Language
Language
1
Dog analogy: Many differences in detail across breeds
and individual dogs. BUT – the deeper truth is that they
are all built on the same body plan and have far more in
common than the superficial differences suggest. 2
What are some features common to all dogs?
1. Social animals
2. Territorial
3. Omnivorous but with a strong preference
for meat
4. Same basic configuration of skeleton
5. Same number and basic shape of teeth
6. Any dog will breed with any other dog,
regardless of large differences in size and
general appearance
7. … (a large number of other features)
3
The Structure of Language
Language works this way as well. All human
languages are built on the same basic design
plan. The broad design features that all
languages have in common run deeper and are
far more important than the differences in
details, as large as those difference may at first
appear.
4
Languages are defined by their grammars – a
collection of rules that allow a speaker (or signer
in the case of a sign language) to generate well-
formed utterances (and the knowledge to
recognize “broken” utterances when they are
encountered).
Your knowledge of English grammar allows you
to figure out:
* = ungrammatical
5
Notes:
(1)A sentence can be perfectly meaningful but still
be ungrammatical:
*This is a four doors car.
*He drove a red big car.
It’s perfectly clear what these sentences mean, but
they are ungrammatical.
(2) The word grammatical here does not mean the
same thing that it meant in grade school.
She ain’t got no crayons.
Where were you at, John. We was waitin’ on you.
6
These sentences conform to the grammar of
some dialects of English; they happen not to
conform to the dialect of standard English.
There are two very different uses of the term
grammar:
(1)descriptive grammar: rules that real speakers
actually use, no matter what teachers, parents, or
“usage experts” say.
(2)prescriptive grammar: rules that English
teachers (and other “experts”), and sometimes
your parents, believe speakers OUGHT to use.
7
Examples of prescriptive grammar rules:
8
Violating a rule of descriptive grammar means that the
utterance would be considered ungrammatical to any
mature (i.e., not a little twirp) native speaker of any dialect.
*That book looks alike.
*I Am America And So Can You. (book title, Stephen
Colbert)
*Frank seems sleeping.
*Bag garbage no good; ski good.
*I did not know how should I dress. (very common
among non-native English speakers)
*I'll be whatever I wanna do. (Philip J. Fry)
*People said I was dumb, but I proved them.
–Philip J. Fry (again)
*I’m going to turn you onto a Poindexter. [Poindexter=nerd]
9
*I am a new tie wearing. (Homer Imposter)
NOTES
(1)All of these sentences would be ungrammatical to
a native speaker in all dialects of English; i.e., there
is more going on here than someone speaking
nonstandard English. This is what makes them
violations of descriptive rules.
(2)The issue is grammaticality; i.e., utterances can be
easy to understand but still ungrammatical.
The science of linguistics is concerned exclusively
with descriptive grammar, not prescriptive grammar.
So, descriptive grammar is the domain of linguistics,
prescriptive grammar is the domain of (mostly self-
appointed and often not very knowledgeable) “usage experts”.
10
More about this later when we discuss dialect.
To do on your own: Which of these sentences violate
English prescriptive rules and which violate
descriptive rules?
Phil has a three-legs cat.
[Its] 5-year mission: to explore strange new worlds, …
to boldly go where no man has gone before.
Badges? We don't need no stinkin’ badges. (The Treasure of
the Sierra Madre)
What Myron has done with my star fish? [descriptive; “What has
Myron done with my star fish?”]
12
One more point: the term grammar is sometimes
used to refer specifically to syntax (word-order
rules), but more recently it refers to all of the rules
of the language, including syntax, semantics
(meaning), morphology (rules for creating words
out of smaller units called morphemes; e.g., to
form walking from walk, readable from read, etc.),
and phonology (sound pattern rules). More later.
13
Now, finally, back to the two uses of the word
language:
•a specific language (English, Dutch, Hungarian,
etc.)
•the general design structure of all human
languages
The 1st meaning is simple and obvious, but what
about the 2nd? What features do all human
languages have in common? Called the Universal
Grammar – it’s a huge, gimongous list (and
incomplete).
14
Here a just a few universal rules:
1. Rules are always structure dependent. E.g.,
English question formation:
John will run. [statement]
Will John run? [question: invert subject & predicate]
Question is formed by reversing the order of the
subject & the predicate – subject & predicate
being structural properties.
One more:
Hanley is the most stubborn person in the
department. [statement]
Is Hanley is the most stubborn person in the 15
department? [question]
How about this hypothetical rule: Form a question
by reversing the order of the last two words in the
sentence.
Hanley is the most stubborn person in the
department.
Hanley is the most stubborn person in
department the?
John will pitch on Thursday.
John will pitch Thursday on?
(1) Not a rule of English; (2) not a structure-
dependent rule; and, most important: (3) no rule
remotely like this in any language, yet this rule
would work just fine. 16
Q: Why is there no rule like this in any of the world’s
~6,000 languages?
A: Because the hypothetical rule violates a rule of the
universal grammar.
Q: Which one? (Hint: I’ve only introduced one
universal rule, so it’s probably that one.)
A: All of the rules of all of languages are structure
dependent. The hypothetic rule (form a question by
reversing the order of the last two words) depends on
serial position, not structure.
(Once again, I have not given you a formal definition of what a
‘structural property’ is. A useful way to think about it is that
structural properties are all those English-major things: whether a
verb is transitive or intransitive, parts of speech, independent vs.
subordinate clauses, subjects vs. predicates, etc. All these – and
17
many others – are examples of structural properties. Serial
One more example:
The soldier that is ill is in the hospital.
How do we make a question of this? Which of the
two instances of is gets moved?
*Is the soldier that ill is in the hospital? (Move the 1st one? Not good.)
19
No child ever makes the mistake of getting mixed
up about which verb to move.
Why? Because they come into the world knowing
that rules are structure dependent and not
dependent on something like serial position –
though serial-position rules would work fine.
More about the significance of this point soon.
20
2. Nearly all languages have agreement rules.
The box is in my office.
The boxes are in my office.
Subject and predicate agree for number (plural vs.
singular).
Languages vary a lot in what kinds of things there
needs to be agreement on. Not all languages
enforce agreement on number, but all except a
very few languages incorporate lots of
agreement rules.
21
Spanish (and French and many other languages)
enforce agreement on gender:
los perros (dogs), las casas (houses), los
árboles (trees), las tablas (tables), las flores
(flowers), las montañas (mountains)
Important: Agreement is not a necessary feature.
•It’s quite easy to imagine a language w/ out
agreement.
•“My shoes are in the closet.”: In English, number
is already specified by the noun (“shoes” in this
case). Why give exactly the same information by
supplying a plural verb (“are”) to go along with a
noun that you already know is plural? 22
• Not all languages have this particular form of
agreement, but all except a very few languages
have agreement rules.
• Is subject-verb number agreement part of the
universal grammar? [No]
• How about agreement for gender as in French
and Spanish? [No]
• What is it that’s (very nearly) universal?
[Agreement rules]
23
3. Phonological rules: All languages incorporate
sound-pattern rules called phonological rules.
• beed – beat
• bid – bit
• league – leak
• cub – cup
• cab – cap
• lag – lack
What do you notice about the lengths of the
vowels on the left vs. those on the right? Rule:
Vowels are lengthened when they precede
voiced consonants.
24
Most languages do not have this particular rule.
However, all languages have large numbers of
sound-pattern rules like this one.
25
Another rule in English. Look at these plural forms:
walks
lips
rats
tracks
--------------
labs
dogs
awards
doors
What sound is added to form the plural in the 1st
group vs. the 2nd group? Orthographically, it’s
always an ‘s’, but what sound is it? (Note: [] --
26
as opposed to [læbz] – is not impossible to
Languages don’t necessarily need to incorporate
phonological rules – though all of them do.
They can’t be essential to communication:
•Nearly every language besides English gets by fine
without the vowel-lengthening rule.
•English gets by fine without the very different set of
sound pattern rules in Spanish, Tamil, Hindi, Korean,
etc.
All languages incorporate phonological rules.
•Is the incorporation of phonological rules part of the
universal grammar?
•Is the vowel lengthening rule (e.g., “cab” vs. “cap”)
27
part of the universal grammar?
4. Recursion
All languages exhibit a property called recursion.
Recursion is a general principle that can be seen in
many areas other than language. In general, recursion is
seen whenever “things” can be embedded inside of
other “things,” which in turn can be embedded inside of
other “things,” which can be …
Simple example: Russian Nesting Dolls (nesting is
another word for embedding).
28
Russian Dolls
31
Ketchup Squeeze Bottle
This example is a little weird (though not hard
to understand). This is an ordinary ketchup
bottle. But notice that the picture on the bottle
shows a lovely waitress holding a tray with a
ketchup bottle on it.
Imagine the unlikely situation in which the
ketchup bottle on the tray is drawn accurately.
If it is, then the bottle on the tray must have a
picture of a waitress holding a tray with a
ketchup bottle on it.
And what about that ketchup bottle? It should
also have a picture of a ketchup bottle on the
tray, right? How far does this recursion go? It
boggles the mind, doesn’t it?
32
Does this ketchup bottle (under the crazy
assumptions) show recursion? (Hint: Yes)
How so? What, if anything, does the ketchup
bottle have in common with the Russian
Nesting Dolls?
What does this have to do with the universal
grammar?
The idea we’re heading toward is simple (sort
of): The grammars of all languages differ in
many details, but they ALL exhibit recursion.
VP
V NP PP
put Det N P NP
the garage
34
An example of recursion in language:
This is the house that Jack built.
Recursion: “This is the house” branches off into the
phrase “that Jack built.”
This is the malt that lay in the house that Jack built.
More recursion: “This is the malt” branches into “that lay
in the house” which branches into “that Jack built.”
This is the rat that ate the malt that lay in the house that
Jack built.
Same thing, but still more of it: A phrase branches into a
phrase which branches into a phrase …
37
Is recursion specific to English or is it part
of the universal grammar?
38
5. Head First/Head Last (I will not go over this in class; read it on your own)
Phrases in (almost) all languages contain a special
“boss” word called the head. The head controls
grammatical features of other words in the phrase.
The fox in socks is in the yard.
*The fox in socks are in the yard.
“fox” is singular, “socks” is plural. Why is it that the verb
agrees with the “fox rather than “socks”? Because it’s
the “boss” word; i.e., the head of the noun phrase “fox
in socks.”
Flying out of Kalamazoo on small planes is scary.
*Flying out of Kalamazoo on small planes are
scary.
“Flying” here is the head of the phrase because the
phrase is mainly about flying, not planes, so the verb
39
agrees with the singular “flying”, not the plural
English is a head-first language – the head precedes all
other words in the phrase. Many other languages reverse
this.
41
There is no reason that languages have to behave
this way.
It is easy to imagine a language that uses Japanese-
like head-last NPs along with English-like PPs:
Kazu sushi ate at home. (head-last NP, head-last
PP)
Or the other way around:
Kazu ate sushi home at. (head-1st NP, head-last
PP)
These mixed rule systems don’t happen. Ever. Why?
It’s more logical? Languages do not work this way.
• I say
• You say (formerly Thou sayest) 42
• He says
[SKIP]
One more example that we saw earlier.
He threw the garbage out.
He threw it out. [it’s ok to substitute a pronoun for
the noun phrase]
He threw out the garbage. [this ordering is ok too]
*
He threw out it. [here it’s not ok to substitute the
pronoun]
Is there anything logical about this system?
So, we can rule out “the-brain-wants-language-to-
be-logical” as an explanation for the
head-first/head-last universal.
43
So, how did we get these grammatical regularities
that comprise the universal grammar (the five we
discussed and a zillion others)?
1. By coincidence, 6,000 separate human
languages happened to adopt these
regularities – without benefit of committee
meetings.
2. Neural circuitry incorporating these
grammatical regularities are built into the brain
– just like the neural circuitry that allows a bat
to convert sonar signals into an image is wired
into bat brains, or the circuitry that allows
spiders to know how to spin a web is wired into
spider brains. 44
Why is the concept of a universal grammar
important? Current thinking among most
linguists:
When children acquire language they do not need
to learn the universal grammar at all. They already
know it.
Children do not need to learn that there are
agreement rules; they need to learn what those
specific agreement rules are.
They do not need to learn that rules are structure
dependent; they do, however, need to learn what
those structure-dependent rules are.
45
Kids do not need to learn that there are sound-
pattern rules; they do need to learn which
particular sound-pattern rules apply to the
language they are learning.
They do not need to learn about the concept of
recursion, but they do need to learn the language-
specific rules that constrain exactly how
recursion occurs. For example, the odd rule that
blocks subject-predicate inversion in
I wasn’t sure what I should do.
is specific to English. Recursion is not. Recursion
is part of the Universal Grammar (and an
extremely important part of it). 46
The Modularity of Language
Central feature of language: It is a layered or
modularized system – the neural substrate for
language is not a blob of brain tissue that
“knows” about language. It is a collection of
interconnected specialists that know a great
deal about just one thing.
Modularity is another term for division of labor –
different modules specialize for different jobs.
This is similar in principle to the way a house is
built – not by a bunch of people who all know
the same thing, but by specialists like
plumbers, electricians, carpenters, roofers,
47
masons, roofers, …
Modularity
Modularity characterizes all complex systems. A
car is not a mass of metal and plastic that
knows how to go. Cars have specialized
modules:
• fuel delivery system (carburetor/fuel injector)
• combustion cylinders
• transmission
• suspension/steering system
• brakes
• exhaust system
• mp3 player
• etc. 48
The human body is modularized. It’s not a blob of
protoplasm that knows how to live – it’s a highly
interconnected system of specialists that each
handle just one kind of job:
• Circulatory system (pump, veins, arteries, etc.)
• Waste management system (kidneys, liver, poo
disposal)
• Central control system (brain, spinal cord, …)
• Musculo-skeletal system
• Sensors (visual, auditory, tactile, …)
• etc.
The heart does not know how to think, the brain
does not know how to pump, the kidneys do
not know how to secrete insulin. Each
specialist has its own act and they stick to it. 49
Language is modularized – it’s a highly
interconnected collection of experts, each of
which handles just one kind of analysis.
Major modules of the language system:
• Semantics (meaning)
• Syntax (structural rules governing word order)
http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=d0BuKYiwhVQ
• Lexicon (mental dictionary)
• Morphology (word-making rules – walk, walked,
walking, …)
• Phonology (sound-pattern rules)
• Phonetics (articulation/sound patterns)
50
The layers of the language system are
interconnected but DISTINCT or AUTONOMOUS –
i.e., different from one another.
A few examples:
Syntax and semantics are not the same thing.
*Colorless green ideas sleep furiously.
(Chomsky)
53
Humor is often derived from semantic clash.
It's easy to quit smoking. I've done it hundreds of times.
-Mark Twain
Wagner’s music is better than it sounds. -Mark Twain
I wish I had an answer to that because I'm tired of
answering that question. -Yogi Berra
Bart, with $10,000 we'd be millionaires. -Homer S.
Nobody goes there anymore, it’s too crowded. -Yogi B.
The report of my death was an exaggeration. –M. Twain
If the people don’t want to come out to the ballpark,
nobody’s going to stop them. –Yogi Berra
My girlfriend left me and moved in with another guy, so I
dumped her. That’s where I draw the line. –G. Shandling
54
• In all of these cases one part of an utterance
contradicts (clashes with) another part, as in
Chomsky’s “Colorless green ideas sleep
furiously.”
55
Phonology (sound pattern rules) and the lexicon (mental
dictionary) are distinct from one another; e.g.,
brick – blick – bnick
• brick: a word
• blick: a non-word, but conforms to English
phonological rules that constrain word shapes
• bnick: a non-word that violates an English
phonological rule that constrains sound sequences
These examples prove specialization, or modularity:
“brick”: Your lexicon specialist tells you this is a word.
“blick”: Your lexicon specialist tells you this is not a
word, but your phonology specialist tells you it could
be.
“bnick”: Lexicon specialist: not a word; phonology
specialist: not a permissible word.
56
“bnick” violates a particular type of
phonological rule is called a phonotactic rule.
phono = sound (speech sound in this case)
tactic = arrangement or ordering
So, phonotactic rules impose limits on
permissible and impermissible arrangements
of speech sounds.
On my web page, there’s a link called
phonotactic rule assignment. It’s due one
week from today.
57
All languages have large numbers of phonotactic
rules. A few others in English:
Words cannot begin with /ŋ/ (/no/ is ok but not /ŋo/)
Words cannot begin with /ʃt/ (/stɑp/ is ok but not /ʃtɑp/).
Words cannot end with /ɛ, ɪ, ʊ/ (/be/, /bi/, and /bu/ are ok
but not /bɛ/, /bɪ/, or /bʊ/.
Terminology: The rules above are called
phonotactic phonological rules, or (more often)
just phonotactic rules.
The other kinds that we talked about (e.g., vowels
are lengthened before voiced consonants) we’ll
simply refer to as non-phonotactic phonological
rules. We’ll discuss many other examples. 58
Morphology: Rules for word formation (e.g., dog dogs;
walk walking)
If boof were a word, what kind would boofable be (noun,
verb, adjective, etc.)?
How can the word understand (verb) be turned into noun?
He used to live in Pakistan, so he has a good
_________ of cricket. (This is a bogus sentence. No one understands
cricket.)
60
Bound vs. Unbound: Morphemes can be either bound
or unbound (also called free).
Unbound (free): Can stand alone as a separate word
(e.g., dog, walk, park, …). Unbound morphemes are
also called free.
Bound: Must appear in combination with one or more
other morphemes; e.g., suffixes like -s, –ed, -ing;
prefixes such as pre-, post-, un-, etc.
premature: pre=bound, mature=unbound
blindness: blind=unbound, ness=bound
Check out the exercise on my web page called
Counting Morphemes.
61
One more point about morphemes: The concept of a
morpheme is pretty straightforward, but counting them is
not always so simple. A few examples:
How many morphemes in these?
uniform Do speakers realize this word is
derived from morphemes meaning one
form? Maybe, but I doubt it.
Do not count:
Dysfluencies, except for most complete form C-c-c-candy, bab-baby
63
Fillers Um-m, ah-h, oh
Last one: A specific sound or sound sequence can
sometimes behave as a morpheme and sometimes not.
worker: ‘er’ (/ɚ/) is a morpheme (‘er’ indicates one who works)
player: ‘er’ (/ɚ/) is a morpheme
sitter: ‘er’ (/ɚ/) is a morpheme
splatter: ‘er’ (/ɚ/) is not a morpheme (‘er’ does not mean
one who splats)
matter: ‘er’ (/ɚ/) is not a morpheme (‘er’ does not mean
one who mats)
(1)
phonological (abstract) layer or underlying
representation, or phonemic layer) – abstract
representation in your head – e.g., you “think”
of the medial sound in “batter” as a /t/.
(2) phonetic layer or surface phonetic form – the
sounds that are actually spoken ([ɾ]in
the example). 66
Rule phonemic (phonological,
/bætɚ/ linguistic, underlying
/t,d/ [ɾ] representation)
when intervocalic,
(betw two vowels) Phonological
but only when the
2nd vowel is Rules
unstressed
69
How to tell whether two speech sounds are
members of the same or different
phoneme class
Are /p/ and /b/ allophones of the same phoneme class, or
are they members of different phoneme classes?
75
Central idea: Contrast.
76
Terminology
In English, [o] and [õ] are:
1. allophonic variants or allophones of /o/
2. phonetically different but phonemically/
phonologically/linguistically equivalent
In French, are [o] and [õ] both phonetically distinct
and phonemically/phonologically/linguistically
distinct: /o/ and /õ/.
Yet another way to say exactly the same thing: In
French, vowel nasalization serves a contrastive
function; i.e., nasalization by itself can serve to
distinguish one word from another. In English, it
does not. 77
Another Example: Tone Languages
In tone languages, the meaning of a word can change
depending on the shape of the pitch contour (intonation).
Mandarin Chinese is just one of many examples of tone
languages.
/mɑ/-tone 1: mother
/mɑ/-tone 2: hemp
/mɑ/-tone 3: horse
/mɑ/-tone 4: scold (Source: http://mandarin.about.com/od/pronunciation/a/tones.htm)
79
Last Example: Phonemic Vowel Duration
There are many languages in which differences in vowel
duration (also called length) are phonemic (Japanese,
Czech, Hawaiian, Finnish, Swedish, Thai, many others) .
Examples from Finnish:
taka (/tɑkɑ/) – back
takaa (/tɑkɑː/) – from behind
this diacritic means ‘long vowel’
Q: Is vowel duration phonemic in Finnish?
A: Yes.
Q: How do you know?
A: We found a minimal pair: a pair of words in which the
distinction is conveyed by a difference in vowel duration
(taka- takaa).
80
Phonemic Vowel Duration (cont’d)
81
Summary
89
/t/
(abstract phoneme type)
English French
/o/ /o/ /õ/
96
A Really Short Story
Bill: I’m leaving you.
Louise: Who is she?
What is the story underlying this conversation?
What do you think Bill means by “leaving”?
Running out to gas up his car? Picking up the dry
cleaning?
How are you able to reconstruct a story based on
two 3- to 4-word sentences?
Is it your linguistic knowledge that allows
understand what is going on here?
97
Language vs. Speech
Last point: I’ve been talking about language and speech
as though they were the same thing. Not.
All speech is language, but not all language is speech.
Two major counterexamples:
• Written language (different in one major way from
spoken language – IT NEEDS TO BE EXPLICITLY
TAUGHT. But it’s still language.)
• Sign language (ASL and all other sign languages)
Sign language is not a stripped down or impoverished
version of spoken language. It conforms to the universal
grammar and contains all of the elements of spoken
language (except sound): structure-dependent rules,
agreement rules, recursion, even movement analogs of
phonological rules and gestural analogs of babbling. It
98
is full-blown language, not a cheap imitation.