The lecture covers sound localization, detailing how interaural time differences (ITD) and interaural level differences (ILD) help determine the direction of sounds. It discusses the physiological mechanisms involved in processing these cues, including the roles of the medial and lateral superior olives. Additionally, it addresses auditory distance perception and the complexities of sound, including timbre and the illusion of the missing fundamental.
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PPTX, PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
0 ratings0% found this document useful (0 votes)
2 views
Lecture10 Hearing
The lecture covers sound localization, detailing how interaural time differences (ITD) and interaural level differences (ILD) help determine the direction of sounds. It discusses the physiological mechanisms involved in processing these cues, including the roles of the medial and lateral superior olives. Additionally, it addresses auditory distance perception and the complexities of sound, including timbre and the illusion of the missing fundamental.
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PPTX, PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
You are on page 1/ 43
Lecture 2:
Lecture 10: Hearing
PSYC 2220 M: Sensation & Perception I
CB 121, Fridays, 11:30 AM Instructor: Raphael Gastrock Sound localization Sound localization • How do we know where the sound is coming from? • Depending on the source of the sound: • Sound reaches each ear at different timepoints • Sounds reaching the closer ear will be more intense Interaural time difference (ITD) • The difference in time that a sound arrives at one ear versus the other Interaural time difference (ITD) • Imagine a circle extending in a horizontal plane around your head (azimuth) Interaural time difference (ITD) • Maximum difference is around 640 µs • Humans can detect differences as small as 10 µs = 1 degree in azimuth threshold ITD physiology • Input from auditory nerves in each ear reach the brain stem • Medial Superior Olive (MSO) is the first binaural stage of the auditory system • Firing rates of neurons in MSO increase when they receive ITD inputs – ITDs encoded in MSO ITD physiology Ladder theory • Differences between the lengths of axons coming from the two ears provide a time delay to detect small time differences • No real evidence for this ITD physiology Interaural frequency differences • It takes time for sound wave to travel from high to low frequencies along basilar membrane of cochlea • Differences in frequencies across two ears are used to measure time Interaural level difference (ILD) • The difference between intensities of sound at one ear versus the other • Sounds are more intense at the ear closer to the source, as the head partially blocks sound pressure waves Interaural level difference (ILD) • Properties for ILD have a lot of similarities with ITD • Sounds are more intense at the ear closer to the source • ILD is largest directly to the right or left of the ears (+/- 90 degrees in azimuth). There is 0 ILD when source is directly in front or behind • ILD correlates with the angle of sound source. BUT, the shape of your head makes this less precise than in ITDs Interaural level difference (ILD) • However, the head blocks high-frequency sounds more than low- frequency sounds • Occurs for frequencies higher than 1000 Hz • Low-frequency sounds wrap around the head Interaural level difference (ILD) • Question: You just purchased a new sound system for your living room, which includes a subwoofer. Where is it optimal to place the subwoofer? • Subwoofer plays very low (bass) frequencies • Since low frequencies do not produce ILDs, it doesn’t matter where the subwoofer is placed ILD physiology • Lateral superior olive (LSO) process intensity differences between the two ears • Excitatory connections to LSO come from ipsilateral ear • Inhibitory connections from contralateral ear Cones of confusion • Notice how ITDs (and ILDs) can be the same from some positions (e.g. 60 and 120 degrees) • A region of positions in space where all sounds produce the same time and intensity differences Cones of confusion • Sounds do not just come from the same plane as azimuth, but also has elevation • This leads to a whole region (cone) where you may confuse the source of the sound • Solution: turn your head • ITDs and ILDs will shift; compare before and after turning Pinnae and head cues • Another cue for sound localization • Pinna funnels sound energy into ear canal • Shape of pinna determines which frequencies are funneled more efficiently than others • Shape of upper body also affects which frequencies reach each ear more easily • Intensity of each frequency varies with direction (sound intensity vs. direction) • This variation provides the other cue for localization • Intensity changes measured with directional transfer function (DTF) Pinnae and head cues • Directional transfer function (DTF) • Measure that describes how the pinna, ear canal, head, and torso change the intensity of sounds with different frequencies that arrive at each ear from different locations in space (both azimuth and elevation) Pinnae and head cues • Directional transfer function (DTF) • How it is measured: • Participant sits in an anechoic room (room where floors and walls are padded – sounds cannot bounce off the walls) • Speakers located in all directions relative to listener (up, down, all around) • Tiny microphones placed near tympanic membrane (eardrum) • Play sounds from different places in the room • Measure how much energy from different frequencies reach the eardrum Pinnae and head cues • Directional transfer function (DTF) Result: • Although the speaker produced the same intensity for different frequencies • Notice how intensities differed at the eardrum for different frequencies • But, we also add in elevation Pinnae and head cues • Directional transfer function (DTF) Result: • If we move the speaker up or down, we see changes in intensities of different frequencies (as both elevation and azimuth changes) The point is: • Relative intensity of different frequencies creates a sense of direction Pinnae and head cues • Directional transfer function (DTF) • Is different for different people (unique/ individualized) • Changes throughout development (head and ear size change, ears can change shape) Pinnae and head cues • Directional transfer function (DTF) • Listening to music in a live concert versus in-ear headphones • Live concert: can pinpoint where the instrument is being played from (piano to the left, guitar from the right) • In-ear headphones: while it is possible to mimic ITDs and ILDs, DTFs are not simulated (pinnae, ear canal, head and body shape are bypassed) • Music from headphones sound like they are coming from inside Pinnae and head cues • Directional transfer function (DTF) • Can we create a sense of direction in recordings? • Binaural recording • Two microphones placed at ear drums, sound source is recorded in these microphones • Play the recording and listener perceives sound as if they were coming from the outside world • Works better if tailored to one’s unique DTF http://courseevaluations.yo rku.ca Auditory distance perception • Although we are good at determining sound direction, we are not as good at judging sound distances • We are best at judging the distance to a sound source when it is about 1 meter away • < 1 m = overestimate distance • > 1 m = underestimate distance • We rely on different cues to judge distances of sound sources Auditory distance perception • Cue # 1: relative intensity • Analogous to relative size in visual depth perception • Sounds become less intense with greater distances • For example, louder croak of a frog would likely mean that the frog is closer • Downside is that it opens you up to mistakes • A softer-sounding croak could come from a nearby frog Auditory distance perception • Cue # 1: relative intensity • But, note that we are good at determining sound distances ~1 m away only • Inverse-square law • As distance from a source increases, intensity decreases faster (decrease in intensity = distance squared) Auditory distance perception • Cue # 2: motion • The sound source can move towards you, or you can move through the environment • Idea is similar to motion parallax in the visual system • Sounds farther away do not seem to change direction as much as nearer sounds Auditory distance perception • Cue # 3: spectral composition of sounds • The air in the atmosphere absorbs high- frequencies more than low-frequencies • When a sound is farther away, higher frequencies decrease in energy more than lower frequencies • We hear sounds that are far away as “muddier” • Change in spectral composition is only noticeable for distances larger than 1000 m Auditory distance perception • Cue # 4: direct and reverberant energy • The sounds we hear can come directly from the source (direct) or bounce off some surfaces in the environment (reverberant) • Closer sound sources will be mostly direct energy, but sound sources farther away will be more reverberant energy • We compare the relative amounts of direct and reverberant energy to judge distance Spatial hearing and blindness • Does your ability to localize sounds in space become better with severe loss of vision? • Yes, with echolocation • Produce sounds which bounce back to the person, and this gives them information about the environment • Some blind people make clicks to sense if something is in their way • Expert echolocators can distinguish sizes, shapes, materials of objects Spatial hearing and blindness • Echolocation • As visual areas in blind people are inactive, echolocators actually use these areas to process auditory input Complex sounds • Recall harmonic spectrum from previous lecture • Spectrum of a complex sound in which energy is at multiples of the fundamental frequency • The fundamental frequency is the lowest • Thefrequency pitch perceived is component of a complex thesound fundamental frequency • Higher harmonics add to the perceived richness, or timbre, of sound (called overtones by musicians) Complex sounds • Illusion of the missing fundamental • If we remove the fundamental frequency and only play the harmonics, we still perceive the same pitch Complex sounds • Illusion of the missing fundamental • Explained by temporal code • All harmonics of a fundamental have common fluctuations in sound pressure at regular intervals corresponding to the fundamental frequency • Auditory nerve and cochlear neurons will fire at every period Complex sounds • Timbre • The ability to distinguish two sounds, even if the sounds have the same loudness and pitch • A trombone and a tenor saxophone can play the exact same note (same fundamental frequency) at the exact same intensity, but we can still tell the sound of these two instruments apart • This is driven by the shape of the harmonics in Complex sounds • Attack and Decay • Attack: The way a complex sound begins (amplitude increases at onset) • Decay: The way it ends (amplitude decreases at offset) Takeaways • Sound localization • Interaural time difference • Interaural level difference • Cones of confusion • Pinnae and head cues • Directional transfer function • Auditory distance perception • 4 cues discussed • Echolocation • Complex sounds • Harmonics, missing fundamental, timbre, attack, decay Thank you so much for your participation in the course!
Final Exam details:
April 16, 2025 (Wednesday) 19:00 (7:00 PM) ACW 206 3 hours to complete (180 minutes) Bring writing materials and student ID
(Ebook) Cochlear Hearing Loss: Physiological, Psychological and Technical Issues, Second Edition by Brian C.J. Moore(auth.) ISBN 9780470516331, 9780470987889, 047051633X, 047098788X - The ebook in PDF format is ready for download