Learning and Perception
Learning and Perception
1. Classical Conditioning?
Discovered by Russian physiologist Ivan Pavlov, classical
conditioning is a type of unconscious or automatic learning.
This learning process creates a conditioned response through
associations between an unconditioned stimulus and a
neutral stimulus
One of the best-known examples of classical conditioning is Pavlov's
classic experiments with dogs. In these experiments, the neutral signal
was the sound of a tone and the naturally occurring reflex was
salivating in response to food. By associating the neutral stimulus
(sound) with the unconditioned stimulus (food), the sound of the tone
alone could produce a salivation response
Key Principles of Classical Conditioning
Behaviorists have described a number of different phenomena
associated with classical conditioning. Some of these elements
involve the initial establishment of the response while others
describe the disappearance of a response. Here is a closer look
at five key principles of classical conditioning.
Acquisition
Acquisition is the initial stage of learning, when a response is
first established and gradually strengthened. During the
acquisition phase of classical conditioning, a neutral stimulus is
repeatedly paired with an unconditioned stimulus.
As you may recall, an unconditioned stimulus is something that
naturally and automatically triggers a response without any learning.
After an association is made, the subject will begin to emit a behavior
in response to the previously neutral stimulus, which is now known as
a conditioned stimulus. It is at this point that we can say that the
response has been acquired.
Once the response has been established, you can gradually reinforce
the response to make sure the behavior is well learned.
Extinction
Extinction is when the occurrences of a conditioned response decrease or
disappear. In classical conditioning, this happens when a conditioned
stimulus is no longer paired with an unconditioned stimulus.
For example, if the smell of food (the unconditioned stimulus) had been
paired with the sound of a whistle (the conditioned stimulus), the sound of
the whistle would eventually come to evoke the conditioned response of
hunger.
However, if the smell of food were no longer paired with the whistle,
eventually the conditioned response (hunger) would disappear.
Spontaneous Recovery
Sometimes a learned response can suddenly re-emerge, even after a
period of extinction. This is called spontaneous recovery.
For example, imagine that after training a dog to salivate to the sound
of a bell, you stop reinforcing the behavior and the response becomes
extinct. After a rest period during which the conditioned stimulus is not
presented, you ring the bell and the animal spontaneously recovers the
previously learned response.
If the conditioned stimulus and unconditioned stimulus are no longer
associated, extinction will return very rapidly after a spontaneous
recovery.
Generalization
Stimulus generalization is the tendency for a conditioned stimulus to
evoke similar responses after the response has been conditioned.
For example, if a dog has been conditioned to salivate at the sound of a
bell, the animal may also exhibit the same response to a sound that's
similar to the bell.
In John B. Watson's famous Little Albert Experiment, for example, a
small child was conditioned to fear a white rat. The child demonstrated
stimulus generalization by also exhibiting fear in response to other
fuzzy white objects, including stuffed toys and Watson's own hair.
Discrimination
Discrimination is the ability to differentiate between a conditioned
stimulus and other stimuli that have not been paired with an
unconditioned stimulus.
For example, if a bell tone were the conditioned stimulus,
discrimination would involve being able to tell the difference between
the bell tone and other similar sounds. Because the subject is able to
distinguish between these stimuli, they will only respond when the
conditioned stimulus is presented.
Operant Conditioning
Operant conditioning was first described by behaviorist
B.F. Skinner, which is why you may occasionally hear it
referred to as Skinnerian conditioning.
As a behaviorist, Skinner believed that it was not really
necessary to look at internal thoughts and motivations in
order to explain behavior. Instead, he suggested, we
should look only at the external, observable causes of
human behavior.
Operant conditioning, sometimes referred to as
instrumental conditioning, is a method of learning that employs
rewards and punishments for behavior. Through operant conditioning,
an association is made between a behavior and a consequence (whether
negative or positive) for that behavior.
For example, when lab rats press a lever when a green light is on, they
receive a food pellet as a reward. When they press the lever when a red
light is on, they receive a mild electric shock. As a result, they learn to
press the lever when the green light is on and avoid the red light.
Skinner used the term operant to refer to any "active
behavior that operates upon the environment to generate
consequences." Skinner's theory explained how we acquire
the range of learned behaviors we exhibit every day.
Types of Behaviors
Skinner distinguished between two different types of behaviors