0% found this document useful (0 votes)
26 views145 pages

Brain Development

Download as ppt, pdf, or txt
Download as ppt, pdf, or txt
Download as ppt, pdf, or txt
You are on page 1/ 145

Brain development

Nature and nurture


Outline

• Part 1: Brain development: A


macroscopic perspective
• Part 2: The development of the cerebral
cortex
• Part 3: Nature and nurture
Part I
Brain development: A macroscopic
perspective
Part I
Brain development: A macroscopic
perspective
See Chronology of Prenatal
Development
p. 110-111
3-4 Weeks
3-4 Weeks
Neural Groove
3-4 Weeks
Neural Groove

Neural Tube
3-4 Weeks
Neural Groove

Neural Tube

Neuroepitheliu
m
3-4 Weeks
Neural Groove

Neural Tube

Neuroepitheliu
m
Brain

Spinal Chord
5 to 6 Weeks
Nervous system begins to function
Hind-, mid-, and forebrain are now distinguishable
5 to 6 Weeks
5 to 6 Weeks
5 to 6 Weeks
Forebrain
5 to 6 Weeks
Forebrain

Telencephalon
5 to 6 Weeks
Forebrain

Telencephalon

Diencephalon
5 to 6 Weeks
Forebrain
5 to 6 Weeks
Forebrain

Midbrain
5 to 6 Weeks
Forebrain

Midbrain

Hindbrain
•Neurons forming rapidly
•1000’s per minute

7 Weeks
Division of the halves of the brain visible

14 Weeks
7 Weeks
•Nerve cell generation complete
•Cortex beginning to wrinkle
•Myelinization

6 Months
14 Weeks
7 Weeks
9 Months

5 Months
14 Weeks
7 Weeks
Telencephalon: C-shaped growth
Cortex: Folding

9 Months

5 Months
14 Weeks
7 Weeks
Telencephalon: C-shaped growth
Cortex: Folding

9 Months

5 Months
14 Weeks
7 Weeks
9 Months
9 Months
9 Months

Medulla

Hindbrain Pons

Cerebellum
9 Months

Medulla

Hindbrain Pons

Cerebellum
9 Months

Medulla

Hindbrain Pons

Cerebellum
9 Months

Medulla

Hindbrain Pons

Cerebellum
9 Months Controls respiration, digestion, circulation,
& fine motor control

Medulla

Hindbrain Pons

Cerebellum
9 Months

Midbrain
9 Months
Basic auditory and visual processing

Midbrain
9 Months

Thalamus
Diencephalon
Hypothalamus
9 Months
Sensory relay station
Long-term memory
Intersection of CNS and hormone system

Thalamus
Diencephalon
Hippocampus

Hypothalamus
9 Months

Telencephalon

2 Cerebral hemispheres

Forms a “cap” over inner


brain structures
9 Months

Cross-sectional view
9 Months

Cerebral
Hemispheres

Cross-sectional view
9 Months

Cerebral
Hemispheres

Thalamus

Hypothalamus

Cross-sectional view Hippocampus


9 Months As the telencephalon
develops, it connects both
with itself, and with
the diencephalon

Cross-sectional view
9 Months As the telencephalon
develops, it connects both
with itself, and with
the diencephalon

Corpus Callosum

Internal Capsule

Cross-sectional view
9 Months

Hippocampus
Telencephalon
9 Months Thin layer of cells covering
both hemispheres

Telencephalon Cortex
Cortex
High-level visual processing

Visual Cortex
Cortex Auditory & visual processing
Receptive language

Visual Cortex

Temporal Cortex
Cortex Sensory integration
Visual-motor processing

Visual Cortex

Temporal Cortex

Parietal Cortex
Cortex Higher-level cognition
Motor control
Expressive language

Visual Cortex

Temporal Cortex

Parietal Cortex

Frontal Cortex
Cortical Development

Begins prenatally

Continues into late


adolescence
II: The development of the
cerebral cortex
A microscopic view
Development of the Cortex

• 2 types of cells:
• Neurons
• Glial cells
Development of the Cortex

• 2 types of cells:
• Neurons
• Glial cells
Development of the Cortex

• 2 types of cells: Dendrite


• Neurons
• Glial cells
Development of the Cortex

• 2 types of cells: Dendrite


• Neurons Cell body
• Glial cells
Development of the Cortex

• 2 types of cells: Dendrite


• Neurons Cell body
• Glial cells
Axon
Development of the Cortex

• 2 types of cells: Dendrite


• Neurons Cell body
• Glial cells
Axon

Synapse
Development of the Cortex

• 2 types of cells: Dendrite


• Neurons Cell body
• Glial cells
Axon

Synapse

Transmit information through the brain


Development of the Cortex

• 2 types of cells:
• Neurons
• Glial cells
Outnumber neurons 10:1
Nourish, repair, & mylenate neurons
Crucial for development
Development of the Cortex

• 2 types of cells:
• Neurons
• Glial cells
Outnumber neurons 10:1
Nourish, repair, & myelinate neurons
Crucial for development
Development of the Cortex

• 2 types of cells:
• Neurons
• Glial cells
Outnumber neurons 10:1
Nourish, repair, & myelinate neurons
Crucial for development
Eg. Oligodendroglia
Development of the Cortex

• 2 types of cells:
• Neurons
• Glial cells
Outnumber neurons 10:1
Nourish, repair, & myelinate neurons
Crucial for development
8 stages of cortical development
1. Neural proliferation
2. Neural migration
3. Neural differentiation
4. Axonal growth
5. Dendritic growth
6. Synaptogenesis
7. Myelination
8. Neuronal death
1. Neural
proliferation
• Begins with neural tube
closure
1. Neural
proliferation
• Begins with neural tube
closure
1. Neural
proliferation
• Begins with neural tube
closure
• New cells born in
ventricular layer
1. Neural
proliferation
• Begins with neural tube
closure
• New cells born in
ventricular layer
• 1 mother cell produces ≈
10,000 daughter cells
1. Neural
proliferation
• Begins with neural tube
closure
• New cells born in
ventricular layer
• 1 mother cell produces ≈
10,000 daughter cells
• All neurons (100 billion in
total) are produced pre-
natally
1. Neural
proliferation
• Begins with neural tube closure
• New cells born in ventricular
layer
• 1 mother cell produces ≈
10,000 daughter cells
• All neurons (100 billion in
total) are produced pre-natally
• Rate of proliferation extremely
high; thousands/minute
2: Cellular
migration
• Non-dividing cells
migrate from
ventricular layer
2: Cellular
migration
• Non-dividing cells
migrate from
ventricular layer
• Creates a radial inside-
out pattern of
development
2: Cellular
migration
• Non-dividing cells
migrate from
ventricular layer
• Creates a radial inside-
out pattern of
development
• Importance of radial
glial cells
2: Cellular
migration
• Non-dividing cells
migrate from
ventricular layer
• Creates a radial inside-
out pattern of
development
• Importance of radial
glial cells
3. Cellular
differentiation
• Migrating cells
structurally and
functionally immature
3. Cellular
differentiation
• Migrating cells
structurally and
functionally immature
• Once new cells reach
their destination,
particular genes are
turned growth of
axons, dendrites, and
synapses
4. Axonal growth

• Growth occurs at a growth cone


4. Axonal growth

• Growth occurs at a growth cone

Growth cone
4. Axonal growth
• Growth occurs at a growth cone
• Axons have specific targets
• Targets often enormous distances away
• Some axons extend a distance that is 40,000
times the width of the cell body it is attached
to
• Finding targets ?  chemical & electrical
gradients, multiple branches
5. Dendritic growth
• Usually begins after migration
• Slow
• Occurs at a growth cone
• Begins prenatally, but continues postnatally
• Overproduction of branches in development
and resultant pruning
• Remaining dendrites continue to branch and
lengthen
6.
Synaptogenesis
• Takes place as dendrites
and axons grow
• Involves the linking
together of the billions of
neurons of the brain
6.
Synaptogenesis
• Takes place as dendrites and
axons grow
• Involves the linking together
of the billions of neurons of
the brain
• 1 neuron makes up to 1000
synapses with other neurons
• Neurotransmitters and
receptors also required
Overproliferation and pruning

• The number of synapses reaches a


maximum at about 2 years of age
• After this, pruning begins
• By 16, only half of the original synapses
remain
7:
Myelinization
• The process whereby glial cells wrap
themselves around axons
7:
Myelinization
• The process whereby glial cells wrap
themselves around axons
• Increases the speed of neural conduction
7:
Myelinization
• The process whereby glial cells wrap
themselves around axons
• Increases the speed of conduction
• Begins before birth in primary motor and
sensory areas
• Continues into adolescence in certain
brain regions (e.g., frontal lobes)
8: Neuronal death

• As many as 50% of neurons created in


the first 7 months of life die
• Structure of the brain is a product of
sculpting as much as growth
III: Nature and nurture in brain
development
III: Nature versus nurture
• The adult brain consists of approximately 100
billion (surviving) neurons that make trillions
of synaptic links
• Functionally highly organized, supporting
various perceptual, cognitive and behavioural
processes
• Perhaps the most complex living system we
know
Question
• Of all the information that is required to
assemble a brain, how much is stored in the
genes?
• Nature view: argues that most of the
information is stored in the genes
• Nurture view: brain is structurally and
functionally underspecified by the genes 
emerges probabilistically over the course of
development
Nature View
• (1) Not much is left to chance
Nature View
• (1) Not much is left to chance
• (2) Brain a collection of genetically-specified
modules
Nature View
• (1) Not much is left to chance
• (2) Brain a collection of genetically-specified
modules
• (3) Each module processes a specific kind of
information & works independently of other
modules
Nature View
• (1) Not much is left to chance
• (2) Brain a collection of genetically-specified
modules
• (3) Each module processes a specific kind of
information & works independently of other
modules
• (4) In evolution: modules get added to the
“collection”
Nature View
• (1) Not much is left to chance
• (2)“The
Braingrammar
a collection of genetically-specified
genes would be stretches modules
of
• (3) Each module processes
DNA that code afor
specific kind of
proteins…
information
that guide,& attract,
works independently
or glue neuronsof other modules
together
into
• (4) In evolution: networks
modules that…to the “collection”
get added
• (5) Inare necessarygenes
development: to compute
that codethe
forsolution
modules are
expressedtoand
some grammatical
modules problem.”
develop according to these
instructions
The nature view: Evidence
• Neurogenesis
• Neuroblasts give rise to a
limited number of
daughter cells
• Cells have a genetically
mediated memory that
allows them to remember
how many times they
have divided
The nature view: Evidence
• Genetics and migration
• Mutant or “knock-out”
mice
The nature view: Evidence
• Genetics and migration
• Mutant or “knock-out”
mice
• Cannot produce a class of
proteins called cell
adhesion molecules
(CAM’s)
• Migration is disrupted
because cells cannot attach
to and migrate along glia
The nature view: Evidence
• Growth of dendrites
and axons
• Undeveloped neuron
needs to establish basic
“polarity:” which end
is which?
The nature view: Evidence
• Growth of dendrites
and axons
• Undeveloped neuron
needs to establish basic
“polarity:” which end
is which?
• Involves specific
proteins
The nature view: Evidence
• Growth of dendrites
and axons
• Undeveloped neuron
needs to establish basic
“polarity:” which end
is which?
• Involves specific
proteins
The nature view: Evidence
• Growth of dendrites and
axons
• Undeveloped neuron needs
to establish basic
“polarity:” which end is
which?
• Involves specific proteins
• Axons: Affords a
sensitivity to chemical
signals emitted by targets
The nature view: Evidence
• Growth of dendrites and
axons
• Undeveloped neuron needs
to establish basic
“polarity:” which end is
which?
• Involves specific proteins
• Axons: Affords a
sensitivity to chemical
signals emitted by targets
The nature view: Evidence
• Formation of synapses
• Knock-out mice
The nature view: Evidence
• Formation of synapses
• Knock-out mice
• Staggerer
• Neurons in the cerebellum make contact, but
receptor surface does not develop
• Thus, a single gene deletion can interfere with
the formation of synapses in the cerebellum
The nature view: Evidence
• Cell death
• Cells seem to possess death genes
• When expressed, enzymes are produced that
effectively cut-up the DNA, and kill the cell
• Similar mechanism may control the timing of
neuronal death
Nurture view
• (1) Brain organization is emergent and
probabilistic not pre-determined
• (2) Genes provide only a broad outline of the
ultimate structural and functional organization
of the brain
• (3) Organization emerges in development
through over-production of structure and
competition for survival
•Gerald Edelman: Neural Darwinism
•OverproliferationNurture view
of structures + sensory experience
produce Darwinian-like selection pressures in
development
• (1) Brain organization is emergent and
•Structures that prove useful in development win the
probabilistic not pre-determined
competition for survival
• (2)rest
•The Genes provide
are cast off only a broad outline of the
ultimate structural and functional organization
of the brain
• (3) Organization emerges in development
through over-production of structure and
competition for survival
The “nurture” view:
Evidence
• Does experience affect developing structures
and functions?
• Is the pruning of brain structures systematic?
• Do developing brain regions competitively
interact?
The “nurture” view: Hubel &
Evidence Weisel
• Raised kittens but deprived them of visual
stimulation to both eyes (binocular
deprivation)
• No abnormality in the retina or thalamus
The “nurture” view: Hubel &
Evidence Weisel
• Raised kittens but deprived them of visual stimulation
to both eyes (binocular deprivation)
• No abnormality in the retina or thalamus
• Gross abnormality in visual cortex
• Disrupted protein production caused fewer and shorter
dendrite to develop, as well as 70% fewer synapses
• Effects only occur early in development, but persist
into adulthood
• Example: Surgery on congenital cataracts in adult
humans
The “nurture” view: Hubel &
Evidence Weisel
• Early monocular deprivation
• After restoring stimulation, vision in this eye is
severely impaired
The “nurture” view: Hubel &
Evidence Weisel
• Early monocular deprivation
• After restoring stimulation, vision in this eye is
severely impaired
• One effect: Monocular deprivation disrupted
the establishment of ocular dominance
columns
The “nurture” view: Development of
mammalian
Evidence visual system
Adult structure

Cortex

Thalamus

Eyes/Retinas
The “nurture” view: Development of
mammalian
Evidence visual system
Adult structure

Cortex

Thalamus

Eyes/Retinas
The “nurture” view: Hubel &
Evidence Weisel
• Early monocular deprivation
• After restoring stimulation, vision in this eye is
severely impaired
• Sensory input competes for available cortex
• With input from one eye eliminated, no
competition
• Therefore, input from uncovered eye assumes
control of available visual cortex and disrupts the
establishment of ocular dominance columns
The “nurture” view: Hubel &
Evidence Weisel
• Early monocular deprivation
• After restoring stimulation, vision in this eye is
severely impaired
• Sensory input competes for available cortex
• With input from one eye eliminated, no
Findings point to the importance of
competition
stimulation from the environment
• Therefore, input from uncovered eye assumes
control of available visual cortex and disrupts the
establishment of ocular dominance columns
The “nurture” view: Kratz, Spear, &
Evidence Smith
• Early monocular deprivation
• After restoring stimulation, vision in this eye is
severely impaired

Hubel &
Weise
The “nurture” view: Kratz, Spear, &
Evidence Smith
• Early monocular deprivation
• After restoring stimulation, vision in this eye is
severely impaired
• A second effect: Residual function of the
deprived eye competitively inhibited by strong
eye
The “nurture” view: Kratz, Spear, &
Evidence Smith
• Early monocular deprivation
• After restoring stimulation, vision in this eye is
severely impaired
• A second effect: Residual function of the
deprived eye competitively inhibited by strong
eye
• Deprived one of experience and then removed
strong eye
The “nurture” view: Kratz, Spear, &
Evidence Smith
• Early monocular deprivation
• After restoring stimulation, vision in this eye is
severely impaired
• A second effect: Residual function of the deprived
eye competitively inhibited by strong eye
• Deprived one of experience and then removed
strong eye
• Prior to surgery, stimulation of deprived eye
elicited activity in only 6% of cortical neurons
The “nurture” view: Kratz, Spear, &
Evidence Smith
• Early monocular deprivation
• After restoring stimulation, vision in this eye is
severely impaired
• A second effect: Residual function of the deprived eye
competitively inhibited by strong eye
• Deprived one of experience and then removed strong
eye
• Prior to surgery, stimulation of deprived eye elicited
activity in only 6% of cortical neurons: After
surgery 31%
The “nurture” view: Kratz, Spear, &
Evidence Smith
• Early monocular deprivation
• After restoring stimulation, vision in this eye is
severely impaired
• A second effect: Residual function of the deprived eye
competitively inhibited by normal eye
• Deprived Findings point to the
one of experience andimportance
then removed ofnormal
eye competitive interaction between
developing
• Prior to surgery, brain
stimulation regionseye elicited
of deprived
activity in only 6% of cortical neurons: After
surgery 31%
Impoverish
The “nurture” view:
ed
Evidence
Environme
• Animal raised in impoverished environments
have brains that are 10 to 20% smallernts
than
animal raised in normal environments. Why?
Impoverish
The “nurture” view:
ed
Evidence
Environme
• Animal raised in impoverished environments
have brains that are 10 to 20% smallernts
than
animal raised in normal environments. Why?
• Decreased glial cell density
• Fewer dendritic spines
• Fewer synapses
• Smaller synapses
The “nurture” view:
Evidence
Sur
• Cortical surgery
• Severed connection between optic nerve and
the occipital cortex as well as the connection
between auditory nerve and auditory cortex
• Reconnected optic nerve to auditory cortex
• Animals developed functionally adequate
vision
The “nurture” view:
Evidence
• Daphnia: A crustacean; easily cloned
• Simple nervous system consisting of several
hundred neurons
• Connection patterns can be studied directly
• Genetically identical individuals show
different patterns of neuronal connectivity
Nurture view: Summary
• Order in the brain is not completely specified
by the genes
• Instead, structures and functions emerge
probabilistically in development through the
combined influence of initial over-production
of structure, neural competition, and
experience
Conclusions
• Genes are a critical source of guidance for
brain development
• Nevertheless, there is abundant shaping and
fine-tuning of brain structure and function with
sensory-experience
4: Studying human brain
development
• Structural and functional change
• How are these changes investigated & what is
known?
4: Studying human brain
development
Structural change: Methods

Magnetic resonance imaging


4: Studying human brain
development
Structural change: Methods

• T1-weighted image
• Segmentation
• Measure thickness
or volume in various
regions
4: Studying human brain
development
Structural change: What do we know?

• Total volume increases


• White matter increases

Girls Boys

Giedd et al, Nature Neuroscience, 1999


4: Studying human brain
development
Structural change: What do we know?

• Total volume increases


• White matter increases
• Grey matter increases
and decreases
• Heterochronic change

Girls Boys Giedd et al, Nature Neuroscience, 1999


4: Studying human brain
development
Structural change: What do we know?

• Total volume increases


• White matter increases
• Grey matter increases
and decreases
• Heterochronic change

Growth Shrinkage

Sowell, J Neuroscience, 2005


4: Studying human brain
development
Functional change: Methods – ERP’s

• Evoked response potentials


• Scalp-measured voltages
• Many trials averaged
• Waveforms
4: Studying human brain
development
Functional change: Methods – ERP’s

• Advantages
• Use with infants
• Inexpensive
• Relatively non-invasive
• Excellent temporal resolution
• Disadvantages
• Poor spatial resolution
4: Studying human brain
development
Functional change: What do we know?

• N170 greater for inverted faces


• Faces special
• Development?
• 12-month-olds show N170 effect
• 6-month-olds do not

de Haan et al, J Cog Neuroscience, 2002


4: Studying human brain
development
Functional change: What do we know?

• N170 greater for inverted faces


• Faces
ERP’s can reveal special
important changes in brain
• Development?
functioning in infancy and
• 12-month-olds
early childhood. show N170 effect
• 6-month-olds do not

de Haan et al, J Cog Neuroscience, 2002


4: Studying human brain
development
Functional change: Methods -- fMRI

• T2*-weighted image
• Blood-oxygenation
dependent response
(BOLD)
4: Studying human brain
development
Functional change: Methods -- fMRI

• T2*-weighted image
• Blood-oxygenation
dependent response
(BOLD)
• Eg. Block design

Time
4: Studying human brain
development
Functional change: Methods -- fMRI

• Advantages
• Excellent spatial resolution
• Disadvantages
• Expensive
• Motion sensitive
• Limited viability with very young children
4: Studying human brain
development
Functional change: What do we know?

shape
12-year-olds & adults
color

Switch Repeat Switch Repeat


18s 36s 18s 36s 18s 36s 18s 36s 18s

Time

Morton & Ansari, 2007


4: Studying human brain
development
Functional change: What do we know?

All participants

Switch > Repeat

• Frontal cortex
• Parietal cortex

Morton & Ansari, 2007


4: Studying human brain
development
Functional change: What do we know?

0.5 Switch Repeat


0.45
0.4
0.35
0.3
0.25

Age x Trial Type interaction reveals


0.2
0.15

development changes
0.1
0.05
0
Children Adults

R Parietal cortex
Switch > Repeat
Only adults
Morton & Ansari, 2007
4: Studying human brain
development
Functional change: What do we know?

• Challenges
• What does an Age x TrialType interaction reveal?
4: Studying human brain
development
Functional change: What do we know?

• Challenges
• What does an Age x TrialType interaction reveal?
• Need to consider behavioral performance
• If perform similarly, could suggest developmental
change
• If perform differently, possible that differences in
brain activity reflect difference in performance not
age
Brain Development Conclusions
• (1) Brain changes throughout development
both structurally and functionally
• (2) Developmental changes occur through and
interaction of genes and experience.

You might also like