Week10 LanguageAndCulture
Week10 LanguageAndCulture
Categories
Language Linguistic relativity
and Cognitive categories
Culture Social categories
Gender
We use the term culture to refer to all the
ideas and assumptions about the nature of
things and people that we learn when we
become members of social groups.
It can be defined as “socially acquired
Culture knowledge.” This is the kind of knowledge
that, like our first language, we initially
acquire without conscious awareness.
We develop awareness of our knowledge,
and hence of our culture, only after having
developed language.
With the words we acquire, we learn to
recognize the types of category
distinctions that are relevant in our social
world.
Very young children may not initially think
of “dog” and “horse” as different types of
Culture entities and refer to both as bow-wow.
In order to use words such as dog or horse,
rain or snow, father or uncle, week or
weekend, we must have a conceptual
system that includes these people, things
and ideas as distinct and identifiable
categories.
A category is a group with certain
features in common and we can think of
the vocabulary we learn as an inherited set
of category labels.
Categories However, evidence from the world’s
languages would suggest that the
organization of external reality actually
varies to some extent according to the
language being used to talk about it.
Some languages may have lots of different
words for types of “rain” or kinds of
“coconut” and other languages may have
only one or two.
Although the Dani of New Guinea can see
all colors of the spectrum, they only use
names for two of them, equivalents of
Categories “black” and “white.”
Observing this difference between the
number of basic color terms in languages,
we can say that there are conceptual
distinctions that are lexicalized
(“expressed as a single word”) in one
language and not in another.
Some of the clearest examples of lexicalized
categories are words used to refer to people
who are members of the same family, or
kinship terms.
In some languages, the equivalent of the word
Kinship father is used not only for “male parent,” but
also for “male parent’s brother.”
Terms Yet, we also use the same word (uncle) for
“female parent’s brother.” That distinction is
not lexicalized in English, but it is in other
languages. For example, in Turkish the word
uncle is used for both female and male
parent’s brothers as dayı and amca.
Having words for units of time such as “two
days” or “seven days” shows that we can
think of time (i.e. something abstract) in
amounts, using noun phrases, in the same
way as “two people” or “seven books” (i.e.
Time something physical).
In another world view, time may not be
Concepts treated in this way. In the Hopi language,
spoken in Arizona, there were traditionally no
terms equivalent to most of our time words
and phrases (two hours, thirty minutes)
because our terms express concepts from a
culture operating on “clock time.”
Linguistic relativity: it seems that the
structure of our language, with its
predetermined categories, must have an
influence on how we perceive the world.
This idea simply captures the fact that we not
Linguistic only talk, but to a certain extent probably also
think about the world of experience, using the
Relativity categories provided by our language.
Our first language seems to have a definite
role in shaping “habitual thought,” that is,
the way we think about things as we go about
our daily lives, without analyzing how we’re
thinking.
Linguistic determinism: holds that
“language determines thought.” If language
does indeed determine thought, then we will
only be able to think in the categories
provided by our language.
For example: English speakers use one word
Linguistic for “snow”. In contrast, Eskimos look out at all
Determinism the white stuff and see it as many different
things because they have lots of different
words for “snow.”
The category system inherent in the language
determines how the speaker interprets and
articulates experience.
Edward Sapir and Benjamin Whorf
produced arguments that the languages of
Native Americans, such as the Hopi, led
them to view the world differently from
those who spoke European languages.
The Sapir– According to Whorf, the Hopi perceive the
world differently from other tribes (including
Whorf the English-speaking tribe) because their
Hypothesis language leads them to do so.
In the grammar of Hopi, there is a distinction
between “animate” and “inanimate” and
among the set of entities categorized as
“animate” are clouds and stones.
Whorf claimed that the Hopi believe that
clouds and stones are living entities and that
it is their language that leads them to believe
this.
The Sapir– English does not mark in its grammar that
Whorf clouds and stones are “animate” so English
Hypothesis speakers do not see the world in the same
way as the Hopi. In Whorf’s words, “We
dissect nature along lines laid down by our
native languages”.
English does lexicalize some conceptual distinctions in
the area of “snow,” with sleet, slush and snowflake as
examples.
However, English speakers can also create expressions,
by manipulating their language, to refer to fresh snow,
powdery snow, spring snow or the dirty stuff that is piled
up on the side of the street after the snow plough has
Example: gone through.
These may be categories of snow for English speakers,
Snow but they are non-lexicalized (“not expressed as a single
word”).
English speakers can express category variation by
making a distinction using lexicalized categories (It’s
more like slush than snow outside) and also by indicating
special reference using non-lexicalized distinctions
(We decorated the windows with some fake plastic snow
stuff).
As a way of analyzing cognition, or how
people think, we can look at language
structure for clues, not for causes.
Cognitive The fact that Hopi speakers inherit a
language system in which clouds have
Categories “animate” as a feature may tell us
something about a traditional belief
system, or way of thinking, that is part of
their culture and not ours.
Classifiers indicate the type or “class” of noun
involved.
The closest English comes to using classifiers is
when we talk about a “unit of” certain types of
things.
There is a distinction in English between things
treated as countable (shirt, word, chair) and
Classifiers those treated as non-countable (clothing,
information, furniture).
It is ungrammatical in English to use a/an or the
plural with non-countable nouns (i.e. *a
clothing, *an information, *two furnitures). To
avoid these ungrammatical forms, we use
classifier-type expressions such as “item of” or
“piece of,” as in an item of clothing, a bit of
information and two pieces of furniture.
Words such as uncle or grandmother provide
examples of social categories.
These are categories of social organization
that we can use to say how we are connected
or related to others.
Social The word «brother» is similarly used among
Categories many groups for someone who is not a family
member.
We can use these words as a means of social
categorization, that is, marking individuals as
members of a group defined by social
connections.
‘Brother, can you spare a dollar?’: the word
brother is being used as an address term (a
word or phrase for the person being talked
Address or written to).
Terms By claiming the kind of closeness in
relationship associated with a family
member, the speaker’s choice of address
term is an attempt to create solidarity (i.e.
being the same in social status).
More typically, an interaction based on an
unequal relationship will feature address
terms using a title (Doctor) or title plus last
name (Professor Buckingham) for the one
with higher status, and first name only for the
Address one with lower status, as in: Professor
Buckingham, can I ask a question?~Yes,
Terms Jennifer, what is it?
More equal relationships have address terms
that indicate similar status of the participants,
such as first names or nicknames: Bucky, ready
for some more coffee? ~ Thanks, Jenny.
In many languages, there is a choice between
pronouns used for addressees who are socially
close versus distant. This is known as the T/V
distinction, as in the French pronouns tu
(close) and vous (distant). A similar type of
social categorization is found in German
(du/Sie) and Spanish (tu´ /Usted).
Address In each of these distinctions, as in older English
Terms usage (thou/you), the second form is used to
indicate that the speakers do not really have a
close relationship.
Traditionally, these forms could be used to
mark a power relationship. The higher status or
more powerful speaker could use tu or thou to
a lower-status addressee, but not vice versa.
Lower-status individuals had to use the
vous or you forms when addressing those
of higher status.
This usage is described as non-reciprocal,
Address but the reciprocal use (both speakers
Terms using the same form) of the tu forms has
generally increased in Europe among
younger speakers, such as students, who
may not know each other really well, but
who find themselves in the same situation.
In English, people without special titles are addressed as
Mr., Mrs., Miss, or Ms.
Only the women’s address terms include information
about their social status. In fact, one of the most
frequently used address terms for a woman indicates that
she is the wife of a particular man (called “Frank Smith,”
for example), as in Mrs. Smith, and sometimes even Mrs.
What he intended: You have wasted the whole term. What he intended: the dear old Queen
What he said: You have tasted the whole worm. What he said: the queer old dean
Psycholinguistics: The Study of Language Processing
Beginning in the 1960s, Victoria Fromkin began to study these and other
naturally occurring slips of the tongue and noted that they can be very revealing of the
manner in which sentences are created in speech. Here’s the important psycholinguistic
point: in order for these exchanges to occur, the sentence would have to be planned out
before the person begins to say it.
Another important observation that Fromkin made was that speech errors
also often involve ‘mixing and matching’ morphemes within words. Consider the
following slips of the tongue.
Intended: rules of word formation Intended: I’d forgotten about that.
Produced: words of rule formation Produced: I’d forgot aboutten that.
Psycholinguistics: The Study of Language Processing
Lexical Decision
In the lexical decision paradigm, the experimental participant (in this example, a
native speaker of English) is seated in front of a computer screen. A word appears in the
middle of the screen and the participant must judge as quickly as possible whether the word
is a real English word and press a button labelled ‘yes’ or a button labelled ‘no’.
Lexical decision experiments usually involve comparing participants’ performance
on one set of stimuli (e.g., nouns) to their performance on another set of stimuli (e.g., verbs).
The key to the importance of the experimental paradigm is that in order for a participant to
respond ‘no’ to a stimulus such as blove or ‘yes’ to a real word such as glove , the participant’s
mental lexicon must be accessed. The lexical decision task can therefore be used to measure
the speed and accuracy with which words in the mental lexicon are accessed.
Psycholinguistics: The Study of Language Processing
Another way in which the lexical decision task can be used to explore language
representation and processing is to investigate the speed and accuracy with which
participants press the ‘no’ button for different types of stimuli. It has been found, for
example, that pronounceable non-words such as plib show slower ‘no’ response times
than unpronounceable non-words such as nlib . Thus, participants’ lexical decisions seem
to take into account the phonotactic constraints of the language.
Psycholinguistics: The Study of Language Processing
Sentence Processing
It is presumed that in sentence processing (i.e., in reading or listening), a
sentence is understood through the analysis of the meanings of its words and through
the analysis of its syntactic structure. Psycholinguists refer to this type of unconscious
automatic analysis as parsing . Much of the research on sentence processing is
concerned with the principles and steps in parsing, its speed, and the manner and
conditions under which it can break down.
Two groups of experimental paradigms that have been used extensively to
study sentence processing: timed-reading experiments and eye-movement
experiments.
Psycholinguistics: The Study of Language Processing
Timed-reading Experiments
One of the more common and revealing timed-reading experimental
paradigms is the bar-pressing paradigm, in which participants are seated in front of a
computer screen and read a sentence one word at a time. The participant begins by
seeing the first word of the sentence in the middle of the screen. When the participant
presses a bar on the keyboard, the first word disappears and the second word of the
sentence appears in its place. This process continues until all the words in the sentence
have been read. The dependent variable in these experiments is the amount of time it
takes participants to press the bar after seeing a particular word (i.e., the amount of time
they need to process that word).
Psycholinguistics: The Study of Language Processing
Eye Movements
When the eyes are at rest they take a ‘snapshot’ of two or three words. These
snapshots usually last from 200 to 250 milliseconds. While the snapshot is being taken, the
language-processing system calculates where to jump to next. During a jump to the next
fixation location (usually about eight letters to the right), the reader is essentially blind.
The details of eye movements in sentence reading are studied with sophisticated
laboratory procedures in which a participant is often seated in front of a computer screen
on which text is displayed. Eye movements are tracked by a device that illuminates the
participant’s eyes with low-intensity infrared light and records the reflection. The eye-
position data are linked to the position of text on the screen so that it is possible to
determine how the eyes move from one text position to another.
Psycholinguistics: The Study of Language Processing
This technique has revealed that fixation times are typically longer for less
frequent words and that the points of fixation are typically centered on words such as
nouns and verbs, rather than on function words such as determiners and conjunctions.
Difficult sentence structures create longer fixation times as well as many more
regressive saccades. Regressive saccades are backward jumps in a sentence and are
usually associated with mis-parsing or miscomprehension. On average, backward
saccades make up 10 to 15 percent of the saccades in sentence reading.
Psycholinguistics: The Study of Language Processing
It turns out that in the processing of implausible sentences, the brain displays a characteristic ERP
sign of surprise. Consider the following sentences:
a. The pizza was too hot to eat.
b. The pizza was too hot to drink.
c. The pizza was too hot to cry.
The sentences are arranged in order of semantic plausibility. In the first case, the last word fits in
perfectly well with the sentence and would typically be expected by the reader. The ERP for this sentence
shows a positive voltage associated with the last word. In the case of (b), however, in which the last word does
not make sense (people do not drink pizza), the ERP is much more negative. This negative wave occurs 400
milliseconds after the onset of the word. For this reason, this signal of semantic anomaly is called the N400
(negative wave at 400 milliseconds after stimulus presentation). The N400 is even stronger in the case of
sentence (c), which is even less congruent with the sentence context (drink is at least associated with food).
Psycholinguistics: The Study of Language Processing
As soon as these segments are identified, you have already accessed the
representation for the word the in your mental lexicon. When the next segment comes up in the
sound stream, you already know that it is the beginning of a new word and you also know that
this word will probably be a noun. The phonetic analysis that follows identifies the segments [d],
[ɑ], and [g] and the corresponding lexical entry dog . Now come the first segments of the word bit
. In principle, the first two phonemes /bɪ/ could be the first two segments of the word believe , but
you are not likely to consider this possibility because your developing interpretation of the
sentence is biasing you toward the word bit , which is associated in your mind with dog .
Psycholinguistics: The Study of Language Processing
As can be appreciated from this example, language processing involves the interplay
of information that develops simultaneously at many different levels of analysis. The person
hearing the sentence The dog bit the cat is performing a phonetic analysis to isolate
phonemes and word boundaries and to relate these to items in the mental lexicon. This
inductive analysis is referred to as bottom-up processing. But we do not wait until we have
analyzed all the phonemes in a sentence before we begin to try to understand it. Rather, we
begin interpretation of a sentence spontaneously and automatically on the basis of whatever
information is available to us. For this reason, by the time we get to the word bit , we are not
only recognizing it using bottom-up processing but are also employing a set of expectations
to guide phonetic processing and word recognition. This is called top-down processing. In
normal language use, we are always engaged in both bottom-up and top-down activities. We
never just process features, or phonemes, or syllables. We process language for the purposes
of understanding each other.
Psycholinguistics: The Study of Language Processing
Morphological Processing
The psycholinguistic study of morphology seeks to understand how word structure
plays a role in language processing.
Morpheme Activation
Words such as blackboard , happiness , and watching are made up of two morphemes.
In the case of the compound blackboard , both morphemes are roots. In the case of happiness ,
one morpheme is a root and the other is a derivational suffix. Finally, in the case of watching, one
morpheme is a root and the other is an inflectional affix.
Psycholinguistics: The Study of Language Processing
There are two proposals about how this activation of morphemes might
occur. The first is that it derives from structured morphological representations in
the mind. According to this view, multi-morphemic words such as happiness and
blackboard are represented in the mind as [happy + ness] and [black + board], and
when we access such words, first their whole word forms and then their constituent
morphemes are automatically activated. This view is termed post-lexical
decomposition, because the constituents of a multi-morphemic word are activated
only after the representation of the whole lexical item is activated. In other words,
whole word access occurs first, morphological decomposition second.
Psycholinguistics: The Study of Language Processing
Syntax
Syntax is the system of rules and categories that underlies sentence formation in human
language. One of the fundamental insights in the study of syntax is that sentences are unique events.
They are typically made up of familiar words, but the particular combination of words and the manner
in which they are arranged are unique to each sentence.
Researchers concluded that there is at least some difference between the rules that native
speakers use to generate and comprehend sentences and the rules that linguists use to characterize
the linguistic knowledge of native speakers. It was therefore necessary to postulate a special module
for sentence processing, called the syntactic parser, and another for grammatical knowledge.
Psycholinguistics: The Study of Language Processing
The correct interpretation for the sentence requires that fell be the head of the main VP and that raced past the barn be
a clause (a reduced version of ‘which was raced past the barn’) that attaches to the NP the horse.
Psycholinguistics: The Study of Language Processing
Sentence Ambiguity
Another important clue to how syntactic processing is accomplished comes from the study of ambiguity.
The tuna can hit the boat = a. tuna meat that is packed in a small round can
b. a large fish swimming toward a boat
This suggests that in fact sentence processing proceeds in two stages. In the first stage, all possible
representations and structures are computed. In the second stage, one of these structures is selected and all others are
abandoned. Of course, all this happens very quickly and subconsciously, so that we as native speakers of a language are
never aware that we compute two possible interpretations. The point of the psycholinguistic experiments just described is
this: no matter which interpretation you arrived at (a or b), you probably considered both of them, chose one, discarded the
other, and forgot about the whole thing in less than a second.
Psycholinguistics: The Study of Language Processing
Psycholinguistic Modelling
Psycholinguistic researchers present their ideas about how language is ‘done’
in terms of models. A psycholinguistic model incorporates the results of experiments
into a proposal about how processing takes place. In other words, it is a statement of
what happens when.
Psycholinguistics: The Study of Language Processing
A parallel psycholinguistic model of sentence processing claims that phonological, lexical, and syntactic
processes are carried out simultaneously. Here information does not flow in a sequential manner. Rather, all modules
operate simultaneously and share information. Therefore, when we hear a sentence, we begin phonological, lexical,
and syntactic processes at the same time. As each type of processing proceeds, it informs the other.
Psycholinguistics: The Study of Language Processing
In recent research, serial and parallel processing models have been very
important for understanding the extent to which bottom-up and top-down processing
interact. Serial models correctly characterize those aspects of language processing that
are modular and are driven by strict bottom-up procedures, such as phonetic perception.
Parallel processing models, in contrast, are more effective than serial models at
characterizing complex processes such as sentence comprehension.
Psycholinguistics: The Study of Language Processing
Option (3) represents a multiple-route model in that it claims that both mechanisms are employed. Usually such
multiple-route models employ the additional metaphor of a horse race by claiming that for some words (e.g., very
frequent short words) the direct route is faster but for others (e.g., rare words) the phonological conversion route is
faster and ‘wins the horse race’. The whole-word recognition route wins the race for frequent bimorphemic words such
as blackboard.
Psycholinguistics: The Study of Language Processing
Clinical Linguistics
(Cummings, 2015)
WEEK 12
CLINICAL LINGUISTICS
CLINICAL LINGUISTICS
So the adult with aphasia and the child with specific language
impairment have a language disorder because they are unable to
encode and decode aspects of language (e.g. syntax, semantics).
However, breakdown in the boxes labelled ‘motor programming’ and
‘motor execution’ typically lead to speech disorders, verbal dyspraxia
(or apraxia) and dysarthria, respectively.
CLINICAL LINGUISTICS
Aphasia
Aphasia is an acquired language disorder that results from damage
to the language centers in the left hemisphere of the brain. Although a range
of neurological diseases and traumas can cause aphasia, the main cause of
the disorder is cerebrovascular accidents or strokes. Aphasia can compromise
language at all levels including phonology, morphology, syntax, semantics
and pragmatics.
It affects language in all its input and output modalities. So, as well
as having problems producing and understanding spoken language, the adult
with aphasia may struggle to produce and understand written language
(acquired dysgraphia and dyslexia, respectively) or produce and
comprehend signs (if the client with aphasia is a user of British Sign
Language, for example).
CLINICAL LINGUISTICS
COMPUTATIONAL LINGUISTICS
COMPUTATIONAL
LINGUISTICS
Computational linguistics seeks to
develop the computational machinery
needed for an agent to exhibit various
COMPU forms of linguistic behavior.
TATION
By “agent,” we mean both human
AL beings and artificial agents such as
LINGUI computer programs.
COMPU
A program called a word-sense
TATION disambiguator is used for this. A
AL word-sense disambiguator
(Kilgarriff and Palmer 2000) can
LINGUI use the context of neighboring
STICS words in the sentence as well as
other words in the document to
figure out which meaning of a
given word is most likely.
A common assumption
COMPU underlying computational
approaches to semantics is that the
TATION meaning of the whole is
AL systematically composed of the
meaning of the parts. A computer
LINGUI program based on compositional
semantics puts together the
STICS sentence meaning from the
meanings of the words and phrases
that compose it.
Information extraction: tries to derive
the semantic content of a document as a
whole as it relates to particular types of
events.
DISCOURSEANALYSIS
Discourse
Analysis
"Discourse is the way in which language is used
socially to convey broad historical meanings. It is
Discourse
language identified by the social conditions of its use,
Analysis
by who is using it and under what conditions.
Language can never be 'neutral' because it bridges
our personal and social worlds" (Henry & Tator,
2002).
Discourse: is the language beyond the sentence
Discourse
identified by
Analysis
1) the social conditions of its use,
2) by who is using it and,
3) under what conditions
Discourse The study of discourse is entirely context-
Analysisdependent because conversation involves situational
knowledge beyond just the words spoken.
(thoughtco.com)
Cohesion: refers to the use of linguistic devices to
Discourse
join sentences together (including conjunctions,
Analysisreference words, substitution, and lexical devices
such as repetition of words, collocations and lexical
groups) and gives a written or spoken text unity and
purpose.
Discourse
Coherence: refers to the general sense that a text
Analysismakes sense through the organization of its content
and gives a written or spoken text unity and
purpose.
Discourse My father bought a Lincoln convertible. The
Analysiscar driven by the police was red. That color doesn’t
suit her. She consists of three letters. However, a
letter isn’t as fast as a phone call.
My father bought a Lincoln convertible. He did
Discourse
it by saving every penny he could. That car would be
Analysisworth a fortune nowadays. However, he sold it to help
pay for my college education. Sometimes I think I’d
rather have the convertible.
In a speech community, communication is
Discourse
organized in terms of events. Hymes (1972) defines
Analysisspeech events as «activities or aspects of activities
that are directly governed by rules or norms for the
use of speech» (p. 56)
Conversation analysis is a systematic analysis of talk that is
produced as a result of normal everyday interactions …
Discourse
According to this approach, social actions hold some sort of
Analysis
significance to those who initiate them.
Conversation analysis … tries to understand the hidden
rules, meanings or structures that create such an order in a
conversation. (communicationtheory.org)
A turn is the time when a speaker is talking
and turn-taking is the skill of knowing when to start
Discourse
and finish a turn in a conversation. There are many
Analysisways that speakers manage turn-taking and they vary
in different cultures: e.g. intonation, grammatical
structures, utterances such as 'ah', 'mm' and 'you
know', body language and gestures.
The cooperative principle is a principle of
Discourse
conversation that was proposed by Grice (1975),
stating that participants expect that each will make a
Analysis
“conversational contribution such as is required, at the
stage at which it occurs, by the accepted purpose or
direction of the talk exchange” (p. 45).
The four conversational maxims of Grice’s
cooperative principle:
Quantity: Say no less or more than the conversation
Discourse
requires.
Analysis
Quality: Do not say things for which you lack evidence.
Manner: Do not be ambiguous. Be brief.
Relevance: Be relevant.
Discourse
AnalysisHedges can be defined as words of phrases used to
indicate that we are not really sure what we are
saying is sufficiently correct or complete.
A parent questions a teenage child on a Sunday
morning. He says, “What time did you come home last
night?” The teenager might respond in a number of ways:
Discourse
“I got home at midnight.”
Analysis
“I got home at around midnight.”
“I got home at midnight, I think.”
“I got home at, like, midnight.”
As far as I know …
I am not absolutely sure …
DiscourseCorrect me if I am wrong …
Analysis almost
maybe
somewhat
think/feel
possible/likely
Conversational implicatures:
(i) are implied by the speaker in making an utterance;
Discourse
Analysis
(ii) are part of the content of the utterance, but (iii) do
not contribute to direct (or explicit) utterance content
and (iv) are not encoded by the linguistic meaning of
what has been uttered.
Carol: Are you coming to the party tonight?
Discourse
Lara: I’ve got an exam tomorrow.
Analysis
Lara’s response adhering to the maxims of
Relation and Quantity.
Background knowledge is information that is not in the text,
but is used from memory by the reader to understand the
Discourse
text.
Analysis
A schema is a general term for a conventional knowledge
structure that exists in memory (e.g., classroom schema).
A script is a series of conventional actions that take place
(e.g., eating at a restaurant).
The language with which we choose to
Discourse
express ourselves and the contexts in which we do
Analysisso displays our social identities and group
affiliations. Thus, discourse analysis can lead to a
better understanding of the values and social
practices of a community (Wennerstrom, 2003).
To summarize, discourse analysis involves the
study of naturally occurring language in the context in
Discourse
which it is used. Discourse analysts, whether they are
Analysis
concerned with the coherence of extended structure or
with the interpretation of more minute texts, are
interested in the language choices people make to
accomplish their social goals (Wennerstrom, 2003) .
Discourse Analysis
Critical discourse analysis (CDA) is a type of
Discourse
discourse analytical research that primarily studies the
Analysis
way social power abuse, dominance, and inequality are
enacted, reproduced, and resisted by text and talk in
the social and political context (Van Dijk, 2001).
Fairclough and Wodak (1997: 271–80) summarize the main
tenets of CDA as follows:
1. CDA addresses social problems
2. Power relations are discursive
Discourse
3. Discourse constitutes society and culture
Analysis
4. Discourse does ideological work
5. Discourse is historical
6. The link between text and society is mediated
7. Discourse analysis is interpretative and explanatory
8. Discourse is a form of social action.
Some research in CDA:
• Gender inequality
• Media discourse
Discourse
• Political discourse
Analysis•
From group domination to professional and
institutional power
• Discourse in educational settings
Topics of discourse analysis in school settings:
• Classroom interaction as cultural practice
Discourse
• Classroom discourse and literacy development
Analysis
• Discourse study of second language development
• Classroom discourse as learning
• School as a venue for talk
“Knowledge and Classroom Discourse” by Jocuns, A.
(2012)
Discourse One approach to classroom discourse has been to
Analysis
examine not only the language that is used within the
classroom or educational setting but the myriad of ways in
which knowledge is constructed, displayed, or both during
social interaction within the classroom, not to mention
content knowledge. …
“Knowledge and Classroom Discourse” by Jocuns, A.
(2012)
Discourse
… Such studies have examined the metacommunicative
Analysis
knowledge at a student’s disposal during learning
interactions including verbal and nonverbal discourse,
and the participant structures available for a student to
use in a specific educational setting (Jocuns, 2007,
2009). …
“Knowledge and Classroom Discourse” by Jocuns, A. (2012)
… The crux of such studies is that they emphasize that
Discourse
students learn more than just content knowledge during
Analysis
classroom interaction: they also learn the metacommunicative
means by which to construct knowledge, as well as the ability
to develop and maintain social roles and identities. What is
more, through classroom discourse students play a role in
constructing content knowledge.
Discourse
Analysis
Discourse
Analysis and
Language
Teaching It is widely accepted in the field that we
teach both “language for communication” and
“language as communication.”
Discourse Discourse analysis and pragmatics are relevant to
Analysislanguage
and teaching and language learning since they
represent two related discourse worlds that characterize
Languagehuman communication. The first represents intended
Teachingmeaning transmitted within context, and is, therefore,
concerned with sequential relationships in production; and
the other explains the interpreted meaning resulting from
linguistic processing and social interaction (Olshtain & Celce-
Murcia, 2001).
Discourse
Analysis and The competent language teacher can no
Languagelonger limit herself or himself to being an educator
Teachingand a grammarian. To a certain extent, she or he
also has to be a sociolinguist, aware of and
interested in various aspects of discourse analysis
(Olshtain & Celce-Murcia, 2001).
Discourse
Analysis and A discourse-based model for language
Languagepedagogy perceives shared knowledge as consisting
Teachingof layers of mutually understood subcategories:
content knowledge, context knowledge, linguistic
knowledge, discourse knowledge, etc. (Johns,
1997).
The discourse analysis of oral interaction is highly relevant to
the teaching of pronunciation in a communicative classroom:
Discourse
• the difference between new and old information is signaled via
Analysisprosody,
and
• contrast and contradiction are also marked by a shift of focus in the
Language
ongoing discourse,
Teaching
• students need to be alerted to similarities and differences in rhythm
and intonation between their NL and the TL,
• the social functions of intonation, which may reveal things such as the
speaker’s degree of interest or involvement, the speaker’s expression
of sarcasm, etc. should be taught.
Knowing grammar can no longer mean knowing only
Discourse
how a form functions within a given sentence, but must also
Analysis and discourse features of grammatical forms.
include
Language It is the context-dependent, pragmatic rules of
Teaching
grammar that play an important role in a discourse approach
to grammar. In English, such grammatical choices as passive
versus active voice, sentential position of adverbs, tense–
aspect–modality sequences, and article use, among others, are
context-dependent (Olshtain & Celce-Murcia, 2001).
In the teaching and learning of vocabulary the discourse
perspective stands out very clearly.
REVISION
WEEK 14
REVISION
First Language Acquisition
ACQUISITION
Language acquisition is the process by which humans
acquire the capacity to perceive and comprehend language (in
other words, gain the ability to be aware of language and to
understand it), as well as to produce and use words and
sentences to communicate.
• Communicative approaches
Second Language Acquisition vs Learning:
Focus on the Learner
• Transfer
• Interlanguage
• Motivation
• Input and output
• Fossilization
Second Language Acquisition vs Learning:
Communicative Competence
Communicative competence can be defined as the
general ability to use language accurately, appropriately,
and flexibly.
• Family trees
• Comparative reconstruction
• The history of English
• Diachronic and synchronic variation
Indo-European is the
language family with the
largest population and
distribution in the world.
Language and Regional Variation
Sociolinguistics
Speech style and style-shifting
African American English
Culture
Categories
Language Linguistic relativity
and Culture Cognitive categories
Social categories
Gender
Psycholinguistics: The Study of Language Processing
Many psycholinguists conceive of the mental lexicon as a collection of individual units as in the figure. In this figure, the lexicon is shown as a space in which entries of
different types are stored and linked together.The main questions that are asked about the mental lexicon are these: (1) How are entries linked? (2) How are entries
accessed? (3) What information is contained in an entry?
Clinical Linguistics: a broad area of academic and
clinical investigation that overlaps with medical and
other fields of study (e.g. neurology, psychology) and
that demands a sound understanding of language at
all of its levels.
CLINICAL LINGUISTICS
Computational linguistics seeks to develop the
computational machinery needed for an agent to exhibit
various forms of linguistic behavior.