Audition
Audition
Under optimum conditions, human hearing is sensitive to sounds that vibrate the eardrum by less
than one-tenth the diameter of an atom, and we can detect a difference between two sounds as little
as 1/30 the interval between two piano notes (Hudspeth, 2014).
Ordinarily, however, we attend to hearing in order to extract useful information. If you hear
footsteps in your home or a snapped twig in the forest, you know you are not alone. If you hear
breathing, you know some person or animal is close. Then you hear the sound of a familiar friendly
voice, and you know that all is well.
Physics and Psychology of Sound
Rube Goldberg (1883–1970) drew cartoons of complicated, far-fetched inventions. For example, a person’s tread on the
front doorstep might pull a string that raised a cat’s tail, awakening the cat, which then chases a bird that had been resting
on a balance, which swings up to strike a doorbell.
The functioning of the ear is complex enough to resemble a Rube Goldberg device, but unlike Goldberg’s inventions, the
ear actually works.
Anatomists distinguish the outer ear, the middle ear, and the inner ear. The outer ear includes the pinna, the familiar
structure of flesh and cartilage attached to each side of the head. By altering the reflections of sound waves, the pinna helps
us locate the source of a sound. We have to learn to use that information because each person’s pinna is shaped differently
from anyone else’s. Rabbits’ large movable pinnas enable them to localize sound sources even more precisely.
“This is a quote, Words full of
wisdom that someone important said
and can make the reader get
inspired.”
—SOMEONE FAMOUS—
Pitch Perception
Your ability to understand speech or enjoy music depends on your ability to differentiate among sounds of different
frequencies. How do you do it?
According to the place theory, the basilar membrane resembles the strings of a piano, with each area along the
membrane tuned to a specific frequency. If you sound a note with a tuning fork near a piano, you vibrate the piano
string tuned to that note.
According to this theory, each frequency activates the hair cells at only one place along the basilar membrane, and the
nervous system distinguishes among frequencies based on which neurons respond. The downfall of this theory is that
the various parts of the basilar membrane are bound together too tightly for any part to resonate like a piano string.
Place Theory
The downfall of this theory in its simplest form is that the refractory period of a neuron, though
variable among neurons, is typically about 1/1,000 second, so the maximum firing rate of a neuron is
about 1000 Hz, far short of the highest frequencies we hear.
The current theory is a modification of both theories. For low-frequency sounds (up to about 100 Hz
—more than an octave below middle C in music, which is 264 Hz), the basilar membrane vibrates in
synchrony with the sound waves, in accordance with the frequency theory, and the auditory nerve
axons generate one action potential per wave.
Soft sounds activate fewer neurons, and stronger sounds activate more. Thus, at low frequencies, the
frequency of impulses identifies the pitch, and the number of firing cells identifies loudness.
The Auditory Cortex
Just as patients with damage in a brain area become motion blind, patients with damage in parts of the
superior temporal cortex become motion deaf. They hear sounds, but they do not detect that a source
of a sound is moving (Ducommun et al., 2004).
Sound Localization
You are walking alone when suddenly you hear a loud noise. You want to know what
produced it (friend or foe), but equally, you want to know where it came from. Sound
localization is less accurate than visual localization, but nevertheless impressive.
Owls localize sounds well enough to capture mice in the dark.
Determining the direction and distance of a sound requires comparing the responses
of the two ears. One method is the difference in time of arrival at the two ears. A
sound coming directly from one side reaches your closer ear about 600 microseconds
(μs) before the other. A smaller difference in arrival times, indicates a sound source
nearer to your midline. Time of arrival is useful for localizing sounds with a sudden
onset. Most birds’ alarm calls increase gradually in loudness, making them difficult
for a predator to localize.
For people with amusia, the auditory cortex appears to be approximately
normal, but it has fewer than average connections to the frontal cortex (Hyde et
al., 2007; Loui et al., 2009; Norman-Haignere et al., 2016). That is, the deficit
is evidently not in hearing itself. Rather, these people have poor memory for
pitch (Tillman, Leveque, Fornoni, Abouy, & Caclin, 2016), and perhaps poor
attention to pitch.
Although few people are totally unable to hear, many people have enough impairment to prevent speech comprehension.
The two categories of hearing loss are conductive deafness and nerve deafness.
Diseases, infections, or tumorous bone growth can prevent the middle ear from transmitting sound waves properly to the
cochlea. The result, conductive deafness or middle-ear deafness, is sometimes temporary.
If it persists, it can be corrected by surgery or by hearing aids that amplify sounds. Because people with conductive
deafness have a normal cochlea and auditory nerve, they readily hear their own voices, conducted through the bones of the
skull directly to the cochlea, bypassing the middle ear. Because they hear themselves clearly, they may accuse others of
mumbling or talking too softly.
Nerve deafness, or inner-ear deafness, results from damage to the cochlea, the hair cells, or the
auditory nerve. If it is confined to one part of the cochlea, it impairs hearing of certain frequencies
and not others.
Nerve deafness can be inherited, it can result from disease, or it can result from exposure to loud
noises. For example, many soldiers, construction workers, and fans of loud rock music expose
themselves to noise levels that damage the synapses and neurons of the auditory system. Gradually
they begin to notice ringing in the ears or impaired hearing (Kujawa & Liberman, 2009).
Tinnitus (tin-EYE-tus) is frequent or constant ringing in the ears.
In some cases, tinnitus may be due to a phenomenon similar to
phantom limb. Damage to part of the cochlea is like an amputation:
If the brain no longer gets its normal input, axons representing
other parts of the body may invade part of the brain area that
usually responds to sounds.
Many older people have hearing problems despite wearing hearing aids. The hearing aids make the sounds
loud enough, but people still have trouble understanding speech, especially in a noisy room or if someone
speaks rapidly.
Part of the explanation is that the brain areas responsible for language comprehension have become less
active (Peelle, Troiani, Grossman, & Wingfield, 2011). This trend might be just a natural deterioration, or
it might be a reaction to prolonged degradation of auditory input. That is, if someone delays getting
hearing aids, the language cortex doesn’t get its normal input and it begins to become less responsive.
APPLICATIONS
We spend much of our day listening to language, and we sometimes forget that the original, primary
function of hearing has to do with simpler but extremely important issues: What do I hear? Where is
it? Is it coming closer? Is it a potential mate, a potential enemy, potential food, or something
irrelevant? The organization of the auditory system is well suited to answering these questions.
Thank You for
Listening!
Josefina C. Ochoa, PhD, RPm, LPT