Constructs, Variables, and Operationalization
Constructs, Variables, and Operationalization
OPERATIONALIZATION
Prof. Jigyasa Bisaria
Asst. Prof. QT & IT
IIFM Bhopal
Constructs in quantitative research
Broadly speaking,
constructs are the Theory
building blocks construc
t
of theories
variable
s
Quality Dissertation
What are constructs?
Constructs are mental abstractions that
express ideas, people, organisations, eventand/or objec
ts/things that we are interested in.
Constructs are a way of bringing theory down to earth,
helping to explain the different components of
theories, as well as measure/observe their behaviour
Properties of constructs
Broad concepts or topics of study :
Abstract
Not directly observable
May be complex (have multiple parts)
Examples of constructs:
Aggression
Love
Intelligence
Life satisfaction
Translating constructs into operational
definitions
Whilst constructs are sometimes mistaken for variables, they are not variables.
Instead, we use variables to operationalize(i.e., measure) the constructs we are
interested in. Constructs can be mistaken for variables because some constructs
may only be represented by one variable, such that the construct name and
the variable name are the same (e.g., the construct and variable, sex).
Therefore, constructs need to be translated from the abstract (i.e., mental ideas;
mental abstractions) to the concrete (i.e., measureable/testable in the form
of variables). In other words, we are re-stating constructs as variables, with
variables also having their own attributes (e.g., gender having the
attributes male/female, which is important, because gender is a classic example
of where constructs/variables, and their attributes, can be confused). The role of
the operational definition is to precisely describe how
to measure the characteristics of a construct. By characteristics, we mean
the mental abstractions/ideas within constructs that ultimately are measureable
in the form of variables and their attributes. It is these variables and their
attributes that are measured.
Translating constructs into operational
definitions
Constructs can be represented by a wide range of variables.
For example, happiness could be associated with love,
financial security, relationships, puppies, a song, ice cream,
and so on. Translating abstract concepts (e.g., happiness)
into concrete variables is not straightforward.
People view constructs in different ways (e.g., in the case
of happiness, people often adopt a perspective that focuses
on actions, such as smoking a cigarette, or possessions,
such as owning a diamond, so you need to be clear how
you intend to operationalize a construct, and why you are
making such choices).
Translating constructs into operational
definitions
Translating constructs into operational definitions can be
an iterative process, but testing (i.e., the measurement
process) should not start until a conceptual and operational
definition of your construct(s) have been selected (i.e., you
cannot have good measurement without
conceptual/operational clarity of constructs).
Ultimately, the operational definition is seldom perfect; that
is, the choice of operational definition may be constrained
by factors such as a lack of access to
operational/measurement data. Also, how we
construct/formulate an operational definition will impact on
the complexity of the measurement process.
Independent vs. Dependent
Independent and Dependent variables (except purely descriptive
research)
All research (except descriptive studies) must have at least two
variables
one can be IV and the other DV
in symmetrical relationships, the question of which is independent and which is
dependent is moot
Having an IV allows you to assume a cause-effect relationship:
changes in the IV result in changes in the DV
If you cannot posit a cause-effect relationship, then you essentially
have two IV's (the level/score of each is independent of the other
[although both may depend on some other variable(s)])
Having an IV allows more control and better inference about what is
going on, especially when you have an active IV.
Active and Attribute Independent Variables
Attribute: level or score of the variable is brought to the
experiment by the subject, usually as a natural
characteristic such as sex, age, etc.
active: the level of the IV is manipulated by the experimenter
Intervening variables: uncontrolled or unobserved
variables that may account for variation in the DV (also
known as extraneous variables)
Control variables: any variable that may affect DV should
be controlled; that is, measured and accounted for
statistically or held constant (age, gender, socioeconomic
status, etc., could be control variables)
Problems with measurement of variables
Qualitative vs. quantitative variables
reliability and validity are essentially measurement
problems
since qualitative variables are basically classificatory,
there is less concern with reliability and validity
Reliability
reliability refers to the observation of variation in scores
earned by an individual on repeated trials of the same
measure (variation can be systematic or random)
so, reliability = consistency
Problems with measurement of variables
Validity
validity is the degree to which the measuring instrument
actually measures the concept in question
validity also refers to the accuracy of the measurement
it is possible to measure a concept more or less
accurately if you are actually measuring the right concept
but it is not possible to measure it accurately if you are
not measuring it at all.
Measurement error
due to sampling
due to subject or experimenter effects
measurement error results in decreased reliability and
validity
Relationship between variables
X and Y are correlated if they vary together
concomitant variation = correlation
correlation can be direct or inverse
Causal relationships
Concomitant variation does not demonstrate causality
Causality is difficult (or impossible) to demonstrate logically
However, we can make the case that X causes Y, if
there is a relationship between X and Y(birds go south in the fall),
and
the relationship is asymmetrical so that a change in X results in a
change in Y, but not vice versa (birds migrate because of fall but
fall does not come because birds migrate), and
a change in X results in a change in Y regardless of the actions of
other variables, and
generally, X should precede Y but sometimes symmetrical causality
and simultaneous causality are allowed; the effect can never
precede the cause
Necessary and sufficient cause
Necessary Y never occurs unless X also occurs (or has occurred)
Sufficient Y occurs every time X occurs (but could also occur
without X; e.g., "smoking causes cancer")
Necessary, but not sufficient (X must occur before Y but, X alone,
is not enough for Y to occur)
Sufficient, but not necessary (X is sufficient to cause Y, but Z can
also cause Y; e.g., Fred is wet (Y) but did he fall into a pond (X)
or did he get caught in the rain (Z)?
Necessary and sufficient Y will never occur without X and will
always occur with X (e.g., the hand grenade will never explode
without you pulling the pin and will always explode when you
pull the pin)
Causality in social science
Difficult to demonstrate theoretically as our
theories are inadequate for the isolation of causes
Difficult to demonstrate methodologically
Survey methods usually do not give temporal
sequences
Laboratory methods help to demonstrate
causality since we control and sequence
independent and dependent variables