0% found this document useful (0 votes)
143 views26 pages

For Full-Screen View, Click On at The Lower Left of Your Screen

This document provides an overview of how to conduct a meta-analysis. It defines a meta-analysis as a systematic review that quantitatively combines results from multiple studies to obtain an overall estimate of effect size. It discusses key aspects of meta-analysis including using a weighted average to give more weight to more precise studies, allowing for heterogeneity between studies, and addressing limitations such as publication bias. The document provides guidance on selecting an appropriate generic measure of effect size, finding and incorporating study characteristics, and assessing study quality and weighting factors.

Uploaded by

Shela Ramos
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PPTX, PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
0% found this document useful (0 votes)
143 views26 pages

For Full-Screen View, Click On at The Lower Left of Your Screen

This document provides an overview of how to conduct a meta-analysis. It defines a meta-analysis as a systematic review that quantitatively combines results from multiple studies to obtain an overall estimate of effect size. It discusses key aspects of meta-analysis including using a weighted average to give more weight to more precise studies, allowing for heterogeneity between studies, and addressing limitations such as publication bias. The document provides guidance on selecting an appropriate generic measure of effect size, finding and incorporating study characteristics, and assessing study quality and weighting factors.

Uploaded by

Shela Ramos
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PPTX, PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
You are on page 1/ 26

· If you are viewing this slideshow within a browser window, select

File/Save as… from the toolbar and save the slideshow to your
computer, then open it directly in PowerPoint.
· When you open the file, use the full-screen view to see the
information on each slide build sequentially.
· For full-screen view, click on this icon at the lower left of your screen.

· To go forwards, left-click or hit the space bar, PdDn or  key.


· To go backwards, hit the PgUp or  key.
· To exit from full-screen view, hit the Esc (escape) key.
An Introduction to Meta-analysis
Will G Hopkins
Victoria University, Melbourne, Australia
· What is a Meta-Analysis?
· Definition, weighted average, heterogeneity, mixed-model meta-
regression
· Limitations to Meta-Analysis
· Individual differences or responses, publication bias
· How to Do a Meta-Analysis
· Generic measures, finding effects, study characteristics, study
quality, weighting factor, model, publication bias
· Summary and References
What is a Meta-Analysis?
· A systematic review of literature to address this question:
on the basis of the research to date, how big is a given effect,
such as…
· the effect of endurance training on resting blood pressure;
· the effect of bracing on ankle injury;
· the effect of creatine supplementation on sprint performance;
· the relationship between obesity and habitual physical activity.
· It is similar to a simple cross-sectional study, in which the
subjects are individual studies rather than individual people.
· But the stats are a bit harder.
· A review of literature is a meta-analytic review only if it
includes quantitative estimation of the magnitude of the effect
and its uncertainty (confidence limits).
· The main outcome is the overall magnitude of the effect.
· The effect in each study (the study estimates) and the meta-
analyzed mean outcome are often shown in a forest plot:
Study1
Study2
Study3
Study4
Study5
Data are means and
Study6 95% confidence
Study7 intervals.
Study8
Study9 Authors should show
Study10 magnitudes on the
Study11 plot.
Study12
Study13
Study14
Study15
Mean
harmful trivial beneficial
-2 -1 0 1 2 3
Effect on power output (%)
· The main outcome is not a simple average of the study
estimates.
· Meta-analysis uses the standard error of each study estimate to
give more weight to studies with more precise estimates.
· The standard error is an SD representing the expected variation
in the study estimate if the study was repeated again and again.
· The weighting factor is 1/(standard error)2.
· Other things being equal, use of this factor is equivalent to
weighting the effect in each study by the study's sample size.
· So, for example, a meta-analysis of 3 studies of 10, 20 and 30
subjects each amounts to a single study of 60 subjects.
· For controlled trials, this factor also takes into account
differences in standard error of measurement between studies.
· You can and you must allow for real differences or heterogeneity
in the magnitude of the effect between studies.
· In early (fixed-effects only) meta-analysis, you did so by testing for
heterogeneity using the Q or chi-squared statistic.
· The test has low power, so you used p<0.10 rather than p<0.05.
· If p<0.10, you excluded "outlier" studies and re-tested, until p>0.10.
· When p>0.10, you declared the effect homogeneous.
• That is, you assumed the differences in the effect between studies
were due only to sampling variation.
• Which made it easy to analyze the weighted mean effect.
· But the approach was unrealistic, limited, and suffered from the
problem of whether statistical non-significance means negligible.
· In random-effect meta-analysis, you accept there are always real
differences between all studies in the magnitude of the effect.
· The “random effect” is the standard deviation representing the
variation in the true magnitude from study to study.
· You get an estimate of this SD and its precision.
• It is sometimes known as tau and is provided as tau2.
· The mean effect ± this SD is what folks can expect typically in
another study or if they try to make use of the effect.
· Instead of this SD, some researchers provide the Q and the related
I2 statistic, representing percent of variation due to real differences.
• Ignore any conclusions based on these uninterpretable statistics.
· Mixed-model meta-analysis or meta-regression is best of all.
· You include study characteristics as fixed effects.
· The study characteristics will partly account for differences in the
magnitude of the effect between studies.
• Example: differences between studies of athletes and non-athletes.
· The random effect now represents residual variation in the effect
between studies (i.e., not explained by the study characteristics).
· Include an extra random effect to account for repeated
measurement (multiple estimates) within studies.
· Custom software or an advanced package (e.g., SAS) is required.
Limitations to Meta-Analysis
· It's focused on mean effects and differences between studies.
· But what really matters is effects on individuals.
· So we should also quantify individual differences or responses.
· These can be expressed as standard deviations, but researchers
usually don't provide enough info to allow their meta-analysis.
· Inclusion of mean subject characteristics (e.g., age, gender,
genotype) as predictors in the meta-analytic model only partly
addresses this problem.
• It would be better if researchers made available all data for all
subjects, to allow individual patient-data meta-analysis.
· A meta-analysis reflects only published effects.
· But statistically significant effects are more likely to get published.
· Hence published effects are biased high.
· Funnel or related plots can be used to assess and reduce
publication bias.
How to Do a Meta-Analysis: Opt for a Generic Measure
· You can combine effects from different studies only when they
are expressed in the same units.
· In most meta-analyses, the effects are converted to a generic
dimensionless measure.
Main measures:
· Standardized difference or change in the mean (Cohen's d)
• Other forms are similar or less useful (Hedges' g, Glass's )
• But it’s better to standardize after the meta-analysis.
· Percent of range (max minus min) for psychometrics.
· Percent or factor difference or change in the mean
· Correlation coefficient and slope (seldom meta-analyzed)
· Risk, proportion, odds, hazard and count ratios.
Standardized Difference or Change in the Mean
· Express the difference or change in the mean as a fraction of
the between-subject standard deviation (mean/SD).
· Also known as Cohen's d (d stands for difference).
· This example of the effect of a treatment on strength shows
why the SD is important:
Trivial effect (0.1x SD) Very large effect (3x SD)
post post
pre pre

strength strength

· mean/SD is biased high for small sample sizes and needs


correcting before including in the meta-analysis.
· A problem with standardization:
· Study samples are often drawn from populations with different
SDs, so some differences in effect size between studies will be
due to the differences in SDs.
· Such differences are irrelevant and tend to mask more
interesting differences.
· The solution:
· Meta-analyze a better generic measure reflecting the biological
effect, usually percent or factor differences or changes.
• Rarely, the raw measure is best; for example, joint angles
representing flexibility.
· Combine the between-subject SDs from the studies selectively
and appropriately, to get one or more population SDs.
· Express the overall effect from the meta-analysis as a
standardized effect using this/these SDs.
· This approach also effectively eliminates the correction for
sample-size bias with standardized effects.
Percent or Factor Difference or Change in the Mean
· Many effects can be expressed as a percent or multiplicative
factor that tends to have the same value for every individual.
· Example: effect of a treatment on performance is +2%, or a factor
of 1.02, regardless of the raw value of the performance.
· For such effects, percent or factor difference or change can be
the most appropriate generic measure in a meta-analysis.
· If all the studies have small percent effects (<10%), use percent
effects directly in the meta-analysis.
· Otherwise express the effects (and their standard errors) as
factors and log-transform them before meta-analysis.
· Back-transform the outcomes into percents or factors.
· Where relevant, calculate standardized differences or changes in
the mean using the log transformed effects and logs of factor SD.
· Measures that are already in percent units (e.g., psychometrics,
fat as %BM) should be analyzed without log transformation.
· Measures of athletic performance need special care.
· The best generic measure is percent change.
· But a given percent change in an athlete's ability to output power
can result in different percent changes in performance in different
exercise modalities.
· Example: a 1% change in endurance power output produces the
following changes…
• 1% in running time-trial speed or time;
• ~0.4% in road-cycling time-trial time;
• 0.3% in rowing-ergometer time-trial time;
• ~15% in time to exhaustion in a constant-power test.
· So convert all published effects to changes in power output.
• A difficult and time-consuming task; you have been warned!
• See recent meta-analyses by my students and colleagues.
· For team-sport fitness tests, convert percent changes into
standardized effects using SD for chosen sports.
Correlation Coefficient and Slope
· These measures of association between two numeric variables
are seldom meta-analyzed.
Performance
· Studies with small between-subject SD
r = 0.80
have small correlations, so correlation
suffers from a similar SD problem as
standardized effects.
r = 0.20
· Solution: meta-analyze the slope.
· The slope is biased low (degraded) Maximum O2 uptake
only by random error in the predictor.
· Adjust for this bias by dividing the slope by the short-term reliability
intraclass correlation coefficient.
· Express the meta-analyzed slope as either…
• a correlation using SD for an appropriate population, or
• the effect of two SD of the predictor in that population.
Risk, Odds, Hazard and Count Ratios
· When the dependent variable is a proportion or count of
something, effects should be expressed as ratios.
· Risk ratio, relative risk, proportion ratio…
· Example: if proportions of inactive and active adults who get heart
disease after 20 years are 25% and 10%, risk ratio = 25/10 = 2.5.
· If proportions are time-independent classifications, convert all
effects to odds ratios for meta-analysis.
· Convert meta-analyzed odds ratios back into proportion ratios by
choosing a sensible proportion for the reference group.
• Use proportion ratio = OR/[1+p1(OR-1)], where OR is the odds ratio
and p1 is the reference proportion.
· Hazard ratio is the risk ratio for new occurrences in the next brief
instant of time (the "right-now" risk ratio).
· If proportions change with time, the proportion and odds ratios also
change, but the hazard ratio usually doesn't.
· So, to meta-analyze studies with different time periods, convert any
proportion and odds ratios to hazard ratios.
• Assume p = (1-e-h.t), where p is the proportion and h is the hazard.
• Therefore hazard ratio h2/h1 = log(1-p2)/log(1-p1).
· But don't convert odds ratios from time-dependent case-control
studies, because (amazingly) these are already hazard ratios.
· If proportions in the two groups in all studies are low (<10%), all
proportion, odds and hazard ratios are effectively equal and
need not be interconverted.
· Express effects on counts as count ratios.
· Express standard errors of ratio effects as ×/÷ factor errors, then
log transform the ratios and errors for meta-analysis.
· Some meta-analysis programs do the log transformation and work
out the standard errors for you.
• Researchers make big mistakes with these programs!
How to Do a Meta-Analysis: Find and Record Effects
· Search the literature for studies of a specific effect.
· If the effect has been meta-analyzed already, you can do another,
if it was done badly or if there are many new studies.
· As you assemble the published papers, broaden or narrow the
focus of your review to make it manageable and relevant via…
• …design (e.g., only randomized controlled trials), population (e.g.,
only competitive athletes), or treatment (e.g., only acute effects).
• Minimum sample size is ~10 studies; many more needed for good
analysis of modifying effects of study characteristics.
· Document your searches, inclusions and exclusions.
· Record each effect magnitude and inferential information
(sample size, p value, confidence limits, SD of change scores).
· Convert effects into values on a single scale of magnitude.
· In studies with more than one group, record the effect in each group
and include relevant fixed and random effects in the analysis.
How to Do a Meta-Analysis: Get Study Characteristics
· Record study characteristics that might account for differences
in the effect magnitude between studies.
· Include the study characteristics as covariates in the meta-
analysis. Examples:
· duration or dose of treatment;
· method of measurement of dependent variable;
· quality score;
· gender and mean characteristics of subjects (age, status…).
• Record separate outcomes for females and males from the same
study, if possible.
• Otherwise analyze gender as a proportion of one gender; for
example, in a study of 3 males and 7 females, “Maleness” = 0.3.
• Use this approach for all problematic dichotomous characteristics
(sedentary vs active, non-athletes vs athletes, etc.).
· group identity (e.g.: control, experimental)
How to Do a Meta-Analysis: Assess Study Quality?
· Most meta-analysts score the quality of a study.
· Examples (scored yes=1, no=0):
• Published in a peer-reviewed journal?
• Experienced researchers?
• Research funded by impartial agency?
• Study performed by impartial researchers?
• Subjects selected randomly from a population?
• Subjects assigned randomly to treatments?
• High proportion of subjects entered and/or finished the study?
• Subjects blind to treatment?
• Data gatherers blind to treatment?
• Analysis performed blind?
· Use the score to exclude some studies, and/or…
· Include as a covariate in the meta-analysis, but…
· Some statisticians advise caution when using quality scores.
How to Do a Meta-Analysis: Get the Weighting Factor
· Calculate the standard error for each effect via one or more of…
· the confidence interval or limits
· the test statistic (t, 2, F)
· the p value
• If the exact p value is not given and you can't calculate the standard
error from the data, try contacting the authors for it.
• Otherwise, if "p<0.05", analyze as p=0.05.
• If "p>0.05" with no other info, deal with the study qualitatively.
· the SD of change scores (for controlled trials)
• For studies lacking sufficient information to calculate standard
errors, calculate the typical error in every other study, impute typical
errors, then estimate standard errors via SD of change scores.
• The spreadsheet for sample-size estimation at Sportscience
converts p values or confidence limits to typical and standard errors
· the data: done for you by the software, depending on the effect.
• But be careful to choose the right options.
How to Do a Meta-Analysis: Develop the Model
· Do a mixed-model meta-regression.
· Estimate and interpret Effect on power output (%)
the effect for interesting 3
types of study or mean
subject. 2
· Show a moderator plot

beneficial
rather than a forest plot: 1
· In a mediator plot, the
subject characteristic
trivial
0
is a difference score
or change score.
-1
harmful

Data are means and -2


90% confidence 0 5 10 15 20
intervals Baseline training (h.wk-1)
· For any linear covariate, estimate and interpret the effect of 2x
the average of between-subject SD from appropriate studies.
· Double the SD representing the between-study random effect to
interpret its magnitude as the unexplained typical differences in
the magnitude of the effect between settings.
· The random effect provided as tau2 may be in log-transformed
units and may need back-transformation to a factor SD.
• Apply the p value for tau2 directly to the SD to get confidence limits.
· For effects where there are control or other reference groups…
· include each group effect separately, if possible;
· include a within-study random effect to account for the resulting
repeated measurement;
· and of course, include a fixed effect to estimate the uncontrolled
effect and the effect relative to control.
· Inspect between-subject SD between and within studies for
evidence of individual differences or responses.
How to Do a Meta-Analysis: Deal with Publication Bias
· Some meta-analysts present the effect magnitude of all the
studies as a funnel plot, to address the issue of publication bias.
· Published effects tend to be larger than true effects, because...
• effects that are larger simply because of non-sig. SE published
sampling variation have smaller p values, missing
studies delete
studies
• and p<0.05 is more likely to be published. funnel of funnel
of all
· A plot of standard error vs effect magnitude allstudies
non-sig
if studies if
should have a triangular or funnel shape. effect=0 effect >0
· But if some non-significant studies weren’t effect 0 value with
published, the plot will be asymmetrical. magnitude huge sample
• The missing studies are generally smaller, therefore have larger SE.
· So delete studies with larger SE to give a symmetrical plot.
· However, effect heterogeneity also disrupts the funnel shape,
making it hard to decide where to draw the line to delete studies.
· A plot of standardized residuals (random-effect solution) vs
standard error works much better. But it requires skill with SAS.
Summary
· Meta-analysis is a statistical review of the magnitude of an effect.
· Meta-analysis uses the magnitude of the effect and its precision
from each study to produce a weighted mean.
· Traditional meta-analysis is based unrealistically on using a test
for heterogeneity to exclude outlier studies.
· Random-effect (mixed-model) meta-analysis estimates
heterogeneity and allows estimation of the effect of study and
subject characteristics on the effect.
· For the analysis, the effects have to be converted into the same
units, usually percent or other dimensionless generic measure,
but do not meta-analyze standardized effects.
· It's possible to account for publication bias and identify outlier
studies using a funnel plot or residuals plot.
References
· Recent meta-analyses co-authored by me. Also my article on improving
meta-analyses http://sportsci.org/2018/metaflaws.htm.
· A good source of meta-analytic wisdom is the Cochrane Collaboration, an
international non-profit academic group specializing in meta-analyses of
healthcare interventions.
· Website http://www.cochrane.org
· Publication: Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of
Interventions http://training.cochrane.org/handbook.
· But the (free) Cochrane meta-analysis software is somewhat limited.
These references are getting out of date:
· Simple reference: Bergman NG, Parker RA (2002). Meta-analysis: neither quick nor easy.
BMC Medical Research Methodology 2, http://www.biomedcentral.com/1471-2288/2/10.
· Glossary: Delgado-Rodríguez M (2001). Glossary on meta-analysis. Journal of Epidemiology
and Community Health 55, 534-536.
· Reference for problems with publication bias: Terrin N, Schmid CH, Lau J, Olkin I (2003).
Adjusting for publication bias in the presence of heterogeneity. Statistics in Medicine 22,
2113-2126.
This presentation is available from:

See Sportscience 8, 2004

You might also like