Exploring The Complexities of Human Action Direct Ebook Download
Exploring The Complexities of Human Action Direct Ebook Download
Visit the link below to download the full version of this book:
https://medipdf.com/product/exploring-the-complexities-of-human-action/
Acknowledgments ix
Introduction: Some Complexities xi
PA RT I . SE T T I N G T H E STAG E
1. Introducing Psychology 3
2. Taking a Theoretical Turn 32
3. Questioning Convention 56
PA RT I I . C O N C E P T UA L I Z I N G AC T IO N
4. Systems Theory and Introducing Action 91
5. Constitutive Processes 113
6. Psychological Processes 144
7. Developmental Processes 199
PA RT I I I . I M P L IC AT IO N S A N D A P P L IC AT IO N S
8. Implications for Understanding Human Functioning
Integratively and Actively 225
9. Implications for Thinking about Individuality and Variability 252
10. Implications for Pondering Some Complexities of
Causality and Meaning 275
11. Evidence and Research Implications 304
12. Casting a Wide Net 332
13. Complex and Empathic Understanding 374
References 399
Index 419
Acknowledgments
I have been pondering the issues discussed in this book for a very long time,
and even though I finished the book (or stopped working on it), I continue
to ponder. Along the way, my pondering has been facilitated, supported, and
challenged in varied ways by varied people with whom I have engaged di-
rectly, as well as indirectly through reading their work.
I am grateful for the intellectual milieu of my childhood where I first began
to ponder. I am grateful that in the late 1980s I went to graduate school at
Clark University, where discussing and questioning ideas provided a strong
foundation for ongoing pondering. I am grateful that in recent years, I have
been fortunate to be in contact with Richard M. Lerner, whose generosity
and support of my work are invaluable and keep me pondering despite nag-
ging doubts about the point of it all.
I thank IUP graduate student Molly Bernoski for diligently proofreading
and checking and rechecking references. I am indebted to artist Jen Blalock
for her illustrations, which transformed my abstract ideas into lively images
of people acting, as well as for her infectious good cheer.
Thank you to Abby Gross at Oxford University Press for taking on this
project and supporting pondering outside the box. I also thank Katharine
Pratt for her hard work and for shepherding this book through the publica-
tion process, along with the production team at Oxford University Press.
And last, as well as probably most (but why quantify?), I am grateful for
long walks with my sister, Anne Raeff. From New York City and the suburbs
of northern New Jersey, to San Francisco and its scenic surrounds, our
conversations never fail to provoke and inspire continued pondering.
Catherine Raeff
Introduction
Some Complexities
Human beings are complex creatures who live complex lives amidst complex
circumstances in all corners of a complex world. To begin thinking about
some human complexities, let us consider the word complex, and what it
means with regard to human functioning. According to my trusty Webster’s
New Collegiate Dictionary, complex refers to “a whole made up of compli-
cated or interrelated parts,” as well as to “a complex substance . . . in which the
constituents are more intimately associated than in a simple mixture.” These
definitions seem quite apt for human functioning, which can be understood
in terms of varied parts, from thinking, remembering, and knowing, to
feeling and constructing self/identity, to interacting with others, to cultural
and neurological processes, to development. These parts are just the tip of
the iceberg, and they do not seem to be simply mixed together. But how can
we get at and understand how they are more intimately associated? As an ad-
jective, complex means that a phenomenon is “hard to separate, analyze, or
solve.” Synonyms (again, from Webster’s) for complex include “complicated,
intricate, involved, knotty” with the “shared meaning element: having con-
fusingly interrelated parts.” Human affairs can be quite intricate and knotty.
Certainly, human functioning is hard to analyze, and some of the problems
that plague humanity are hard to solve. And aspects of human functioning
are also utterly intertwined and hard to separate. Complexity in the sense of
hard to analyze is evident in the dynamics, variability, individuality, and de-
velopment of what people do as they go about their lives in all corners of the
world. We are all human beings who act in some common ways, yet we also
lead individual lives and act in culturally particular ways. It can sometimes
be overwhelmingly confusing to make sense of it all.
This book is about what people do as they go about their complex lives in
all corners of the world. I wrote this book because I have long been befuddled
by the complexities of what people do. It represents my attempt to explore
some of those complexities, and my attempt to articulate a theoretical
Exploring the Complexities of Human Action. Catherine Raeff, Oxford University Press (2020). © Oxford
University Press. DOI: 10.1093/oso/9780190050436.001.0001
xii Introduction
Of course, human beings the world over have been trying to make sense of
human functioning for millennia. People think about themselves and others
every day as they go about their complex lives in all corners of the world. And
within varied jobs and careers, some people ponder and investigate aspects
of human functioning professionally. Artists—from novelists and poets, to
playwrights and film makers, to painters and songwriters—ponder and illu-
minate aspects of human functioning. Since the field of psychology emerged
in the late 19th century as a distinct science, academic discipline, and profes-
sion, it has contributed to our understanding of human functioning in varied
important ways.
Today, one can detect some restlessness in some corners of psychology
that points to the utility of reflecting on the field and considering alterna-
tive theoretical frameworks. For example, one indication of restlessness in
psychology can be found in discussions about why it is so difficult to rep-
licate research results (Pashler & Wagenmakers, 2012). In addition, psy-
chology has become a vast and fragmented discipline in which psychologists
study aspects of human functioning in relative isolation. Within this context,
some are calling for collaboration among psychologists who work on dis-
parate topics, as well as between psychology and other disciplines. In 2015,
the Association for Psychological Science inaugurated the International
Convention of Psychological Science as a forum for “scientific advances that
are integrative” (https://www.psychologicalscience.org/conventions/icps/
about). There are calls for integrative approaches to conceptualize human
functioning holistically and to conceptualize the functioning of the whole
person (Diriwächter & Valsiner, 2008; Raeff, 2016, 2017a; Robinson, 2007;
Shotter, 1975). The term intersectionality seems to crop up a lot to address
how people function at the intersection of varied characteristics, such as race,
gender, socioeconomic status, and sexuality. In 1989, Crenshaw conceptu-
alized intersectionality to refer to how Black women experience overlap-
ping or intersecting forms of discrimination, that is, racism and sexism. She
uses intersectionality to counteract the “tendency to treat race and gender
as mutually exclusive categories of experience and analysis” (p. 139). Using
the analogy of a four-way traffic intersection, she explains that when traffic
flows into and out of an intersection, an accident can happen there “by cars
traveling from any number of directions and, sometimes, from all of them”
(p. 149). It may be difficult to assign blame to one particular driver because
more than one contributed to the accident simultaneously.
xiv Introduction
Restlessness is not new in psychology. There were debates about how the
field of psychology should proceed when it was being established as a dis-
tinct and scientific discipline in the late 19th century. Although research in
psychology came to be dominated by and identified with quantitative and ex-
perimental methods, some psychologists continued to advocate for other re-
search methods. Drawing on those traditions, and in keeping with the social
and political movements of the 1960s, critically analyzing psychology gained
ground from the 1970s through the 1990s. For example, social construc-
tionist and feminist perspectives were being used to question psychology’s
dominant assumptions and methods. To counteract the hegemony of white,
middle-class American values within psychology, there were calls to focus
on cultural diversity and indigenous psychologies. In the 1980s and 1990s,
questioning truth claims was the wider postmodern order of the day. Critical
analyses raised questions about defining psychology in terms of strict adher-
ence to a particular scientific method that originated in the natural sciences.
During this time, some classics of critical psychology were published, in-
cluding Seymour Sarason’s Psychology Misdirected, in 1981; Carol Gilligan’s
In a Different Voice, in 1982; Kurt Danziger’s Constructing the Subject, in
1990; and Kenneth Gergen’s The Saturated Self, in 1991.
Critical analyses in the 1980s and 1990s also pointed to a need for more
elaborate theorizing in psychology. At the time, I was in graduate school at
Clark University (from 1988 to 1993), where many psychology department
faculty emphasized theory. There was also no shortage of classes that explored
and embraced a variety of critical perspectives. There was much arguing over
how to define terms and how to conceptualize varied topics in psychology.
We talked about how methods are not theory neutral. We talked about how
theory and science are historically situated and ideologically infused. We
talked about how psychologists study objects that they themselves construct
conceptually, as well as through using particular methods. I was hooked by
these discussions. (And I remain particularly indebted to Ina Č. Užgiris,
Bernard Kaplan, James V. Wertsch, Nancy Budwig, Michael Bamberg, and
Seymour Wapner.) I began to see that how we understand human func-
tioning depends on the theoretical frameworks we use. I came to understand
that different ways of conceptualizing human functioning may demand dif-
ferent methods of empirical validation. Theory comes first, then method.
And the method does not necessarily have to be experimental or quantita-
tive. I embraced it all. I left Clark and continued to question psychology’s
dominant practices. I became increasingly overwhelmed by the increasing
Introduction xv