2.1 Language
2.1 Language
Language is a structured system of communication that consists of grammar and vocabulary. It is the
primary means by which humans convey meaning, both in spoken and signed forms, and may also be
conveyed through writing. Human language is characterized by its cultural and historical diversity, with
significant variations observed between cultures and across time.[1] Human languages possess the
properties of productivity and displacement, which enable the creation of an infinite number of sentences,
and the ability to refer to objects, events, and ideas that are not immediately present in the discourse. The
use of human language relies on social convention and is acquired through learning.
Estimates of the number of human languages in the world vary between 5,000 and 7,000. Precise
estimates depend on an arbitrary distinction (dichotomy) established between languages and dialects.[2]
Natural languages are spoken, signed, or both; however, any language can be encoded into secondary
media using auditory, visual, or tactile stimuli – for example, writing, whistling, signing, or braille. In
other words, human language is modality-independent, but written or signed language is the way to
inscribe or encode the natural human speech or gestures.
Depending on philosophical perspectives regarding the definition of language and meaning, when used as
a general concept, "language" may refer to the cognitive ability to learn and use systems of complex
communication, or to describe the set of rules that makes up these systems, or the set of utterances that
can be produced from those rules. All languages rely on the process of semiosis to relate signs to
particular meanings. Oral, manual and tactile languages contain a phonological system that governs how
symbols are used to form sequences known as words or morphemes, and a syntactic system that governs
how words and morphemes are combined to form phrases and utterances.
The scientific study of language is called linguistics. Critical examinations of languages, such as
philosophy of language, the relationships between language and thought, how words represent
experience, etc., have been debated at least since Gorgias and Plato in ancient Greek civilization.
Thinkers such as Jean-Jacques Rousseau (1712–1778) have argued that language originated from
emotions, while others like Immanuel Kant (1724–1804) have argued that languages originated from
rational and logical thought. Twentieth century philosophers such as Ludwig Wittgenstein (1889–1951)
argued that philosophy is really the study of language itself. Major figures in contemporary linguistics of
these times include Ferdinand de Saussure and Noam Chomsky.
Language is thought to have gradually diverged from earlier primate communication systems when early
hominins acquired the ability to form a theory of mind and shared intentionality.[3][4] This development is
sometimes thought to have coincided with an increase in brain volume, and many linguists see the
structures of language as having evolved to serve specific communicative and social functions. Language
is processed in many different locations in the human brain, but especially in Broca's and Wernicke's
areas. Humans acquire language through social interaction in early childhood, and children generally
speak fluently by approximately three years old. Language and culture are codependent. Therefore, in
addition to its strictly communicative uses, language has social uses such as signifying group identity,
social stratification, as well as use for social grooming and entertainment.
Languages evolve and diversify over time, and the history of their
evolution can be reconstructed by comparing modern languages
to determine which traits their ancestral languages must have had
in order for the later developmental stages to occur. A group of
languages that descend from a common ancestor is known as a
language family; in contrast, a language that has been
demonstrated not to have any living or non-living relationship
with another language is called a language isolate. There are also
A mural in Teotihuacan, Mexico
many unclassified languages whose relationships have not been
(c. 2nd century) depicting a person
established, and spurious languages may have not existed at all. emitting a speech scroll from his
Academic consensus holds that between 50% and 90% of mouth, symbolizing speech
languages spoken at the beginning of the 21st century will
probably have become extinct by the year 2100.[5][6][7]
Definitions
The English word language derives ultimately from Proto-Indo-
European *dn̥ ǵʰwéh₂s "tongue, speech, language" through Latin
lingua, "language; tongue", and Old French language.[8] The
word is sometimes used to refer to codes, ciphers, and other kinds
of artificially constructed communication systems such as
formally defined computer languages used for computer
programming. Unlike conventional human languages, a formal
language in this sense is a system of signs for encoding and
decoding information. This article specifically concerns the
properties of natural human language as it is studied in the Cuneiform is the first known form of
written language, but spoken
discipline of linguistics.
language predates writing by at
As an object of linguistic study, "language" has two primary least many tens of thousands of
years.
meanings: an abstract concept, and a specific linguistic system,
e.g. "French". The Swiss linguist Ferdinand de Saussure, who
defined the modern discipline of linguistics, first explicitly
formulated the distinction using the French word langage for
language as a concept, langue as a specific instance of a language
system, and parole for the concrete use of speech in a particular
language.[9]
Some proponents of Saussure's view of language have advocated a formal approach which studies
language structure by identifying its basic elements and then by presenting a formal account of the rules
according to which the elements combine in order to form words and sentences. The main proponent of
such a theory is Noam Chomsky, the originator of the generative theory of grammar, who has defined
language as the construction of sentences that can be generated using transformational grammars.[19]
Chomsky considers these rules to be an innate feature of the human mind and to constitute the rudiments
of what language is.[20] By way of contrast, such transformational grammars are also commonly used in
formal logic, in formal linguistics, and in applied computational linguistics.[21][22] In the philosophy of
language, the view of linguistic meaning as residing in the logical relations between propositions and
reality was developed by philosophers such as Alfred Tarski, Bertrand Russell, and other formal
logicians.
Communication systems used by other animals such as bees or apes are closed systems that consist of a
finite, usually very limited, number of possible ideas that can be expressed.[27] In contrast, human
language is open-ended and productive, meaning that it allows humans to produce a vast range of
utterances from a finite set of elements, and to create new words and sentences. This is possible because
human language is based on a dual code, in which a finite number of elements which are meaningless in
themselves (e.g. sounds, letters or gestures) can be combined to form an infinite number of larger units of
meaning (words and sentences).[28] However, one study has demonstrated that an Australian bird, the
chestnut-crowned babbler, is capable of using the same acoustic elements in different arrangements to
create two functionally distinct vocalizations.[29] Additionally, pied babblers have demonstrated the
ability to generate two functionally distinct vocalisations composed of the same sound type, which can
only be distinguished by the number of repeated elements.[30]
Several species of animals have proved to be able to acquire forms of communication through social
learning: for instance a bonobo named Kanzi learned to express itself using a set of symbolic lexigrams.
Similarly, many species of birds and whales learn their songs by imitating other members of their species.
However, while some animals may acquire large numbers of words and symbols,[note 1] none have been
able to learn as many different signs as are generally known by an average 4 year old human, nor have
any acquired anything resembling the complex grammar of human language.[32]
Human languages differ from animal communication systems in that they employ grammatical and
semantic categories, such as noun and verb, present and past, which may be used to express exceedingly
complex meanings.[32] It is distinguished by the property of recursivity: for example, a noun phrase can
contain another noun phrase (as in "[[the chimpanzee]'s lips]") or a clause can contain another clause (as
in "[I see [the dog is running]]").[4] Human language is the only known natural communication system
whose adaptability may be referred to as modality independent. This means that it can be used not only
for communication through one channel or medium, but through several. For example, spoken language
uses the auditive modality, whereas sign languages and writing use the visual modality, and braille
writing uses the tactile modality.[33]
Human language is unusual in being able to refer to abstract concepts and to imagined or hypothetical
events as well as events that took place in the past or may happen in the future. This ability to refer to
events that are not at the same time or place as the speech event is called displacement, and while some
animal communication systems can use displacement (such as the communication of bees that can
communicate the location of sources of nectar that are out of sight), the degree to which it is used in
human language is also considered unique.[28]
Origin
Theories about the origin of language differ in regard to their
basic assumptions about what language is.[35] Some theories are
based on the idea that language is so complex that one cannot
imagine it simply appearing from nothing in its final form, but
that it must have evolved from earlier pre-linguistic systems
among our pre-human ancestors. These theories can be called
continuity-based theories. The opposite viewpoint is that language
is such a unique human trait that it cannot be compared to
anything found among non-humans and that it must therefore The Tower of Babel by Pieter
have appeared suddenly in the transition from pre-hominids to Bruegel the Elder. Oil on board,
early man. These theories can be defined as discontinuity-based. 1563.
Similarly, theories based on the generative view of language Humans have speculated about the
origins of language throughout
pioneered by Noam Chomsky see language mostly as an innate
history. The Biblical myth of the
faculty that is largely genetically encoded, whereas functionalist
Tower of Babel is one such account;
theories see it as a system that is largely cultural, learned through other cultures have different stories
social interaction.[36] of how language arose.[34]
Researchers on the evolutionary origin of language generally find it plausible to suggest that
language was invented only once, and that all modern spoken languages are thus in some
way related, even if that relation can no longer be recovered ... because of limitations on the
methods available for reconstruction.[40]
Because language emerged in the early prehistory of man, before the existence of any written records, its
early development has left no historical traces, and it is believed that no comparable processes can be
observed today. Theories that stress continuity often look at animals to see if, for example, primates
display any traits that can be seen as analogous to what pre-human language must have been like. Early
human fossils can be inspected for traces of physical adaptation to language use or pre-linguistic forms of
symbolic behaviour. Among the signs in human fossils that may suggest linguistic abilities are: the size of
the brain relative to body mass, the presence of a larynx capable of advanced sound production and the
nature of tools and other manufactured artifacts.[41]
It was mostly undisputed that pre-human australopithecines did not have communication systems
significantly different from those found in great apes in general. However, a 2017 study on Ardipithecus
ramidus challenges this belief.[42] Scholarly opinions vary as to the developments since the appearance of
the genus Homo some 2.5 million years ago. Some scholars assume the development of primitive
language-like systems (proto-language) as early as Homo habilis (2.3 million years ago) while others
place the development of primitive symbolic communication only with Homo erectus (1.8 million years
ago) or Homo heidelbergensis (0.6 million years ago), and the development of language proper with
anatomically modern Homo sapiens with the Upper Paleolithic revolution less than 100,000 years
ago.[43][44]
In March 2024, researchers reported that the beginnings of human language began about 1.6 million years
ago.[47]
Study
The study of language, linguistics, has been developing into a science since the first grammatical
descriptions of particular languages in India more than 2000 years ago, after the development of the
Brahmi script. Modern linguistics is a science that concerns itself with all aspects of language, examining
it from all of the theoretical viewpoints described above.[48]
Subdisciplines
The academic study of language is conducted within many different
disciplinary areas and from different theoretical angles, all of which
inform modern approaches to linguistics. For example, descriptive
linguistics examines the grammar of single languages, theoretical
linguistics develops theories on how best to conceptualize and define the
nature of language based on data from the various extant human
languages, sociolinguistics studies how languages are used for social
William Jones discovered
purposes informing in turn the study of the social functions of language the family relation between
and grammatical description, neurolinguistics studies how language is Latin and Sanskrit, laying
processed in the human brain and allows the experimental testing of the ground for the discipline
theories, computational linguistics builds on theoretical and descriptive of historical linguistics.
linguistics to construct computational models of language often aimed at
processing natural language or at testing linguistic hypotheses, and
historical linguistics relies on grammatical and lexical descriptions of languages to trace their individual
histories and reconstruct trees of language families by using the comparative method.[49]
Early history
The formal study of language is often considered to have started in India
with Pāṇini, the 5th century BC grammarian who formulated 3,959 rules
of Sanskrit morphology. However, Sumerian scribes already studied the
differences between Sumerian and Akkadian grammar around 1900 BC.
Subsequent grammatical traditions developed in all of the ancient cultures
that adopted writing.[50]
Modern linguistics
In the 1960s, Noam Chomsky formulated the generative theory of
language. According to this theory, the most basic form of language is a
set of syntactic rules that is universal for all humans and which underlies
the grammars of all human languages. This set of rules is called Universal
Grammar; for Chomsky, describing it is the primary objective of the
discipline of linguistics. Thus, he considered that the grammars of
individual languages are only of importance to linguistics insofar as they
allow us to deduce the universal underlying rules from which the
observable linguistic variability is generated.[53]
In opposition to the formal theories of the generative school, functional Noam Chomsky is one of
theories of language propose that since language is fundamentally a tool, the most important linguistic
theorists of the 20th
its structures are best analyzed and understood by reference to their
century.
functions. Formal theories of grammar seek to define the different
elements of language and describe the way they relate to each other as
systems of formal rules or operations, while functional theories seek to define the functions performed by
language and then relate them to the linguistic elements that carry them out.[22][note 2] The framework of
cognitive linguistics interprets language in terms of the concepts (which are sometimes universal, and
sometimes specific to a particular language) which underlie its forms. Cognitive linguistics is primarily
concerned with how the mind creates meaning through language.[55]
The brain
The brain is the coordinating center of all linguistic activity; it controls both the production of linguistic
cognition and of meaning and the mechanics of speech production. Nonetheless, our knowledge of the
neurological bases for language is quite limited, though it has advanced considerably with the use of
modern imaging techniques. The discipline of linguistics dedicated to studying the neurological aspects
of language is called neurolinguistics.[58]
Early work in neurolinguistics involved the study of language in
people with brain lesions, to see how lesions in specific areas
affect language and speech. In this way, neuroscientists in the 19th
century discovered that two areas in the brain are crucially
implicated in language processing. The first area is Wernicke's
area, which is in the posterior section of the superior temporal
gyrus in the dominant cerebral hemisphere. People with a lesion in
this area of the brain develop receptive aphasia, a condition in
which there is a major impairment of language comprehension, Language Areas of the brain.
while speech retains a natural-sounding rhythm and a relatively Angular gyrus
normal sentence structure. The second area is Broca's area, in the Supramarginal gyrus
posterior inferior frontal gyrus of the dominant hemisphere. Broca's area
People with a lesion to this area develop expressive aphasia, Wernicke's area
meaning that they know what they want to say, they just cannot Primary auditory cortex
With technological advances in the late 20th century, neurolinguists have also incorporated non-invasive
techniques such as functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and electrophysiology to study
language processing in individuals without impairments.[58]
Anatomy of speech
Spoken language relies on human physical ability to produce sound, which is a longitudinal wave
propagated through the air at a frequency capable of vibrating the ear drum. This ability depends on the
physiology of the human speech organs. These organs consist of the lungs, the voice box (larynx), and the
upper vocal tract – the throat, the mouth, and the nose. By controlling the different parts of the speech
apparatus, the airstream can be manipulated to produce different speech sounds.[62]
The sound of speech can be analyzed into a combination of segmental and suprasegmental elements. The
segmental elements are those that follow each other in sequences, which are usually represented by
distinct letters in alphabetic scripts, such as the Roman script. In free flowing speech, there are no clear
boundaries between one segment and the next, nor usually are there any audible pauses between them.
Segments therefore are distinguished by their distinct sounds which are a result of their different
articulations, and can be either vowels or consonants. Suprasegmental phenomena encompass such
elements as stress, phonation type, voice timbre, and prosody or intonation, all of which may have effects
across multiple segments.[63]
Consonants and vowel segments combine to form syllables, which in
turn combine to form utterances; these can be distinguished
phonetically as the space between two inhalations. Acoustically,
these different segments are characterized by different formant
structures, that are visible in a spectrogram of the recorded sound
wave. Formants are the amplitude peaks in the frequency spectrum
of a specific sound.[63][64]
Vowels are those sounds that have no audible friction caused by the
narrowing or obstruction of some part of the upper vocal tract. They
vary in quality according to the degree of lip aperture and the The human vocal tract
placement of the tongue within the oral cavity.[63] Vowels are called
close when the lips are relatively closed, as in the pronunciation of
the vowel [i] (English "ee"), or open when the lips are relatively
open, as in the vowel [a] (English "ah"). If the tongue is located
towards the back of the mouth, the quality changes, creating vowels
such as [u] (English "oo"). The quality also changes depending on
whether the lips are rounded as opposed to unrounded, creating
distinctions such as that between [i] (unrounded front vowel such as
Spectrogram of American
English "ee") and [y] (rounded front vowel such as German "ü").[65]
English vowels [i, u, ɑ] showing
the formants f1 and f2
Consonants are those sounds that have audible friction or closure at
some point within the upper vocal tract. Consonant sounds vary by
place of articulation, i.e. the place in the vocal tract where the
airflow is obstructed, commonly at the lips, teeth, alveolar ridge,
palate, velum, uvula, or glottis. Each place of articulation produces a
different set of consonant sounds, which are further distinguished by
manner of articulation, or the kind of friction, whether full closure, in
which case the consonant is called occlusive or stop, or different
degrees of aperture creating fricatives and approximants. Consonants
can also be either voiced or unvoiced, depending on whether the
vocal cords are set in vibration by airflow during the production of Real time MRI scan of a person
the sound. Voicing is what separates English [s] in bus (unvoiced speaking in Mandarin Chinese
sibilant) from [z] in buzz (voiced sibilant).[66]
Some speech sounds, both vowels and consonants, involve release of air flow through the nasal cavity,
and these are called nasals or nasalized sounds. Other sounds are defined by the way the tongue moves
within the mouth such as the l-sounds (called laterals, because the air flows along both sides of the
tongue), and the r-sounds (called rhotics).[64]
By using these speech organs, humans can produce hundreds of distinct sounds: some appear very often
in the world's languages, whereas others are much more common in certain language families, language
areas, or even specific to a single language.[67]
Modality
Human languages display considerable plasticity[1] in their deployment of two fundamental modes: oral
(speech and mouthing) and manual (sign and gesture).[note 3] For example, it is common for oral language
to be accompanied by gesture, and for sign language to be accompanied by mouthing. In addition, some
language communities use both modes to convey lexical or grammatical meaning, each mode
complementing the other. Such bimodal use of language is especially common in genres such as story-
telling (with Plains Indian Sign Language and Australian Aboriginal sign languages used alongside oral
language, for example), but also occurs in mundane conversation. For instance, many Australian
languages have a rich set of case suffixes that provide details about the instrument used to perform an
action. Others lack such grammatical precision in the oral mode, but supplement it with gesture to convey
that information in the sign mode. In Iwaidja, for example, 'he went out for fish using a torch' is spoken as
simply "he-hunted fish torch", but the word for 'torch' is accompanied by a gesture indicating that it was
held. In another example, the ritual language Damin had a heavily reduced oral vocabulary of only a few
hundred words, each of which was very general in meaning, but which were supplemented by gesture for
greater precision (e.g., the single word for fish, l*i, was accompanied by a gesture to indicate the kind of
fish).[68]
Secondary modes of language, by which a fundamental mode is conveyed in a different medium, include
writing (including braille), sign (in manually coded language), whistling and drumming. Tertiary modes –
such as semaphore, Morse code and spelling alphabets – convey the secondary mode of writing in a
different medium. For some extinct languages that are maintained for ritual or liturgical purposes, writing
may be the primary mode, with speech secondary.
Structure
When described as a system of symbolic communication, language is traditionally seen as consisting of
three parts: signs, meanings, and a code connecting signs with their meanings. The study of the process of
semiosis, how signs and meanings are combined, used, and interpreted is called semiotics. Signs can be
composed of sounds, gestures, letters, or symbols, depending on whether the language is spoken, signed,
or written, and they can be combined into complex signs, such as words and phrases. When used in
communication, a sign is encoded and transmitted by a sender through a channel to a receiver who
decodes it.[69]
Semantics
Languages express meaning by relating a sign form to a meaning, or its content. Sign forms must be
something that can be perceived, for example, in sounds, images, or gestures, and then related to a
specific meaning by social convention. Because the basic relation of meaning for most linguistic signs is
based on social convention, linguistic signs can be considered arbitrary, in the sense that the convention is
established socially and historically, rather than by means of a natural relation between a specific sign
form and its meaning.[17]
Thus, languages must have a vocabulary of signs related to specific meaning. The English sign "dog"
denotes, for example, a member of the species Canis familiaris. In a language, the array of arbitrary signs
connected to specific meanings is called the lexicon, and a single sign connected to a meaning is called a
lexeme. Not all meanings in a language are represented by single words. Often, semantic concepts are
embedded in the morphology or syntax of the language in the form of grammatical categories.[72]
All languages contain the semantic structure of predication: a structure that predicates a property, state, or
action. Traditionally, semantics has been understood to be the study of how speakers and interpreters
assign truth values to statements, so that meaning is understood to be the process by which a predicate
can be said to be true or false about an entity, e.g. "[x [is y]]" or "[x [does y]]". Recently, this model of
semantics has been complemented with more dynamic models of meaning that incorporate shared
knowledge about the context in which a sign is interpreted into the production of meaning. Such models
of meaning are explored in the field of pragmatics.[72]
Sounds as part of a linguistic system are called phonemes.[74] Phonemes are abstract units of sound,
defined as the smallest units in a language that can serve to distinguish between the meaning of a pair of
minimally different words, a so-called minimal pair. In English, for example, the words bat [bæt] and pat
[pʰæt] form a minimal pair, in which the distinction between /b/ and /p/ differentiates the two words,
which have different meanings. However, each language contrasts sounds in different ways. For example,
in a language that does not distinguish between voiced and unvoiced consonants, the sounds [p] and [b]
(if they both occur) could be considered a single phoneme, and consequently, the two pronunciations
would have the same meaning. Similarly, the English language does not distinguish phonemically
between aspirated and non-aspirated pronunciations of consonants, as many other languages like Korean
and Hindi do: the unaspirated /p/ in spin [spɪn] and the aspirated /p/ in pin [pʰɪn] are considered to be
merely different ways of pronouncing the same phoneme (such variants of a single phoneme are called
allophones), whereas in Mandarin Chinese, the same difference in
pronunciation distinguishes between the words [pʰá] 'crouch' and [pá] 'eight'
(the accent above the á means that the vowel is pronounced with a high
tone).[75]
Writing systems represent language using visual symbols, which may or may
not correspond to the sounds of spoken language. The Latin alphabet (and
those on which it is based or that have been derived from it) was originally
based on the representation of single sounds, so that words were constructed
from letters that generally denote a single consonant or vowel in the The syllable "wi" in the
Hangul script
structure of the word. In syllabic scripts, such as the Inuktitut syllabary, each
sign represents a whole syllable. In logographic scripts, each sign represents
an entire word,[80] and will generally bear no relation to the sound of that
word in spoken language.
In order to represent the sounds of the world's languages in writing, linguists have developed the
International Phonetic Alphabet, designed to represent all of the discrete sounds that are known to
contribute to meaning in human languages.[82]
Grammar
Grammar is the study of how meaningful elements called morphemes within a language can be combined
into utterances. Morphemes can either be free or bound. If they are free to be moved around within an
utterance, they are usually called words, and if they are bound to other words or morphemes, they are
called affixes. The way in which meaningful elements can be combined within a language is governed by
rules. The study of the rules for the internal structure of words are called morphology. The rules of the
internal structure of phrases and sentences are called syntax.[83]
Grammatical categories
Grammar can be described as a system of categories and a set of rules that determine how categories
combine to form different aspects of meaning.[84] Languages differ widely in whether they are encoded
through the use of categories or lexical units. However, several categories are so common as to be nearly
universal. Such universal categories include the encoding of the grammatical relations of participants and
predicates by grammatically distinguishing between their relations to a predicate, the encoding of
temporal and spatial relations on predicates, and a system of grammatical person governing reference to
and distinction between speakers and addressees and those about whom they are speaking.[85]
Word classes
Languages organize their parts of speech into classes according to their functions and positions relative to
other parts. All languages, for instance, make a basic distinction between a group of words that
prototypically denotes things and concepts and a group of words that prototypically denotes actions and
events. The first group, which includes English words such as "dog" and "song", are usually called nouns.
The second, which includes "think" and "sing", are called verbs. Another common category is the
adjective: words that describe properties or qualities of nouns, such as "red" or "big". Word classes can be
"open" if new words can continuously be added to the class, or relatively "closed" if there is a fixed
number of words in a class. In English, the class of pronouns is closed, whereas the class of adjectives is
open, since an infinite number of adjectives can be constructed from verbs (e.g. "saddened") or nouns
(e.g. with the -like suffix, as in "noun-like"). In other languages such as Korean, the situation is the
opposite, and new pronouns can be constructed, whereas the number of adjectives is fixed.[86]
Word classes also carry out differing functions in grammar. Prototypically, verbs are used to construct
predicates, while nouns are used as arguments of predicates. In a sentence such as "Sally runs", the
predicate is "runs", because it is the word that predicates a specific state about its argument "Sally". Some
verbs such as "curse" can take two arguments, e.g. "Sally cursed John". A predicate that can only take a
single argument is called intransitive, while a predicate that can take two arguments is called
transitive.[87]
Many other word classes exist in different languages, such as conjunctions like "and" that serve to join
two sentences, articles that introduce a noun, interjections such as "wow!", or ideophones like "splash"
that mimic the sound of some event. Some languages have positionals that describe the spatial position of
an event or entity. Many languages have classifiers that identify countable nouns as belonging to a
particular type or having a particular shape. For instance, in Japanese, the general noun classifier for
humans is nin (人), and it is used for counting humans, whatever they are called:[88]
Morphology
In linguistics, the study of the internal structure of complex words and the processes by which words are
formed is called morphology. In most languages, it is possible to construct complex words that are built
of several morphemes. For instance, the English word "unexpected" can be analyzed as being composed
of the three morphemes "un-", "expect" and "-ed".[89]
Morphemes can be classified according to whether they are independent morphemes, so-called roots, or
whether they can only co-occur attached to other morphemes. These bound morphemes or affixes can be
classified according to their position in relation to the root: prefixes precede the root, suffixes follow the
root, and infixes are inserted in the middle of a root. Affixes serve to modify or elaborate the meaning of
the root. Some languages change the meaning of words by changing the phonological structure of a word,
for example, the English word "run", which in the past tense is "ran". This process is called ablaut.
Furthermore, morphology distinguishes between the process of inflection, which modifies or elaborates
on a word, and the process of derivation, which creates a new word from an existing one. In English, the
verb "sing" has the inflectional forms "singing" and "sung", which are both verbs, and the derivational
form "singer", which is a noun derived from the verb with the agentive suffix "-er".[90]
Languages differ widely in how much they rely on morphological processes of word formation. In some
languages, for example, Chinese, there are no morphological processes, and all grammatical information
is encoded syntactically by forming strings of single words. This type of morpho-syntax is often called
isolating, or analytic, because there is almost a full correspondence between a single word and a single
aspect of meaning. Most languages have words consisting of several morphemes, but they vary in the
degree to which morphemes are discrete units. In many languages, notably in most Indo-European
languages, single morphemes may have several distinct meanings that cannot be analyzed into smaller
segments. For example, in Latin, the word bonus, or "good", consists of the root bon-, meaning "good",
and the suffix -us, which indicates masculine gender, singular number, and nominative case. These
languages are called fusional languages, because several meanings may be fused into a single morpheme.
The opposite of fusional languages are agglutinative languages which construct words by stringing
morphemes together in chains, but with each morpheme as a discrete semantic unit. An example of such a
language is Turkish, where for example, the word evlerinizden, or "from your houses", consists of the
morphemes, ev-ler-iniz-den with the meanings house-plural-your-from. The languages that rely on
morphology to the greatest extent are traditionally called polysynthetic languages. They may express the
equivalent of an entire English sentence in a single word. For example, in Persian the single word
نفهمیدمش, nafahmidamesh means I didn't understand it consisting of morphemes na-fahm-id-am-esh
with the meanings, "negation.understand.past.I.it". As another example with more complexity, in the
Yupik word tuntussuqatarniksatengqiggtuq, which means "He had not yet said again that he was going to
hunt reindeer", the word consists of the morphemes tuntu-ssur-qatar-ni-ksaite-ngqiggte-uq with the
meanings, "reindeer-hunt-future-say-negation-again-third.person.singular.indicative", and except for the
morpheme tuntu ("reindeer") none of the other morphemes can appear in isolation.[91]
Many languages use morphology to cross-reference words within a sentence. This is sometimes called
agreement. For example, in many Indo-European languages, adjectives must cross-reference the noun
they modify in terms of number, case, and gender, so that the Latin adjective bonus, or "good", is
inflected to agree with a noun that is masculine gender, singular number, and nominative case. In many
polysynthetic languages, verbs cross-reference their subjects and objects. In these types of languages, a
single verb may include information that would require an entire sentence in English. For example, in the
Basque phrase ikusi nauzu, or "you saw me", the past tense auxiliary verb n-au-zu (similar to English
"do") agrees with both the subject (you) expressed by the n- prefix, and with the object (me) expressed by
the – zu suffix. The sentence could be directly transliterated as "see you-did-me"[92]
Syntax
Another way in which languages convey
meaning is through the order of words within a
sentence. The grammatical rules for how to
produce new sentences from words that are
already known is called syntax. The syntactical
rules of a language determine why a sentence in
English such as "I love you" is meaningful, but
"*love you I" is not.[note 4] Syntactical rules
determine how word order and sentence structure
is constrained, and how those constraints
contribute to meaning.[94] For example, in
English, the two sentences "the slaves were
cursing the master" and "the master was cursing In addition to word classes, a sentence can be
analyzed in terms of grammatical functions: "The cat"
the slaves" mean different things, because the
is the subject of the phrase, "on the mat" is a locative
role of the grammatical subject is encoded by the
phrase, and "sat" is the core of the predicate.
noun being in front of the verb, and the role of
object is encoded by the noun appearing after the
verb. Conversely, in Latin, both Dominus servos vituperabat and Servos vituperabat dominus mean "the
master was reprimanding the slaves", because servos, or "slaves", is in the accusative case, showing that
they are the grammatical object of the sentence, and dominus, or "master", is in the nominative case,
showing that he is the subject.[95]
Latin uses morphology to express the distinction between subject and object, whereas English uses word
order. Another example of how syntactic rules contribute to meaning is the rule of inverse word order in
questions, which exists in many languages. This rule explains why when in English, the phrase "John is
talking to Lucy" is turned into a question, it becomes "Who is John talking to?", and not "John is talking
to who?". The latter example may be used as a way of placing special emphasis on "who", thereby
slightly altering the meaning of the question. Syntax also includes the rules for how complex sentences
are structured by grouping words together in units, called phrases, that can occupy different places in a
larger syntactic structure. Sentences can be described as consisting of phrases connected in a tree
structure, connecting the phrases to each other at different levels.[96] To the right is a graphic
representation of the syntactic analysis of the English sentence "the cat sat on the mat". The sentence is
analyzed as being constituted by a noun phrase, a verb, and a prepositional phrase; the prepositional
phrase is further divided into a preposition and a noun phrase, and the noun phrases consist of an article
and a noun.[97]
The reason sentences can be seen as being composed of phrases is because each phrase would be moved
around as a single element if syntactic operations were carried out. For example, "the cat" is one phrase,
and "on the mat" is another, because they would be treated as single units if a decision was made to
emphasize the location by moving forward the prepositional phrase: "[And] on the mat, the cat sat".[97]
There are many different formalist and functionalist frameworks that propose theories for describing
syntactic structures, based on different assumptions about what language is and how it should be
described. Each of them would analyze a sentence such as this in a different manner.[22]
All languages structure sentences into Subject, Verb, and Object, but languages differ in the way they
classify the relations between actors and actions. English uses the nominative-accusative word typology:
in English transitive clauses, the subjects of both intransitive sentences ("I run") and transitive sentences
("I love you") are treated in the same way, shown here by the nominative pronoun I. Some languages,
called ergative, Gamilaraay among them, distinguish instead between Agents and Patients. In ergative
languages, the single participant in an intransitive sentence, such as "I run", is treated the same as the
patient in a transitive sentence, giving the equivalent of "me run". Only in transitive sentences would the
equivalent of the pronoun "I" be used.[99] In this way the semantic roles can map onto the grammatical
relations in different ways, grouping an intransitive subject either with Agents (accusative type) or
Patients (ergative type) or even making each of the three roles differently, which is called the tripartite
type.[102]
The shared features of languages which belong to the same typological class type may have arisen
completely independently. Their co-occurrence might be due to universal laws governing the structure of
natural languages, "language universals", or they might be the result of languages evolving convergent
solutions to the recurring communicative problems that humans use language to solve.[23]
The form of linguistic expression often does not correspond to the meaning that it actually has in a social
context. For example, if at a dinner table a person asks, "Can you reach the salt?", that is, in fact, not a
question about the length of the arms of the one being addressed, but a request to pass the salt across the
table. This meaning is implied by the context in which it is spoken; these kinds of effects of meaning are
called conversational implicatures. These social rules for which ways of using language are considered
appropriate in certain situations and how utterances are to be understood in relation to their context vary
between communities, and learning them is a large part of acquiring communicative competence in a
language.[106]
Acquisition
All healthy, normally developing human beings learn to use language. Children acquire the language or
languages used around them: whichever languages they receive sufficient exposure to during childhood.
The development is essentially the same for children acquiring sign or oral languages.[107] This learning
process is referred to as first-language acquisition, since unlike many other kinds of learning, it requires
no direct teaching or specialized study. In The Descent of Man, naturalist Charles Darwin called this
process "an instinctive tendency to acquire an art".[15]
First language acquisition proceeds in a fairly regular sequence,
though there is a wide degree of variation in the timing of
particular stages among normally developing infants. Studies
published in 2013 have indicated that unborn fetuses are capable
of language acquisition to some degree.[108][109] From birth,
newborns respond more readily to human speech than to other
sounds. Around one month of age, babies appear to be able to A lesson at Kituwah Academy, a
distinguish between different speech sounds. Around six months school where English and the
of age, a child will begin babbling, producing the speech sounds Cherokee language are mediums of
or handshapes of the languages used around them. Words appear instruction
around the age of 12 to 18 months; the average vocabulary of an
eighteen-month-old child is around 50 words. A child's first
utterances are holophrases (literally "whole-sentences"), utterances that use just one word to
communicate some idea. Several months after a child begins producing words, the child will produce
two-word utterances, and within a few more months will begin to produce telegraphic speech, or short
sentences that are less grammatically complex than adult speech, but that do show regular syntactic
structure. From roughly the age of three to five years, a child's ability to speak or sign is refined to the
point that it resembles adult language.[110][111]
Acquisition of second and additional languages can come at any age, through exposure in daily life or
courses. Children learning a second language are more likely to achieve native-like fluency than adults,
but in general, it is very rare for someone speaking a second language to pass completely for a native
speaker. An important difference between first language acquisition and additional language acquisition is
that the process of additional language acquisition is influenced by languages that the learner already
knows.[112]
Culture
Languages, understood as the particular set of speech norms of a
particular community, are also a part of the larger culture of the
community that speaks them. Languages differ not only in
pronunciation, vocabulary, and grammar, but also through having
different "cultures of speaking." Humans use language as a way of
signalling identity with one cultural group as well as difference
from others. Even among speakers of one language, several
different ways of using the language exist, and each is used to
signal affiliation with particular subgroups within a larger culture.
Linguists and anthropologists, particularly sociolinguists, Arnold Lakhovsky, The
ethnolinguists, and linguistic anthropologists have specialized in Conversation (c. 1935)
studying how ways of speaking vary between speech
communities.[113]
Linguists use the term "varieties" to refer to the different ways of speaking a language. This term includes
geographically or socioculturally defined dialects as well as the jargons or styles of subcultures.
Linguistic anthropologists and sociologists of language define communicative style as the ways that
language is used and understood within a particular culture.[114]
Because norms for language use are shared by members of a specific group, communicative style also
becomes a way of displaying and constructing group identity. Linguistic differences may become salient
markers of divisions between social groups, for example, speaking a language with a particular accent
may imply membership of an ethnic minority or social class, one's area of origin, or status as a second
language speaker. These kinds of differences are not part of the linguistic system, but are an important
part of how people use language as a social tool for constructing groups.[115]
However, many languages also have grammatical conventions that signal the social position of the
speaker in relation to others through the use of registers that are related to social hierarchies or divisions.
In many languages, there are stylistic or even grammatical differences between the ways men and women
speak, between age groups, or between social classes, just as some languages employ different words
depending on who is listening. For example, in the Australian language Dyirbal, a married man must use
a special set of words to refer to everyday items when speaking in the presence of his mother-in-law.[116]
Some cultures, for example, have elaborate systems of "social deixis", or systems of signalling social
distance through linguistic means.[117] In English, social deixis is shown mostly through distinguishing
between addressing some people by first name and others by surname, and in titles such as "Mrs.", "boy",
"Doctor", or "Your Honor", but in other languages, such systems may be highly complex and codified in
the entire grammar and vocabulary of the language. For instance, in languages of east Asia such as Thai,
Burmese, and Javanese, different words are used according to whether a speaker is addressing someone
of higher or lower rank than oneself in a ranking system with animals and children ranking the lowest and
gods and members of royalty as the highest.[117]
The invention of the first writing systems is roughly contemporary with the beginning of the Bronze Age
in the late 4th millennium BC. The Sumerian archaic cuneiform script and the Egyptian hieroglyphs are
generally considered to be the earliest writing systems, both emerging out of their ancestral proto-literate
symbol systems from 3400 to 3200 BC with the earliest coherent texts from about 2600 BC. It is
generally agreed that Sumerian writing was an independent invention; however, it is debated whether
Egyptian writing was developed completely independently of Sumerian, or was a case of cultural
diffusion. A similar debate exists for the Chinese script, which developed around 1200 BC. The pre-
Columbian Mesoamerican writing systems (including among others Olmec and Maya scripts) are
generally believed to have had independent origins.[81]
Change
All languages change as speakers adopt or invent new ways of speaking
and pass them on to other members of their speech community. Language
change happens at all levels from the phonological level to the levels of
vocabulary, morphology, syntax, and discourse. Even though language
change is often initially evaluated negatively by speakers of the language
who often consider changes to be "decay" or a sign of slipping norms of
language usage, it is natural and inevitable.[119]
Another example is the Great Vowel Shift in English, which is the reason that the spelling of English
vowels do not correspond well to their current pronunciation. This is because the vowel shift brought the
already established orthography out of synchronization with pronunciation. Another source of sound
change is the erosion of words as pronunciation gradually becomes increasingly indistinct and shortens
words, leaving out syllables or sounds. This kind of change caused Latin mea domina to eventually
become the French madame and American English ma'am.[121]
Change also happens in the grammar of languages as discourse patterns such as idioms or particular
constructions become grammaticalized. This frequently happens when words or morphemes erode and
the grammatical system is unconsciously rearranged to compensate for the lost element. For example, in
some varieties of Caribbean Spanish the final /s/ has eroded away. Since Standard Spanish uses final /s/ in
the morpheme marking the second person subject "you" in verbs, the Caribbean varieties now have to
express the second person using the pronoun tú. This means that the sentence "what's your name" is
¿como te llamas? [ˈkomo te ˈjamas] in Standard Spanish, but [ˈkomo ˈtu te ˈjama] in Caribbean Spanish.
The simple sound change has affected both morphology and syntax.[122] Another common cause of
grammatical change is the gradual petrification of idioms into new grammatical forms, for example, the
way the English "going to" construction lost its aspect of movement and in some varieties of English has
almost become a full-fledged future tense (e.g. I'm gonna).
Language change may be motivated by "language internal" factors, such as changes in pronunciation
motivated by certain sounds being difficult to distinguish aurally or to produce, or through patterns of
change that cause some rare types of constructions to drift towards more common types.[123] Other
causes of language change are social, such as when certain pronunciations become emblematic of
membership in certain groups, such as social classes, or with ideologies, and therefore are adopted by
those who wish to identify with those groups or ideas. In this way, issues of identity and politics can have
profound effects on language structure.[124]
Contact
One source of language change is contact and the resulting diffusion of
linguistic traits between languages. Language contact occurs when
speakers of two or more languages or varieties interact on a regular
basis.[125] Multilingualism is likely to have been the norm throughout
human history and most people in the modern world are multilingual.
Before the rise of the concept of the ethno-national state, monolingualism
was characteristic mainly of populations inhabiting small islands. But with
the ideology that made one people, one state, and one language the most
desirable political arrangement, monolingualism started to spread
throughout the world. There are only 250 countries in the world
corresponding to some 6,000 languages, which means that most countries
are multilingual and most languages therefore exist in close contact with
Multilingual sign outside the
other languages.[126]
mayor's office in Novi Sad,
written in the four official When speakers of different languages interact closely, it is typical for their
languages of the city: languages to influence each other. Through sustained language contact
Serbian, Hungarian, Slovak, over long periods, linguistic traits diffuse between languages, and
and Pannonian Rusyn languages belonging to different families may converge to become more
similar. In areas where many languages are in close contact, this may lead
to the formation of language areas in which unrelated languages share a
number of linguistic features. A number of such language areas have been documented, among them, the
Balkan language area, the Mesoamerican language area, and the Ethiopian language area. Also, larger
areas such as South Asia, Europe, and Southeast Asia have sometimes been considered language areas
because of the widespread diffusion of specific areal features.[127][128]
Language contact may also lead to a variety of other linguistic phenomena, including language
convergence, borrowing, and relexification (the replacement of much of the native vocabulary with that
of another language). In situations of extreme and sustained language contact, it may lead to the
formation of new mixed languages that cannot be considered to belong to a single language family. One
type of mixed language called pidgins occurs when adult speakers of two different languages interact on a
regular basis, but in a situation where neither group learns to speak the language of the other group
fluently. In such a case, they will often construct a communication
form that has traits of both languages, and that has a simplified
grammatical and phonological structure. The language comes to
contain mostly the grammatical and phonological categories that
exist in both languages. Pidgin languages are defined by not
having any native speakers, but only being spoken by people who
have another language as their first language. But if the Pidgin
language becomes the main language of a speech community, then
eventually children will grow up learning the Pidgin language as Multilingualism is also common in
their first language. As the generation of child learners grows up, the Indian Republic. The signboard
the pidgin will often be seen to change its structure and acquire a is displayed in the Imphal
greater degree of complexity. This type of language is generally International Airport in Meitei, Hindi
called a creole language. An example of such mixed languages is and English, some of the official
languages of the Indian Republic.
Tok Pisin, the official language of Papua New Guinea, which
originally arose as a Pidgin based on English and Austronesian
languages; others are Kreyòl ayisyen, the French-based creole language spoken in Haiti, and Michif, a
mixed language of Canada, based on the Native American language Cree and French.[129]
Linguistic diversity
SIL Ethnologue defines a "living language" as "one that has at least one
Native
speaker for whom it is their first language". The exact number of Language speakers
known living languages varies from 6,000 to 7,000, depending on the (millions)[130]
precision of one's definition of "language", and in particular, on how Mandarin 848
one defines the distinction between a "language" and a "dialect". As of
2016, Ethnologue cataloged 7,097 living human languages.[132] The Spanish 329 [note 5]
The language family of the world that has the most speakers is the Indo-European languages, spoken by
46% of the world's population.[137] This family includes major world languages like English, Spanish,
French, German, Russian, and Hindustani (Hindi/Urdu). The Indo-European family spread first through
hypothesized Indo-European migrations that would have taken place some time in the period c. 8000–
1500 BCE,[138] and subsequently through much later European colonial expansion, which brought the
Indo-European languages to a politically and often numerically dominant position in the Americas and
much of Africa. The Sino-Tibetan languages are spoken by 20%[137] of the world's population and
include many of the languages of East Asia, including Hakka, Mandarin Chinese, Cantonese, and
hundreds of smaller languages.[139]
Africa is home to a large number of language families, the largest of which is the Niger-Congo language
family, which includes such languages as Swahili, Shona, and Yoruba. Speakers of the Niger-Congo
languages account for 6.9% of the world's population.[137] A similar number of people speak the
Afroasiatic languages, which include the populous Semitic languages such as Arabic, Hebrew language,
and the languages of the Sahara region, such as the Berber languages and Hausa.[139]
The Austronesian languages are spoken by 5.5% of the world's population and stretch from Madagascar
to maritime Southeast Asia all the way to Oceania.[137] It includes such languages as Malagasy, Māori,
Samoan, and many of the indigenous languages of Indonesia and Taiwan. The Austronesian languages are
considered to have originated in Taiwan around 3000 BC and spread through the Oceanic region through
island-hopping, based on an advanced nautical technology. Other populous language families are the
Dravidian languages of South Asia (among them Kannada, Tamil, and Telugu), the Turkic languages of
Central Asia (such as Turkish), the Austroasiatic (among them Khmer), and Tai–Kadai languages of
Southeast Asia (including Thai).[139]
The areas of the world in which there is the greatest linguistic diversity, such as the Americas, Papua New
Guinea, West Africa, and South-Asia, contain hundreds of small language families. These areas together
account for the majority of the world's languages, though not the majority of speakers. In the Americas,
some of the largest language families include the Quechua, Arawak, and Tupi-Guarani families of South
America, the Uto-Aztecan, Oto-Manguean, and Mayan of Mesoamerica, and the Na-Dene, Iroquoian, and
Algonquian language families of North America. In Australia, most indigenous languages belong to the
Pama-Nyungan family, whereas New Guinea is home to a large number of small families and isolates, as
well as a number of Austronesian languages.[136] Due to its remoteness and geographical fragmentation,
Papua New Guinea emerges in fact as the leading location worldwide for both species (8% of world total)
and linguistic richness – with 830 living tongues (12% of world total).[140]
Language endangerment
Language endangerment occurs when a language
is at risk of falling out of use as its speakers die
out or shift to speaking another language.
Language loss occurs when the language has no
more native speakers, and becomes a dead
language. If eventually no one speaks the
language at all, it becomes an extinct language.
While languages have always gone extinct Together, these eight countries contain more than
throughout human history, they have been 50% of the world's languages.
disappearing at an accelerated rate in the 20th These areas are the most linguistically diverse [141]
and 21st centuries due to the processes of in the world, and the locations of most of the world's
globalization and neo-colonialism, where the endangered languages.
economically powerful languages dominate other
languages.[6]
The more commonly spoken languages dominate the less commonly spoken languages, so the less
commonly spoken languages eventually disappear from populations. Of the between 6,000[5] and 7,000
languages spoken as of 2010, between 50 and 90% of those are expected to have become extinct by the
year 2100.[6] The top 20 languages, those spoken by more than 50 million speakers each, are spoken by
50% of the world's population, whereas many of the other languages are spoken by smaller communities,
most of them with less than 10,000 speakers.[6]
The United Nations Educational, Scientific and Cultural Organization (UNESCO) operates with five
levels of language endangerment: "safe", "vulnerable" (not spoken by children outside the home),
"definitely endangered" (not spoken by children), "severely endangered" (only spoken by the oldest
generations), and "critically endangered" (spoken by a few members of the oldest generation, often semi-
speakers). Despite claims that the world would be better off if most adopted a single common lingua
franca, such as English or Esperanto, there is a consensus that the loss of languages harms the cultural
diversity of the world. It is a common belief, going back to the biblical narrative of the tower of Babel in
the Old Testament, that linguistic diversity causes political
conflict,[34] but many of the world's major episodes of violence
have taken place in situations with low linguistic diversity, such as
the Yugoslav and American Civil War, or the genocide of
Rwanda.[142]
UNESCO's five levels of language
Many projects aim to prevent or slow this loss by revitalizing
endangerment
endangered languages and promoting education and literacy in
minority languages. Across the world, many countries have
enacted specific legislation to protect and stabilize the language of indigenous speech communities. A
minority of linguists have argued that language loss is a natural process that should not be counteracted
and that documenting endangered languages for posterity is sufficient.[143]
The University of Waikato is using the Welsh language as a model for their Māori language revitalisation
programme, as they deem Welsh to be the world's leading example for the survival of languages.[144][145]
In 2019, Hawaiian TV company Oiwi visited a Welsh language centre in Nant Gwrtheyrn, North Wales,
to help find ways of preserving their Ōlelo Hawaiʻi language.[146]
See also
Language portal
Linguistics portal
Society portal
Religion portal
Notes
1. The gorilla Koko reportedly used as many as 1000 words in American Sign Language, and
understands 2000 words of spoken English. There are some doubts about whether her use
of signs is based on complex understanding or simple conditioning.[31]
2. "Functional grammar analyzes grammatical structure, as do formal and structural grammar;
but it also analyzes the entire communicative situation: the purpose of the speech event, its
participants, its discourse context. Functionalists maintain that the communicative situation
motivates, constrains, explains, or otherwise determines grammatical structure, and that a
structural or formal approach is not merely limited to an artificially restricted data base, but is
inadequate even as a structural account. Functional grammar, then, differs from formal and
structural grammar in that it purports not to model but to explain; and the explanation is
grounded in the communicative situation".[54]
3. While sign is usually a visual medium, there is also tactile signing; and while oral speech is
usually an aural medium, there is also lipreading and tadoma.
4. The prefixed asterisk * conventionally indicates that the sentence is ungrammatical, i.e.
syntactically incorrect.[93]
5. Ethnologue's figure is based on numbers from before 1995. A more recent figure is 420
million.[131]
References
1. Nicholas Evans & Stephen Levinson 5. Moseley (2010): "Statistics (http://www.une
(2009) 'The Myth of Language Universals: sco.org/culture/languages-atlas/index.php)
Language Diversity and Its Importance for Archived (https://web.archive.org/web/201
Cognitive Science'. Behavioral and Brain 40812000456/http://www.unesco.org/cultur
Sciences 32, 429–492. e/languages-atlas/index.php) 12 August
2. Kamusella, Tomasz (2016). "The History of 2014 at the Wayback Machine"
the Normative Opposition of 'Language 6. Austin & Sallabank (2011)
versus Dialect': From Its Graeco-Latin 7. Graddol, David (27 February 2004). "The
Origin to Central Europe's Ethnolinguistic Future of Language". Science. 303 (5662):
Nation-States" (http://ispan.waw.pl/journal 1329–1331. Bibcode:2004Sci...303.1329G
s/index.php/ch/article/download/ch.2016.0 (https://ui.adsabs.harvard.edu/abs/2004Sc
11/2342). Colloquia Humanistica. 5 (5): i...303.1329G).
189–198. doi:10.11649/ch.2016.011 (http doi:10.1126/science.1096546 (https://doi.o
s://doi.org/10.11649%2Fch.2016.011). rg/10.1126%2Fscience.1096546).
Archived (https://web.archive.org/web/202 ISSN 0036-8075 (https://search.worldcat.o
00226064031/https://ispan.waw.pl/journal rg/issn/0036-8075). PMID 14988552 (http
s/index.php/ch/article/download/ch.2016.0 s://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/14988552).
11/2342) from the original on 26 February S2CID 35904484 (https://api.semanticscho
2020. Retrieved 9 February 2020. lar.org/CorpusID:35904484).
3. Tomasello (1996) 8. "language". The American Heritage
4. Hauser, Chomsky & Fitch (2002) Dictionary of the English Language
(3rd ed.). Boston: Houghton Mifflin
Company. 1992.
9. Lyons (1981:2)
10. Lyons (1981:1–8)
11. Trask (2007:129–131)
12. Bett 2010. 30. Engesser, Sabrina; Ridley, Amanda R.;
13. Devitt & Sterelny 1999. Townsend, Simon W. (20 July 2017).
14. Hauser & Fitch (2003) "Element repetition rates encode
functionally distinct information in pied
15. Pinker (1994) babbler 'clucks' and 'purrs' " (http://wrap.wa
16. Trask 2007, p. 93. rwick.ac.uk/89798/7/WRAP-element-repetit
17. Saussure (1983) ion-rates-encode-functionally-Townsend-2
18. Campbell (2001:96) 017.pdf) (PDF). Animal Cognition. 20 (5):
953–960. doi:10.1007/s10071-017-1114-6
19. Trask 2007, p. 130. (https://doi.org/10.1007%2Fs10071-017-11
20. Chomsky (1957) 14-6). PMID 28730513 (https://pubmed.nc
21. Trask (2007:93, 130) bi.nlm.nih.gov/28730513).
22. Newmeyer (1998:3–6) S2CID 21470061 (https://api.semanticscho
23. Evans & Levinson (2009) lar.org/CorpusID:21470061). Archived (http
s://web.archive.org/web/20190428232241/
24. Van Valin (2001) http://wrap.warwick.ac.uk/89798/7/WRAP-
25. Nerlich 2010, p. 192. element-repetition-rates-encode-functionall
26. Hockett, Charles F. (1966). "The Problem y-Townsend-2017.pdf) (PDF) from the
of Universals in Language" (https://web.arc original on 28 April 2019. Retrieved
hive.org/web/20121110023354/http://www. 9 November 2018.
ling.ohio-state.edu/~swinters/371/designfe 31. Candland (1993)
atures.html). Archived from the original (htt 32. Deacon (1997)
p://www.ling.ohio-state.edu/~swinters/371/
33. Trask (2007:165–166)
designfeatures.html) on 10 November
2012. Retrieved 11 May 2013. 34. Haugen (1973)
27. Hockett (1960); Deacon (1997) 35. Shanahan, D. (2011). Language, feeling,
and the brain: The evocative vector.
28. Trask (1999:1–5)
Transaction Publishers.
29. Engesser, Sabrina; Crane, Jodie M. S.;
Savage, James L.; Russel, Andrew F.; 36. Ulbaek (1998)
Townsend, Simon W. (29 June 2015). 37. Tomasello (2008)
"Experimental Evidence for Phonemic 38. Fitch 2010, pp. 466–507.
Contrasts in a Nonhuman Vocal System" 39. Anderson (2012:107)
(https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/
40. Anderson (2012:104)
PMC4488142). PLOS Biology. 13 (6):
e1002171. 41. Fitch 2010, pp. 250–292.
doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.1002171 (https://d 42. Clark, Gary; Henneberg, Maciej (2017).
oi.org/10.1371%2Fjournal.pbio.1002171). "Ardipithecus ramidus and the evolution of
PMC 4488142 (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.g language and singing: An early origin for
ov/pmc/articles/PMC4488142). hominin vocal capability". HOMO. 68 (2):
PMID 26121619 (https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm. 101–121. doi:10.1016/j.jchb.2017.03.001
nih.gov/26121619). (https://doi.org/10.1016%2Fj.jchb.2017.03.
001). PMID 28363458 (https://pubmed.ncb
i.nlm.nih.gov/28363458).
43. Foley 1997, pp. 70–74.
44. Fitch 2010, pp. 292–293.
45. Chomsky 1972, p. 86.
46. Chomsky 2000, p. 4.
47. Keys, David (25 March 2024). "The 1.6 77. Stokoe, William C. (1960). Sign Language
million-year-old discovery that changes Structure: An Outline of the Visual
what we know about human evolution - Communication Systems of the American
New research suggests language is eight Deaf, Studies in linguistics: Occasional
times older than previously thought" (http papers (No. 8). Buffalo: Dept. of
s://www.independent.co.uk/news/science/a Anthropology and Linguistics, University of
rchaeology/human-evolution-language-orig Buffalo.
in-archaeology-b2517744.html). The 78. Stokoe, William C.; Dorothy C. Casterline;
Independent. Archived (https://archive.toda Carl G. Croneberg (1965). A dictionary of
y/20240325041616/https://www.independe American sign languages on linguistic
nt.co.uk/news/science/archaeology/human principles. Washington, D.C.: Gallaudet
-evolution-language-origin-archaeology-b2 College Press
517744.html) from the original on 25
79. Sandler & Lillo-Martin (2001:539–540)
March 2024. Retrieved 25 March 2024.
80. Trask (2007:326)
48. Newmeyer (2005)
81. Coulmas (2002)
49. Trask (2007)
82. Trask (2007:123)
50. Campbell (2001:82–83)
83. Lyons (1981:103)
51. Bloomfield 1914, p. 310
84. Allerton (1989)
52. Clarke (1990:143–144)
85. Payne (1997)
53. Foley (1997:82–83)
86. Trask (2007:208)
54. Nichols (1984)
87. Trask (2007:305)
55. Croft & Cruse (2004:1–4)
88. Senft (2008)
56. Trask (1999:11–14, 105–113)
89. Aronoff & Fudeman (2011:1–2)
57. Fisher, Lai & Monaco (2003)
90. Bauer (2003); Haspelmath (2002)
58. Lesser (1989:205–206)
91. Payne (1997:28–29)
59. Trask (1999:105–107)
92. Trask (2007:11)
60. Trask (1999:108)
93. Denham, Kristin; Lobeck, Anne (2009).
61. Sandler & Lillo-Martin (2001:554) Linguistics for Everyone: An Introduction (h
62. MacMahon (1989:2) ttps://books.google.com/books?id=n0KVSv
63. MacMahon (1989:3) qvZKYC&pg=PA9). Cengage Learning.
64. International Phonetic Association p. 9. ISBN 978-1-4130-1589-8. Archived (h
(1999:3–8) ttps://web.archive.org/web/202005221014
50/https://books.google.com/books?id=n0
65. MacMahon (1989:11–15)
KVSvqvZKYC&pg=PA9) from the original
66. MacMahon (1989:6–11) on 22 May 2020. Retrieved 16 April 2020.
67. Ladefoged & Maddieson (1996) "An ungrammatical sentence is one that is
68. Nicholas Evans (2017) Listening Here: impossible in a given language, one that a
Ngûrrahmalkwonawoniyan. Humanities native speaker of that variety would never
Australia, Journal of the Australian utter naturally. (Remember that
Academy of the Humanities, vol 8, p 39. ungrammatical sentences are marked with
69. Lyons (1981:17–24) an asterisk, *."
70. Trask (1999:35) 94. Baker (2001:265)
71. Lyons (1981:218–224) 95. Trask (2007:179)
72. Levinson (1983) 96. Baker 2001, pp. 269–270.
73. Goldsmith (1995) 97. Trask (2007:218–219)
74. International Phonetic Association 98. Nichols (1992);Comrie (1989)
(1999:27) 99. Croft (2001:340)
75. Trask (2007:214) 100. Greenberg (1966)
76. International Phonetic Association (1999:4) 101. Comrie (2009:45); McMahon (1994:156)
102. Croft (2001:355)
103. Campbell (2004) 131. "Primer estudio conjunto del Instituto
104. Levinson (1983:54–96) Cervantes y el British Council sobre el
peso internacional del español y del
105. Levinson (1983:226–278)
inglés" (http://www.cervantes.es/sobre_inst
106. Levinson (1983:100–169) ituto_cervantes/prensa/2012/noticias/nota-l
107. Bonvillian, John D.; Michael D. Orlansky; ondres-palabra-por-palabra.htm). Instituto
Leslie Lazin Novack (December 1983). Cervantes (www.cervantes.es). Archived (h
"Developmental milestones: Sign language ttps://web.archive.org/web/201709162041
acquisition and motor development". Child 18/http://www.cervantes.es/sobre_instituto
Development. 54 (6): 1435–1445. _cervantes/prensa/2012/noticias/nota-lond
doi:10.2307/1129806 (https://doi.org/10.23 res-palabra-por-palabra.htm) from the
07%2F1129806). JSTOR 1129806 (https:// original on 16 September 2017. Retrieved
www.jstor.org/stable/1129806). 17 September 2012.
PMID 6661942 (https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.ni 132. "Ethnologue statistics" (http://www.ethnolo
h.gov/6661942). gue.com/statistics). Summary by world
108. Scientific American (2015:24) area | Ethnologue. SIL. Archived (https://w
109. Skwarecki, Beth (26 August 2013). "Babies eb.archive.org/web/20160108221353/htt
Learn to Recognize Words in the Womb" p://www.ethnologue.com/statistics) from
(https://web.archive.org/web/20151104124 the original on 8 January 2016. Retrieved
655/http://news.sciencemag.org/brain-beh 11 September 2014.
avior/2013/08/babies-learn-recognize-word 133. Rickerson, E.M. "What's the difference
s-womb). Science. Archived from the between dialect and language?" (https://we
original (https://www.science.org/content/ar b.archive.org/web/20101219070627/http://
ticle/babies-learn-recognize-words-womb) spinner.cofc.edu/linguist/archives/2005/08/
on 4 November 2015. whats_the_diffe.html?referrer=webcluster
110. O'Grady, William; Cho, Sook Whan (2001). &). The Five Minute Linguist. College of
"First language acquisition". Contemporary Charleston. Archived from the original (htt
Linguistics: An Introduction (4th ed.). p://spinner.cofc.edu/linguist/archives/2005/
Boston: Bedford St. Martin's. 08/whats_the_diffe.html?referrer=webclust
111. Kennison (2013) er&) on 19 December 2010. Retrieved
17 July 2011.
112. Macaro (2010:137–157)
134. Lyons (1981:26)
113. Duranti (2003)
135. Krämer, Philipp; Vogl, Ulrike; Kolehmainen,
114. Foley (1997)
Leena; et al. (2022). "What is language
115. Agha (2006) making?" (https://doi.org/10.1515%2Fijsl-2
116. Dixon (1972:32–34) 021-0016). International Journal of the
117. Foley (1997:311–328) Sociology of Language (274): 1–27.
118. Olson (1996) doi:10.1515/ijsl-2021-0016 (https://doi.org/
10.1515%2Fijsl-2021-0016). hdl:1854/LU-
119. Aitchison (2001); Trask (1999:70) 8745135 (https://hdl.handle.net/1854%2FL
120. Clackson (2007:27–33) U-8745135).
121. Aitchison (2001:112) 136. Katzner (1999)
122. Zentella (2002:178) 137. Lewis (2009), "Summary by language
123. Labov (1994) family (http://www.ethnologue.com/statistic
124. Labov (2001) s/family) Archived (https://web.archive.org/
web/20160101054116/http://www.ethnolog
125. Thomason (2001:1)
ue.com/statistics/family) 1 January 2016 at
126. Romaine (2001:513) the Wayback Machine"
127. Campbell (2002)
128. Aikhenvald (2001)
129. Thomason & Kaufman (1988); Thomason
(2001); Matras & Bakker (2003)
130. Lewis (2009)
138. Heggarty, P.; Anderson, C.; Scarborough, 144. "University of Waikato Launches a
M.; King, B.; Bouckaert, R.; Jocz, L.; Strategic Partnership with Cardiff
Kümmel, M.J.; Jügel, T.; Irslinger, B.; University in Wales" (https://www.scoop.co.
Pooth, R.; Liljegren, H (28 July 2023). nz/stories/ED2111/S00030/university-of-w
"Language trees with sampled ancestors aikato-launches-a-strategic-partnership.ht
support a hybrid model for the origin of m) (Press release). University of Waikato.
Indo-European languages" (http://urn.kb.s 10 November 2021. Archived (https://web.
e/resolve?urn=urn:nbn:se:su:diva-220000). archive.org/web/20211124055642/https://
Science. 381 (6656): eabg0818. www.scoop.co.nz/stories/ED2111/S00030/
doi:10.1126/science.abg0818 (https://doi.o university-of-waikato-launches-a-strategic-
rg/10.1126%2Fscience.abg0818). partnership.htm) from the original on 24
hdl:10234/204329 (https://hdl.handle.net/1 November 2021. Retrieved 21 December
0234%2F204329). PMID 37499002 (http 2021 – via Scoop News.
s://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/37499002). 145. Rhiannon James (10 November 2021).
139. Comrie (2009); Brown & Ogilvie (2008) "Council investing £6.4m in the future of
140. Briand, Frederic (February 2013). "Silent the Welsh language" (https://nation.cymru/
Plains … the Fading Sounds of Native news/council-investing-6-4m-in-the-future-
Languages" (https://www.researchgate.net/ of-the-welsh-language/). Nation Cymru.
publication/235742552). National Archived (https://web.archive.org/web/202
Geographic. 11111205030/https://nation.cymru/news/co
141. Greenberg, Joseph H. (1956). "The uncil-investing-6-4m-in-the-future-of-the-w
Measurement of Linguistic Diversity". elsh-language/) from the original on 11
Language. 32 (1): 109–115. November 2021. Retrieved 21 December
2021.
doi:10.2307/410659 (https://doi.org/10.230
7%2F410659). JSTOR 410659 (https://ww 146. "Hawaiian TV company seeks help to
w.jstor.org/stable/410659). promote language" (https://www.cambrian-
142. Austin & Sallabank (2011:10–11) news.co.uk/article.cfm?id=130057&headlin
e=Hawaiian%20TV%20company%20seek
143. Ladefoged (1992) s%20help%20to%20promote%20language
§ionIs=news&searchyear=2019).
Cambrian News. 20 August 2019. Archived
(https://web.archive.org/web/20211205155
429/https://www.cambrian-news.co.uk/artic
le.cfm?id=130057&headline=Hawaiian%20
TV%20company%20seeks%20help%20t
o%20promote%20language§ionIs=ne
ws&searchyear=2019) from the original on
5 December 2021. Retrieved 21 August
2021.
Works cited
Agha, Asif (2006). Language and Social Relations. Cambridge University Press.
Aikhenvald, Alexandra (2001). "Introduction". In Alexandra Y. Aikhenvald; R.M.W. Dixon (eds.).
Areal diffusion and genetic inheritance: problems in comparative linguistics. Oxford: Oxford
University Press. pp. 1–26.
Aitchison, Jean (2001) [1981]. Language Change: Progress or Decay? (3rd ed.). Cambridge,
New York, Melbourne: Cambridge University Press.
Allerton, D.J. (1989). "Language as Form and Pattern: Grammar and its Categories". In
Collinge, N.E. (ed.). An Encyclopedia of Language. London:NewYork: Routledge.
Anderson, Stephen (2012). Languages: A Very Short Introduction. Oxford: Oxford University
Press. ISBN 978-0-19-959059-9.
Aronoff, Mark; Fudeman, Kirsten (2011). What is Morphology. John Wiley & Sons.
Austin, Peter K; Sallabank, Julia (2011). "Introduction". In Austin, Peter K; Sallabank, Julia
(eds.). Cambridge Handbook of Endangered Languages. Cambridge University Press.
ISBN 978-0-521-88215-6.
Baker, Mark C. (2001). "Syntax". In Mark Aronoff; Janie Rees-Miller (eds.). The Handbook of
Linguistics. Blackwell. pp. 265–295.
Bauer, Laurie (2003). Introducing linguistic morphology (2nd ed.). Washington, D.C.:
Georgetown University Press. ISBN 978-0-87840-343-1.
Bett, R. (2010). "Plato and his Predecessors". In Alex Barber; Robert J Stainton (eds.). Concise
Encyclopedia of Philosophy of Language and Linguistics. Elsevier. pp. 569–570.
Bloomfield, Leonard (1914). An introduction to the study of language (https://archive.org/details/
introductiontos00bloo). New York: Henry Holt and Company.
Brown, Keith; Ogilvie, Sarah, eds. (2008). Concise Encyclopedia of Languages of the World.
Elsevier Science. ISBN 978-0-08-087774-7.
Clackson, James (2007). Indo-European Linguistics: An Introduction. Cambridge University
press.
Campbell, Lyle (2002). "Areal linguistics". In Bernard Comrie, Neil J. Smelser and Paul B. Balte
(ed.). International Encyclopedia of Social and Behavioral Sciences. Oxford: Pergamon.
pp. 729–733.
Campbell, Lyle (2004). Historical Linguistics: an Introduction (2nd ed.). Edinburgh and
Cambridge, MA: Edinburgh University Press and MIT Press.
Campbell, Lyle (2001). "The History of Linguistics". In Mark Aronoff; Janie Rees-Miller (eds.).
The Handbook of Linguistics. Blackwell. pp. 81–105.
Candland, Douglas Keith (1993). Feral Children and Clever Animals: Reflections on Human
Nature (https://archive.org/details/feralchildrencle00cand). Oxford University Press US.
pp. 293 (https://archive.org/details/feralchildrencle00cand/page/293)–301. ISBN 978-0-19-
510284-0. "koko gorilla operant conditioning."
Chomsky, Noam (1957). Syntactic Structures. The Hague: Mouton.
Chomsky, Noam (1972). Language and Mind (https://archive.org/details/languagemind00chom).
Harcourt Brace Jovanovich. ISBN 978-0-15-549257-8.
Chomsky, Noam (2000). The Architecture of Language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Clarke, David S. (1990). Sources of semiotic: readings with commentary from antiquity to the
present. Carbondale: Southern Illinois University Press.
Comrie, Bernard (1989). Language universals and linguistic typology: Syntax and morphology
(2nd ed.). Oxford: Blackwell. ISBN 978-0-226-11433-0.
Comrie, Bernard, ed. (2009). The World's Major Languages. New York: Routledge. ISBN 978-0-
415-35339-7.
Coulmas, Florian (2002). Writing Systems: An Introduction to Their Linguistic Analysis.
Cambridge University Press.
Croft, William; Cruse, D. Alan (2004). Cognitive Linguistics. Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press.
Croft, William (2001). "Typology". In Mark Aronoff; Janie Rees-Miller (eds.). The Handbook of
Linguistics. Blackwell. pp. 81–105.
Deacon, Terrence (1997). The Symbolic Species: The Co-evolution of Language and the Brain.
New York: W.W. Norton & Company. ISBN 978-0-393-31754-1.
Devitt, Michael; Sterelny, Kim (1999). Language and Reality: An Introduction to the Philosophy
of Language. Boston: MIT Press.
Dixon, Robert M.W. (1972). The Dyirbal Language of North Queensland (https://archive.org/det
ails/dyirballanguageo00dixo). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. ISBN 978-0-521-
08510-6.
Duranti, Alessandro (2003). "Language as Culture in U.S. Anthropology: Three Paradigms".
Current Anthropology. 44 (3): 323–348. doi:10.1086/368118 (https://doi.org/10.1086%2F368
118). S2CID 148075449 (https://api.semanticscholar.org/CorpusID:148075449).
Evans, Nicholas; Levinson, Stephen C. (2009). "The myth of language universals: Language
diversity and its importance for cognitive science" (https://doi.org/10.1017%2Fs0140525x09
99094x). Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 32 (5): 429–492. doi:10.1017/s0140525x0999094x
(https://doi.org/10.1017%2Fs0140525x0999094x). hdl:11858/00-001M-0000-0012-C29E-4
(https://hdl.handle.net/11858%2F00-001M-0000-0012-C29E-4). PMID 19857320 (https://pu
bmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/19857320).
"First Impressions: We start to pick up words, food preferences and hand-eye coordination long
before being born". Scientific American. Vol. 313, no. 1. July 2015. p. 24.
Fisher, Simon E.; Lai, Cecilia S.L.; Monaco, Anthony P. (2003). "Deciphering the Genetic Basis
of Speech and Language Disorders". Annual Review of Neuroscience. 26: 57–80.
doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.26.041002.131144 (https://doi.org/10.1146%2Fannurev.neuro.2
6.041002.131144). hdl:11858/00-001M-0000-0012-CB7D-6 (https://hdl.handle.net/11858%2
F00-001M-0000-0012-CB7D-6). PMID 12524432 (https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/1252443
2).
Fitch, W. Tecumseh (2010). The Evolution of Language. Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press.
Foley, William A. (1997). Anthropological Linguistics: An Introduction. Blackwell.
Goldsmith, John A (1995). "Phonological Theory". In John A. Goldsmith (ed.). The Handbook of
Phonological Theory. Blackwell Handbooks in Linguistics. Blackwell Publishers. ISBN 978-
1-4051-5768-1.
Greenberg, Joseph (1966). Language Universals: With Special Reference to Feature
Hierarchies. The Hague: Mouton & Co.
Haspelmath, Martin (2002). Understanding morphology. London: Arnold, Oxford University
Press. (pbk)
Haugen, Einar (1973). "The Curse of Babel". Daedalus. 102 (3, Language as a Human
Problem): 47–57.
Hauser, Marc D.; Chomsky, Noam; Fitch, W. Tecumseh (2002). "The Faculty of Language: What
Is It, Who Has It, and How Did It Evolve?". Science. 298 (5598): 1569–1579.
doi:10.1126/science.298.5598.1569 (https://doi.org/10.1126%2Fscience.298.5598.1569).
PMID 12446899 (https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/12446899).
Hauser, Marc D.; Fitch, W. Tecumseh (2003). "What are the uniquely human components of the
language faculty?" (https://web.archive.org/web/20140808045239/http://mind.cog.jhu.edu/fa
culty/smolensky/050.341-641/Readings/Hauser%20%26%20Fitch%2003%20Evolution%20
of%20Lg.pdf) (PDF). In M.H. Christiansen; S. Kirby (eds.). Language Evolution: The States
of the Art. Oxford University Press. Archived from the original (http://mind.cog.jhu.edu/facult
y/smolensky/050.341-641/Readings/Hauser%20&%20Fitch%2003%20Evolution%20of%20
Lg.pdf) (PDF) on 8 August 2014.
Hockett, Charles F. (1960). "Logical considerations in the study of animal communication". In W.
E. Lanyon; W. N. Tavolga (eds.). Animals sounds and animal communication. pp. 392–430.
International Phonetic Association (1999). Handbook of the International Phonetic Association:
A guide to the use of the International Phonetic Alphabet. Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press. ISBN 978-0-521-65236-0.
Katzner, Kenneth (1999). The Languages of the World. New York: Routledge.
Kennison, Shelia (2013). Introduction to Language Development. SAGE.
Labov, William (1994). Principles of Linguistic Change vol.I Internal Factors. Blackwell.
Labov, William (2001). Principles of Linguistic Change vol.II Social Factors. Blackwell.
Ladefoged, Peter (1992). "Another view of endangered languages". Language. 68 (4): 809–811.
doi:10.1353/lan.1992.0013 (https://doi.org/10.1353%2Flan.1992.0013). S2CID 144984900
(https://api.semanticscholar.org/CorpusID:144984900).
Ladefoged, Peter; Maddieson, Ian (1996). The sounds of the world's languages. Oxford:
Blackwell. pp. 329–330. ISBN 978-0-631-19815-4.
Lesser, Ruth (1989). "Language in the Brain: Neurolinguistics". In Collinge, N.E. (ed.). An
Encyclopedia of Language. London:NewYork: Routledge.
Levinson, Stephen C. (1983). Pragmatics (https://archive.org/details/pragmatics00levi).
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Lewis, M. Paul, ed. (2009). "Ethnologue: Languages of the World" (http://www.ethnologue.com/
ethno_docs/distribution.asp?by=size) (16th ed.). Dallas, Tex.: SIL International. Archived (htt
ps://web.archive.org/web/20120404174730/http://www.ethnologue.com/ethno_docs/distribut
ion.asp?by=size) from the original on 4 April 2012. Retrieved 25 August 2012.
Lyons, John (1981). Language and Linguistics (https://archive.org/details/languagelinguist0000l
yon). Cambridge University Press. ISBN 978-0-521-29775-2.
Macaro, Ernesto, ed. (2010). Continuum companion to second language acquisition. London:
Continuum. pp. 137–157. ISBN 978-1-4411-9922-5.
McMahon, April M.S. (1994). Understanding Language Change. Cambridge University Press.
ISBN 978-0-521-44119-3.
MacMahon, M.K.C. (1989). "Language as available sound:Phonetics". In Collinge, N.E. (ed.).
An Encyclopedia of Language. London:NewYork: Routledge.
Matras, Yaron; Bakker, Peter, eds. (2003). The Mixed Language Debate: Theoretical and
Empirical Advances. Berlin: Walter de Gruyter. ISBN 978-3-11-017776-3.
Moseley, Christopher, ed. (2010). Atlas of the World's Languages in Danger (http://www.unesco.
org/culture/en/endangeredlanguages/atlas) (3rd ed.). Paris: UNESCO Publishing. Archived
(https://web.archive.org/web/20140820135739/http://www.unesco.org/culture/en/endangere
dlanguages/atlas/) from the original on 20 August 2014. Retrieved 7 August 2012.
Nerlich, Brigitte (2010). "History of pragmatics". In Cummings, Louise (ed.). The Pragmatics
Encyclopedia. London/New York: Routledge. pp. 192–193.
Newmeyer, Frederick J. (2005). The History of Linguistics (http://www.linguisticsociety.org/resou
rce/history-modern-linguistics). Linguistic Society of America. ISBN 978-0-415-11553-7.
Archived (https://web.archive.org/web/20141231094048/http://www.linguisticsociety.org/reso
urce/history-modern-linguistics) from the original on 31 December 2014. Retrieved
18 September 2014.
Newmeyer, Frederick J. (1998). Language Form and Language Function (https://web.archive.or
g/web/20111216100353/http://www.isc.cnrs.fr/FN_chapter1.pdf) (PDF). Cambridge, MA:
MIT Press. Archived from the original (http://www.isc.cnrs.fr/FN_chapter1.pdf) (PDF) on 16
December 2011. Retrieved 29 August 2012.
Nichols, Johanna (1992). Linguistic diversity in space and time. Chicago: University of Chicago
Press. ISBN 978-0-226-58057-9.
Nichols, Johanna (1984). "Functional Theories of Grammar". Annual Review of Anthropology.
13: 97–117. doi:10.1146/annurev.an.13.100184.000525 (https://doi.org/10.1146%2Fannure
v.an.13.100184.000525).
Olson, David R. (1996). "Language and Literacy: what writing does to Language and Mind".
Annual Review of Applied Linguistics. 16: 3–13. doi:10.1017/S0267190500001392 (https://d
oi.org/10.1017%2FS0267190500001392). S2CID 145677801 (https://api.semanticscholar.or
g/CorpusID:145677801).
Payne, Thomas Edward (1997). Describing morphosyntax: a guide for field linguists (https://boo
ks.google.com/books?id=LC3DfjWfCiwC&q=%22perfect+aspect%22+%22perfective+aspec
t%22&pg=PA239). Cambridge University Press. pp. 238–241. ISBN 978-0-521-58805-8.
Archived (https://web.archive.org/web/20210331142644/https://books.google.com/books?id
=LC3DfjWfCiwC&q=%22perfect+aspect%22+%22perfective+aspect%22&pg=PA239) from
the original on 31 March 2021. Retrieved 14 November 2020.
Pinker, Steven (1994). The Language Instinct: How the Mind Creates Language. Perennial.
Romaine, Suzanne (2001). "Multilingualism". In Mark Aronoff; Janie Rees-Miller (eds.). The
Handbook of Linguistics. Blackwell. pp. 512–533.
Sandler, Wendy; Lillo-Martin, Diane (2001). "Natural Sign Languages". In Mark Aronoff; Janie
Rees-Miller (eds.). The Handbook of Linguistics. Blackwell. pp. 533–563.
Saussure, Ferdinand de (1983) [1913]. Bally, Charles; Sechehaye, Albert (eds.). Course in
General Linguistics. Translated by Roy Harris. La Salle, Illinois: Open Court. ISBN 978-0-
8126-9023-1.
Senft, Gunter, ed. (2008). Systems of Nominal Classification. Cambridge University Press.
ISBN 978-0-521-06523-8.
Tomasello, Michael (1996). "The Cultural Roots of Language". In B. Velichkovsky and D.
Rumbaugh (ed.). Communicating Meaning: The Evolution and Development of Language.
Psychology Press. pp. 275–308. ISBN 978-0-8058-2118-5.
Tomasello, Michael (2008). Origin of Human Communication. MIT Press.
Thomason, Sarah G.; Kaufman, Terrence (1988). Language Contact, Creolization and Genetic
Linguistics. University of California Press.
Thomason, Sarah G. (2001). Language Contact – An Introduction. Edinburgh University Press.
Trask, Robert Lawrence (1999). Language: The Basics (2nd ed.). Psychology Press.
Trask, Robert Lawrence (2007). Stockwell, Peter (ed.). Language and Linguistics: The Key
Concepts (2nd ed.). Routledge.
Ulbaek, Ib (1998). "The Origin of Language and Cognition". In J. R. Hurford & C. Knight (ed.).
Approaches to the evolution of language. Cambridge University Press. pp. 30–43.
Van Valin, Robert D. Jr. (2001). "Functional Linguistics". In Mark Aronoff; Janie Rees-Miller
(eds.). The Handbook of Linguistics. Blackwell. pp. 319–337.
Zentella, Ana Celia (2002). "Spanish in New York". In García, Ofelia; Fishman, Joshua (eds.).
The Multilingual Apple: Languages in New York City. Walter de Gruyter.
Further reading
Crystal, David (1997). The Cambridge Encyclopedia of Language. Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.
Cysouw, Michael; Good, Jeff (2013). "Languoid, doculect and glossonym: Formalizing the
notion 'language' " (http://scholarspace.manoa.hawaii.edu/handle/10125/4606). Language
Documentation and Conservation. 7: 331–359. hdl:10125/4606 (https://hdl.handle.net/1012
5%2F4606).
Swadesh, Morris (1934). "The phonemic principle". Language. 10 (2): 117–129.
doi:10.2307/409603 (https://doi.org/10.2307%2F409603). JSTOR 409603 (https://www.jstor.
org/stable/409603).
External links
World Atlas of Language Structures: a large database of structural (phonological,
grammatical, lexical) properties of languages (http://wals.info/)
Ethnologue: Languages of the World (http://www.ethnologue.com/) is a comprehensive
catalog of all of the world's known living languages