0% found this document useful (0 votes)
20 views18 pages

CH3 8

This document provides a comprehensive guide to setting up a meta-analysis, emphasizing the importance of a thorough literature search and clear criteria for study selection. It outlines the process of defining variables, coding study results, and addressing issues such as study quality and publication bias. The document serves as a resource for researchers to ensure their meta-analyses are replicable and valid.

Uploaded by

Vujica Zivkovic
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
0% found this document useful (0 votes)
20 views18 pages

CH3 8

This document provides a comprehensive guide to setting up a meta-analysis, emphasizing the importance of a thorough literature search and clear criteria for study selection. It outlines the process of defining variables, coding study results, and addressing issues such as study quality and publication bias. The document serves as a resource for researchers to ensure their meta-analyses are replicable and valid.

Uploaded by

Vujica Zivkovic
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
You are on page 1/ 18

Setting Up a

3 Meta-Analysis
Traditional, narrative reviews of research literature are selective. Critics of such studies
rightly ask, "Why were some studies selected but not others? What criteria were used?
What kind of bias did the selection process introduce into the review?"

A meta-analysis should be comprehensive and replicable. It should not only examine as


much of the research as possible, it should also describe how you found the research so
that other researchers can evaluate your work.

This chapter describes the tasks required to organize and conduct a meta-analysis.

Focusing Your Aim . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 35

Conducting the Literature Search . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 36


Handling Questions of Study Quality . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 37
Dealing with Publication Bias . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 38
Documenting Your Search . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 38
Creating the Meta-Analysis in Meta-Stat . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 38

Defining Variables . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 39
Classes of Variables . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 39
Variables for a Meta-Analysis of the Effects of
Psychotherapy on Asthma . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 39
Variables for a Meta-Analysis of the Effects of
Coaching on SAT Scores . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 40
Deciding Which Variables to Use for Your Meta-Analysis . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 41
Setting Up Variables in Meta-Stat . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 42

continued

Chapter 3 Page 33
Setting Up a Meta-Analysis Meta-Stat User's Guide

Coding the Results of Each Study . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 43


Dealing with Data Errors . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 43
Entering Study Data into Meta-Stat . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 44

Analyzing and Displaying Results . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 45


First steps . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 45
A comment on Schmidt and Hunter meta-analysis . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 47

Summary of Steps for a Meta-Analysis . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 49

Chapter 3 Page 34
Meta-Stat User's Guide Setting Up a Meta-Analysis

Focusing Your Aim

Before setting out to collect studies, you must first decide the range of topics your review
will encompass. Meta-analyses typically cover broad topics, sometimes loosely defined
topics. They examine questions such as, Does psychotherapy work? Does computer-
assisted instruction lead to more learning than traditional instruction? Is mastery learning
better than traditional learning?

These questions, by themselves, are too broad to be meaningful. Does "psychotherapy,"


for example, mean reading a self-help book, or spending a few sessions with a college
counselor, or completing a multi-year psychoanalysis?

To avoid vague generalities, you must make the focus of your meta-analysis much more
explicit by establishing criteria for including or excluding studies. However, the criteria
cannot be too restricting, or you might not find a sufficient number of studies; and even if
you do find enough studies, you might not find anything interesting or illuminating.

To focus your aim, read a good sample of the literature and develop a thorough
understanding of the concepts and methods that you want to analyze. Determine the effect,
or outcome, you want to study, as well as the predictors, or independent variables, that you
want to measure.

Chapter 3 Page 35
Setting Up a Meta-Analysis Meta-Stat User's Guide

Conducting the Literature Search

A thorough literature search is critical to the validity of your meta-analysis:

How one searches determines what one finds; and what one finds is the basis of the
conclusions of one's integration of studies. (Glass, 1976)

Finding research studies is difficult and time-consuming. The studies can be located in a
variety of places, and often you must look beyond the titles. For example, if you are
conducting a meta-analysis of sex differences, you will find that some studies show such
differences even when that is not the principal variable of interest in the study and even
when the study title makes no mention of sex differences.

To find studies, check the following general categories described by Rosenthal (1984):

! Books

- Authored books
- Edited books
- Chapters in edited books

! Journals

- Professional journals
- Published newsletters
- Magazines
- Newspapers

! Theses

- Doctoral theses
- Master's theses
- Bachelor's theses

! Unpublished work

- Technical reports
- Grant reports
- Convention papers not published in proceedings
- ERIC reports
- Films and cassette recordings
- Other unpublished work

Chapter 3 Page 36
Meta-Stat User's Guide Setting Up a Meta-Analysis

Also consider soon-to-be-published works. Given the time lags between data collection,
write-up, journal acceptance, and publication, the latest word on a topic may well be in the
pipeline to publication. Your literature search should produce a list of names you can
contact with inquiries about recent research. These researchers will also know about others
who are doing research on similar topics.

Last, and perhaps most important, consult secondary sources such as review periodicals
and the myriad abstract archives that are available in most fields of study. These sources
will help you identify the primary research that will be the subject of your meta-analysis.

Handling Questions of Study Quality

One of the most controversial questions related to meta-analysis is the question of whether
to include studies that are of doubtful or poor quality. Some critics invoke the Garbage-In,
Garbage-Out principle by arguing that any meta-analysis that summarizes studies of widely
differing quality is likely to be uninformative or flawed. These critics argue that studies with
methodological flaws should be eliminated from consideration in the meta-analysis.

Others counter by noting that it is often difficult to assess methodological quality and
researchers often disagree on quality. Despite a researcher's best attempts to provide an
objective measure of quality, decisions to include or exclude studies introduce bias into the
meta-analysis.

Others note that the quality of a study may not have an effect on the study's outcome.
When in doubt, include the study in the meta-analysis and use an independent variable to
code the quality of a study. Then examine empirically whether the outcome does in fact
vary with study quality. You can do this with Meta-Stat by examining the relationship
between study quality and effect size.

Dealing with Publication Bias

Well-recognized publication biases can produce bias in your meta-analysis.

First, publication policy is biased toward statistically significant findings. In other words, it
is easier to get published if you have something statistically significant to report. A number
of meta-analyses have indeed found that effect sizes reported in journals differ widely from
unpublished work. The effect size is about 33 percent larger in published research.

Chapter 3 Page 37
Setting Up a Meta-Analysis Meta-Stat User's Guide

Second, publication biases can skew the direction of effect. For example, Smith (1980)
reported that, in the published literature, counselors and therapists tend to view women
more negatively than men. In the unpublished literature, the opposite is true. As Strube
(1985) theorized:

Such biases probably reflect the fact that some findings better "fit" the prevailing
scientific atmosphere (Zeitgeist) and are scrutinized less closely than are novel or
counterintuitive results.

To overcome publication bias, gather everything you can find, both published and
unpublished. You can then use empirical methods to examine the question of publication
bias. For example, with Meta-Stat you can compare the effect sizes for published research
and unpublished work.

Documenting Your Search

A traditional, narrative review of literature often devotes only one or two paragraphs to a
discussion of the methods that were used to conduct the literature search. A meta-analysis
requires a much more rigorous approach. Only through a comprehensive description can
other researchers evaluate the validity of your work and, perhaps, gather clues about
overlooked sources.

Creating the Meta-Analysis in Meta-Stat

Before you can define your variables in Meta-Stat, as described in the following sections,
you must create the meta-analysis in Meta-Stat. Chapter 4 of this manual describes how to
create and modify the set of data files that, together, make up a meta-analysis.

See Chapter 4 for complete information.

Chapter 3 Page 38
Meta-Stat User's Guide Setting Up a Meta-Analysis

Defining Variables

After you collect the literature for your meta-analysis, you must determine which study
features your meta-analysis will examine. These features become the variables in the meta-
analysis.

Classes of Variables

There are three classes of variable:

! Variables that identify characteristics of the study

You will need some variables to identify when and where the research was
published, whether a control group was used, the type of effect that was measured,
and so forth.

These variables will help you show the relationships, if any, between study
methodology and results.

! Variables that identify characteristics of the sample

Use these variables to identify the subject population—age, educational level,


socioeconomic status, and so on.

! Variables that identify characteristics of the intervention

These variables could include the type of treatment or intervention, its duration, the
type of effect that was measured, and so on.

Variables for a Meta-Analysis of the Effects of Psychotherapy on Asthma

Schlesinger et al. (1978) conducted a meta-analysis that evaluated the effects of


psychotherapy on asthma. This meta-analysis found that psychotherapy does indeed
significantly lessen both the medical and nonmedical effects of asthma.

In the meta-analysis, the following variables were used:

Chapter 3 Page 39
Setting Up a Meta-Analysis Meta-Stat User's Guide

This variable ... Was used to identify ...


Therapy type Type of psychotherapy received by treatment
subjects

For example, some subjects received


hypnotherapy while others received group
therapy.
Age Average age of subjects
Hours of Therapy Number of hours that subjects received therapy
Control Group Type of therapy received by the control group

Some control subjects received no treatment,


others received relaxation therapy, and others
received medical treatment.
Follow-Up Time Amount of time between the end of treatment
and the measurement of the outcome variable
Dependent Variable The type of outcome or effect that was
measured

For example, some studies measured the effect


of psychotherapy on the use of drugs to treat
asthma; others measured the effect on asthma
attacks and hospitalization.
Effect size The size of the effect of treatment

Variables for a Meta-Analysis of the Effects of Coaching on SAT Scores

Becker (1990) conducted a meta-analysis that tested the effects of coaching on Scholastic
Aptitude Test scores. Among other results, this meta-analysis found that the effectiveness
of coaching varied widely across the studies, with much of the variation resulting from
studies without comparison groups. The magnitude of the coaching effect is related to study
design and to the duration of coaching intervention.

The following table describes some of the variables that were used in the meta-analysis:

This variable ... Was used to identify ...


Year of publication Year the study was published

Chapter 3 Page 40
Meta-Stat User's Guide Setting Up a Meta-Analysis

This variable ... Was used to identify ...


Type of publication Whether the article was published in an
academic journal
ETS Authorship Whether any author was affiliated with
Educational Testing Service, which develops
and administers the SAT
Control Group Type of control group
Use of Whether subjects were randomly assigned to
randomization control and coaching groups
Selectivity The type of students in the study (e.g. low
achievers, public-school students, college-prep
students)
Voluntariness Whether participation in coaching was
voluntary or compulsory
Duration Length of coaching program
Presence of Test Whether students practiced taking complete
Practice sample tests
Homework Whether students were given coaching-related
exercises for completion at home
Use of Whether students were given computerized
Computerized practice
Instruction

Deciding Which Variables to Use for Your Meta-Analysis

In some cases, simple analysis will help you decide which variables are required for a meta-
analysis. But it's not always so simple. Definitions of study features may vary from study
to study, and different researchers often use different terminology.

The safest procedure is to have two researchers independently code all study features, then
negotiate an agreement in places where they disagree.

Chapter 3 Page 41
Setting Up a Meta-Analysis Meta-Stat User's Guide

Setting Up Variables in Meta-Stat

Chapter 5 describes how to create and modify variables in Meta-Stat. The program comes
with many variables already predefined for you. There are variables to measure:

! Study characteristics, including the year of publication and the type of publication
in which the research appeared

! Sample characteristics, including the sizes of the treatment and control groups

! Intervention characteristics, including the effect size and the unbiased effect size

You can also create your own variables to handle other study features. You can create
different types of variables to handle different types of data.

See Chapter 4 for complete information about Meta-Stat variables.

Chapter 3 Page 42
Meta-Stat User's Guide Setting Up a Meta-Analysis

Coding the Results of Each Study

After defining the variables you will use to measure studies, you must obtain the studies
and code data for each variable.

Unfortunately, this task is not as straightforward as it might seem. Meta-analysts


sometimes find that the required data are missing and must be estimated. Sometimes,
researchers find errors in the original research. Errors in the publication are relatively easy
to correct. But that is not the case for errors that were made when the data were originally
recorded. These errors will contaminate your meta-analysis, but it is no easy matter to
determine whether that contamination is severe.

Dealing with Data Errors

In social science research, errors commonly occur in favor of the anticipated outcome. In
one study, researchers gave genetically identical white rats to students. The researchers told
the students that some of the rats were fast runners and some were slow. When the
students recorded running times in a maze, the "fast" rats had faster times than the "slow"
ones.

Another study collected the original data from 27 published studies. The median error rate
was about 1 percent, but rates of 3 percent and 4 percent were not uncommon. Two-thirds
of these errors supported the researcher's hypothesis. Of course, if the researchers were
unbiased, half of the errors should have supported the hypothesis and half should have
countered it.

Dealing with Data Reporting Deficiencies

The scientific method requires researchers to report their studies in enough detail that
anyone who wants to replicate a study can do so. Unfortunately, published research seldom
exhibits the required clarity and detail. Further, there is no standardization among different
journals—or even within a journal, as editors change—about what all studies must contain.
About 40 percent of all studies even lack the means and standard deviations that are
important to most meta-analyses. Fortunately, this deficiency does not make a meta-
analysis impossible.

Deficient reporting can affect your meta-analysis by forcing you to exclude variables that,

Chapter 3 Page 43
Setting Up a Meta-Analysis Meta-Stat User's Guide

although potentially important, are not described in the study results. Deficient reporting
can also force you to abandon precise variables in favor of rougher groupings of data. This
problem causes less variance in your variables and attenuates important relations in your
data.

In your meta-analysis, it may be useful to add a variable that you can use to rate data
quality. If you rate the quality of each study's data on a three-point scale, you can then
examine whether the quality of reporting systematically affects the outcomes of studies.

Entering Study Data into Meta-Stat

Chapter 6 describes how to enter each study into your meta-analysis. For each study, you
must identify the author and title. You must then enter data from the study for each
variable that you have set up.

See Chapter 6 for full information.

Chapter 3 Page 44
Meta-Stat User's Guide Setting Up a Meta-Analysis

Analyzing and Displaying Results

Chapters 7 and 8 describe how to analyze and display the results of your meta-analysis.

Chapter 7 describes the statistical techniques you can use to explore your data. Chapter 8
describes the graphs you can use to analyze and display results.

First steps

The meta-analyst is well advised to perform a number of very basic analyses before
launching into more complex analyses in which some of the more basic features of the data
base are obscured. Specifically, after entering the data, it is wise to perform simple
frequency distributions and scatter plots to see if data have been entered correctly or
whether some studies contain data that are clearly aberrant in the context of all studies in
the analysis. These aberrations can occur in a variety of ways. Most simply, perhaps, are
clerical errors committed in the entry of the data into Meta-Stat. Bad data points can enter
the process earlier: typos in printed reports of the studies; data analysis errors in the
primary data analysis that yield an F-ratio in error by a factor of two or three, for example;
or conditions invalidating the transformation of a reported statistic into an effect size or
correlation coefficient. However they arise, these errors produce odd values of variables
that can distort the analysis of the study outcomes and their relationships with study
characteristics. The errors need to be detected and corrected or removed.

Two analyses are particularly useful to this end. Under CHARTS can be found the options
of BAR GRAPH and EFFECT SIZE BY STUDY. An early analysis should be the
graphing of EFFECT SIZE (or CORRELATION in the case of correlations as the study
outcome) as a BAR CHART. The results are then inspected for a small number of entries
that are clearly separated from the bulk of the distribution of effects. How far removed
from the mean or the next closest entries is "clearly separated"? No definitive answer can
be given. Values more than five standard deviations from the mean in samples of 100 or
fewer cases would certainly raise suspicions about errors in reporting or calculation. But it
is difficult to be more precise than this here. (The reader interested in pursuing this point to
a more precise answer should consult Dixon & Massey, 1969, or Tukey, 1977.)

The graph of EFFECT SIZE BY STUDY can add information to the BAR GRAPH. All
effect sizes are arrayed in either ascending or descending order and each is bracketed by a
confidence interval that reflects on the size of the samples in the study yielding the effect
size. If the largest or smallest of these effects are very distant from their neighbors and the
confidence intervals are likewise quite distinct and not overlapping, then it might be well to

Chapter 3 Page 45
Setting Up a Meta-Analysis Meta-Stat User's Guide

return to the studies that produced these effects and see if some error is apparent. Not
every aberration will have a simple explanation; when they lack reasons, the analyst is
faced with the difficult question of eliminating the odd data point without good reasons or
leaving it in and having it exercise undue influence on the later statistical analyses.

The problem of detecting outliers can be more complex than indicated above. Consider the
follow data, which for the sake of example can be thought of as effect size from nine
experiments in which a drug is tested against a placebo: .40, .45, .50. .55, .60, .75, .80, .85,
.90. There appear to be no outliers or aberrant data points here. But suppose that the first
four and the ninth effect sizes are from studies that are double blind and the fifth through
the eighth effects are from single blind experiments: when the effects are grouped and
inspected, the value of .90 appears aberrant and sends the analyst looking for errors or
explanations:

Single Blind Effects: .60 .75 .80 .85


Double Blind Effects: .40 .45 .50 .55 .90

Consequently, it is advisable not only to inspect the entire data set for outliers but to break
down the whole data set into various groups to see if aberrations appear when effects are
compared with effects observed under similar conditions. Perhaps the best way to
accomplish this analysis is to select EFFECT SIZE BY STUDY under the CHARTS menu
and then use SELECT IF to construct selection criteria to view the effects for various
subgroups of discrete study characteristics.

For continuous study characteristics, the counterpart to the above analysis is a scatter plot
relating the study characteristic to the study outcome (e.g., EFFECT SIZE). When
SCATTER PLOT is selected from the CHARTS menu and EFFECT SIZE plotted against
a study characteristic, a quick visual fitting of the relationship will reveal points that fall far
away from the general relationship between the two variables. The studies contributing
these outliers can be identified with the POINT SHOOT feature, and reread or checked for
obvious errors.

An alternative means of detecting bad data points is to perform REGRESSION analyses


under the ANALYSIS menu and inspect the residuals graph that the analysis produces.
Very large and aberrant residuals identify data points that have peculiarities unrelated to the
study characteristics used in the regression.

Although outliers can be critical when they occur in the study outcome variable, say
EFFECT SIZE, they can occur in other places in a data set. Occasionally they cause
problems in subsequent analyses. The methods suggested above for searching for outliers in
study outcomes can also be applied to any other variable (with the exception of the

Chapter 3 Page 46
Meta-Stat User's Guide Setting Up a Meta-Analysis

EFFECT SIZE BY STUDY graph that is only available for the EFFECT SIZE variable). In
addition, the BREAKDOWN tables under ANALYSIS are useful in detecting bad data
points in Grouping Variables that describe study characteristics.

A comment on Schmidt and Hunter meta-analysis

The effect-size meta-analysis approach in Meta-Stat tends to follow the concepts outlined
by Glass and by Hedges. These techniques are very similar to those of Schmidt-Hunter, but
they do differ in several critical ways. Schmidt and Hunter argue that

1) the effect size should be based on the pooled variance estimate rather than that of
the control group,

2) the meta-analyst should correct for sampling error, and

3) the meta-analyst should correct for measurement error.

The argument for using the pooled variance estimate is that it is based on more
observations and subject to less error than the control group variance estimate. The counter
argument is that the control group variance is unaffected by the treatment. The effect-size
calculator in Meta-Stat lets you choose your technique. In practice there will be very little
difference.

Schmidt and Hunter recommend grouping, trimming, or selecting your data until the ratio of
the error variance to the variance of the effect sizes is .75 or greater. They argue that if
75% of the variance is due to error, then the rest of the variance is probably also due to
error. Therefore the population variance is zero and the model of a single effect size is
consistent with the data. Meta-Stat provides you with their variance ratio under
Analysis/Descriptive when the criterion variable is UNBIASED.

Their last key recommendation is to correct for study artifacts such as the lack of perfect
reliability of the criterion measure and data dichotomization. For example, they recommend
correcting for measurement error by dividing the effect-size estimate by the square root of
the reliability of the criterion variable. They note that measurement error inflates the
standard deviation and thus lowers the effect size. Meta-Stat can accommodate this
correction. The program allows you to override values entered into an equation when one
or more of the variables are missing. Thus, you can correct for measurement error and
other artifacts by following these steps.

1. Use the effect size calculator to compute EFFECTSZ..

2. Manually divide the EFFECTSZ (or UNBIASED) value by the square root of the

Chapter 3 Page 47
Setting Up a Meta-Analysis Meta-Stat User's Guide

dependent measure reliability (reliability for commercial tests can be found in the
Buros Mental Measurements Yearbook).

3. Divide the above value by other correction formulas if desired and available.

4. Delete the value for EFFECTSZ.

5. Insert the corrected effect-size in UNBIASED.

Not all meta-analysis methodologists will agree with each of Hunter & Schmidt's
recommendations. We diverge from their plan in at least three respects. It seems unwise to
contaminate the variance estimate with the treatment effect. Furthermore the 75%
rule-of-thumb test is quite arbitrary and not in the spirit of intelligent, contextualized data
analysis. Third, corrections for measurement unreliability often make conceptual sense but
are impossible to implement. Finding a test reliability (in a book or manual or from a
computer printout) is one thing; but assessing a sensible and appropriate error variance is
quite another. The error that enters the control group variance estimate and contributes to
observed differences among persons is generally not the error assessed by ordinary
convenient measures of test reliability like Cronbach's Alpha, Kuder-Richardson or various
split-half coefficients. In addition, after reanalyzing several meta-analysis studies using the
Glass and the Schmidt-Hunter techniques, Hough and Hall (1991) demonstrated that the
corrections are trivial and that both approaches lead to the same conclusions.

Chapter 3 Page 48
Meta-Stat User's Guide Setting Up a Meta-Analysis

Summary of Steps for a Meta-Analysis

To conduct a meta-analysis:

1. Focus your aim by deciding on an area of the literature that you want to explore and a
specific topic that you want to analyze.

2. Conduct a thorough literature search.

3. Create the meta-analysis in Meta-Stat. See Chapter 4 of this manual.

4. Add variables that you will use to code study features. See Chapter 5.

5. Read the studies and code the variables for each study.

6. Enter the data for each study into Meta-Stat. See Chapter 6.

7. Explore and display the data with various statistical techniques. See Chapter 7 and 8.

Chapter 3 Page 49
Setting Up a Meta-Analysis Meta-Stat User's Guide

Chapter 3 Page 50

You might also like