Jean Piaget
Jean Piaget
html
1. Developmental Psychology
2. Piaget
o sensorimotor,
o preoperational,
o concrete operational,
o formal operational.
Schemas
Imagine what it would be like if you did not have a mental model of
your world. It would mean that you would not be able to make so
much use of information from your past experience or to plan future
actions.
Schemas are the basic building blocks of such cognitive models, and
enable us to form a mental representation of the world. Piaget (1952,
p. 7) defined a schema as:
"a cohesive, repeatable action sequence possessing component
actions that are tightly interconnected and governed by a core
meaning."
In more simple terms Piaget called the schema the basic building
block of intelligent behavior – a way of organizing knowledge. Indeed,
it is useful to think of schemas as “units” of knowledge, each relating
to one aspect of the world, including objects, actions, and abstract
(i.e., theoretical) concepts.
Wadsworth (2004) suggests that schemata (the plural of schema) be
thought of as 'index cards' filed in the brain, each one telling an
individual how to react to incoming stimuli or information.
When Piaget talked about the development of a person's mental
processes, he was referring to increases in the number and
complexity of the schemata that a person had learned.
When a child's existing schemas are capable of explaining what it can
perceive around it, it is said to be in a state of equilibrium, i.e., a state
of cognitive (i.e., mental) balance.
Piaget emphasized the importance of schemas in cognitive
development and described how they were developed or acquired. A
schema can be defined as a set of linked mental representations of the
world, which we use both to understand and to respond to situations.
The assumption is that we store these mental representations and
apply them when needed.
For example, a person might have a schema about buying a meal in a
restaurant. The schema is a stored form of the pattern of behavior
which includes looking at a menu, ordering food, eating it and paying
the bill. This is an example of a type of schema called a 'script.'
Whenever they are in a restaurant, they retrieve this schema from
memory and apply it to the situation.
The schemas Piaget described tend to be simpler than this - especially
those used by infants. He described how - as a child gets older - his or
her schemas become more numerous and elaborate.
Piaget believed that newborn babies have a small number of innate
schemas - even before they have had many opportunities to
experience the world. These neonatal schemas are the cognitive
structures underlying innate reflexes. These reflexes are genetically
programmed into us.
For example, babies have a sucking reflex, which is triggered by
something touching the baby's lips. A baby will suck a nipple, a
comforter (dummy), or a person's finger. Piaget, therefore, assumed
that the baby has a 'sucking schema.'
Similarly, the grasping reflex which is elicited when something
touches the palm of a baby's hand, or the rooting reflex, in which a
baby will turn its head towards something which touches its cheek,
are innate schemas. Shaking a rattle would be the combination of two
schemas, grasping and shaking.
Assimilation and
Accommodation
Jean Piaget (1952; see also Wadsworth, 2004) viewed intellectual
growth as a process of adaptation (adjustment) to the world. This
happens through:
Assimilation
– Which is using an existing schema to deal with a new object or
situation.
Accommodation
– This happens when the existing schema (knowledge) does not
work, and needs to be changed to deal with a new object or
situation.
Equilibration
– This is the force which moves development along. Piaget
believed that cognitive development did not progress at a steady
rate, but rather in leaps and bounds.
Example of Assimilation
A 2-year-old child sees a man who is bald on top of his head and has
long frizzy hair on the sides. To his father’s horror, the toddler shouts
“Clown, clown” (Siegler et al., 2003).
Example of Accommodation
In the “clown” incident, the boy’s father explained to his son that the
man was not a clown and that even though his hair was like a clown’s,
he wasn’t wearing a funny costume and wasn’t doing silly things to
make people laugh.
With this new knowledge, the boy was able to change his schema of
“clown” and make this idea fit better to a standard concept of
“clown”.
Educational Implications
Piaget (1952) did not explicitly relate his theory to education,
although later researchers have explained how features of Piaget's
theory can be applied to teaching and learning.
Piaget has been extremely influential in developing educational policy
and teaching practice. For example, a review of primary education by
the UK government in 1966 was based strongly on Piaget’s theory.
The result of this review led to the publication of the Plowden
report (1967).
Discovery learning – the idea that children learn best through doing
and actively exploring - was seen as central to the transformation of
the primary school curriculum.
'The report's recurring themes are individual learning, flexibility in
the curriculum, the centrality of play in children's learning, the use of
the environment, learning by discovery and the importance of the
evaluation of children's progress - teachers should 'not assume that
only what is measurable is valuable.'
Because Piaget's theory is based upon biological maturation and
stages, the notion of 'readiness' is important. Readiness concerns
when certain information or concepts should be taught. According to
Piaget's theory children should not be taught certain concepts until
they have reached the appropriate stage of cognitive development.
According to Piaget (1958), assimilation and accommodation require
an active learner, not a passive one, because problem-solving skills
cannot be taught, they must be discovered.
Within the classroom learning should be student-centered and
accomplished through active discovery learning. The role of the
teacher is to facilitate learning, rather than direct tuition. Therefore,
teachers should encourage the following within the classroom:
o Focus on the process of learning, rather than the end product of
it.
o Using active methods that require rediscovering or
reconstructing "truths."
o Using collaborative, as well as individual activities (so children
can learn from each other).
o Devising situations that present useful problems, and create
disequilibrium in the child.
o Evaluate the level of the child's development so suitable tasks can
be set.
Critical Evaluation
Support
The influence of Piaget’s ideas in developmental psychology has
been enormous. He changed how people viewed the child’s world
and their methods of studying children.
Criticisms
Are the stages real? Vygotsky and Bruner would rather not talk
about stages at all, preferring to see development as a continuous
process. Others have queried the age ranges of the stages. Some
studies have shown that progress to the formal operational
stage is not guaranteed.
Back to top
report this ad
Print Download
report this ad
Article Content
Introduction SchemasAssimilation & AccommodationPiaget
EvaluationReferences
report this ad
Stages of Development
Sensorimotor Stage Preoperational StageConcrete Operational
report this ad