NLP Unit-4
NLP Unit-4
Can we define a notion of sentence meaning that is independent of context? In other words, is
there a level at which the sentence "Do you know what gate you are going to?" has a single
meaning, but may be used for different purposes? This is a complex issue, but there are many
advantages to trying to make such an approach work. The primary argument is modularity. If
such a division can be made, then we can study sentence meaning in detail without all the
complications of sentence usage. In particular, if sentences have no context-independent
meaning, then we may not be able to separate the study of language from the study of general
For the moment let us assume the knowledge representation language is the first-order predicate
calculus (FOPC). Given that assumption, what is the status of the logical form? In some
approaches the logical form is defined as the literal meaning of the utterance, and the logical
form language is the same as the final knowledge representation language. If this is to be a viable
approach in the long run, however, it would mean that the knowledge representation must be
considerably more complex than representations in present use in Al systems. For instance, the
logical form language must allow indexical terms, that is, terms that are defined by context. The
pronouns "I" and "you" are indexical because their interpretation depends on the context of who
is speaking and listening. In fact most definite descriptions (such as "the red ball") are indexical,
as the object referred to can only be identified with respect to a context. Many other aspects of
language, including the interpretation of tense and determining the scope of quantifiers, depend
on context as well and thus cannot be uniquely determined at the logical form level. Of course,
all of this could be treated as ambiguity at the logical form level, but this would be impractical,
as every sentence would have large numbers of possible logical forms (as in the sentence "The
red ball dropped", which would have a different logical form for every possible object that could
be described as a ball that is red).
But if the logical form language is not part of the knowledge representation language, what is its
formal status? A promising approach has been developed in linguistics over the last decade that
2
suggests an answer that uses the intuitive notion of the meaning of "situation" in English. For
instance, when attending a class, you are in a situation where there are fellow students and an
instructor, where certain utterances are made by the lecturer, questions asked, and so on. Also,
there will be objects in the lecture hail, say a blackboard and chairs, and so on. More formally,
you might think of a situation as a set of objects and relations between those objects. A very
simple situation might consist of two objects, a ball B0005 and a person P86, and include the
relationship that the person owns the ball. Let us encode this situation as the set {(BALL B0005),
(PERSON P86), (OWNS P86B0005)}.
Language creates special types of situations based on what information is conveyed. These issues
will be explored in detail later, but for now consider the following to help your intuition. In any
conversation or text, assume there is a discourse situation that records the information conveyed
so far. A new sentence is interpreted with respect to this situation and produces a new situation
that includes the information conveyed by the new sentence. Given this view, the logical form is
a function that maps the discourse situation in which the utterance was made to a new discourse
situation that results from the occurrence of the utterance. For example, assume that the situation
we just encoded has been created by some preceding sentences describing the ball and who owns
it. The utterance "The ball is red" might produce a new situation that consists of the old situation
plus the new fact that B0005 has the property RED: ((BALL B0005), (PERSON P86), (OWNS
P86 B0005), (RED B0005)}. Figure 8.2 shows this view of the interpretation process, treating
the logical form as a function between situations. The two organizations presented in Figures 8.1
and 8.2 differ in that the latter might not include a single identifiable expression in the
knowledge representation that fully captures the ‖meaning‖ of the sentence. Rather, the logical
form might make a variety of changes to produce the updated situation. This allows other
implications to be derived from an utterance that are not directly captured in the semantic content
of the sentence. Such issues will become important later when we discuss contextual
interpretation.
3
4.2 Word senses & ambiguity
To develop a theory of semantics and semantic interpretation, we need to develop a structural
model, just as we did for syntax. With syntax we first introduced the notion of the basic syntactic
classes and then developed ways to constrain how simple classes combine to form larger
structures. We will follow the same basic strategy for semantics. You might think that the basic
semantic unit could be the word or the morpheme, but that approach runs into problems because
of the presence of ambiguity. For example, it is not unusual for the verb go to have more than 40
entries in a typical dictionary. Each one of these definitions reflects a different sense of the word.
Dictionaries often give synonyms for particular word senses. For go you might find synonyms
such as move, depart, pass, vanish, reach, extend, and set out. Many of these highlight a different
sense of the verb go. Of course, if these are true synonyms of some sense of go, then the verbs
themselves will share identical senses. For instance, one of the senses of go will be identical to
one of the senses of depart.
If every word has one or more senses then you are looking at a very large number of senses, even
given that some words have synonymous senses. Fortunately, the different senses can be
organized into a set of broad classes of objects by which we classify the world. The set of
different classes of objects in a representation is called its ontology. To handle a natural
language, we need a much broader ontology than commonly found in work on formal logic.
Such classifications of objects have been of interest for a very long time and arise in the writings
of Aristotle (384—322 B.C.). The major classes that Aristotle suggested were substance
(physical objects), quantity (such as numbers), quality (such as bright red), relation, place, time,
position, state, action, and affection. To this list we might add other classes such as events, ideas,
concepts, and plans. Two of the most influential classes are actions and events. Events are things
that happen in the world and are important in many semantic theories because they provide a
structure for organizing the interpretation of sentences. Actions are things that agents do, thus
causing some event. Like all objects in the ontology, actions and events can be referred to by
pronouns, as in the discourse fragment
4
4.3 The Basic Logical Form Language
The basic logical form language defines a language in which you can combine word
senses elements to form meanings for more complex expressions. This language will follow
First-Order Predicative Calculus (FOPC). The FOPC are many equivalent forms of
representation, such as network-based representations, that use the same basic ideas. The word
senses will serve as the atoms or constants of the representation. These constants can be
classified by the types of things they describe. For instance, constants that describe objects in the
world,
including abstract objects such as events and situations, are called terms. Constants that describe
relations and properties are called predicates. A proposition in the language is formed from a
predicate followed by an appropriate number of terms to serve as its arguments. For instance, the
proposition corresponding to the sentence "Fido is a dog" would be constructed from the term
FIDO1 and the predicate constant DOG1 and is written as
(DOG1 FIDO1)
Predicates that take a single argument are called unary predicates or properties; those that
take two arguments, such as LOVES1, are called binary predicates; and those that take n
arguments are called n-ary predicates. The proposition corresponding to the sentence Sue
loves Jack would involve a binary predicate LOVES1 and would be written as
(LOVES1 SUE1 JACK1)
You can see that different word classes in English correspond to different types of constants in
the logical form. Proper names, such as Jack, have word senses that are terms; common nouns,
such as dog, have word senses that are unary predicates; and verbs, such as run, love, and put,
have word senses that correspond to n-ary predicates, where n depends on how many terms the
verb subcategorizes for.
Note that while the logical forms are presented in a predicate-argument form here, the same
distinctions are made in most other meaning representations. For instance, a network
representation would have nodes that correspond to the word senses and arcs that indicate the
predicate-argument structure. The meaning of the sentence Sue loves Jack in a semantic network
like representation might appear in one of the two forms shown in Figure 8.3. For most purposes
all of these representation formalisms are equivalent.
5
More complex propositions are constructed using a new class of constants called logical
operators. For example, the operator NOT allows you to construct a proposition that says that
some proposition is not true. The proposition corresponding to the sentence Sue does not love
Jack would be
(NOT (LOVES1 SUE1 JACK1))
English also contains operators that combine two or more propositions to form a complex
proposition. FOPC contains operators such as disjunction (v), conjunction (&), what is often
called implication (`), and other forms (there are 16 possible truth functional binary operators in
FOPC). English contains many similar operators including or, and, if only if, and so on. Natural
language connectives often involve more complex relationships between sentences. For instance,
the conjunction "and" might correspond to the logical operator "&" but often also involves
temporal sequencing, as in "I went home and had a drink", in which going home preceded
having the drink. The connective "but", on the other hand, is like "and" except that the second
argument is something that the hearer might not expect to be true given the first argument. The
general form for such a proposition is (connective proposition proposition). For example, the
logical form of the sentence "Jack loves Sue or Jack loves Mary" would be (OR1 (LOVES1
JACK1 SUE1) (LOVES1 JACK1 MARY1)). The logical form language will allow both
operators corresponding to word senses and operators like "&" directly from FOPC. The logic
based operators will be used to connect propositions not explicitly conjoined in the sentence.
The Quantifier is used to present more complex sentences by using FOPC and there are only
two quantifiers: Ž and j. English contains a much larger range of quantifiers, including all, some,
most, many, a few, the, and so on. Thus two instances of the same variable x occurring in two
different formulas - say in the formulas jx.P(x) and jx. Q(x).
For the standard existential and universal quantifiers, there are formulas in standard FOPC
equivalent to the generalized quantifier forms. In particular, the formula
(EXISTS x : Px Qx)
is equivalent to
j x . Px & Qx)
and the universally quantified form
(ALL x : Px Qx)
is equivalent to
Žx . Px ` Qx)
6
These generalized quantifier forms can be thought of simply as abbreviations. But the other
quantifiers do not have an equivalent form in standard FOPC. To see this, consider trying to
define the meaning of "Most dogs bark" using the standard semantics for quantifiers. Clearly Žx .
Dog(x) ` Bark(x)is too strong, and jx . Dog(x) ` Bark(x) is too weak.
Thus the sentence "Sue watched the ball" is ambiguous out of context. A single logical form can
represent these two possibilities, however:
1. (THE b1 : ({BALL1 BALL2} b1) (PAST (WATCH1 SUE1 b1 )))
This abbreviates two possible logical forms, namely
2. (THE b1 : (BALL1 b1) (PAST (WATCH1 SUE1 b1 )))
and
3. (THE b1 : (BALL2 b1) (PAST (WATCH1 SUE1 b1 )))
Encoding Ambiguity
A typical sentence will have multiple possible syntactic structures, each of which might
have multiple possible logical forms.
The words in the sentence will have multiple senses. Many researchers view this ambiguity
encoding as a separate level of representation from the logical form, and it is often referred to as
the quasi-logical form.
For example, the logical forms for the sentence "Every boy loves a dog" are captured by a single
ambiguous form
(LOVES1 <EVERY b1 (BOY1 b1)> <A d1 (DOG1 d1)>)
This abbreviates an ambiguity between the logical form
(EVERY b1 : (BOY1 b1) (A d1 (DOG1 d1) (LOVES1 b1 d1)))
and
(A d1: (DOG1 d1) (EVERY b1 : (BOY1 b1) (LOVES1 b1 d1)))
In addition, operators such as negation and tense are also scope sensitive. For example, the
sentence "Every boy didn't run" is ambiguous between the read ing in which some boys didn't
run and some did, that is,
(NOT (EVERY b1 : (BOY1 b1) (RUN1 b1 )))
In fact, proper names must be interpreted in context, and the name John will refer to different
people in different situations. We will introduce this construct as a special function, namely
(NAME <variable> <name>)
which produces the appropriate object with the name in the current context. Thus, the logical
form of "John ran" would be (<PAST RUN1> (NAME j1 "John")).
7
4.5 Verbs & States in Logical Form
The verbs have mapped to appropriate senses acting as predicates in the logical form. This
treatment can handle all the different forms but loses some generalities that could be captured. It
also has some annoying properties. Consider the following sentences, all using the verb ―break":
In particular, the quasi-logical form for the sentence "John broke the window" using this
abbreviation is
(<PAST BREAK!> e1 [AGENT (NAME j1 "John")]
[THEME <THE w1 WINDOW1>])
It turns out that similar arguments can be made for verbs other than event verbs. Consider the
sentence "Mary was unhappy". If it is represented using a unary predicate as
(<PAST UNHAPPY> (NAME j1 "Mary"))
In many situations using explicit event and state variables in formulas is cumbersome and
interferes with the development of other ideas. As a result, we will use different representations
depending on what is best for the presentation. For example, the logical form of "Mary sees
John" will sometimes be written as
(PRES (SEES1 l1 [AGENT (NAME j1 "Mary")]
[THEME (NAME m1 "John")]))
which of course is equivalent to
(PRES (j l1 (& (SEES1 l1) (AGENT l1 (NAME j1 "Mary"))
(THEME l1 (NAME m1 "John")))))
8
4.6 Thematic roles
Thematic Roles and theories based on the notion of thematic roles, or cases. One of the
motivating example is given below.
John", " the hammer", and "the window" play the same semantic roles in each of these sentences.
"John" is the actor, "the window" is the object, and "the hammer" is the instrument used in the
act of breaking of the window.
We introduced relations such as AGENT, THEME, and INSTR to capture these intuitions.
Perhaps the easiest thematic role to define is the AGENT role.
A noun phrase fills the AGENT role if it describes the instigator of the action described by the
sentence. The following sentences are acceptable: (bcz Agent role)
For example, given the sentence "The gray eagle saw the mouse", the NP "the mouse" is the
THEME and is the answer to the question "What was seen?"
For intransitive verbs, the THEME role is used for the subject NPs that are not AGENTs. Thus in
"The clouds appeared over the horizon", the NP "the clouds" fills the THEME role. More
examples follow, with the THEME NP in italics:
The rock broke.
John broke the rock.
I gave John the book.
Other phrases describe changes in location, direction of motion, or paths:
I walked from here to school yesterday.
It fell to the ground.
The birds flew from the lake along the river gorge.
There are at least three different types of phrases here: those that describe where something came
from (the FROM-LOC role), such as "from here"; those that describe the destination (the TO-
LOC role), such as "to the ground"; and those that describe the trajectory or path (the PATH-
LOC role), such as "along the gorge".
9
You can see other specializations of these roles when you consider the abstract
relation of possession:
I threw the ball to John. (the TO-LOC role)
I gave a book to John. (the TO-POSS role)
I caught the ball from John. (the FROM-LOC role)
I borrowed a book from John. (the FROM-POSS role)
The box contains a ball. (the AT LOC role)
John owns a book. (the AT POSS role)
Similarly, you might define AT-TIME, TO-TIME, and FROM-TIME roles, as in
I saw the car at 3 o'clock. (the AT-TIME role)
I worked from one until three. (the FROM-TIME and TO-TIME role)
The temperature remains at zero. (AT VALUE)
The temperature rose from zero. (FROM-VALUE)
Sentences are used for many different purposes. Each sentential mood indicates a different
relation between the speaker and the propositional content of the context. The logical form
language is extended to capture the distinctions. Each of the major sentence types has a
corresponding operator that takes the sentence interpretation as an argument is called a surface
speech act.
For ASSERT declarative sentences, such as "The man ate a peach", the complete LF is (logical
form)
(ASSERT (<PAST EAT> e1 [AGENT <THE m1 MAN1] [THEME <A p1 PEACH1>]))
For YES/NO questions, such as "Did the man eat a peach?", the LF is
(Y/N-QUERY (<PAST EAT> e1 [AGENT <THE m1 MAN1>] [THEME <A p1
PEACH1>]))
For WH-QUERY, the logical form of the sentence "What did the man eat?" is
(WH-QUERY (<PAST EAT> e1 (AGENT <THE m1 MAN1>] [THEME <WH w1
PHYSOBJ>]))
10
Embedded sentences
Embedded sentences, such as relative clauses, end up as complex restrictions within the noun
phrase construction and thus do not need any new notation. For example, the logical form of the
sentence "The man who ate a peach left" would be
(ASSERT
(<PAST LEAVE> l1
[AGENT <THE m1 (& (MAN1 m1)
(<PAST EAT1> e2 [AGENT m1]
[THEME <A p1 PEACH>]))>]))
For instance, the standard models for logic, called Tarskian models, are complete in that they
must map every legal term in the language into the domain and assign every statement to be true
or false. Model theory is an excellent method for studying context-independent meaning, because
the meanings of sentences are not defined with respect to one specific model but rather by how
they relate to any possible model.
Formally, a model m is a tuple <Dm, Im>, where Dm is the domain of interpretation (that is, a
set of primitive objects), and I is the interpretation function.
To handle natural language, the domain of interpretation would have to allow objects of
all the different types of things that can be referred to, including physical objects, times,
Locations, events, and situations.
The interpretation function maps senses and larger structures into structures defined on
the domain. For example, the following describe how an interpretation function will
interpret the senses based on some lexical classes:
Senses of noun phrases - refer to specific objects; the interpretation function maps each to an
element of Dm.
Senses of singular common nouns (such as "dog", "idea", "party") - identify classes of objects
in the domain; the interpretation function maps them to sets of elements from Dm (that is,
subsets of Dm).
Senses of verbs - identify sets of n-ary relations between objects in D. The arity depends on the
verb.
11
12
4.9 Language Modelling-Introduction and n-Gram Models
The Models that assign probabilities to sequences of words are called language models
(LMs). The simplest language model that assigns probabilities to sentences and sequences of
words is the n-gram model.
An n-gramis a sequence of N words: For example ―Please turn your homework‖ can be
written as:
–A 1-gram (unigram) is a single word sequence of words like ―please‖, ―turn‖, ―your‖,
―homework‖
–A 2-gram (bigram) is a two-word sequence of words like ―please turn‖, ―turn your‖, or ‖your
homework‖.
–A 3-gram (trigram) is a three-word sequence of words like ―please turn your‖, or ―turn your
homework‖.
We can use n-gram models to estimate the probability of the last word of an n-gram given the
previous words, and also to assign probabilities to entire word sequences.
13
Example: P(the man from jupiter) =
P(the) P(man|the) P(from|the man) P(jupiter|the man from)
N-Grams
The aim of the n-gram model (simplifying assumption) is to predict the next word in the given
text. Instead of computing the probability of a word given its entire history, we can approximate
the history by just the last few words.
N-Grams
Computing probabilities of word sequences (Sentences)
Unigram
P(<s> the man from Jupiter came </s>)
P(the) P(man) P(from) P(jupiter) P(came)
Bigram
P(<s> the man from Jupiter came </s>)
P(the|<s>) P(man|the) P(from|man) P(jupiter|from) P(came|jupiter) P(</s>|came)
Trigram
P(<s> the man from Jupiter came </s>)
P(the|<s><s>) P(man|<s>the) P(from|the man) P(jupiter|man from) P(came|from jupiter)
P(</s>|Jupiter came) P(</s>|came </s>)
14
N-Grams and Markov Models
The assumption that the probability of a word depends only on the previous word(s) is called
Markov assumption.
Markov models are the class of probabilistic models that assume that we can predict the
probability of some future unit without looking too far into the past.
•A bigram is called a first-order Markov model (because it looks one token into the past)
•A trigram is called a second-order Markov model;
•In general a N-Gram is called a N-1 order Markov model.
15
Estimating N-Gram Probabilities
A Bigram Example
A mini-corpus: We augment each sentence with a special symbol <s> at the
beginning of the sentence, to give us the bigram context of the first word, and
special end-symbol </s>.
<s> I am Sam </s>
<s> Sam I am </s>
<s> I fly </s>
Bigrams: <s> and </s> are also tokens. There are 6(4+2) tokens and 6*6=36 bigrams
16
4.10 Language model Evaluation
Language model evaluation in Natural Language Processing (NLP) is a crucial step to assess
the performance and quality of a language model. Evaluating language models helps researchers
and developers understand how well a model performs on various tasks and benchmark it against
other models or baselines. Here are some common evaluation techniques used in NLP:
1. Perplexity: Perplexity is a widely used metric for evaluating language models. It measures how well
a language model predicts a given text corpus. Lower perplexity indicates better performance.
Perplexity is calculated using the probabilities assigned by the model to each word in a test set.
2. Accuracy: Accuracy is a common evaluation metric for tasks like sentiment analysis, text
classification, or named entity recognition. It measures the proportion of correctly predicted
instances compared to the total number of instances in a test set. Accuracy is usually calculated by
comparing the predicted labels or outputs of the model with the ground truth labels.
3. F1 Score: F1 score is a widely used metric for evaluating models in tasks like named entity
recognition, part-of-speech tagging, and information extraction. It balances precision (the proportion
of correctly predicted positive instances) and recall (the proportion of actual positive instances
correctly predicted). F1 score is the harmonic mean of precision and recall and provides a single
measure of model performance.
4. BLEU Score: BLEU (Bilingual Evaluation Understudy) is a metric commonly used in machine
translation to evaluate the quality of generated translations. It measures the overlap between the
model's predicted translations and reference translations. BLEU score ranges from 0 to 1, with higher
scores indicating better translation quality.
5. ROUGE Score: ROUGE (Recall-Oriented Understudy for Gisting Evaluation) is a family of metrics
used to evaluate text summarization systems. ROUGE measures the overlap between the model's
generated summaries and reference summaries. Different variants of ROUGE, such as ROUGE-N
(measuring n-gram overlap) and ROUGE-L (measuring longest common subsequence), are used to
evaluate different aspects of summarization quality.
6. Human Evaluation: In addition to automated metrics, human evaluation is often conducted to
assess the quality of language models. Human evaluators provide judgments on various aspects of
model outputs, such as fluency, coherence, grammaticality, relevance, and overall quality. Human
evaluation provides valuable insights that may not be captured by automated metrics.
It is important to note that the choice of evaluation metric depends on the specific NLP task and the
goals of the evaluation. Different metrics have their strengths and weaknesses, and a combination of
metrics is often used to provide a comprehensive evaluation of a language model's performance.
17
4.11 Parameter Estimation
In the context of NLP, MLE is often used to estimate the probabilities of words or
sequences of words in a language model. The basic idea is to count the occurrences of
different words or sequences of words in a given training corpus and use these counts
to calculate the probabilities as follows.
1. Data Collection: Collect a large corpus of text that represents the domain or language
you want to model. This corpus will be used as the training data.
2. Data Preprocessing: Preprocess the training data by tokenizing it into words or
subword units, removing punctuation, normalizing case, etc. This step ensures that the
data is in a suitable format for modeling.
3. Parameter Estimation: Calculate the probabilities of words or sequences of words
based on the training data. For example, to estimate the probability of a word, count the
number of occurrences of that word in the corpus and divide it by the total number of
words in the corpus.
4. Smoothing: One challenge in using MLE is that unseen words or sequences in the
training data will have zero probabilities. To address this issue, smoothing techniques
are often applied to assign non-zero probabilities to unseen events. Common
smoothing methods include Laplace smoothing (add-one smoothing), Lidstone
smoothing, and Good-Turing smoothing.
18
5. Model Evaluation: Evaluate the performance of the language model using evaluation
metrics such as perplexity, accuracy, or F1 score. These metrics help assess how well the
model predicts the target language or performs on specific NLP tasks.
In Bayesian parameter estimation, the goal is to estimate the posterior distribution of the
parameters given the observed data and prior knowledge. The posterior distribution represents
the updated belief about the parameter values after considering the observed data. This
distribution is obtained by combining the likelihood of the data and the prior distribution of the
parameters using Bayes' theorem.
1. Define a Prior Distribution: Specify a prior distribution that captures the initial belief or
knowledge about the parameters before observing any data. The prior distribution represents the
uncertainty or prior information about the parameter values.
2. Likelihood Calculation: Calculate the likelihood of the observed data given the parameters. The
likelihood measures how probable the observed data is under the current parameter values.
3. Bayesian Inference: Apply Bayes' theorem to update the prior distribution based on the
likelihood and obtain the posterior distribution of the parameters. The posterior distribution
reflects the updated belief about the parameter values after considering the observed data.
4. Posterior Analysis: Analyze the posterior distribution to obtain estimates of the parameters. This
can be done by calculating the mean, median, or mode of the posterior distribution, which
represent the point estimates of the parameters. Additionally, credible intervals can be computed
to quantify the uncertainty in the parameter estimates.
5. Model Evaluation: Evaluate the performance of the Bayesian language model using evaluation
metrics such as perplexity, accuracy, or F1 score, similar to the evaluation of MLE-based
models.
19
Bayesian parameter estimation provides several advantages over MLE in NLP. It allows the
incorporation of prior knowledge, which can be particularly useful when the amount of available
training data is limited. Additionally, Bayesian estimation provides a richer representation of
uncertainty by yielding a posterior distribution rather than a point estimate. This uncertainty
information can be valuable for decision-making or downstream applications.
Overall, Bayesian parameter estimation offers a principled and flexible framework for estimating
parameters in language models, incorporating prior knowledge, and capturing uncertainty in
NLP tasks.
Language model adaptation in NLP refers to the process of fine-tuning a pre-trained language
model on a specific task or domain to improve its performance. Language models like GPT-3.5
are trained on a large corpus of diverse text from the internet, which gives them a general
understanding of language. However, fine-tuning or adapting them to a specific task or domain
can enhance their performance on that particular task.
1. Dataset Collection: Gather a dataset that is specific to the target task or domain you want to
adapt the language model to. This dataset should be representative of the target task and should
include text samples that the language model is likely to encounter during inference.
2. Dataset Preprocessing: Preprocess the collected dataset by cleaning the text, removing
irrelevant information, and ensuring the dataset is in a format that can be used for training.
3. Model Architecture: Decide on the specific architecture you want to use for adaptation. This
can be the same architecture as the pre-trained language model or a modified version depending
on the task requirements.
4. Task-Specific Labels: If your target task involves supervised learning, you will need to annotate
or label your dataset with the appropriate task-specific labels. This step is crucial for tasks like
sentiment analysis, named entity recognition, or machine translation.
5. Fine-tuning: Initialize the pre-trained language model with the weights learned during pre-
training, and then fine-tune it on your target dataset. During fine-tuning, the model learns task-
specific patterns and representations. The process typically involves minimizing a task-specific
loss function using techniques such as backpropagation and gradient descent.
6. Hyperparameter Tuning: Experiment with different hyperparameters such as learning rate,
batch size, and regularization techniques to optimize the performance of the adapted model. This
step helps to find the best set of hyperparameters that work well for the specific task.
7. Evaluation and Iteration: Evaluate the performance of the adapted language model on a
validation set or using other evaluation metrics specific to your task. Iterate on the fine-tuning
20
process by making changes to the architecture, hyperparameters, or dataset, if necessary, to
further improve performance.
It's worth noting that language model adaptation requires a substantial amount of task-specific
data to achieve good performance. If only a limited amount of data is available, transfer learning
techniques such as few-shot or zero-shot learning can be used to leverage the knowledge learned
from the pre-trained language model. Additionally, it's important to strike a balance between
task-specific adaptation and preserving the general language understanding of the original pre-
trained model.
1. N-gram Language Models: N-gram models are simple and widely used language models that
predict the probability of a word given its preceding context of n-1 words. They rely on counting
and statistical techniques to estimate the probabilities. For example, a trigram model predicts the
next word based on the previous two words.
2. Neural Language Models: Neural language models leverage deep learning techniques, such as
recurrent neural networks (RNNs), long short-term memory (LSTM) networks, and transformers,
to capture complex patterns and dependencies in language. These models learn distributed
representations of words and generate probabilities based on the context.
3. Transformer-based Language Models: Transformer models, such as OpenAI's GPT
(Generative Pre-trained Transformer), have gained significant attention in recent years. These
models use self-attention mechanisms to capture global dependencies and parallelize
computation, making them highly effective for tasks like language generation, machine
translation, and question answering.
4. Contextualized Language Models: Contextualized language models, such as ELMo
(Embeddings from Language Models) and BERT (Bidirectional Encoder Representations from
Transformers), generate word representations that are sensitive to the context in which the word
appears. These models provide contextual embeddings that are useful for downstream NLP tasks
like sentiment analysis, named entity recognition, and text classification.
5. Encoder-Decoder Models: Encoder-decoder models, often based on the sequence-to-sequence
architecture, are used for tasks like machine translation and text summarization. These models
consist of an encoder that processes the input sequence and a decoder that generates the output
sequence based on the encoded representation.
6. Hybrid Models: Some language models combine multiple approaches or architectures to
leverage their individual strengths. For example, ULMFiT (Universal Language Model Fine-
tuning) combines features of transformer models with techniques like transfer learning and fine-
tuning to improve performance on specific tasks.
7. Pre-trained Language Models: Pre-trained language models, such as GPT, BERT, and
RoBERTa, are models that are trained on large amounts of data before being fine-tuned for
specific tasks. These models capture a broad understanding of language and can be adapted to
various downstream tasks with additional training.
21
These are just a few examples of the types of language models used in NLP. Different models
have different strengths and are suited for specific tasks and scenarios. Researchers continue to
explore new architectures and techniques to develop more powerful and efficient language
models for a wide range of NLP applications.
In natural language processing (NLP), there are several language-specific modeling problems
that arise due to the unique characteristics and complexities of different languages. Here are
some common language-specific modeling challenges in NLP:
1. Morphological Variations: Many languages exhibit rich morphology, where words can have
multiple forms depending on factors like tense, number, gender, and case. Modeling
morphological variations can be challenging, especially for languages with highly inflected
forms, as it requires capturing the intricate relationships between word forms and their meanings.
2. Word Order and Syntax: Languages vary in their word order and sentence structure. For
example, English follows a subject-verb-object (SVO) order, while languages like Japanese
follow a subject-object-verb (SOV) order. Modeling the syntactic structure and understanding
the correct word order for different languages is crucial for tasks like parsing, machine
translation, and text generation.
3. Named Entity Recognition (NER): Named Entity Recognition involves identifying and
classifying named entities such as names of people, organizations, locations, and dates in text.
Different languages have specific naming conventions and variations, which pose challenges for
building language-independent NER systems. Language-specific rules and resources are often
required to handle these variations effectively.
4. Sentiment Analysis: Sentiment analysis aims to determine the sentiment or opinion expressed in
a text. The sentiment lexicons, patterns, and linguistic cues that indicate sentiment can differ
across languages. Building accurate sentiment analysis models requires language-specific
sentiment resources and training data that capture the nuances of sentiment expression in
different languages.
5. Language Idiosyncrasies: Each language has its own set of idiosyncrasies, such as idiomatic
expressions, proverbs, and cultural references. These language-specific nuances pose challenges
for models that aim to understand and generate natural language text. Capturing and representing
these idiosyncrasies is crucial for building language models that accurately handle language-
specific contexts.
6. Low-Resource Languages: Low-resource languages, which have limited amounts of training
data and linguistic resources, present unique modeling challenges. Limited resources make it
difficult to develop robust models for these languages. Techniques like cross-lingual transfer
learning, data augmentation, and unsupervised methods are often employed to address the
scarcity of language-specific resources.
7. Code-Switching and Multilingualism: Many languages exhibit code-switching, where speakers
alternate between two or more languages within a conversation. Modeling code-switching and
multilingual text requires techniques that can handle language mixing, language identification,
and understanding the context-dependent language usage.
22
Addressing these language-specific modeling challenges in NLP often involves a combination of
linguistic knowledge, domain expertise, and language-specific resources. Researchers and
practitioners work on developing techniques that can effectively handle these challenges and
improve the performance and applicability of NLP models across different languages.
23
learning, a model can generate outputs in a language it has not been explicitly trained
on, using only high-level instructions or prompts. Few-shot learning involves training a
model on a limited amount of labeled data from the target language, allowing it to
perform well on that language. These techniques are useful when resources in the target
language are scarce.
6. Cross-lingual Named Entity Recognition (NER): Cross-lingual NER involves
recognizing named entities in multiple languages. By leveraging multilingual training
data and shared representations, cross-lingual NER models can generalize across
languages and recognize entities even in low-resource languages. Techniques like cross-
lingual transfer learning and alignment methods are used to improve cross-lingual NER
performance.
24