This document outlines the content of two lectures on the visual system, focusing on the processes of photo transduction and retinal computation. It discusses the anatomy of the eye, the importance of the retina, and the mechanisms of eye movements, including smooth pursuit, vergence, and saccades. The lectures aim to explain how the eye captures light energy and processes visual information, emphasizing the retina's role in extracting features and compressing data for the brain.
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as DOCX, PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
0 ratings0% found this document useful (0 votes)
5 views12 pages
Vision
This document outlines the content of two lectures on the visual system, focusing on the processes of photo transduction and retinal computation. It discusses the anatomy of the eye, the importance of the retina, and the mechanisms of eye movements, including smooth pursuit, vergence, and saccades. The lectures aim to explain how the eye captures light energy and processes visual information, emphasizing the retina's role in extracting features and compressing data for the brain.
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as DOCX, PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
You are on page 1/ 12
Good morning. So welcome.
Um, this is the going to be the first of two lectures
that you're getting both on the same day about the visual system. So vision, as you probably know, is a very important primary sense for humans and very many different species. Does anyone know who's iris is, by the way? There will be a prize this afternoon if you. Almost know the right answer. It's a cuttlefish. Yeah. Close. Um, so vision is very important. Primary sensor. And in the course of these two lectures today, we're going to only scratch the surface of the visual system. But we will try to look at two important processes that are mediated by the eye. So the first is the conversion of light energy and environmental signal into an electrical biological signal. This is the process of photo transduction which we will look at this morning. And then we'll move on again this morning to look at the role of the AI in neural computation. So we'll examine how circuits in the retina, um, process the visual scene. And we'll see how feature extraction and parallel processing generate a compressed representation of important sensory salient sensory information that is important for guiding behaviour and is communicated to the brain through the optic nerve. Um, then in this afternoon's lecture, we will firstly, we'll have a mental meta quiz to see if you learnt anything this morning. Then we will talk a little bit more about retinal processing, and then follow the rest of the physical pathway from the retina to the visual cortex, which is thought to be the main route to supporting conscious visual processing. And then after that, we'll have another mental meta quiz. Um, and you can see if you do any better on the second quiz in the first. So this morning's lecture is going to cover some very basic stuff about the AI in the visual fields. Um, introduce some important, um, principles of visual processing in the retina. We'll look at its structure and connectivity, and then we'll spend most of the time looking at photo transduction. Um, a very important process which is going to capture light energy and generate the biological signal and photoreceptors and then start looking at retinal computation, focusing on the famous on and off pathways. This is the bit where you'll need to pay special attention. So the eye, as you know, is the organ of vision. And across the, uh, across the, uh, uh, natural world, there's a wide variety of different eye designs. Um, humans and other vertebrates have what's known as a camera type eye, which is shown here. So you probably are familiar with the anatomy of the eye. But these elements that the at the front, these optical elements, the crystalline lens and the cornea are refractive elements that are responsible for bending incoming rays of light and generating focussed images on the photosensitive surface at the back of the eye. Much like the lens of a camera generates a focussed image on a film, or nowadays on the CMOs sensor in the eye. Despite sensitive surfaces, the remarkable neural retina which we're going to be focusing on a lot today. There are also various muscles associated with the eye, so muscles in the ciliary body can change the shape and optical power of the lens. This is a process known as accommodation, which allows you to focus on objects at different distances than the outside world, and muscles associated with the iris can change the size of the entrance pupil of the eye, so you can adapt to different light intensities, see in bright sunlight and also in dimmer conditions. There are also six extra ocular muscles, only two of which are shown in this cross section, which are attached to the outside of the eye. The globe which allow the eye to rotate around three different axes in the orbit. So that's the sort of basic anatomy. But as I mentioned, there's a wide variety of different eye designs in nature. We've looked at cuttlefish eye. This is the eye of the compound either fly which has a radically different organisation. Unfortunately, in these lectures we're really going to focus on, um, mammalian or specifically primate vision. But you should be aware that there's a lot of variety. So the eye is obviously a sense organ. And in thinking about a sense organ, we have to consider, um, what region of of stimulus space, what region of the environment is accessible to the eye. And this is known as the visual field. So the visual field is defined as the region of space in angular terms that the eye can see. Um, the monocular visual field for the human eye is about 170 degrees in the horizontal plane. So these red rays here indicate the limits of the visual field. For the right eye it's about 170 degrees quite large a bit less than the vertical dimension. Um, but one thing you have probably noticed is you have two eyes. And this has a number of important consequences. So the first is that your total visual field is enlarged beyond what one I would see on its own. And secondly, because you're a primate and you have both eyes on the front of your head, your monocular visual fields overlap, generating a so-called binocular region. This is about 120 degrees in humans. And binocular vision has a number of important consequences for the visual system, one of which is that it supports a mechanism for depth perception called stereo. This. This is a binocular mechanism for depth perception, and this arises because your eyes are separated on the head. They obtain slightly different parallax views of objects in the outside world, and your brain can fuse those two images to generate three dimensional perception and depth. Perception is one of the functions of binocular vision. Um, in thinking about this visual fields, though, we said 170 degrees for one eye. It's not sampled evenly, and this arises due to the structure of the eye and particularly the retina. So one important feature is that the retina has a region called the optic disk. This is where the optic nerve joins the eye and blood vessels enter and exit the eye. At this point, there's a gap in the photoreceptor layer which generates a blind spot, so any regions of visual space cast onto the optic disk are invisible to this. I usually don't notice this when you're viewing the world of two Eyes. It's one of the advantages of binocular vision. The second sort of feature of the retina is that in many species, including humans, there's a specialised region called the fovea, which is responsible for very detailed high acuity vision. So regions of visual space cast onto the fovea a perceived in greater detail. We'll see why later in this lecture. But the fovea only covers a couple of degrees of visual space. So it's about the width of two fingers at arm's length. Only the small sliver of visual space can be viewed at high resolution by the phobia. So again the retina is a heterogeneous structure, something we're going to keep returning to in this lecture and thinking about the eyes and the visual field, though. One thing you might realise that the eyes can move. So this is illustrated here by one of the masters of eye movements. Obviously it's a chameleon, not a primate. Eye movements serve a whole variety of important functions of the visual system, which unfortunately, we're not going to go into in any depth. But I'm going to briefly give you a flavour of three types of voluntary eye movement in these next couple of slides. So one voluntary eye movement that you should be aware of is known as smooth pursuit. The function of smooth pursuit eye movements is to maintain images of moving objects on the fovea. So this process of tracking objects and keeping them fixed on the face is known as your fovea. Eating the object. So this interesting object moves backwards and forwards your eye. Both eyes will rotate alternately clockwise and then counter clockwise in order to maintain foliation of this target. So you're probably doing smooth pursuit now if you're tracking this laser pointer and slowly getting hypnotised. So this example of an eye movement is quite slow and both eyes rotate in the same direction. They're both rotating at any instant, either clockwise. The target's moving this way or counterclockwise this way. That's known as a conjugate eye movement because both eyes are rotating in the same sense, clockwise or counterclockwise. And that contrasts with our second example, the vergence eye movements, which are important for fabricating targets in depth. So if I, for example, fo gating this laser pointer and it moves towards my nose, my eyes need to converge. They both rotate towards the nose in order to maintain focus of this target. So viewed from above, one eye is going clockwise and the other counterclockwise. That's known as a disjunctive eye movement. And as you can imagine, and obviously if the target moves away, the eyes diverge. So these eye movements, as you can imagine, are very intimately linked with stereotypes. And both classes Neath the seat and vergence are controlled by quite complex circuits that span from the brainstem all the way through to the frontal cortex. These are also quite slow, smooth eye movements, and that contrasts them with our third example of an eye movement, which are saccades. Saccades are very fast, ballistic eye movements which allow the eye to very abruptly change position in the orbit, and they're separated by, um, by periods when the eye is relatively still known as fixations. So these rapid, abrupt changes in eye position, um, allow you, in combination of head movements and movements of the, of the whole body, sometimes to redirect gaze, to explore a visual scene. Um, this is illustrated here. So this is the pattern of saccades, these quick lines and fixations, the little blobs that the human is doing as they inspect this interesting artefacts at the museum. And, um, the important reason is the reason you need to perform this sequence of psychotic eye movements is to allow you to successively sample different parts of the visual scene with your fovea. Remember I told you the favourite only covers a tiny little slit? There are a couple of degrees of visual space. If you want to build up a high resolution picture of an object of interest, you need to successfully sample from the visual scene using these successive psychotic environments. Saccades are extremely fast, so the eye can rotate around 900 degrees per second. In primates, it's one of the fastest movements that you can make. But even during the fixations in between the saccades, the eye is not really static. So small fixations, eye movements, they're called vertical drifts, tremors and microseconds mean that the eye is constantly moving even during the fixation. And that is important to prevent images being perfectly stabilised on the retina, because stabilised images fade very rapidly from visual perception. So these fixation eye movements are tied to character actors. So this is a very inadequate survey of eye movements. You should be aware that there's several other types of reflex eye movement in addition to the ones I've talked about, which also important for us for normal vision. But we won't go into any more detail about eye movements today. So we're going to touch now on some basic principles of vision and start thinking about how the how the eye functions in, in, in capturing light energy and generating visual perception. And one very important thing you should take away from these lectures is the eye is not going to perform like a simple camera. It's not going to acquire a faithful image of the outside world and then sort of ship this image off to the brain, but to do all the processing instead, it's going to do a lot of data compression and feature extraction. So let's start by to try to understand what I'm talking about, looking at this very simple image. So on this uniform background, this grey bar appears to have uniform intensity from one end to the other. Pretty boring right? But if it's superimposed on a gradient background, it doesn't look uniform anymore. So the bar hasn't changed, of course, but the way that you perceive it has when it's overlaid on a different background. And this very simple example illustrates a very important principle of retinal visual processing, which is that the retina is very interested in differences in light intensity across space and also across time. So the right hand edge of the bar, the bar is relatively brighter than the background, relatively dark in the background, sorry. Whereas the left hand end is relatively brighter than the background. And the retina is particularly interested, as I said, in these relative differences in light intensity, in this case across space, which is known as spatial contrast, and spatial contrast is but one of several. What's going on now? Several visual features that the retina extracts were sensitive to. A couple of others are listed, a few others listed here. So the wavelength. The retina is also sensitive to the wavelength of light, which as we will see, will lead is responsible for the perception of colour. So this is the visual feature and this is the first set. And as we've seen writing is sensitive to spatial contrast. The differences in the lighting intensity across space, which allows the perception of edges in visual scenes. And it's also sensitive to spatial frequency, which approximately corresponds to the size of visual features. There are many other aspects of feature sensitivity two one, for example, is motion, so the retina can detect motion and motion direction. But we're not going to touch on this slightly more complicated topic. Uh, very much. We'll mention it briefly in the second lecture. So before we go on, it's very important that you have clear in your mind what spatial contrast is and what spatial frequency is. on this slide is supposed to help illustrate this. So this is an image. It's a sinusoidal grating. It doesn't really matter in which both contrast and spatial frequency vary. So contrast as I mentioned before, refers to differences in light intensity across space. At the top of this image, contrast is high. So there's a very large difference in intensity between these bright white bars and these dark black bars. And towards the bottom of the image contrast decreases where both the white and the black bars move towards this mid grey level, and you find it very hard to see them. Spatial frequency refers to the length scale over which these contrast modulations happen, so on the right hand side of the image, spatial frequency is low. That corresponds to long wavelengths. And you can see these very broad white and dark bars. Whereas on the left hand side of the image the spatial frequency is high, that corresponds a short wavelength. And you can see these very narrow closely spaced bars. So the reason this is important to understand the difference in spatial frequency and contrast, and think about these features, is that natural scenes contain a broad range of contrast and spatial frequencies that the retina needs to be sensitive to. So one, um, uh, uh, aspect of retinal processing, which we're going to encounter in these lectures is the concept of parallel processing. And what this means is that essentially there are going to be different populations of cells in the retina which are responsible for extracting a particular, in this case, spatial frequencies from the same input image. So panel A is supposed to show an input image might be familiar to you, which has been cast onto the retina. And in these three other panels we're looking at the representation generated by a particular population of cells. So in B, a population of cells which are tuned to low spatial frequencies have extracted a low spatial frequency representation of Westminster Tower. Whereas other populations of cells are extracting medium or very high spatial frequency information from the same scene. So this is happening by separate population cells at the same time, in parallel to generate multiple representations of the same input image, which are going to be communicated to the brain to support different aspects of behaviour. And over the course of this lecture this morning and this afternoon, you will hopefully come to understand exactly how this comes about, or more or less exactly how it comes about in the retina. So this idea of taking an image and splitting it into multiple representations feature tuned representations as is parallel processing. So let's now dive into the retina and see how it does, how it detects light and does all this neural computation and parallel processing. So we'll start by reviewing the structure of the retina. You've probably already somewhat familiar. So the retina is actually a very well studied part of the central nervous system. And importantly it is part of the central nervous system, not the peripheral nervous system. So you might know if you've done some developmental neurobiology that early in development, the neural the neural plate if originate some complicated morphogenetic movements generate the cup shaped retina of the two eyes. The it's part of the CNS, as I've said, um, it's a layered structure. So it has multiple layers of cell bodies and synaptic neuro pills. And here we can see the, the basic arrangement. So up here at the top are the photoreceptors. Um, these are going to be the site of light capture that are really going to convert an environmental signal light energy on the one hand into an internal neural representation. So they're kind of the interface between the outside world and the internal neural representation. And that's going to support vision. Um, rather counterintuitively, they sit right at the back of the retina. So incoming light has to pass through all of the other cell body layers, at least in the mammalian retina, before reaching the photoreceptors in an octopus, by the way, it's the other way around. But for primates, the facial receptors at the back information then is passed from the photoreceptors to bipolar cells down into neurones in the retina, and then from the bipolar cell, information can be passed to retinal ganglion cells. These are the output cells of the retina, which are going to project long axons through the optic nerve to reach the retina recipient targets in the brain. So that pathway just went through from photoreceptor to bipolar to ganglion cell is the most direct route for visual information. It's the so-called vertical pathway. If I. Guess the vertical pathway through the retina. But there are also other populations of injured neurones which mediate a process of lateral information flow. And we're going to encounter two examples on this morning and on this afternoon of of that and why it's important. Another important thing to note is that a lot of the neural signals in the retina are mediated by graded changes in membrane potential. They support a lot of the neural activity in the retina, but ganglion cells, the output cells, are spiking neurones. They fire action potentials suitable for long distance communication along the long optic nerve all the way to the brain. Hopefully you know the difference in graded potential changes and action potentials. So let's start by looking at the photoreceptors then and look at the process of photo transduction. As you might know, there are two main flavours of photoreceptor in the eye the rods and the cones shown here in this beautiful light micrograph. So these are rods and these are cones. Two cones here or three actually. And these two main populations of photoreceptors have different structures, different physiologies different functions and different distributions across the retina. So the rods this is now a scanning electron micrograph. You can see lots of rods here. These things are responsible for very high sensitivity Vision. So they're extremely sensitive to light. And they support your vision at very, very low light intensity, so-called psychotropic vision at night. Um, the rods are very abundant. So they outnumber cones about 20 to 1 in the primate retina, and they're distributed across almost the entire retina. So in this plot here, you're looking at the distribution of the photoreceptor sites measured across the surface of the retina, where we're measuring in degrees from the phobia. So the fever is here at zero degrees. And then we're measuring it degrees eccentricity from the five year. And hopefully you can see that the rods are kind of distributed everywhere except at the phobia where they're absent. So that's the one region that they're excluded from. And instead at the table we find the other type of photoreceptors, the cones. So the cones are responsible for vision at higher light intensities, which is called for topic vision. So you're using your cones now in this bright, um, lecture theatre. Um, there are three types of cones which as we will see, support colour vision. And they are very sparse across most of the retina, except for the phobia, where they're the only photoreceptor type present. So again, the the retina has a very heterogeneous structure. I mentioned at the start of the lecture that the phobia is responsible for high acuity vision, and this arises for several reasons. So this cartoon doesn't really illustrate this very clearly, actually had a phobia. We've said only cones are present, but the cones that are found at the phobia are very small and squished together. So this means that because they're packed in very tightly, each individual cone only views a tiny sliver a fraction of a degree of visual space. And the circuitry of the retina is designed to keep this visual information separate. So what this means is that these individual fovea cones have dedicated private connections through the retinal circuit onto ganglion cells. In fact, one cone can connect to, on average, about 2.5 ganglion cells. Um, this means that the rational circuitry does not mix visual information from adjacent cones. It's kept separated, and that's responsible for visual acuity. This tight spacing of the cones really sets the limit on the resolution of your vision, the limit of visual acuity. This sort of private connection scheme is not a present in the peripheral retina. So out in the periphery, many cones. In this cartoon only three. But in reality it can be thousands of cones or thousands of rods and converge onto individual ganglion cells. So this results in a pooling of visual information from many photoreceptors, which decreases visual acuity, visual acuity. So the resolution of vision is poorer, but it does have the advantage of it increases visual sensitivity because you're summing the light signal that's derived from many photoreceptors. The peripheral retina is better for visual sensitivity and for motion perception. The flavour is better for high acuity, detailed vision. Okay, so let's have zoom in on some photoreceptors and look at their, um, the structure of the cells. So photoreceptors, the cells and cells can be divided into three main sections. The important business end is called the outer segment. So rods shown here in this micrograph have very long rod shaped outer segments. Um and these outer segments contain very large numbers of flattened membranous compartments called discs. The discs are a little bit like a stack of coins inside the outer segment. They're shown here in cross-section. And these, as we're going to see, are going to be the size of light capture. So the rods have very long outer segments containing tons of discs full of photo pigment for light capture. Cones, on the other hand, have shorter cone shaped outer segments. And they're these flattened, membranous compartments at all lamellae. They have a different name in cones. The second part of the photoreceptor is that the inner segment. So this contains the cell body in the nucleus and lots of mitochondria to generate energy for potential destruction. And then finally the cell has a synaptic terminal where it releases neurotransmitter onto downstream cells such as bipolar cells in the retina. And in the case of photoreceptors, the neurotransmitter is glutamate. So as I briefly mentioned, it's in these disc or lamellae membranes that light capture is going to happen. And the reason that the light is captured there is that these disc membranes, in the case of rods, are packed full of molecules of photo pigment in this slide. So photo pigment itself is made up of two main ingredients. The first is the protein and oxygen. In rods. It's called rhodopsin. Easy to remember I've shown here in blue this is a seven transmembrane protein. And you can see its seven transmembrane helices helix crossing the membrane. The second ingredient is a retinol cofactor derived from vitamin A, which is covalently bound that shown here in red and is covalently bound to a lysine residue on one of these transmembrane helices. So together, the option and the rationale cofactor comprise the photo pigment. And this retinal cofactor is important because this is actually the chromosphere which is going to absorb light. And the way that light capture works is that it's going to induce a conformation will change what isomerisation of the retinal cofactor, converting a cis carbon carbon double bond to a trans carbon carbon double bond, so that retinol changes from 11 cis retinol into all trans retinal. And that process is known as photo isomerisation. The absorption of a photon causes the change in the structure of rationale from 11 cis to all trans. And this is really the crucial event that this bridge British represents the capture of the photon, and it's going to be propagated because the change in the shape of the retinal chromosphere will induce the conformational change in the opt in protein, which allows it to interact with the hetero trimeric G protein, as we will see, and kicks off a whole cascade of events in the photoreceptor, which eventually results in a change in glutamate release. We're going to come to that cascade in a couple of slides time, but it's all initiated by this initial photo isomerisation event. So both rods and cones are two main types of um photoreceptor contain um photo pigment um, but they um contain slightly different types of photo pigment which result in sensitivities to different wavelengths of light. So this sensitivity is shown here in this plot. So this is wavelength on the x axis. And this is log sensitivity on the y axis. All rods share the same photo pigment which we said is called rhodopsin. And as a result they all have the same spectral sensitivity, which is illustrated here in black. It peaks around 500 nanometres, a sort of greenish colour, and then drops off at longer wavelengths. Um. However, there are three types of these. In primates, there are three types of cone, each with a different type of cone of sin. So for example, the s cones contain s Hopsin s for short, uh, and they're colloquially known as blue cones because short wavelengths correspond to the perception of blue colours at their sensitivity shown here. This blue trace. There are m cones M for medium containing an option with the sensitivity shown here in this green trace. And then finally L cones containing an L and L for long, which are tuned to the longest wavelengths globally known as red cones. Um, the reason that each population of cones has a different spectral sensitivity is because of differences in the amino acid sequence of the oxygen, so the retinal cofactor is the same in all cases. The amino acid sequence of the oxygen that's illustrated here is responsible for these differences in spectral sensitivity. And this difference in spectral sensitivity is illustrated again on this slide. Now we're not on a log scale which is why these curves look a bit different. Um, because we have three populations of cones in the primate retina with different spectral sensitivity. This is this supports colour vision. And the reason that it allows you to perceive colour is going to be hopefully illustrated here. So in response to any individual wavelength of light, the light of this particular wavelength, let's call it 590 nanometres is incident on your retina. As you can see, this wavelength is going to cause very strong activation of your L cones. About half as much activation of the M cones. And no activation at all of your S cones. And that ratio of activation levels, the relative levels of activation. High, medium and none can be decoded to perceive um in this case for colour yellow. So the relative activations of the cones is responsible for colour vision. Um, this also makes clear to you hopefully. Why the why road vision or topic vision at night is is achromatic because all the rods share the same photo pigment. They all have the same spectral sensitivity, so we can't compare any activation levels across the rod population in order to in order to decode wavelength, because the rods all share the same offset. Okay, so let's dive into, um, the process of the photo transduction in the photo receptor. So we started we've already seen the first step which is the absorption of a photon by the retinal cofactor which calls this photo summarisation. And I already mentioned that this is going to induce a conformational change in the oxygen protein. And because it's a certain transmembrane protein, this will allow it to interact with a heterotrophic G protein inside the photoreceptor outer segment. And in the case of photoreceptors, that trimeric G protein has a special name. It's called transduced in and. As usual, this interaction between the seven transmembrane protein and the G protein is going to catalyse the exchange of GDP to GTP and activate the hetero trimeric G protein. Hopefully, that's an interaction you're already familiar with. So activate a transducer can then activate an effector enzyme, which in the case of photoreceptors is called phosphodiesterase. This yellow guy here and phosphodiesterase will then um, catalyse the breakdown of cyclic GMP in the outer segment to GMP, which of course results in a decrease in cyclic GMP concentration. the consequence of that is that these cyclic nucleotide gated ion channels will close. So normally in the dark there's a high level of cyclic GMP. It's generated by quite late cycles. It gets these channels open and it allows cations to flow into the outer segment. So this inward current in the dark is called the dark current. And it means that under dark conditions photoreceptors are quite depolarised. They sit at around -30 millivolts. They're quite depolarised compared to a neurone. But when light is instant on the phase receptor, and through this cascade, the cyclic GMP is broken down and the channels close, the outer segment is going to hyper polarise. So membrane potential. There's an undated potassium conductance and membrane potential ahead of the reversal potential of potassium. And as a result we see hyper polarisation of the membrane potential. So this is very important in response to light photoreceptors hyper polarised. They do not depolarise somewhat counter-intuitive And as a result of this hyper polarisation, they're going to release less neurotransmitter. Less glutamate is released. This is illustrated here. This is the pulse of light. This is a decrease in the inward current. Notice the axis is the negative is decreasing. And we're going to see hyper polarisation of the receptor and the release of less glutamate. Another important thing to bear in mind is like all second messenger pathways this benefits from a large amount of amplification. And as I've illustrated, here are some numbers. You can see how the absorption of a single photon by one rhodopsin molecule through this amplification cascade can result in a final change in membrane potential of one millivolt, which is quite substantial, but given just one incident photon on a rod photoreceptor. So this signal is something that has been measured in some landmark studies. So this is a beautiful example of trying to measure the responses of Individual photoreceptors, individual rods in I think it was an unfortunate monkey whose retina was, um, donated for this experiment. So in this experiment, the individual photoreceptors have sucked into this, um, patch electrode, and it's exposed to very dim flashes of light at the time of these dots. And hopefully what you can see are each of these dim flashes of light causes a very brief hyper polarisation of the photoreceptor, measured using perforated patch recording. Um, in some cases, this misses where the receptor doesn't respond, and there's also occasionally a false positive. We get a little dip even with no light flash. That's due to thermal isomerisation of retinal. But in general, you can see that the photoreceptor is very good at reporting these very dim light flashes, which are designed to explain receptors to just one photon each time. Another important aspect of this response is that it's graded according to the intensity of our of the light flash. So this is illustrated here now for cones. So the time of this little blip, the cone is being exposed to flashes of increasing brightness. And hopefully what you can see is this results in an increasing amount of hyper polarisation as the flashes get brighter and brighter, until eventually the response saturates. So this means that the cone is kind of an analogue sensor, where the intensity of the light flash is converted into a graded change in hyper polarisation, and therefore a graded change in glutamate decrease onto downstream neurones. Um, with this type of recording technique, you can also measure um, currents. So here's some current recordings are being made. Um, in real time cones. You can see that under dark conditions there's an inward current -30 ish pico amps. And in response to a light flash, that dark current that we talked about is shut off as a cyclic nucleotide gated cation channels close. You can see this decrease in the inward Inwood Current. And you can also see that the dynamics of this, of these current changes in the photoreceptors are quite different from rods and cones and cones have a much faster response kinetics than rods. So we've sort of mentioned now over the course of the last few slides, a number of different differences between rods and cones, and they sort of captured on this slide to make things easier. So rods as I mentioned, are very abundant in the retina. They're extremely sensitive. They're about a thousand times more sensitive to light compared to cones. And they mediate a vision at very low light levels known as topic vision. And they're sensitive in part because they have very long outer segments containing tons of discs and lots of photo pigment and show very high levels of amplification. However, they all have the same type of opposition to the toxin, and so they have the same shared spectral sensitivity, which results in achromatic vision. The cones, on the other hand, have lower sensitivity. They only support vision at of higher light intensities is no this topic vision. But there are three types of cones with three different types of cone option, and as a result of this they support colour vision. So averaging for time. Good. So now we've only touched on the first cell in the retina the photo the photoreceptors. And we're now going to have a look downstream of these cells some of these vertical pathways through the retina for the remainder of this lecture. So. Downstream of the photoreceptors these vertical pathways are going to be the first point that contributes to some of this feature extraction and parallel processing that I referred to at the beginning. And one very important distinction is that these vertical pathways can be broken down into so-called on and off pathways that kind of illustrate in very simple terms downstream of the cones in this slide. So these on and off pathways sometimes cause some confusion. But if you pay attention now you won't be confused. So the important aspect of these pathways is they're named according to the change in light intensity, which is going to evoke spiking in the pathway. So in the En pathway, cells are going to respond by increasing spiking in response to an increase in light intensity i.e on whereas in the off pathway cells are going to increase that spiking in response to a decrease in light intensity so-called off response okay. Both pathways can modulate in both directions. Right. So the on pathway where it spikes in response to the increase in light intensity, a decrease in light intensity will cause a suppression of spiking. We just get the opposite of response. And then off pathway which which as we saw prefers light off. So it increases spiking when the intensity goes down. If the intensity goes up. Spiking will be suppressed. So both pathways can modulate bi directionally, but they have opposite preferences. The off pathway prefers preferring, meaning it's going to spike more. A decrease in light intensity. Whereas the On pathway is looking for an increase in light intensity. Okay. Remember this. The key thing here is we're talking about changes in light intensity. So these are increments or decrements in light intensity that the pathways are interested in. Um the pathway is more complicated as we will see. So these on and off pathways start at the level of the bipolar cells. All the phase receptors as we've seen, have the same response to an increase in light intensity, which is like hyper polarised and release less glutamate that we've already seen. So this distinction between on and off is starting with our bipolar cells. And in the mammalian retina. There's lots of different types of bipolar cell. So they have different morphologies and um, different functions. Um, all of them make connections with photoreceptors in the outer flexible layer, and then they generate their they elaborate synaptic terminals here in the inner plexus form layer, which in the case of cone bipolar cells will connect onto retinal ganglion cells. There's a whole bunch of different types of cone bipolar cell. All of these are cone bipolar cells and just one lonely Roger bipolar cell, which we'll see its special properties. Uh, see? So why do we need so many types of bipolar cell? You're probably wondering. Well, remember that in the cone system we have three different types of cone. And they all contribute in various ways to on and off pathways, which subdivides the number of channels in the retina to some largish number. So it's quite complicated. But this key distinction that I mentioned between on and off is always true. So the on pathways always respond to an increase in light intensity with increased spiking. And the off pathway does the opposite. An increase in light intensity will cause a decrease in spiking. So now let's see exactly how this happens. By stepping through the on and off pathways, starting with the cones, which is a slightly simpler case. So that's why you need to pay special attention. So the photo receptors are shown up here. These are the bipolar cells and these are the ganglion cells. And we're going to imagine the response to an increase in light intensity. Um and this light of course is first absorbed by the cone. So imagine there's an increase in light intensity. We already know what's going to happen in the cones. Right. This is going to cause the cell to hyper polarise and release less glutamate. So that hyper polarisation is illustrated here. The key distinction between the on and off pathways, um, is in the cone system results on this first synapse between the photoreceptor and the bipolar cell. So on bipolar the cells expressing that postsynaptic membrane, a specific glutamate receptor called MK six, which is inhibitory. So this glutamate receptor acts on a cation conductance in the bipolar cell to inhibit it. This means that when there's a decrease in glutamate release, unless activation of mg R6, we're going to see a decrease in inhibition. It's a bit like taking your foot off the brake is decrease in inhibition results in activation and depolarisation of the bipolar cell. So you can see this is also known as a sign inverting synapse. Because we've converted a negative the decrease in glutamate release into a positive, which is the depolarisation of the bipolar cell and the depolarised bipolar cell. Then in a more usual way, we'll release neurotransmitter glutamate onto the downstream ganglion cell and cause it to spike more. So you can follow that pathway here. Open polarisation of the photoreceptor caused through the sign inverting synapse, the depolarisation of the bipolar cell, and that was relayed to the ganglion cell. And then because that's the spiking neurone, we see an increase in spike rate, okay. In the off pathway the off bipolar is expressed a different type of glutamate receptor, which looks a lot more like an Ampa receptor that you're probably familiar with. So as a result, a decrease in glutamate release from the receptor will simply cause less Ampa activation and therefore hyper polarisation of the off bipolar cell. It will release less neurotransmitter onto the ganglion cell. And so spiking rates decrease. That's illustrated here. So hopefully this is clear. The key distinction in these on and off pathways right comes down to this synapse at least in the cone system which is sign inverting in this case where we have this inhibitory L6 glutamate receptor but its sign conserving in lost most cases where we have a straightforward appetite receptor. Some people are nodding. Yeah. You want me to go for it again? Or one person at least. Okay. So all so the difference between these two pathways remembers starting at the bipolar cells, the cones always being the same thing. So they're going to respond to an increase in light intensity by hyper polarising and releasing less glutamate. So that's that's the same across both pathways. However the on bipolar cell responds to that decrease in neurotransmitter like depolarising because it has this inhibitory and glucose six, which is now less activated by because glutamate concentration is decreased. And so the cell depolarise activate this ganglion cells, we see more spiking. Whereas on the off side that decrease in neurotransmitter simply results in less activation of an AMP receptor, therefore less inward current into the polar it will hyper polarise. Or at least less. Neurotransmitter. And so the ganglion cell will spike glass. Okay, so. That's the cone on and off pathways. What about the rod system. So as you will remember there's only one type of rod bipolar cell. And it looks like a cone on bipolar. So it expresses m l6. And in response to an increase in light intensity and less glutamate release, the rod bipolar will depolarise. Now the interesting thing is that rod bipolar cells don't make direct connections onto ganglion cells. Instead they're actually going to hijack the cone pathway. And that the way they're going to do this is um, by means of our well, it's our first example of lateral information flow in the retina. They hijack the cone pathway, um, by way of an interneuron called an omicron's cell, which extends lots of processes in the in the inner place the form layer. Here's the console. Here's some pipe mounted rest and they're showing what they look like. And what this alma principal does is it gets contacted by the rod bipolar and it's in turn makes contacts with the presynaptic terminals of cone bipolar cells. And it can do this in two ways. So in the on pathway the console makes electrical synapses onto the presynaptic terminals of the cone on bipolar. So this means that when the rod bipolar depolarise that depolarisation is propagated to this presynaptic terminal resulting in more neurotransmitter release and therefore excitation of the ganglion cell. However, in the off pathway, the American cell is going to make a sign inverting glycine inhibitory synapse onto the presynaptic terminal of off cone bipolar, which will hyper polarise this terminal, resulting in less neurotransmitter release and as a result, a decrease in firing rate of the ganglion cell. So there's sort of two modes that the cell can connect the rod bipolar to the cone system. One is for an electrical synapse which propagates the excitation right through. So we get an increase in spiking. And the other is by sign inverting it through another inhibitory synapse which suppresses spiking. Um, the very cool thing about this system is that it's actually the same American cell that does both tasks. The very famous A2 American cell. And, um, you can read more about the American cell and some of the selected reading. But the way you can do this is because it has different layers of neuritis, some of which are connect onto the on system to make electrical synapses, and a different kind of layer of neuritis and the inner form layer, which I do this glycine allergic transmission. So it's a very hard working neurone. Um, I think you can hopefully appreciate it a little bit like the coin system. The secret to these on and off responses comes down to either sign conserving or sign inverting synapses. But in the case of the rod system, the critical sign ups is this one between ATM and cell and the cone by polars. Whereas in the cone system the critical sign ups was the first one. It was from the cone itself onto the bipolar cell. So this might all be quite a lot to um, uh, remember, um, this table sort of summarises the difference between the on and off pathways for both the cones and the rods. The key thing to remember is that the difference between on and off responses starts at the level of the bipolar cells. It depends on the presence of a sign inverting or sign conserving synapse in the cone system. As we mentioned, it's the first sign ups from the cone to the bipolar cell. One is expressed as R6, the other is an emperor sceptre. But in the rod system, the rod bipolar doesn't make direct connections onto ganglion cells. It uses this omicron's cell, and the omicron's cell makes either a sign conserving or sign inverting synapse. Okay, um. Let's summarise. So what you should have learned this morning is about some basic features of the visual field and stereo and eye movements. We spent some time looking at the process of transduction. You do need to know the details of the approach, understand selection pathway and understand it completely. Um, particularly knowing that in response to an increase in light intensity, the photoreceptors hyper polarised. Um, we've talked a little bit about the, the mainly the differences between rod and cone pathways and the different visual functions, those two types of photoreceptor play. And then we've started to examine the roles of the retina in neural computation, starting with these on and off pathways. If you're wondering what the point of the on and off pathways is, then, um, your, uh, questions hopefully will be answered this afternoon when we see how they used to build the interesting response properties of retinal ganglion cells and what that in turn, is useful for. So, um, that's all the, um, on the last slide, I don't know if these slides are these slides on Moodle. Yes. Okay. So on the last slide you can um see some some recommended readings, especially this one if you want to do some uh, book chapter, uh, reading alongside the lecture slides. Um, I will see you again, uh, for I think here. And we will start with, um, a Moodle quiz to see if you, um, learned anything about the on and off pathways, then see what they are used for and then have another Moodle quiz. Okay.