DEFINATIONS
DEFINATIONS
various media and sources.[1] It excludes specialized learning that can only be
obtained with extensive training and information confined to a single medium.
General knowledge is an essential component of crystallized intelligence. It is
strongly associated with general intelligence and with openness to experience.[2]
Studies have found that people who are highly knowledgeable in a particular domain
tend to be knowledgeable in many.[3][4] General knowledge is thought to be
supported by long-term semantic memory ability.[5] General knowledge also supports
schemata for textual understanding.[6]
Individual differences
Intelligence
High scorers on tests of general knowledge tend to also score highly on
intelligence tests. IQ has been found to robustly predict general knowledge scores
even after accounting for differences in age, and five-factor model personality
traits.[7][8][9] However, many general knowledge tests are designed to create a
normal distribution of answers, creating a bell-shaped curve.[10]
General knowledge is also moderately associated with verbal ability, though only
weakly or not at all with numerical and spatial ability.[3] As with crystallized
intelligence, general knowledge has been found to increase with age.[11]
Personality
People high in general knowledge tend to be highly open to new experiences[7][8][9]
[11] and in typical intellectual engagement.[8][9] The relationship between
openness to experience and general knowledge remains robust even when IQ is taken
into account.[7][9] People high in openness may be more motivated to engage in
intellectual pursuits that increase their knowledge.[9] Relationships between
general knowledge and other five factor model traits tend to be weak and
inconsistent. Though one study found that extraversion and neuroticism were
negatively correlated with general knowledge,[8] others found that they were
unrelated.[7][11] Inconsistent results have also been found for conscientiousness.
[note 1]
Predictor of achievement
This section needs additional citations for verification. Please help improve this
article by adding citations to reliable sources in this section. Unsourced material
may be challenged and removed. (April 2021) (Learn how and when to remove this
message)
A number of studies have assessed whether performance on a general knowledge test
can predict achievement in particular areas, namely in academics,[13] proofreading,
[14] and creativity.[15]
Academic achievement
General knowledge helps to perform well on government exams [13] The study examined
cognitive ability and personality predictors of exam performance and found that
general knowledge was positively correlated with GCSE English, mathematics,
Grammar, History, Science and overall exam results. General knowledge test scores
predicted exam results, even after controlling for IQ, five-factor model
personality traits, and learning styles.
Proofreading
General knowledge has been found to robustly predict proofreading skills in
university students.[14] A study found that proofreading had a larger correlation
with general knowledge than with general intelligence, verbal reasoning, or
openness to experience. In a multiple regression analysis using general knowledge,
general intelligence, verbal reasoning, five factor personality traits, and
learning styles as predictors, only general knowledge was a significant predictor.
Creativity
General knowledge has been found to have weak associations with measures of
creativity.[15] In a study examining contributions of personality and intelligence
to creativity, general knowledge was positively correlated with divergent thinking
tests, but was unrelated to a biographical measure of creative achievement, self-
rated creativity, or a composite measure of creativity. The relationship between
general knowledge and divergent thinking became non-significant when controlling
for fluid intelligence.
[1][16]
This section does not cite any sources. Please help improve this section by adding
citations to reliable sources. Unsourced material may be challenged and removed.
(April 2021) (Learn how and when to remove this message)
Many game shows use general knowledge questions for entertainment purposes. Game
shows such as Who Wants to Be a Millionaire? and Fifteen to One centre their
questions on general knowledge, while other shows focus questions more on specific
subjects. Some shows ask questions both on specific subjects and on general
knowledge, including Eggheads and Mastermind. In Mastermind, contestants choose
their own "specialist subject" before answering general knowledge questions,
whereas in Eggheads the subjects are chosen at random. The game show Jeopardy!
tests contestants' knowledge.
Questions drawn from the game Trivial Pursuit have been used in a number of
psychological experiments concerning general knowledge.[17][18]
See also
IQ
Intelligence
High culture
Cattell–Horn–Carroll theory
Cultural literacy
Core Knowledge Foundation
Upper ontology – Ontology applicable across domains of knowledge
Notes
Furnham et al.[7] found positive correlations with general knowledge in his first
and third studies, but no significant relationship in his second. Studies by others
have found no significant relationship.[8][11]
References
"GENERAL KNOWLEDGE". Cambridge English Dictionary. Retrieved 2019-08-24.
T. C. Bates and A. Shieles. (2003). Crystallized Intelligence is a product of
Speed and Drive for Experience: The Relationship of Inspection Time and Openness to
g and Gc. Intelligence, 31, 275-287
Rolfhus, Eric L.; Ackerman, Phillip L. (1999). "Assessing individual differences
in knowledge: Knowledge, intelligence, and related traits" (PDF). Journal of
Educational Psychology. 91 (3): 511–526. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.91.3.511. Archived
from the original (PDF) on 2012-04-15. Retrieved 2019-12-25.
Ackerman, Phillip L.; Bowen, Kristy R.; Beier, Margaret E.; Kanfer, Ruth (2001).
"Determinants of individual differences and gender differences in knowledge".
Journal of Educational Psychology. 93 (4): 797–825. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.93.4.797.
Archived from the original on 2016-04-05. Retrieved 2011-12-04.
Smith, Amy M.; Hughes, Gregory I.; Davis, F. Caroline; Thomas, Ayanna K. (March
2019). "Acute stress enhances general-knowledge semantic memory". Hormones and
Behavior. 109: 38–43. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2019.02.003. PMID 30742829. S2CID
72334918.
An, Shuying (2013-01-01). "Schema Theory in Reading". Theory and Practice in
Language Studies. 3 (1). doi:10.4304/tpls.3.1.130-134. ISSN 1799-2591. S2CID
26167875.
Furnham, Adrian; Chamorro-Premuzic, Tomas (2006). "Personality, intelligence, and
general knowledge". Learning and Individual Differences. 16: 79–90.
doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2005.07.002.
Chamorro-Premuzic, Tomas; Furnham, Adrian; Ackerman, Phillip L. (2006). "Ability
and personality correlates of general knowledge" (PDF). Personality and Individual
Differences. 41 (3): 419–429. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2005.11.036. Archived from the
original (PDF) on 2012-04-26.
Furnham, Adrian; Swami, Viren; Arteche, Adriane; Chamorro-Premuzic, Tomas (2008).
"Cognitive ability, learning approaches and personality correlates of general
knowledge". Educational Psychology. 28 (4): 427–437. doi:10.1080/01443410701727376.
S2CID 144132702.
Stuart-Hamilton, Ian (6 April 2014). "What Constitutes General Knowledge?".
Psychology Today. Retrieved 2019-08-24.
Furnham, Adrian; Christopher, Andrew N.; Garwood, Jeanette; Martin, G. Neil
(2007). "Approaches to learning and the acquisition of general knowledge".
Personality and Individual Differences. 43 (6): 1563–1571.
doi:10.1016/j.paid.2007.04.013.
Glisky, Elizabeth L. (2007), Riddle, David R. (ed.), "Changes in Cognitive
Function in Human Aging", Brain Aging: Models, Methods, and Mechanisms, Frontiers
in Neuroscience, CRC Press/Taylor & Francis, ISBN 9780849338182, PMID 21204355,
retrieved 2019-08-24
Furnham, Adrian; Monsen, J. & Ahmetoglu (2009). "Typical intellectual engagement,
Big Five personality traits, approaches to learning and cognitive ability
predictors of academic performance". British Journal of Educational Psychology. 79
(4): 769–782. doi:10.1348/978185409X412147. PMID 19245744.
Furnham, Adrian (2010). "Proofreading as an index of crystallised intelligence".
Educational Psychology. 30 (6): 735–754. doi:10.1080/01443410.2010.506005. S2CID
145775446.
Batey, Mark; Furnham, Adrian; Safiullina, Xeniya (2010). "Intelligence, general
knowledge and personality as predictors of creativity". Learning and Individual
Differences. 20 (5): 532–535. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2010.04.008.
Achary, K ram (3 April 2024). "30+ GK Questions and Answers in English | General
Awareness Questions". Just Know. Retrieved 10 April 2024.
Dijksterhuis, A.; van Knippenberg, A. (1998). "The relation between perception and
behavior, or how to win a game of trivial pursuit" (PDF). Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology. 74 (4): 865–77. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.74.4.865. PMID
9569649. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2010-06-14.
Dijksterhuis, A. (October 1998). "Seeing one thing and doing another: Contrast
effects in automatic behavior". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 75
(4): 862–871. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.75.4.862.