Piagets Theory
Piagets Theory
Cognitive Development
simplypsychology.org/piaget.html
Key Features
Stages of Development
Jean Piaget’s theory of cognitive development suggests that children move through
four different stages of intellectual development which reflect the increasing
sophistication of children’s thought.
Each child goes through the stages in the same order (but not all at the same rate), and child
development is determined by biological maturation and interaction with the environment.
1/43
At each stage of development, the child’s thinking is qualitatively different from the other
stages, that is, each stage involves a different type of intelligence.
Although no stage can be missed out, there are individual differences in the rate at which
children progress through stages, and some individuals may never attain the later stages.
Piaget did not claim that a particular stage was reached at a certain age – although
descriptions of the stages often include an indication of the age at which the average child
would reach each stage.
During the sensorimotor stage (birth to age 2) infants develop basic motor skills and learn to
perceive and interact with their environment through physical sensations and body
coordination.
2/43
The infant learns about the world through their senses and through their actions
(moving around and exploring their environment).
During the sensorimotor stage, a range of cognitive abilities develop. These include:
object permanence; self-recognition (the child realizes that other people are separate
from them); deferred imitation; and representational play.
Cognitive abilities relate to the emergence of the general symbolic function, which is
the capacity to represent the world mentally.
At about 8 months, the infant will understand the permanence of objects and that they
will still exist even if they can’t see them, and the infant will search for them when they
disappear.
At the beginning of this stage, the infant lives in the present. It does not yet have a mental
picture of the world stored in its memory, so it does not have a sense of object permanence.
If the child cannot see something, then it does not exist. This is why you can hide a toy from
an infant, while it watches, but it will not search for the object once it has gone out of sight.
The main achievement during this stage is object permanence – knowing that an object still
exists, even if it is hidden. It requires the ability to form a mental representation (i.e., a
schema) of the object.
Towards the end of this stage the general symbolic function begins to appear where
children show in their play that they can use one object to stand for another.
Language starts to appear because they realise that words can be used to represent
objects and feelings.
The child begins to be able to store information about the world, recall it, and label it.
Individual Differences
Cultural Practices: In some cultures, babies are carried on their mothers’ backs
throughout the day. This constant physical contact and varied stimuli can influence
how a child perceives their environment and their sense of object permanence.
Gender Norms: Toys assigned to babies can differ based on gender expectations. A
boy might be given more cars or action figures, while a girl might receive dolls or
kitchen sets. This can influence early interactions and sensory explorations.
3/43
The Preoperational Stage
Ages: 2 – 7 Years
Piaget’s second stage of intellectual development is the preoperational stage, which occurs
between 2 and 7 years. At the beginning of this stage, the child does not use operations (a
set of logical rules), so thinking is influenced by how things look or appear to them rather
than logical reasoning.
For example, a child might think a tall, thin glass contains more liquid than a short, wide
glass, even if both hold the same amount, because the child focuses on the height rather
than considering both dimensions.
Furthermore, the child is egocentric; he assumes that other people see the world as he
does, as shown in the Three Mountains study.
As the preoperational stage develops, egocentrism declines, and children begin to enjoy the
participation of another child in their games, and let’s pretend play becomes more
important.
Toddlers often pretend to be people they are not (e.g. superheroes, policemen), and may
play these roles with props that symbolize real-life objects. Children may also invent an
imaginary playmate.
4/43
Major Characteristics and Developmental Changes:
Toddlers and young children acquire the ability to internally represent the world
through language and mental imagery.
During this stage, young children can think about things symbolically. This is the ability
to make one thing, such as a word or an object, stand for something other than itself.
A child’s thinking is dominated by how the world looks, not how the world is. It is not
yet capable of logical (problem-solving) type of thought.
Moreover, the child has difficulties with class inclusion; he can classify objects but
cannot include objects in sub-sets, which involves classifying objects as belonging to
two or more categories simultaneously.
Infants at this stage also demonstrate animism. This is the tendency for the child to
think that non-living objects (such as toys) have life and feelings like a person’s.
By 2 years, children have made some progress toward detaching their thoughts from the
physical world. However, have not yet developed logical (or “operational”) thought
characteristics of later stages.
Thinking is still intuitive (based on subjective judgments about situations) and egocentric
(centered on the child’s own view of the world).
Individual Differences
Cultural Storytelling: Different cultures have unique stories, myths, and folklore.
Children from diverse backgrounds might understand and interpret symbolic elements
differently based on their cultural narratives.
Race & Representation: A child’s racial identity can influence how they engage in
pretend play. For instance, a lack of diverse representation in media and toys might
lead children of color to recreate scenarios that don’t reflect their experiences or
background.
Ages: 7 – 11 Years
5/43
By the beginning of the concrete operational stage, the child can use operations (a set of
logical rules) so they can conserve quantities, realize that people see the world in a different
way (decentring), and demonstrate improvement in inclusion tasks.
During this stage, children begin to think logically about concrete events.
Children begin to understand the concept of conservation; understanding that,
although things may change in appearance, certain properties remain the same.
During this stage, children can mentally reverse things (e.g., picture a ball of plasticine
returning to its original shape).
During this stage, children also become less egocentric and begin to think about how
other people might think and feel.
The stage is called concrete because children can think logically much more successfully if
they can manipulate real (concrete) materials or pictures of them.
Piaget considered the concrete stage a major turning point in the child’s cognitive
development because it marks the beginning of logical or operational thought. This means
the child can work things out internally in their head (rather than physically try things out in
the real world).
6/43
Children can conserve number (age 6), mass (age 7), and weight (age 9). Conservation is
the understanding that something stays the same in quantity even though its appearance
changes.
But operational thought is only effective here if the child is asked to reason about materials
that are physically present. Children at this stage will tend to make mistakes or be
overwhelmed when asked to reason about abstract or hypothetical problems.
Individual Differences
The formal operational period begins at about age 11. As adolescents enter this stage, they
gain the ability to think abstractly, the ability to combine and classify items in a more
sophisticated way, and the capacity for higher-order reasoning.
7/43
Adolescents can think systematically and reason about what might be as well as what is
(not everyone achieves this stage). This allows them to understand politics, ethics, and
science fiction, as well as to engage in scientific reasoning.
Adolescents can deal with abstract ideas; for example, they can understand division and
fractions without having to actually divide things up, and solve hypothetical (imaginary)
problems.
Concrete operations are carried out on physical objects, whereas formal operations
are carried out on ideas. Formal operational thought is entirely freed from physical
and perceptual constraints.
During this stage, adolescents can deal with abstract ideas (e.g., they no longer need
to think about slicing up cakes or sharing sweets to understand division and fractions).
They can follow the form of an argument without having to think in terms of specific
examples.
Adolescents can deal with hypothetical problems with many possible solutions. For
example, if asked, ‘What would happen if money were abolished in one hour?’ they
could speculate about many possible consequences.
Piaget described reflective abstraction as the process by which individuals become
aware of and reflect upon their own cognitive actions or operations (metacognition).
8/43
From about 12 years, children can follow the form of a logical argument without reference to
its content. During this time, people develop the ability to think about abstract concepts, and
logically test hypotheses.
This stage sees the emergence of scientific thinking, formulating abstract theories and
hypotheses when faced with a problem.
Individual Differences
Piaget’s Theory
Piaget’s theory places a strong emphasis on the active role that children play in their
own cognitive development.
According to Piaget, children are not passive recipients of information; instead, they
actively explore and interact with their surroundings.
This active engagement with the environment is crucial because it allows them to
gradually build their understanding of the world.
Piaget was employed at the Binet Institute in the 1920s, where his job was to develop
French versions of questions on English intelligence tests. He became intrigued with the
reasons children gave for their wrong answers to the questions that required logical
thinking.
He believed that these incorrect answers revealed important differences between the
thinking of adults and children.
Piaget branched out on his own with a new set of assumptions about children’s
intelligence:
9/43
Children’s intelligence differs from an adult’s in quality rather than in quantity. This
means that children reason (think) differently from adults and see the world in different
ways.
Children actively build up their knowledge about the world. They are not passive
creatures waiting for someone to fill their heads with knowledge.
The best way to understand children’s reasoning is to see things from their point of
view.
Piaget did not want to measure how well children could count, spell, or solve problems as a
way of grading their I.Q.
He was more interested in how fundamental concepts emerged, such as the very ideas of
number, time, quantity, causality, and justice.
Piaget studied children from infancy to adolescence using naturalistic observation of his
own three babies and sometimes controlled observation too. From these, he wrote diary
descriptions charting their development.
He also used clinical interviews and observations of older children who were able to
understand questions and hold conversations.
It is a research approach for understanding children’s thinking that Piaget adapted from the
diagnostic clinical interview used in psychopathology.
Unlike standardized tests, the clinical method uses flexible, open-ended questions to explore
the child’s thinking in depth.
The interviewer adapts their questions based on the child’s initial responses, prompting
further explanation and clarification.
10/43
One key aspect of this approach is the use of open-ended and non-judgmental
questions that allow the child to express their thoughts freely. The questions should be
based on the spontaneous questions asked by children of the same age or younger.
The interviewer must carefully observe the child’s responses and follow up with
additional questions to clarify their reasoning.
The interviewer must also be able to distinguish between truly spontaneous
responses and those that are influenced by suggestion or other external factors.
Piaget’s clinical method has been influential in the field of developmental psychology and
has helped to shape the way researchers and educators understand children’s thinking.
What is the “critical method” and how does it differ from the clinical method?
It’s important to note that the clinical method evolved throughout Piaget’s career.
While the clinical method primarily relies on verbal dialogue, the critical method involves
children actively manipulating objects, allowing researchers to observe their actions and
reasoning in relation to physical phenomena.
For example, a researcher might ask a child to predict whether a ball of clay will weigh more
or less after being rolled into a snake (conservation of mass).
By observing the child’s actions and explanations, researchers can gain insight into their
understanding of the concept.
Piaget believed that engaging children with a concrete challenge helped to put them at ease,
minimized the amount of verbal instruction required, and allowed the interviewer to use the
child’s language when discussing the phenomenon.
By incorporating physical objects into his research, he could observe how children’s actions
and manipulations influenced their thinking.
For example, to understand how children transition from animistic to mechanistic thinking,
Piaget explored children’s conceptions of material force through a series of experiments
involving physical demonstrations
11/43
However, the core principles of the clinical examination, such as open-ended inquiry, a focus
on underlying reasoning, and sensitive interviewing, remained essential elements of his
research, even as his methods became more complex
Piaget’s (1936, 1950) theory of cognitive development explains how a child constructs a
mental model of the world.
He disagreed with the idea that intelligence was a fixed trait, and regarded cognitive
development as a process that occurs due to biological maturation and interaction with the
environment.
Children’s ability to understand, think about, and solve problems in the world develops in a
stop-start, discontinuous manner (rather than gradual changes over time).
Piaget’s theory:
The goal of the theory is to explain the mechanisms and processes by which the infant, and
then the child, develops into an individual who can reason and think using hypotheses.
4. Schemas
12/43
A schema is a mental framework or concept that helps us organize and interpret
information. It’s like a mental file folder where we store knowledge about a particular object,
event, or concept.
Wadsworth (2004) suggests that schemata (the plural of schema) be thought of as “index
cards” filed in the brain, each one telling an individual how to react to incoming stimuli or
information.
According to Piaget, we are born with a few primitive schemas, such as sucking, which give
us the means to interact with the world. These initial schemas are physical, but as the child
develops, they become mental schemas.
Babies have a sucking reflex, triggered by something touching their lips. This
corresponds to a “sucking schema.”
The grasping reflex, elicited when something touches the palm of a baby’s hand,
represents another innate schema.
The rooting reflex, where a baby turns its head towards something that touches its
cheek, is also considered an innate schema.
Operations are more sophisticated mental structures that allow us to combine schemas in
a logical (reasonable) way. For example, picking up a rattle would combine three schemas,
gazing, reaching and grasping.
13/43
As children grow, they can carry out more complex operations and begin to imagine
hypothetical (imaginary) situations.
Operations are learned through interaction with other people and the environment, and they
represent a key advancement in cognitive development beyond simple schemas.
As children grow and interact with their environment, these basic schemas become more
complex and numerous, and new schemas are developed through the processes of
assimilation and accommodation.
Assimilation is fitting new information into existing schemas without changing one’s
understanding. For example, a child who has only seen small dogs might call a cat a
“dog” due to similar features like fur, four legs, and a tail.
14/43
Accommodation occurs when existing schemas must be revised to incorporate new
information. For instance, a child who believes all animals have four legs would need
to accommodate their schema upon seeing a snake.
A baby tries to use the same grasping schema to pick up a very small object. It
doesn’t work. The baby then changes (accommodates) the schema using the
forefinger and thumb to pick up the object.
When a child’s existing schemas are capable of explaining what it can perceive
around it, it is said to be in a state of equilibrium, i.e., a state of cognitive (i.e.,
mental) balance.
Disequilibrium occurs when new information conflicts with existing schemas, creating
cognitive discomfort. This cognitive conflict drives cognitive development and learning.
These processes are continuous and interactive, allowing schemas to evolve and become more
sophisticated.
Example
When encountering new information, a child first attempts to assimilate it into existing
schemas.
15/43
If assimilation fails, disequilibrium occurs, prompting the need for accommodation.
Importance of Equilibration:
16/43
Constant accommodation without assimilation would make every experience seem
novel, leading to cognitive exhaustion.
Applications to Education
Think of old black-and-white films you’ve seen where children sat in rows at desks with
inkwells. They learned by rote, all chanting in unison in response to questions set by an
authoritarian figure like Miss Trunchbull in Matilda.
Children who were unable to keep up were seen as slacking and would be punished by
variations on the theme of corporal punishment. Yes, it really did happen and in some parts
of the world still does today.
Piaget is partly responsible for the change that occurred during the 1960s to early children’s
education.
17/43
“Children should be able to do their own experimenting and their own research.
Teachers, of course, can guide them by providing appropriate materials, but the
essential thing is that in order for a child to understand something, he must construct
it himself, he must re-invent it. Every time we teach a child something, we keep him
from inventing it himself. On the other hand that which we allow him to discover by
himself will remain with him visibly”.
Plowden Report
This was notable because Piaget’s (1952) work was not originally designed for educational
applications.
The Plowden Report emphasized several recurring themes aligned with Piaget’s
work:
Individual learning
Curriculum flexibility
The central role of play in children’s learning
18/43
Utilizing the environment for learning
Discovery-based learning
Assessment of children’s progress
Importantly, the report cautioned that teachers should “not assume that only what is
measurable is valuable,” encouraging a holistic approach to assessing children’s
development.
Teachers should encourage students to take an active role in discovering and constructing
knowledge. The teacher’s role is to facilitate learning rather than direct tuition.
Because Piaget’s theory is based upon biological maturation and stages, the notion of
“readiness” is important. Readiness concerns when certain information or concepts should
be taught.
According to Piaget’s theory, children should not be taught certain concepts until they have
reached the appropriate stage of cognitive development.
Assimilation and accommodation require an active learner, not a passive one, because
problem-solving skills cannot be taught, they must be discovered (Piaget, 1958).
19/43
3. Provide challenges that promote growth without causing frustration: Devising
situations that present useful problems and create disequilibrium in the child.
4. Focus on the process of learning rather than the end product: Instead of
checking if children have the right answer, the teacher should focus on the students’
understanding and the processes they used to arrive at the answer.
5. Encourage active learning: Learning must be active (discovery learning). Children
should be encouraged to discover for themselves and to interact with the material
instead of being given ready-made knowledge. Using active methods that require
rediscovering or reconstructing “truths.”
6. Foster social interaction: Using collaborative, as well as individual activities (so
children can learn from each other). Implement cooperative learning activities, such as
group problem-solving tasks or role-playing scenarios.
7. Differentiated teaching: Adapt lessons to suit the needs of the individual child. For
example, observe a child’s ability to classify objects by color, shape, and size. If they
can easily sort by one attribute but struggle with multiple attributes, tailor future
activities to gradually increase complexity, such as sorting buttons first by color, then
by color and size together.
8. Providing support for the “spontaneous research” of the child: Provide
opportunities and resources for children to explore topics of their own interest,
encouraging their natural curiosity and self-directed learning. Create a “Wonder Wall”
in the classroom where children can post questions about topics that interest them.
Classroom Activities
Although most kids in this age range are not in a traditional classroom setting, they
can still benefit from games that stimulate their senses and motor skills.
20/43
2. Preoperational Stage (2-7 years):
Role Playing: Set up pretend play areas where children can act out different
scenarios, such as a kitchen, hospital, or market.
Use of Symbols: Encourage drawing, building, and using props to represent
other things.
Hands-on Activities: Children should interact physically with their environment,
so provide plenty of opportunities for hands-on learning.
Egocentrism Activities: Use exercises that highlight different perspectives. For
instance, having two children sit across from each other with an object in
between and asking them what the other sees.
21/43
4. Formal Operational Stage (11 years and older):
While Piaget’s stages offer a foundational framework, they are not universally experienced
in the same way by all children.
Social identities play a critical role in shaping cognitive development, necessitating a more
nuanced and culturally responsive approach to understanding child development.
Piaget’s stages may manifest differently based on social identities like race, gender,
and culture:
Race & Teacher Interactions: A child’s race can influence teacher expectations and
interactions. For example, racial biases can lead to children of color being perceived
as less capable or more disruptive, influencing their cognitive challenges and support.
Racial and Cultural Stereotypes: These can affect a child’s self-perception and self-
efficacy. For instance, stereotypes about which racial or cultural groups are “better” at
certain subjects can influence a child’s self-confidence and, subsequently, their
engagement in that subject.
Gender & Peer Interactions: Children learn gender roles from their peers. Boys
might be mocked for playing “girl games,” and girls might be excluded from certain
activities, influencing their cognitive engagements.
22/43
Language: Multilingual children might navigate the stages differently, especially if
their home language differs from their school language. The way concepts are framed
in different languages can influence cognitive processing. Cultural idioms and
metaphors can shape a child’s understanding of concepts and their ability to use
symbolic representation, especially in the pre-operational stage.
Purposeful Play: Ensuring that play is not just free time but a structured learning
experience requires careful planning. Educators must identify clear learning objectives
and create play environments that facilitate these goals.
Alignment with Standards: Striking a balance between child-initiated play and
curriculum expectations can be challenging. Educators need to find ways to integrate
play-based learning with broader educational goals and standards.
Pace of Learning: The curriculum’s focus on specific content by certain ages can
create pressure to accelerate student learning, potentially contradicting Piaget’s
notion of developmental stages. Teachers should regularly assess students’
understanding to identify areas where they need more support or challenge.
Assessment Focus: The emphasis on standardized testing can shift the focus from
process-oriented learning (as Piaget advocated) to outcome-based teaching.
Educators should use assessments that reflect real-world tasks and allow students to
demonstrate their understanding in multiple ways.
Parents
Other challenges
23/43
Individual Differences: Piaget emphasized individual differences in cognitive
development, but classrooms often have diverse learners. Meeting the needs of all
students while maintaining a play-based approach can be demanding.
Time Constraints: In some educational settings, there may be pressure to cover
specific content or prepare students for standardized tests. Prioritizing play-based
learning within these constraints can be difficult.
Cultural Sensitivity: Recognizing and respecting cultural differences is essential.
Piaget’s theory may need to be adapted to fit the specific cultural context of the
children being taught.
Yes, Piaget’s ideas can be adapted to support children with special educational needs and
disabilities (SEND), though with important considerations:
While Piaget’s theory offers valuable insights, it should be part of a broader, evidence-
based approach that recognizes the diverse factors influencing development in children with
SEND.
24/43
Jean Piaget could not have anticipated the expansive digital age we now live in.
Today, knowledge dissemination and creation are democratized by the Internet, with
platforms like blogs, wikis, social media, and generative AI allowing for vast collaboration
and shared knowledge. This development has prompted a reimagining of the future of
education.
Classrooms, traditionally seen as primary sites of learning, are being overshadowed by the
rise of mobile technologies and platforms like MOOCs (Passey, 2013).
The millennial generation, the first to grow up with cable TV, the internet, and cell phones,
relies heavily on technology.
They view it as an integral part of their identity, with most using it extensively in their daily
lives, from keeping in touch with loved ones to consuming news and entertainment
(Nielsen, 2014).
Social media platforms offer a dynamic environment conducive to Piaget’s principles. These
platforms allow interactions that nurture knowledge evolution through cognitive processes
like assimilation and accommodation.
They emphasize communal interaction and shared activity, fostering both cognitive and
socio-cultural constructivism. This shared activity promotes understanding and exploration
beyond individual perspectives, enhancing social-emotional learning (Gehlbach, 2010).
This inclusivity can equalize learning opportunities, potentially diminishing biases based on
factors like race or socio-economic status, resonating with Kegan’s (1982) concept of
“recruitability.”
However, there are challenges. While social media’s potential in learning is vast, its
practical application necessitates intention and guidance. Cuban, Kirkpatrick, and Peck
(2001) note that certain educators and students are hesitant about integrating social media
into educational contexts.
25/43
This hesitancy can stem from technological complexities or potential distractions. Yet, when
harnessed effectively, social media can provide a rich environment for collaborative learning
and interpersonal development, fostering a deeper understanding of content.
In essence, the rise of social media aligns seamlessly with constructivist philosophies.
Social media platforms act as tools for everyday cognition, merging daily social interactions
with the academic world, and providing avenues for diverse, interactive, and engaging
learning experiences.
Some critics argue that Piaget’s clinical method lacked standardization and objectivity,
making it difficult to replicate findings and generalize results.
The open-ended nature of questioning, while allowing for flexibility, also introduced potential
biases from the researcher’s interpretations.
Additionally, the focus on verbal explanations may have underestimated the cognitive
abilities of younger children or those with limited language skills.
1. Small sample size: Piaget often used small, non-representative samples, frequently
including only his own children or those from similar backgrounds (European children
from families of high socio-economic status). This limits the generalizability of his
findings (Lourenço & Machado, 1996).
2. Potential researcher bias: Piaget’s methods, including studying his own children and
conducting solo observations, risked subjective interpretation.
The lack of inter-rater reliability and potential issues with clinical interviews (e.g.,
children misunderstanding questions or trying to please the experimenter) may have
led to biased or inaccurate conclusions.
Using multiple researchers and more standardized methods could have improved
reliability (Donaldson, 1978).
26/43
3. Age-related issues: Some critics argue that Piaget underestimated the cognitive
abilities of younger children. This may be due to the complex language used in his
tasks, which could have masked children’s true understanding.
4. Cultural limitations: Piaget’s research was primarily conducted with Western,
educated children from relatively affluent backgrounds. This raises questions about the
universality of his developmental stages across different cultures (Rogoff, 2003).
5. Task design: Some of Piaget’s tasks may have been too abstract or removed from
children’s everyday experiences. This could have led to underestimating children’s
actual cognitive abilities in more familiar contexts.
As several studies have shown Piaget underestimated the abilities of children because
his tests were sometimes confusing or difficult to understand (e.g., Hughes, 1975).
Piaget’s concept of fixed developmental stages has been challenged by other theorists and
researchers.
Vygotsky and Bruner would rather not talk about stages at all, preferring to see development
as a continuous process.
Others have queried the age ranges of the stages. Some studies have shown that progress
to the formal operational stage is not guaranteed.
For example, Keating (1979) reported that 40-60% of college students fail at formal
operational tasks, and Dasen (1994) states that only one-third of adults ever reach the formal
operational stage.
27/43
There’s greater recognition of the brain’s plasticity and the potential for cognitive growth
throughout the lifespan.
This challenges the idea of fixed developmental endpoints proposed in stage theories.
The fact that the formal operational stage is not reached in all cultures and not all individuals
within cultures suggests that it might not be biologically based.
Cross-cultural studies show that the stages of development (except the formal operational
stage) occur in the same order in all cultures suggesting that cognitive development is a
product of a biological maturation process.
However, the age at which the stages are reached varies between cultures and individuals
which suggests that social and cultural factors and individual differences influence cognitive
development.
Dasen (1994) cites studies he conducted in remote parts of the central Australian desert with
8—to 14-year-old Indigenous Australians.
He gave them conservation of liquid tasks and spatial awareness tasks. He found that the
ability to conserve came later in the Aboriginal children, between the ages of 10 and 13 (as
opposed to between 5 and 7, with Piaget’s Swiss sample).
However, he found that spatial awareness abilities developed earlier among Aboriginal
children than among Swiss children.
28/43
Such a study demonstrates that cognitive development is not purely dependent on
maturation but on cultural factors as well—spatial awareness is crucial for nomadic groups of
people.
Lev Vygotsky, a contemporary of Piaget, emphasized the social nature of learning in his
sociocultural theory.
Vygotsky argued that cognitive development occurs through social interactions, particularly
with more knowledgeable others (MKOs) such as parents, teachers, or skilled peers.
He introduced the concept of the Zone of Proximal Development (ZPD), which represents
the gap between what a child can do independently and what they can achieve with
guidance.
Emotional factors, including motivation, self-esteem, and relationships, also play significant
roles in learning and development – aspects not thoroughly addressed in Piaget’s cognitive-
focused theory.
Critics argue that Piaget may have underestimated children’s cognitive abilities due to
methodological issues.
Piaget failed to distinguish between competence (what a child can do) and performance
(what a child can show when given a particular task).
When tasks were altered, performance (and therefore competence) was affected. Therefore,
Piaget might have underestimated children’s cognitive abilities.
29/43
For example, a child might have object permanence (competence) but still be unable to
search for objects (performance). When Piaget hid objects from babies, he found that it
wasn’t until after nine months that they looked for them.
However, Piaget relied on manual search methods – whether the child was looking for the
object or not.
Later, researchers such as Baillargeon and Devos (1991) reported that infants as young as
four months looked longer at a moving carrot that didn’t do what it expected, suggesting they
had some sense of permanence, otherwise they wouldn’t have had any expectation of what
it should or shouldn’t do.
He inspired many who followed and took up his ideas. Piaget’s theories inspired extensive
research in the field of cognitive development.
Theoretical Contributions
Seminal Theory: Piaget (1936) was one of the first psychologists to study cognitive
development systematically. His contributions include a stage theory of child cognitive
development, detailed observational studies of cognition in children, and a series of
simple but ingenious tests to reveal different cognitive abilities.
Neo-Piagetian theories: Researchers have built upon Piaget’s stage theory of
cognitive development, incorporating information processing and brain development to
explain cognitive growth, emphasizing individual differences and more gradual
developmental progressions (Case, 1985; Fischer, 1980; Pascual-Leone, 1970).
30/43
Clinical Methods: Piaget’s clinical and critical methods, revolutionized the study of
child psychology. They shifted the focus from simply measuring children’s performance
to understanding the underlying processes of their thinking.
The National Association for the Education of Young Children (NAEYC) has
incorporated Piagetian principles into its DAP framework, influencing early childhood
education policies worldwide.
Parenting Practices
Parents can use Piaget’s stages to have realistic developmental expectations of their
children’s behavior and cognitive capabilities.
31/43
For instance, understanding that a toddler is in the pre-operational stage can help parents be
patient when the child is egocentric.
Play Activities
Recognizing the importance of play in cognitive development, many parents provide toys and
games suited for their child’s developmental stage.
Parents can offer activities that are slightly beyond their child’s current abilities, leveraging
Vygotsky’s concept of the “Zone of Proximal Development,” which complements Piaget’s
ideas.
Memory Games: Using cards with pictures, place them face down, and ask
students to find matching pairs.
Role Playing and Pretend Play: Let children act out roles or stories that
enhance symbolic thinking. Encourage symbolic play with dress-up clothes,
playsets, or toy cash registers. Provide prompts or scenarios to extend their
imagination.
Story Sequencing: Give children cards with parts of a story and have them
arranged in the correct order.
32/43
3. Concrete Operational Stage (7-11 years):
Number Line Jumps: Create a number line on the floor with tape. Ask students
to jump to the correct answer for math problems.
Classification Games: Provide a mix of objects and ask students to classify
them based on different criteria (e.g., color, size, shape).
Logical Puzzle Games: Games that involve problem-solving using logic, such as
simple Sudoku puzzles or logic grid puzzles.
Debate and Discussion: Provide a topic and let students debate the pros and
cons. This promotes abstract thinking and logical reasoning.
Hypothesis Testing Games: Present a scenario and have students come up
with hypotheses and ways to test them.
Strategy Board Games: Games like chess, checkers, or Settlers of Catan can
help in developing strategic and forward-thinking skills.
This allows for creating holistic learning experiences that support cognitive, social, and
emotional growth.
By recognizing various developmental factors, professionals can tailor their practices to each
child’s unique needs and background.
Differences:
33/43
Individual vs. Social Emphasis: Piaget maintains that cognitive development stems
largely from independent explorations in which children construct knowledge of their
own.
Vygotsky argues that children learn through social interactions, building knowledge by
learning from more knowledgeable others, such as peers and adults.
In other words, Vygotsky believed that culture affects cognitive development.
Discovery Learning vs. Guided Scaffolding: Piaget argued that the teacher should
provide opportunities that challenge the children’s existing schemas and encourage
them to discover for themselves.
Alternatively, Vygotsky would recommend that teachers assist the child to progress
through the zone of proximal development by using scaffolding.
Similarities:
1. Both theories view children as actively constructing their own knowledge of the world;
they are not seen as just passively absorbing knowledge.
2. They also agree that cognitive development involves qualitative changes in thinking,
not only a matter of learning more things.
Piaget Vygotsky
34/43
Piaget Vygotsky
Role of the Provide opportunities for children to Assist the child to progress
Teacher learn about the world for themselves through the ZPD by using
(discovery learning) scaffolding
At each stage, individuals face a conflict between two opposing states that shapes
personality. Successfully resolving conflicts leads to virtues like hope, will, purpose, and
integrity. Failure leads to outcomes like mistrust, guilt, role confusion, and despair.
Differences:
35/43
Learning Process vs. Identity Formation: Piaget emphasizes how children learn and
understand the world. Erikson focuses on how individuals develop their sense of self
and place in society through resolving psychosocial conflicts.
Similarities:
Differences:
36/43
Cognitive Structures vs. Proximal Processes: Piaget focused on the development of
cognitive structures (schemas). Bronfenbrenner emphasized proximal processes –
regular, enduring interactions between the individual and their immediate environment
– as key drivers of development.
Discovery Learning vs. Contextual Learning: Piaget advocated for discovery
learning to challenge existing schemas. Bronfenbrenner would emphasize the
importance of understanding and leveraging the various ecological contexts in which
learning occurs, from family to cultural systems.
Similarities:
FAQs
This includes the growth and maturation of the brain, as well as the acquisition and
refinement of various mental skills and abilities.
Cognitive development is a major aspect of human development, and both genetic and
environmental factors heavily influence it. Key domains of cognitive development include
attention, memory, language skills, logical reasoning, and problem-solving.
Various theories, such as those proposed by Jean Piaget and Lev Vygotsky, provide different
perspectives on how this complex process unfolds from infancy through adulthood.
Piaget divided children’s cognitive development into four stages; each of the stages
represents a new way of thinking and understanding the world.
He called them (1) sensorimotor intelligence, (2) preoperational thinking, (3) concrete
operational thinking, and (4) formal operational thinking. Each stage is correlated with an age
period of childhood, but only approximately.
37/43
According to Piaget, intellectual development takes place through stages that occur in a fixed
order and which are universal (all children pass through these stages regardless of social or
cultural background).
Development can only occur when the brain has matured to a point of “readiness”.
Cross-cultural studies show that the stages of development (except the formal operational
stage) occur in the same order in all cultures suggesting that cognitive development is a
product of a biological maturation process.
However, the age at which the stages are reached varies between cultures and individuals,
suggesting that social and cultural factors and individual differences influence cognitive
development.
Schemas are mental structures that contain all of the information relating to one aspect of
the world around us.
According to Piaget, we are born with a few primitive schemas, such as sucking, which give
us the means to interact with the world.
These are physical, but as the child develops, they become mental schemas. These
schemas become more complex with experience.
According to Piaget, how does a child’s verbal thought relate to their active
and concrete thought?
Piaget acknowledged the complex relationship between a child’s verbal expressions and
their active engagement with the concrete world.
He recognized that children, like adults, possess a layer of “purely verbal thought” that can
be superimposed over their “active and concrete thought”.
Inventing Stories: Piaget observed that children frequently invent stories, both during
questioning and in their everyday lives.
38/43
He argued that these stories offer insights into the child’s thought processes, as they often
reflect the child’s understanding of causality, relationships, and the workings of the world
He recognized that these verbal scenarios, while not directly mirroring the child’s lived
experience, could still elicit valuable insights into their moral reasoning processes.
However, Piaget also expressed concerns about the limitations of relying solely on verbal
expressions to understand children’s thinking.
Verbal Thought as a Potential Distraction: Piaget cautioned that focusing too heavily on a
child’s verbalizations might lead researchers away from observing their active engagement
with the world, which he considered a crucial aspect of their cognitive development.
He emphasized the need to balance verbal inquiry with observations of the child’s actions
and manipulations of physical objects.
The Risk of Misinterpreting Verbal Responses: Piaget emphasized that children’s verbal
expressions could be influenced by various factors, such as a desire to please the
interviewer or a misunderstanding of the question.
He stressed the importance of careful interpretation and the need to distinguish between
“liberated” or “spontaneous” responses and those that are influenced by suggestion or other
external factors.
To address these challenges, Piaget advocated for integrating verbal inquiry with
observations of the child’s active and concrete engagement with the world:
Combining Verbal and Concrete Tasks: As his clinical method evolved, Piaget increasingly
incorporated concrete tasks and manipulations into his research protocols.
By engaging children in activities that involved interacting with physical objects, he believed
he could gain a more comprehensive understanding of their reasoning processes.
This shift is exemplified in his research on physical causality, where he presented children
with concrete demonstrations, such as dropping a pebble into a glass of water, and then
questioned them about their observations and explanations.
39/43
Using Language Rooted in the Child’s Experience: Piaget emphasized the importance of
using language and concepts that were familiar to the child and connected to their concrete
experiences.
He believed that this approach helped to ensure that the child understood the question and
that their responses reflected their genuine thinking.
Ultimately, Piaget saw the relationship between verbal and concrete thought as a dynamic
interplay that evolves as the child develops.
References
Baillargeon, R., & DeVos, J. (1991). Object permanence in young infants: Further
evidence. Child development, 1227-1246.
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development: Experiments by nature
and design. Harvard University Press.
Bruner, J. S. (1966). Toward a theory of instruction. Cambridge, Mass.: Belkapp Press.
Case, R. (1985). Intellectual development: Birth to adulthood. Academic Press.
Cuban, L., Kirkpatrick, H., & Peck, C. (2001). High access and low use of technologies
in high school classrooms: Explaining an apparent paradox. American Educational
Research Journal, 38(4), 813-834.
Daniels, H., & Diack, H. (1977). Piagetian tests for the primary school. Routledge and
Kegan Paul.
Dasen, P. (1994). Culture and cognitive development from a Piagetian perspective. In
W .J. Lonner & R.S. Malpass (Eds.), Psychology and culture (pp. 145–149). Boston,
MA: Allyn and Bacon.
Donaldson, M. (1978). Children’s minds. Fontana Press.
Fisher, K., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Golinkoff, R. M., Singer, D. G., & Berk, L. (2011). Playing
around in school: Implications for learning and educational policy. In A. D. Pellegrini
(Ed.), The Oxford handbook of the development of play (pp. 341-360). Oxford
University Press.
Erickson, E. H. (1958). Young man Luther: A study in psychoanalysis and history. New
York: Norton.
40/43
Gehlbach, H. (2010). The social side of school: Why teachers need social
psychology. Educational Psychology Review, 22, 349-362.
Guy-Evans, O. (2024, January 17). Bronfenbrenner’s Ecological Systems Theory.
Simply Psychology. https://www.simplypsychology.org/bronfenbrenner.html
Hughes, M. (1975). Egocentrism in preschool children. Unpublished doctoral
dissertation. Edinburgh University.
Inhelder, B., & Piaget, J. (1958). The growth of logical thinking from childhood to
adolescence. New York: Basic Books.
Keating, D. (1979). Adolescent thinking. In J. Adelson (Ed.), Handbook of adolescent
psychology (pp. 211-246). New York: Wiley.
Kegan, R. (1982). The evolving self: Problem and process in human development.
Harvard University Press.
Lee, K., & Zentall, S. S. (2012). Psychostimulant and sensory stimulation interventions
that target the reading and math deficits of students with ADHD. Journal of Attention
Disorders, 16(4), 308-329.
Nielsen. 2014. “Millennials: Technology = Social Connection.”
http://www.nielsen.com/content/corporate/us/en/insights/news/2014/millennials-
technology-social-connecti on.html.
Lourenço, O. (2012). Piaget and Vygotsky: Many resemblances, and a crucial
difference. New Ideas in Psychology, 30(3), 281-295.
Lourenço, O., & Machado, A. (1996). In defense of Piaget’s theory: A reply to 10
common criticisms. Psychological Review, 103(1), 143-164.
Morra, S., & Borella, E. (2015). Working memory training: From metaphors to models.
Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1097.
McLeod, S. (2024, January 24). Vygotsky’s Theory Of Cognitive Development. Simply
Psychology. https://www.simplypsychology.org/vygotsky.html
McLeod, S. (2024, January 24). Piaget’s Sensorimotor Stage of Cognitive
Development. Simply Psychology. https://www.simplypsychology.org/sensorimotor.html
McLeod, S. (2024, January 24). Piaget’s Preoperational Stage (Ages 2-7). Simply
Psychology. https://www.simplypsychology.org/preoperational.html
McLeod, S. (2024, January 24). The Concrete Operational Stage of Cognitive
Development. Simply Psychology. https://www.simplypsychology.org/concrete-
operational.html
41/43
McLeod, S. (2024, January 24). Piaget’s Formal Operational Stage: Definition &
Examples. Simply Psychology. https://www.simplypsychology.org/formal-
operational.html
McLeod, S. (2024, January 25). Erik Erikson’s Stages Of Psychosocial Development.
Simply Psychology. https://www.simplypsychology.org/erik-erikson.html
Mayer, S. J. (2005). The early evolution of Jean Piaget’s clinical method. History of
Psychology, 8(4), 362–382. https://doi.org/10.1037/1093-4510.8.4.362
Ojose, B. (2008). Applying Piaget’s theory of cognitive development to mathematics
instruction. The Mathematics Educator, 18(1), 26-30.
Pascual-Leone, J. (1970). A mathematical model for the transition rule in Piaget’s
developmental stages. Acta Psychologica, 32, 301-345.
Passey, D. (2013). Inclusive technology enhanced learning: Overcoming cognitive,
physical, emotional, and geographic challenges. Routledge.
Piaget, J. (1932). The moral judgment of the child. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.
Piaget, J. (1936). Origins of intelligence in the child. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.
Piaget, J. (1945). Play, dreams and imitation in childhood. London: Heinemann.
Piaget, J. (1952). The child’s conception of number. Routledge & Kegan Paul.
Piaget, J. (1952). The origins of intelligence in children. (M. Cook, Trans.). W W Norton
& Co. (Original work published 1936)
Piaget, J. (1957). Construction of reality in the child. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.
Piaget, J., & Cook, M. T. (1952). The origins of intelligence in children. New York, NY:
International University Press.
Piaget, J. (1962). The language and thought of the child (3rd ed.). (M. Gabain, Trans.).
Routledge & Kegan Paul. (Original work published 1923)
Piaget, J. (1970). The Psychology of Intelligence. New York: Basic Books.
Piaget, J. (1981). Intelligence and affectivity: Their relationship during child
development. (Trans & Ed TA Brown & CE Kaegi). Annual Reviews.
Piaget, J. (1985). The equilibration of cognitive structures: The central problem of
intellectual development. (T. Brown & K. J. Thampy, Trans.). University of Chicago
Press. (Original work published 1975)
Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. (1956). The child’s conception of space. Routledge & Kegan
Paul.
Piaget, J., & Szeminska, A. (1952). The child’s conception of number. Routledge &
Kegan Paul.
42/43
Plowden, B. H. P. (1967). Children and their primary schools: A report (Research and
Surveys). London, England: HM Stationery Office.
Ratcliff, M. J. (2024). Jean Piaget and the autonomous disciples, Alina Szeminska and
Bärbel Inhelder: From the “critical method” to the appropriation of research
culture.History of Psychology, 27(4), 317–332. https://doi.org/10.1037/hop0000261
Rogoff, B. (2003). The cultural nature of human development. Oxford University Press.
Shayer, M. (1997). The Long-Term Effects of Cognitive Acceleration on Pupils’ School
Achievement, November 1996.
Siegler, R. S., DeLoache, J. S., & Eisenberg, N. (2003). How children develop. New
York: Worth.
Smith, L. (Ed.). (1996). Critical readings on Piaget. London: Routledge.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological
processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Wadsworth, B. J. (2004). Piaget’s theory of cognitive and affective development:
Foundations of constructivism. New York: Longman.
Further Reading
BBC Radio Broadcast about the Three Mountains Study
Piagetian stages: A critical review
Bronfenbrenner’s Ecological Systems Theory
43/43