Children at Risk of Specific Learning Disorder
Children at Risk of Specific Learning Disorder
Article
Children at Risk of Specific Learning Disorder: A Study on
Prevalence and Risk Factors
Leyla Bozatlı 1, * , Hasan Cem Aykutlu 1 , Açelya Sivrikaya Giray 2 , Tuğçe Ataş 3 , Çisem Özkan 4 ,
Burcu Güneydaş Yıldırım 5 and Işık Görker 1
1 Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Trakya University, Edirne 22000, Turkey;
[email protected] (H.C.A.); [email protected] (I.G.)
2 Department of Disability Studies, Faculty of Education, Trakya University, Edirne 22000, Turkey;
[email protected]
3 Artvin State Hospital, Artvin 08000, Turkey; [email protected]
4 İnegöl State Hospital, Bursa 16000, Turkey; [email protected]
5 Kırklareli Training and Research Hospital, Kırklareli 39000, Turkey; [email protected]
* Correspondence: [email protected]
Brazil, the prevalence was 7.6% [5]. The number of prevalence studies using diagnostic
criteria or scales for SLD is small. However, it is accepted that SLD is relatively common
and under-recognised [1].
Although the characteristics of SLD may begin to appear in the preschool years or even
at a younger age, the diagnostic process for SLD usually begins with the child’s difficulties
with reading comprehension or learning, difficulties with writing or written expression,
difficulties with number perception/calculation, and when, as a result of these difficulties,
the child’s academic performance is below what would be expected for his or her age. In
addition to academic difficulties, these children also have difficulties in other areas during
the preschool years. Research has demonstrated that early signs of SLD encompass delays
in social skill development, challenges in adhering to rules, and difficulties with individual
and group work [6]. Additionally, SLD may manifest as motor skill delay, difficulty with
ball/balance games requiring hand-eye coordination, limited comprehension of speech,
and delays in receptive and expressive language development [7].
In recent years, there has been a growing emphasis among researchers on the early
detection of SLD during preschool years. They have focused on recognising the initial
symptoms and implementing early intervention strategies [7–9]. Studies highlight that
if children at risk of learning disabilities are identified during preschool and provided
with suitable intervention programs, the likelihood of receiving an SLD diagnosis during
their school years is significantly reduced [10–12]. The benefits of early intervention in
SLD extend beyond the academic realm. Research suggests that an early diagnosis of
SLD in children and interventions employed to address the issues related to the learning,
socialisation, and emotional development of these children can help to ameliorate the SLD
condition and its negative consequences for the child [13].
Although it is possible to identify and intervene in the early stages of SLD, studies
have mostly examined the risk factors, and there is not enough research on the prevalence of
the characteristics of SLD in the preschool years. Therefore, our objective was to assess the
prevalence of preschool children who are at risk of SLD and related psychiatric disorders.
following the completion of at least six months of the first grade. In addition to assessing
SLD, the comprehensive psychiatric evaluation also assessed other psychiatric diagnoses
that may be comorbid with SLD, using the Kiddie Schedule for Affective Disorders and
Schizophrenia, Current and Lifetime Version [15,16].
3. Results
Our primary objective was to conduct a survey involving 671 students. However,
due to incomplete responses on the measurement scales or a lack of willingness to partici-
pate, we were only able to analyse data from 490 students, as provided by their parents
(Figure 1). The mothers completed 89.9% of the forms. The mean age of the participants
was 72.5 ± 5.6 SD months, and 50.7% of the children were female (Table 1).
3. Results
Our primary objective was to conduct a survey involving 671 students. However, due
to incomplete responses on the measurement scales or a lack of willingness to participate,
we were only able to analyse data from 490 students, as provided by their parents (Figure
Children 2024, 11, 759 1). The mothers completed 89.9% of the forms. The mean age of the participants was 72.54 of 14
± 5.6 SD months, and 50.7% of the children were female (Table 1).
Flowchart
Figure1.1.Flowchart
Figure ofof
thethe study
study sample.
sample.
Table 1. Cont.
A total of 27.6% (n = 135) of the study sample was identified as at risk for SLD
(Figure 2). The SLD risk group had statistically significantly higher scores in four subscales
of the Early Symptoms of Learning Disabilities Screening Scale (language development
and communication skills, cognitive skills, psychomotor skills, psychosocial skills, and
social–emotional skills) compared to the low-risk group (Table 2).
Table 2. Early symptoms of learning disabilities screening scale scores between the risk and low-
risk groups.
(n = 355) (n = 135)
consanguineous parents (p = 0.023) (Table 3).
Language Development and Communication Skills 3 (1–9) 6 (1–20) <0.001
Cognitive Skills 6 (1–9) 10 (1–19) <0.001
Table 3. Comparison of the specific learning disorder risk groups for variables.
Psychomotor Skills 3 (1–9) 6 (1–20) <0.001
Social–Emotional Skills 3 (1–19)
Low-Risk Group (n = 355) 7 (1–19)
Risk Group (n = 135) <0.001 p
Total Score 87 (4–131) 120 (20–247) <0.001
Age Month
a: Mann–Whitney U test. 72 (IQR = 6) 71 (IQR = 7) 0.510 MU
Boy 48.2% (n = 171) 52.6% (n = 71) CS
Sex We compared the socio-demographic
Girl 51.8% (n = 184)data between the groups
47.4% 64) found that 0.382
(n = and fathers
of children in the high-risk group had a lower level of education (p = 0.017), mothers
Yes 68.5% (n = 243) 69.6% (n = 94)
Siblings 0.801 CS
smoked more during pregnancy (p = 0.004), and children stayed longer in the neonatal
No 31.5% (n = 112) 30.4% (n = 41)
Number of siblings 1 (IQR = 0) 1 (IQR = 0) 0.728 MU
Together 94% (n = 330) 97.7% (n = 130)
Divorced 4.3% (n = 15) 2.3% (n = 3)
Marital Status
Separated 1.4% (n = 5) 0% 0.325 CS
Parental loss 0.3% (n = 1) 0%
Number of people living in
4 (IQR = 1) 4 (IQR = 1) 0.685 MU
the home
Children 2024, 11, 759 7 of 14
Table 3. Cont.
Table 3. Cont.
95% I. for
B S.E. Wald df Sig. Exp(B) Exp(B)
Lower Lower
Low 3.015 2 0.222
Father’s education Medium −0.887 0.512 3.001 1 0.083 0.412 0.151 1.124
High −0.793 0.536 2188 1 0.139 0.452 0.158 1.294
Low 0.951 2 0.622
Family income Medium −0.230 0.294 0.614 1 0.433 0.794 0.447 1.413
High 0.033 0.449 0.005 1 0.941 1.034 0.429 2.493
Smoking before pregnancy (yes) 0.114 0.272 0.176 1 0.674 1.121 0.658 1.909
Smoking during pregnancy (yes) 0.191 0.456 0.176 1 0.675 1.211 0.496 2.958
Postpartum intensive care unit
0.375 0.343 1.192 1 0.275 1.454 0.742 2.850
hospitalistion (yes)
Walking milestone 0.004 0.042 0.008 1 0.929 1.004 0.925 1.090
Screentime 0.002 0.001 4.527 1 0.033 1.002 1.000 1.004
Kinship (yes) 1.792 0.729 6.040 1 0.014 6.000 1.437 25.043
Constant −0.683 0.783 0.760 1 0.383 0.505
4. Discussion
We aimed to estimate the prevalence of preschool children at risk of SLD and to follow
them up in primary school to see if they received a clinical diagnosis of SLD. Our sample’s
total scale score indicated that 5.7% of preschool children were at risk of SLD. When we
also examined the subscale scores, the prevalence of children at risk for SLD increased to
27.6%. To our knowledge, no other study has explored the prevalence of children at risk of
SLD in this population.
The prevalence of specific learning disorders (SLD) varies across studies from different
countries [1]. Epidemiological studies and government reports in 2004–2005 indicated that
the prevalence of learning disabilities in Greece was between 1.2% and 1.4%. In contrast, the
prevalence rate of SLD among children aged 3–17 years was 9.7% in the United States [4].
Moreover, the prevalence of SLD in primary school children was 7.7% in Pakistan [18] and
13.6% in Turkey [14]. According to the DSM, the prevalence of SLD in reading, writing, and
mathematics ranged from 5% to 15% among school-aged children in different languages
and cultures [2]. However, some studies have found a higher prevalence of SLD. For
example, in a study conducted in India, the prevalence of SLD was 30.77% [19], while
in a review article analysing studies conducted in India, this rate was between 2% and
33% [20]. Similarly, in a study in Turkey where symptoms of SLD were screened through
a questionnaire form, this rate was reported as 36.8% on teacher forms and 37.9% on
parent forms [21]. As a result of the studies and meta-analyses, the reported rates for
the prevalence of SLD spanned a very wide range. This wide range is explained by the
population and country in which the studies were conducted, the language used, the
presence and variety of screening tools in the country, the study method (screening tools,
clinical assessment), and whether the study was conducted according to the DSM-IV or
DSM-5 criteria before or after 2013 [1]. In our study, the prevalence rate of SLD symptoms
was close to the SLD rate reported in the DSM and the literature. Therefore, our SLD risk
ratio results (5.7% with the total scale scoring and 27.6% when considering the subscale
scores and medium risk group) are consistent with the wide range of SLD prevalence
reported in the literature.
When assessing for SLD risk in preschool children, it is important not to overlook other
psychiatric disorders that share common symptoms and causes with SLD. In this study,
we invited children at risk for specific learning disabilities to undergo a comprehensive
Children 2024, 11, 759 10 of 14
psychiatric assessment at our outpatient clinic after a one-year follow-up upon finishing
first grade. However, only 19.3% of the children completed the assessment, which was a
significantly lower rate than expected. To understand the reason for the low participation,
the characteristics of the families who attended the clinical assessment and those who
did not (child’s gender, siblings, marital status, parents’ education, employment status,
health, family income, social security, and kinship) were analysed, but no difference was
found. Although the reason for the low participation is unknown, it suggests that parents’
awareness of SLD may be low. Furthermore, parents may seek assessments for their
children who exhibit pronounced behavioural symptoms or developmental delays relative
to their age-matched peers.
The psychiatric assessment results showed that 92.3% of the children evaluated had a
psychiatric disorder such as attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), SLD, anxiety
disorder, obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD), intellectual disability, or an eating disorder.
Notably, SLD was diagnosed in 38.46% of these cases. Interestingly, ADHD was diagnosed
in 83.3% of the patients, indicating a strong correlation between ADHD and SLD. This
association is supported by previous studies, such as the research by Visser et al. (2020),
which found that 28% of 3014 children with SLD also had ADHD [22]. Moreover, studies
focusing on hyperactive children frequently report academic failure and learning difficul-
ties. Numerous epidemiological studies have also found associations between SLD and
hyperactivity, as well as between specific subtypes of ADHD and SLD and their respective
symptoms [23]. These findings emphasise the importance of early identification and inter-
vention for children who are at risk of SLD. They also suggest that SLD is a heterogeneous
disorder that can manifest in various ways and interact with other disorders. Further
research is necessary to investigate the causes, mechanisms, and outcomes of SLD and its
comorbidities during the preschool period.
The prevalence of smoking during pregnancy was higher among mothers of children
in the risk group for SLD (p = 0.004). Studies investigating the effects of maternal smoking
have reported an increased risk of preterm birth, low birth weight, ADHD, and learning
difficulties [24]. Research has shown that smoking during pregnancy is linked to ADHD in
the child [25,26]. Additionally, mothers who smoke more than 20 cigarettes per day during
pregnancy have an increased risk of their children developing intellectual disability [27].
Furthermore, smoking during pregnancy has long-term negative effects. According to Fer-
gusson et al. (1998), smoking during pregnancy is associated with an increase in psychiatric
symptoms in late adolescence. The study also reported that exposure to cigarette smoke
during pregnancy is associated with increased rates of psychiatric symptoms with increasing
doses. In conclusion, studies have shown that smoking during pregnancy is associated with
early and late psychiatric symptoms and diagnoses [28]. Our study found that the SLD risk
group was diagnosed with ADHD (83.3%), anxiety disorder (20.8%), an eating disorder
(8.3%), a speech disorder (8.3%), OCD (4.2%), and intellectual disability (4.2%) in addition
to SLD. This evidence further corroborates the existing body of research in this field.
Our study revealed that fathers of children at risk for SLD had lower educational
levels (p = 0.017). This finding aligns with a previous study conducted in the same region,
which associated lower paternal education with an increased risk of SLD in children [14]. In
the broader context, research on early childhood development often considers maternal or
parental education as a whole [29,30]. However, few studies have separately examined the
impact of the father’s educational level [31]. One such study, conducted across 44 low- and
middle-income countries, analysed the relationship between both maternal and paternal
education levels and children’s early development. The results indicated a correlation be-
tween the parents’ educational level and early childhood development. Specifically, higher
maternal and paternal education levels were linked to better early childhood development
outcomes. Furthermore, the parents’ educational levels were positively associated not
only with their own parenting practices but also with the other parents’ interactions with
the child. The study also found that the manner in which caregivers support the child’s
learning process influences the child’s development significantly [31].
Children 2024, 11, 759 11 of 14
Intensive care in the neonatal period can have long-term consequences for a baby’s
health. However, these situations are complex and depend on many factors. Babies may
need intensive care for a variety of reasons, including prematurity, complications during
labour, infections, or other health problems. On the other hand, intensive care can disrupt
the natural bonding process essential for children’s growth and development. In our study,
the rate of neonatal intensive care stay was higher in cases identified as being at risk of SLD
(p = 0.039). Similarly, many researchers have reported that children treated in the intensive
care unit have significant educational problems [32,33].
We found that children born from consanguineous marriages had a higher likelihood
of being in the SLD risk group (p = 0.023). When we applied multiple regression to the data,
consanguineous marriage was one of the two risk factors that remained significant for SLD
risk (p = 0.014, Exp(B) = 6). Consanguineous marriage is defined as a marriage between
close biological relatives (first and second cousins). The rate of consanguineous marriages
in different countries depends on various factors such as education level, religion, local
traditions, and socio-economic status [34]. Consanguineous marriages, especially between
first cousins, are associated with an increased incidence of several inherited diseases,
including intellectual disability and psychiatric disorders [35,36]. Studies have suggested
possible negative consequences of consanguineous marriage, such as reduced fertility and
an increased risk of infant mortality, congenital disease, and intellectual disability [37].
There is also considerable evidence that reading disability may be inherited as a family trait.
A study investigating reading disability in children born into consanguineous marriages
reported a significant association between consanguineous marriage and an increased risk
of reading disability, highlighting the effect of genetics. The study assessed the reading
skills of 770 students and compared the performance of two experimental groups with
reading difficulties: 22 students from first-cousin marriages and 21 children from unrelated
parents. The control group consisted of 21 children with typical reading skills. The results
showed that children of cousin parents were more likely to have reading difficulties than
children of parents from other families [38].
In the current technological era, screens have become an increasingly common aspect
of daily life. The impact of these devices (smartphones, tablets, computers, and televisions)
that make our lives easier on children’s development and education is of great interest to
researchers. Although the use of digital devices is spreading rapidly among very young
children, the effects of screen time on emotional and cognitive functions are still being
debated. A longitudinal study (4–8 years) assessing emotional regulation and academic
performance reported that children’s screen time was positively and significantly associ-
ated with mood dysregulation at age 4 and negatively associated with mathematics and
literacy performance at age 8 [39]. In a further study that focused on the preschool period,
researchers noted that a child’s first screen exposure to television may be formative because
of developmental differences between preschool and later childhood. The importance of
this period is explained by the fact that habit formation and overexposure in the early
period increase the likelihood of overexposure later in life and that screen use tends to
increase over time to include more entertainment as opposed to viewing for educational
purposes only [40]. Screen time has also been linked to attention span. A meta-analysis
of studies examining this relationship reported a negative association between increased
screen time and attention span [41]. On the other hand, time spent with parents is also
important. In a study that examined the time children spent with four digital media devices
(TV, computer, smartphone, and tablet computer) using a nationally representative sample
of more than 2300 parents of children aged 0–8 years, results from linear regression analy-
ses showed that parents’ own screen time was strongly related to their children’s screen
time. Further analysis shows that children’s screen time use is the result of an interaction
between child and parent factors and is strongly influenced by parental attitudes [42]. Due
to the role of parents in the development of their children’s physical activity and sedentary
behaviour, especially in the early years, the effect of parental influence on screen time
and physical activity in young children has been investigated. The results suggest that
Children 2024, 11, 759 12 of 14
parental encouragement and support can increase children’s physical activity and that
reducing parents’ own screen time can lead to a reduction in children’s screen time. In light
of these findings, it has been reported that improving parental self-efficacy or changing
parenting styles may be beneficial in increasing young children’s physical activity and
reducing screen time [43]. Although it is noted that screens can have a positive effect on
accessing information outside the daily routine, learning vocabulary/language, or general
cultural information, the key point here is that screen time and content should be under
parental control [40].
The significant increase in screen time among the group at risk for SLD (p = 0.009),
which remained significant after regression analysis (p = 0.033), coupled with a high rate of
psychiatric diagnosis (92.4%) among those assessed, underscores the need for comprehen-
sive studies on the effects of screen use. This is particularly relevant given the ubiquity of
digital media tools in various areas such as education, work, communication, and entertain-
ment. The recent surge in mental health issues among young people has been hypothesised
to be linked to increased screen-based technology usage. However, these hypotheses are
primarily based on cross-sectional studies. Therefore, more longitudinal studies examining
screen content and the motivations for screen use are warranted to elucidate the relation-
ship between screen time and mental health symptoms [44]. Concurrently, it is crucial to
conduct studies on rational digital use and disseminate the findings to society, given the
mobile and easily accessible nature of digital media tools.
5. Conclusions
Although the diagnostic process for SLD typically commences after primary school,
early symptoms can be observed during the preschool years. Our study found a high
prevalence of preschool children at risk for SLD, many of whom also have a wide range of
psychiatric comorbidities. However, parents are often oblivious to the symptoms. It has
been reported that there is a four-year gap between the time a mother first suspects SLD
symptoms and the child’s actual diagnosis [45]. This gap can be reduced by identifying
the risk factors and clinical signs of SLD in preschool children, as well as by increasing the
awareness and involvement of teachers in the referral process. Teachers can facilitate early
intervention by recognising the learning difficulties and preferences of each child and by
collaborating with child and adolescent psychiatrists to design individualised educational
programmes. Further research is necessary to enhance understanding of the risk of SLD
and its management.
Supplementary Materials: The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://
www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/children11070759/s1, The Learning Disability Early Symptoms
Screening Scale (LDESSS).
Author Contributions: Conceptualisation, L.B., H.C.A. and I.G.; methodology, L.B., H.C.A. and I.G.;
validation, L.B., H.C.A. and I.G.; formal analysis, L.B. and H.C.A.; investigation, resources, data
curation, L.B., H.C.A., A.S.G., T.A., Ç.Ö., B.G.Y. and I.G.; writing—original draft preparation, L.B.,
H.C.A., A.S.G., T.A., Ç.Ö., B.G.Y. and I.G; writing—review and editing, L.B., H.C.A. and I.G. All
authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.
Funding: This research received no external funding.
Institutional Review Board Statement: Approval for the study was granted by the Trakya University
Social Science Ethical Committee (number E-76244175-050.01.04-230032), ethics date: 12 May 2023.
Informed Consent Statement: Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.
Data Availability Statement: The data presented in this study are available on request from the
corresponding author due to privacy restriction.
Acknowledgments: We would like to thank all participants who volunteered for this research.
Conflicts of Interest: The authors declare no conflicts of interest.
Children 2024, 11, 759 13 of 14
References
1. Al-Yagon, M.; Cavendish, W.; Cornoldi, C.; Fawcett, A.J.; Grünke, M.; Hung, L.-Y.; Jiménez, J.E.; Karande, S.; van Kraayenoord,
C.E.; Lucangeli, D. The proposed changes for DSM-5 for SLD and ADHD: International perspectives—Australia, Germany,
Greece, India, Israel, Italy, Spain, Taiwan, United Kingdom, and United States. J. Learn. Disabil. 2013, 46, 58–72. [CrossRef]
2. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th ed.; American Psychiatric Association:
Arlington, VA, USA, 2013.
3. Scaria, L.M.; Bhaskaran, D.; George, B. Prevalence of Specific Learning Disorders (SLD) among children in India: A systematic
review and meta-analysis. Indian J. Psychol. Med. 2023, 45, 213–219. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
4. Altarac, M.; Saroha, E. Lifetime prevalence of learning disability among US children. Pediatrics 2007, 119 (Suppl. S1), 77–83.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]
5. Fortes, I.S.; Paula, C.S.; Oliveira, M.C.; Bordin, I.A.; de Jesus Mari, J.; Rohde, L.A. A cross-sectional study to assess the prevalence
of DSM-5 specific learning disorders in representative school samples from the second to sixth grade in Brazil. Eur. Child Adolesc.
Psychiatry 2016, 25, 195–207. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
6. Mustard, J.F. Early Child Development and the Brain-the Base for Health, Learning, and Behavior Throughout Life. In From Early
Child Development to Human Development: Investing in Our Children’s Future; Young, M., Ed.; World Bank: Washington, DC, USA,
2002; pp. 23–61.
7. Balikci, O.S.; Melekoglu, M.A. Early signs of specific learning disabilities in early childhood. Int. J. Early Child. Spec. Educ. 2020,
12, 84–95. [CrossRef]
8. Aslan, K. Review on Early Symptoms of Specific Learning Disability and Early Intervention Practices. Hacet. Univ. Fac. Health Sci.
J. 2015, 1, 577–588.
9. Firat, T.; Bildiren, A. Developmental characteristics of children with learning disabilities aged 0–6 based on parental observations.
Curr. Psychol. 2024, 43, 2909–2921. [CrossRef]
10. Şentürk Gülhan, N.; Burak, Y. Investigation of the Relationship between Early Symptoms of Learning Disability of Preschool
Children and Early Literacy and Executive Functioning Skills. J. Mother Tongue Educ. 2023, 11, 707–729.
11. Schneider, W.; Ennemoser, M.; Roth, E.; Küspert, P. Kindergarten Prevention Od Dyslexia:Does Training İn Phonological
Awareness Work For Everybody. J. Learn. Disabil. 1999, 32, 429–436. [CrossRef]
12. Metwally, A.M.; Aboulghate, A.; Elshaarawy, G.A.; Abdallah, A.M.; Abdel Raouf, E.R.; El-Din, E.M.S.; ElRifay, A.S. Prevalence
and Risk Factors of Disabilities among Egyptian Preschool Children: A Community-based Population Study. BMC Psychiatry
2023, 23, 689. [CrossRef]
13. Mercer, C.D. Students with Learning Disabilities; Merrill: St Princeton, NC, USA, 1997.
14. Görker, I.; Bozatli, L.; Korkmazlar, Ü.; Karadağ, M.Y.; Ceylan, C.; Söğüt, C.; Aykutlu, H.C.; Subay, B.; Turan, N. The Probable
Prevalence and Sociodemographic Characteristics of Specific Learning Disorder in Primary School Children in Edirne. Arch.
Neuropsychiatry 2017, 54, 343–349. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
15. Kaufman, J.; Birmaher, B.; Brent, D.; Rao, U.M.A.; Flynn, C.; Moreci, P.; Williamson, D.; Ryan, N. Schedule for Affective Disorders
and Schizophrenia for School-age Children-present and lifetime version (K-SADS-PL): Initial reliability and validity data. J. Am.
Acad. Child. Adolesc. Psychiatry 1997, 36, 980–988. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
16. Ünal, F.; Öktem, F.; Çetin Çuhadaroğlu, F.; Çengel Kültür, S.E.; Akdemir, D.; Foto Özdemir, D.; Çak, H.T.; Ünal, D.; TIRAŞ, K.;
Aslan, C.; et al. Reliability and Validity of the Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-age Children-present
and lifetime version, DSM-5 November 2016-Turkish adaptation (K-SADS-PL-DSM-5-T). Turk Psikiyatr. Derg. 2019, 30, 1–8.
[CrossRef]
17. Okur, M. Determination of Psychometric Properties of Learning Disability Early Symptoms Screenıng Scale. Master’s Thesis,
Department of Special Education Anadolu University, Graduate School of Educational Sciences, Eskisehir, Türkiye, 2019.
18. Kausar, N.; Farhat, N.; Maqsood, F.; Qurban, H. Specific learning disorder among primary school children of Sarai Alamgir. JPMA
2021, 71, 1193.
19. Kumari, R.; Ojha, A.A.R.P.; Dubey, G. Prevalence of Learning Disability among Tribal, Rural and Urban School Going Children of
Eastern Uttar Pradesh. J. Nehru Gram Bharati Univ. 2018, 7, 1–10.
20. Beri, N.; Singh, K.U.V. Learning Disability in India: A Review. Ensemble 2021, 1, 67–71.
21. Doğan, O.; Ersan, E.; Doğan, S. The Probable Learning Disorders in Primary School Students: A preliminary study. Anadolu
Psikiyatr. Derg.-Anatol. J. Psychiatry 2009, 10, 62–70.
22. Visser, L.; Linkersdörfer, J.; Hasselhorn, M. The Role of ADHD Symptoms in the Relationship between Academic Achievement
and Psychopathological Symptoms. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2020, 97, 103552. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
23. Cantwell, D.P.; Baker, L. Association between Attention Deficit-Hyperactivity Disorder and Learning Disorders. J. Learn. Disabil.
1991, 24, 88–95. [CrossRef]
24. Batstra, L.; Hadders-Algra, M.; Neeleman, J. Effect of Antenatal Exposure to Maternal Smoking on Behavioural Problems and
Academic Achievement in Childhood: Prospective Evidence from a Dutch Birth Cohort. Early Hum. Dev. 2003, 75, 21–33.
[CrossRef]
25. Gustavson, K.; Ystrom, E.; Stoltenberg, C.; Susser, E.; Surén, P.; Magnus, P.; Knudsen, G.P.; Smith, G.D.; Langley, K.; Rutter, M.
Smoking in Pregnancy and Child ADHD. Pediatrics 2017, 139, e20162509. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Children 2024, 11, 759 14 of 14
26. He, Y.; Chen, J.; Zhu, L.H.; Hua, L.L.; Ke, F.F. Maternal Smoking During Pregnancy and ADHD: Results from a Systematic Review
and Meta-analysis of Prospective Cohort Studies. J. Atten. Disord. 2020, 24, 1637–1647. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
27. Braun, J.M.; Daniels, J.L.; Kalkbrenner, A.; Zimmerman, J.; Nicholas, J.S. The Effect of Maternal Smoking During Pregnancy on
Intellectual Disabilities among 8-Year-Old Children. Paediatr. Perinat. Epidemiol. 2009, 23, 482–491. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
28. Fergusson, D.M.; Woodward, L.J.; Horwood, L.J. Maternal Smoking during Pregnancy and Psychiatric Adjustment in Late
Adolescence. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 1998, 55, 721–727. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
29. McCoy, D.C.; Zuilkowski, S.S.; Fink, G. Poverty, Physical Stature, and Cognitive Skills: Mechanisms Underlying Children’s
School Enrollment in Zambia. Dev. Psychol. 2015, 51, 600–614. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
30. McCoy, D.C.; Peet, E.D.; Ezzati, M.; Danaei, G.; Black, M.M.; Sudfeld, C.R.; Fawzi, W.; Fink, G. Early Childhood Developmental
Status in Low-and Middle-Income Countries: National, Regional, and Global Prevalence Estimates Using Predictive Modeling.
PLoS Med. 2016, 13, e1002034. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
31. Jeong, J.; McCoy, D.C.; Fink, G. Pathways between Paternal and Maternal Education, Caregivers’ Support for Learning, and Early
Child Development in 44 Low-and Middle-Income Countries. Early Child. Res. Q. 2017, 41, 136–148. [CrossRef]
32. Resnick, M.B.; Gomatam, S.V.; Carter, R.L.; Ariet, M.; Roth, J.; Kilgore, K.L.; Bucciarelli, R.L.; Mahan, C.S.; Curran, J.S.; Eitzman,
D.V. Educational Disabilities of Neonatal Intensive Care Graduates. Pediatrics 1998, 102, 308–314. [CrossRef]
33. Sell, E.J.; Gaines, J.A.; Gluckman, C.; Williams, E. Early Identification of Learning Problems in Neonatal Intensive Care Graduates.
Am. J. Dis. Child. 1985, 139, 460–463. [CrossRef]
34. Hamamy, H.; Antonarakis, S.E.; Cavalli-Sforza, L.L.; Temtamy, S.; Romeo, G.; Ten Kate, L.P.; Bennett, R.L.; Shaw, A.; Megarbane,
A.; Van Duijn, C. Consanguineous Marriages, Pearls and Perils: Geneva International Consanguinity Workshop Report. Genet.
Med. 2011, 13, 841–847. [CrossRef]
35. Lakhan, R.; Bipeta, R.; Yerramilli, S.S.; Nahar, V.K. A Family Study of Consanguinity in Children with Intellectual Disabilities in
Barwani, India. J. Neurosci. Rural Pract. 2017, 8, 551–555. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
36. Saad, H.A.; Elbedour, S.; Hallaq, E.; Merrick, J.; Tenenbaum, A. Consanguineous Marriage and Intellectual and Developmental
Disabilities among Arab Bedouins Children of The Negev Region in Southern Israel: A Pilot Study. Front. Media 2014, 2, 1–3.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]
37. Jaber, L.; Merlob, P.; Gabriel, R.; Shohat, M. Effects of Consanguineous Marriage on Reproductive Outcome in an Arab Community
in Israel. J. Med. Genet. 1997, 34, 1000–1002. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
38. Talaat, F.M. The Effect of Consanguineous Marriage on Reading Disability Based on Deep Neural Networks. Multimed. Tools Appl.
2023, 83, 51787–51807. [CrossRef]
39. Cerniglia, L.; Cimino, S.; Ammaniti, M. What are The Effects of Screen Time on Emotion Regulation and Academic Achievements?
A Three-Wave Longitudinal Study on Children From 4 to 8 Years of Age. J. Early Child. Res. 2021, 19, 145–160. [CrossRef]
40. Canadian Paediatric Society. Screen Time and Young Children: Promoting Health and Development in a Digital World. Paediatr.
Child Health 2017, 461–468.
41. Santos, R.M.S.; Mendes, C.G.; Marques Miranda, D.; Romano-Silva, M.A. The Association between Screen Time and Attention in
Children: A Systematic Review. Dev. Neuropsychol. 2022, 47, 175–192. [CrossRef]
42. Lauricella, A.R.; Wartella, E.; Rideout, V.J. Young Children’s Screen Time: The Complex Role of Parent and Child Factors. J. Appl.
Dev. Psychol. 2015, 36, 11–17. [CrossRef]
43. Xu, H.; Wen, L.M.; Rissel, C. Associations of Parental Influences with Physical Activity and Screen Time among Young Children:
A systematic review. J. Obes. 2015, 546925. [CrossRef]
44. Tang, S.; Werner-Seidler, A.; Torok, M.; Mackinnon, A.J.; Christensen, H. The Relationship between Screen Time and Mental
Health in Young People: A Systematic Review of Longitudinal Studies. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2021, 86, 102021. [CrossRef]
45. Blake, R.A. Parents of Children with Learning Disabilities: Attitudes toward Education. Master’s Thesis, The College at Brockport,
Brockport, NY, USA, 1997.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual
author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to
people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.