Cognitive digit span
Cognitive digit span
information. It is an active system that receives information from the senses, puts information
into a usable form, organises it as it stores away, and then retrieves the information from
storage (adapted from Baddeley, 1996, 2003). The three processes of memory include the
following:
As per the information- processing model (also known as the Atkinson Shiffrin
• Sensory memory (raw information from the senses is held for a very brief
• Short-term memory (information is held for a brief period of time while being
used)
Short-term Memory:
Short-term memory is the second level store of memory and is limited in capacity and
retention time. Short-term memory is responsible for holding any information currently
needed to complete a task as well as important information from sensory memory. However,
this information is displaced by new information after just a few seconds (Miller, 2011).
Short-term memory has three essential features: capacity, duration, and encoding. Its duration
1
is usually between 15 and 30 seconds, and its capacity is approximately seven items. Without
Working memory and short-term memory are not entirely distinct from each other.
The term "working memory" was first introduced by Miller and colleagues (1960) to describe
memory as it is used for planning and executing behaviour. In the field, "working memory"
became more widely used after Baddeley and Hitch's (1974) research showed that a single
module could not account for all types of temporary memory. Based on their findings, an
influential model came into existence (Baddeley, 1986) where verbal-phonological and
visual-spatial representations were held separately and managed and manipulated with the
The Digit Span test is one of the memory span tests used for measuring short-term
memory. The concept of Digit Span as a psychological construct was first proposed by
Gottfried Leibniz (1646-1716). He proposed that individuals have a finite capacity to hold
and process information from their environment. He called this capacity the span of
apperception. In the 19th century, cognitive scientist Herman Ebbinghaus used span as an
experimental paradigm to study memory and learning. From the beginning, virtually all
Wechsler intelligence and memory scales for both children and adults have included a Digit
Span subtest. Wechsler combined the results of the digits forward and digits backward tasks
The Digit Span test is a commonly used method for assessing attention-concentration
and working memory (Ryan, Lopez & Paolo, 1996; Ostrosky-Solís & Lozano, 2006). This
test is often included as part of neuropsychological and mental state examinations (Ryan,
Lopez & Paolo, 1996) due to its portability and ease of use. The test includes both digit
2
forward and digit backward conditions, wherein a series of digit strings are read out to
participants who are then required to repeat them in the same or reverse order of presentation.
Successful performance on the test relies on auditory attention and short-term verbal memory
(Ostrosky-Solís & Lozano, 2006). Studies have shown that performance on the Digit Span
test is more closely tied to the integrity of the left hemisphere of the brain, rather than the
The Digit Span test is commonly used in Indian settings by researchers and clinicians
to evaluate attention and working memory. In clinical settings, variations of the Digit Span
test are used to evaluate memory and working memory deficits. For example, the Backward
Digits Test (BDT) assesses working memory deficits in dementia. It involves trials of 3, 4,
Theoretical Framework
This model of memory was proposed by Craik and Lockhart in 1972. According to this
model, the more deeply information is processed during encoding, the better it will be
remembered.
semantic processing, but it still does not deeply engage with the meaning or
the information. This level of processing involves thinking about the meaning,
3
significance, and relevance of the information being encoded. When information is
The levels of processing model suggests that memory is not simply a product of
repetition or rote memorization but rather a result of the depth of cognitive processing during
encoding. According to this model, deep processing leads to better memory performance
Tulving’s Model:
Tulving in the year 1972 proposed a model that categorizes memory as short-term and
long-term. Long-term memory is further divided into three categories: episodic memory,
experiences, while semantic memory involves the accumulation of facts and organized
knowledge over a lifetime. Procedural memory deals with skill learning and the acquisition of
how-to knowledge.
Nelson Cowan defined working memory as a cognitive process that allows us to retain
both old and new information in an accessible state, which we can use to perform mental
from attention and long-term memory. It is worth noting that if a whole process is activated
without aiding a task, it's still considered a part of working memory (like the verbal encoding
of meaningless shapes).
4
Validity
The Digit Span test displays moderate criterion validity with the Stanford-Binet-IV
composite score and short-term memory, with correlation coefficients of r = 0.48 and r =
0.52, respectively.
Review of Literature
Ramirez & Harris (2023)- In this research, the relationship between Hispanic Spanish-
English bilingualism and performance on the Digit Span task was investigated. The study
education: 15.68 years; 53.7% female). Participants completed the Language and Social
Background Questionnaire (LSBQ) and the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale - Fourth
Edition (WAIS-IV) Digit Span subtest via Zoom. LSBQ composite factor scores significantly
predicted Digit Span Forward and Longest Digit Span Forward scores. Bilingual experiences
did not significantly predict Digit Span Backward, Digit Span Sequencing, Digit Span
Total, Longest Digit Span Backward, or Longest Digit Span Sequencing scores.
Tripathi et al. (2019)-The study aimed to explore the usefulness of the Digit Span
test for Indian older adults with different levels of education. The sample included 258
screening, which included a semi-structured interview using the modified MINI Screen, the
living, and a verbal administration of the Digit Span test. Participants with higher educational
levels performed significantly better than those with low education on the Digit Span test.
Low-educated participants often struggled with the test and resorted to guessing the digits.
Choi et al. (2014)-The study aimed to explore the effect of demographic variables
on Digit Span test (DS) performance in an educationally diverse elderly population and to
5
provide normative information. The DS was administered to 784 community-dwelling
volunteers aged 60-90 years with an educational history ranging from zero to 25 years of full-
time education. Individuals with serious neurological, medical, and psychiatric disorders
(including dementia) were excluded. Age, education, and gender were significantly
associated with DS performance. Based on the results obtained, DS norms were stratified by:
age (two strata), education (three strata) and gender (two strata).
Muangpaisan, Intalapaporn, & Assantachai (2010)- The purpose of the study was to
compare the performance of normal subjects and patients with amnestic MCI on the Digit
Span test and CVFT, while also examining the potential influence of age, gender, and
education on task performance. The authors gathered data from 77 participants diagnosed
with amnestic MCI and 30 normal subjects aged 50 or over who were recruited from
communities in Bangkok. The Digit Span test and CVFT (which included semantic fluency
and Controlled word association test for letter fluency) were utilized to evaluate the
participants. The results of the study indicated that MCI patients scored significantly lower on
the Digit Span test, both in Digits Forward and Digits Backward. Additionally, they exhibited
poorer performance on the semantic fluency task for animals and fruits, as well as on the
The Digit Span test evaluates working memory, attention span, and short-term
tasks that call for quick memory and processing of information. This experiment guarantees
precision and consistency in data collecting by using technologies such as PEBL software and
standard operating procedures. The test is a useful tool for both research and clinical settings
because of its ease of use and capacity to detect cognitive deficits under a variety of
6
scenarios. In order to provide light on cognitive processes essential for day-to-day
functioning and the identification of neurological diseases, this study attempts to clarify the
Sample
For this experiment, 10 participants aged between 20-25 were selected, comprising 5
Apparatus
The administration of forward Digit Span test was conducted using the PEBL
software. To conduct the experiment, the 'battery' section in PEBL software has to be opened
and the 'dspan' option needs to be selected. Then the 'digitspan.pbl' file has to be run. For
accurate results, it is recommended that the participant wears headphones and is in complete
silence.
Procedure
The participant will be shown a sequence of digits, one at a time on the screen, and
will be asked to type them in the exact order they were presented (i.e., forward direction). If
they are unsure of the next digit, they can skip it using the '-' key. It's encouraged that they try
to recall the digits in their original positions. If a mistake is made, they can correct it using
the backspace key. The participant will start with a list of 3 digits and will receive three
different lists at each length. If they can correctly recall two out of three lists, they will
Instructions
7
“You are about to take part in a memory test. You will be presented with a sequence of
digits. Each digit will occur only once during a list. You will then be asked to type the list of
digits exactly in the order you saw them in. If you do not know what digit comes next, you
can skip over it by typing the ‘-’ key. Try to put the numbers in the original list positions. If
you make a mistake, you can use the backspace key to make a correction. You will start with
a list of three items and will get three different lists at each length. If you are able to recall
two out of three lists completely correctly, you will move on to the next longest list length”.
Ethical Considerations
To ensure ethical and proper conduct, it is necessary to obtain informed consent from
all participants. A comprehensive overview of the rules and instructions must be provided,
including an explanation of the objective and procedure of the test, while ensuring complete
confidentiality. Participation must be entirely voluntary and the test should be conducted in
an environment that is quiet and free from distractions. In case of any difficulty in typing,
participants may vocalize their response and request assistance from the examiner in typing it
out.
Score Table
Sl. No. Name of the Gender Memory Total Words Total Time
minutes)
8
6 P. U. B. Male 6 6 3.17 minutes
9
Line Graph Showing The Memory Span of The Participants
12
10 10
8
Memory Span
7 7
6 6 6 6 6
Female
Male
4
0
1 2 3 4 5
Participants (5 females, 5 males)
10
Discussion
The Digit Span test is a measure of working memory capacity, specifically the ability
to temporarily hold and manipulate information. Participants are required to repeat a series of
A high score on the forward Digit Span test suggests a better working memory
capacity. Individuals with higher scores tend to have better abilities to hold and manipulate
information in their mind temporarily. They are likely to excel in tasks that require attention,
concentration, and quick processing of information. Conversely, a low score on the forward
Digit Span test indicates a limited working memory capacity. Individuals with lower scores
might find it challenging to remember and manipulate information in their mind, which could
In terms of clinical aspects, individuals who are in good health typically have a digit
span range of seven with an acceptable margin of plus or minus two. If the digit span falls
below five, it is regarded as subpar, while a range beyond nine is deemed exceptional. If the
outcome falls below average, further examinations may be necessary to identify any potential
• P. S. has the highest memory span of 10 digits and the highest total words correct (15
digits). This suggests superior short-term memory capacity and cognitive processing
• M. K. also demonstrates a high memory span of 9 digits and performs well in terms of
total words correct (13 digits), indicating strong short-term memory ability.
11
• G. S., H. C., and P. S. also have relatively high scores, indicating above-average short-
• M. P. P., A. M. B., S. D., P. U. B., A. K., and A. R. have lower scores, suggesting more
retaining and manipulating information in their minds. They are well-suited for tasks that
demand focus, concentration, and swift processing of data. G. S., H. C., and P. S., on the
minds, making them ideal for tasks that require high levels of concentration, attention, and
quick data processing. Conversely, M. P. P., A. M. B., S. D., P. U. B., A. K., and A. R. may
encounter some difficulty in recalling and manipulating information in their minds, which
Females: In this dataset, females generally have memory span ranging from 6 to 7 in memory
span. This indicates an average working memory capacity. The total words correct also range
from 6 to 9. Overall, the performance seems consistent with average working memory
Males: Males in this dataset show a wider range of memory span. While some males have
memory span similar to females, there are also males with higher memory span (up to 10).
This suggests a more varied working memory capacity among males in this dataset. Some
memory span.
12
Comparison
The Digit Span test can be compared with a similar test called the Corsi block-tapping
The Corsi block-tapping task, also known as the Corsiblock-tapping test (CBT),
originated from Milner's (1971) research and was later explained by her PhD student, Corsi
(1972), in his dissertation. The test is a variation of the Hebb Digit Span task (Hebb, 1961)
and aims to assess the participant's "spatial span," which is now commonly referred to as the
Corsi Span.
In the standard Corsi task, individuals are tasked with recalling the order in which a set
of rectangles illuminate. They must then select the rectangles in the same sequence as they
appeared. The Corsi span measures the maximum length of a sequence that a person can
accurately repeat. Typically, the average Corsi span is 6 blocks, with a maximum span of 9
blocks. The backward Corsi task presents an additional level of difficulty to participants. In
this task, individuals must recall and select the illuminated rectangles in reverse order.
Starting with the last illuminated rectangle, they must work their way backward. The
backward Corsi span measures the longest sequence that a person can accurately reproduce in
reverse order.
The Digit Span and Corsi block-tapping tasks are widely utilized for assessing Working
Memory (WM) (Kessels, van den Berg, Ruis & Brands, 2008). In both assessments, the
subject is required to repeat a sequence of stimuli in either the same order (forward) or the
opposite order (backward). The Digit Span involves an increasing sequence of numbers from
1 to 9 randomized to evaluate verbal WM, while the Corsi block-tapping task assesses
visuospatial WM through nine cubes on a wooden board. Both assessments are commonly
13
pathological aging, including minor and major neurocognitive disorders. Digit Span is
commonly used in clinical settings to assess verbal working memory deficits, such as those
seen in conditions like attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), dementia, or brain
injuries. Corsi block-tapping Test is commonly used to assess visuospatial working memory
deficits, such as those seen in conditions like Alzheimer's disease, traumatic brain injury, or
reliability of two working memory tasks: the Digit Span and Corsi block-tapping tasks. The
study included 25 older adults with low formal education who were referred for
etiological diagnosis). Both the Digit Span and Corsi block-tapping tasks were administered
to all participants, with two trials conducted for each span length in both forward and reverse
order. Reliability was estimated using the split-half method, with the first trials from each
span forming the first half, and the second trials forming the second half. Reliable Change
Indexes (RCIs) were calculated for all test measures. The study found that the Digit Span had
high reliability for the forward version, but low reliability for the backward version. The
A study conducted by Kessels and colleagues (2008) analysed the backward Corsi
procedure in a group of 246 healthy older adults, ranging from ages 50 to 92. They compared
the results to the Digit Span subtest of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale—Third Edition
(WAIS-III). Through principal component analysis, a two-factor model was revealed: the
verbal working memory factor and the spatial working memory factor. Interestingly, the Corsi
backward task was not found to be more challenging than the Corsi forward task, which
differs from the Digit Span backward task that is more difficult than the Digit Span forward
14
task. These findings suggest that the Corsi block-tapping Task backward task depends on
processing within working-memory's slave systems, while the Digit Span backward also
Conclusion
working memory capacity, which is essential for tasks that demand attention and rapid data
processing. The varying performance levels among participants emphasize the unique
cognitive abilities of each individual. Additionally, comparing the results with the Corsi
Familiarizing with these assessments can facilitate the diagnosis and treatment of cognitive
15
REFERENCES
Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. Oxford Psychology Series No. 11. Oxford:
Clarendon Press.
Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The
Choi, H. J., Lee, D. Y., Seo, E. H., Jo, M. K., Sohn, B. K., Choe, Y. M., Byun, M. S., Kim, J.
W., Kim, S. G., Yoon, J. C., Jhoo, J. H., Kim, K. W., & Woo, J. I. (2014). A normative
Corsi, P. M. (1972). Human memory and the medial temporal region of the brain.
memory assessment in neurocognitive disorders: a study of the Digit Span and Corsi
Hebb, D. O. (1961). Distinctive features of learning in the higher animal. Brain mechanisms
Kessels, R. P., van den Berg, E., Ruis, C., & Brands, A. M. (2008). The backward span of the
Corsi Block-Tapping Task and its association with the WAIS-III Digit
Miller, G. A., Galanter, E., & Pribram, K. H. (1960). Plans and the structure of behavior.
16
Miller, M. D. (2011). What College Teachers Should Know About Memory: A Perspective
https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordjournals.bmb.a070866
Muangpaisan, W., Intalapaporn, S., & Assantachai, P. (2010). Digit Span and verbal fluency
tests in patients with mild cognitive impairment and normal subjects in Thai-
Ostrosky-Solís, F., & Lozano, A. (2006). Digit Span: Effect of education and culture.
Ramirez, N. R., & Harris, R. N. (2023). 27 Examining the Relationship Between Spanish-
Ryan, J. J., Lopez, S. J., & Paolo, A. M. (1996). Digit Span performance of persons 75-96
years of age: Base rates and associations with selected demographic variables.
Tripathi, R., Kumar, K., Bharath, S., P, M., Rawat, V. S., & Varghese, M. (2019). Indian older
adults and the Digit Span A preliminary report. Dementia & neuropsychologia, 13(1),
111–115. https://doi.org/10.1590/1980-57642018dn13-010013
Wambach, D., Lamar, M., Swenson, R., Penney, D. L., Kaplan, E., & Libon, D. J. (2011).
79948-3_1288
17