0% found this document useful (0 votes)
7 views4 pages

True Experiments

True experiments are considered the gold standard in research designs due to their rigorous methodology, involving manipulation of independent variables and random assignment of subjects to control and experimental groups. They are effective for establishing causality and internal validity, particularly in explanatory research questions, though they can be resource-intensive and challenging to implement in practice. Various designs such as classical experiments, posttest only designs, and the Solomon four-group design are discussed, each with their own strengths and weaknesses in addressing testing effects and establishing causal relationships.
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as DOCX, PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
0% found this document useful (0 votes)
7 views4 pages

True Experiments

True experiments are considered the gold standard in research designs due to their rigorous methodology, involving manipulation of independent variables and random assignment of subjects to control and experimental groups. They are effective for establishing causality and internal validity, particularly in explanatory research questions, though they can be resource-intensive and challenging to implement in practice. Various designs such as classical experiments, posttest only designs, and the Solomon four-group design are discussed, each with their own strengths and weaknesses in addressing testing effects and establishing causal relationships.
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as DOCX, PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
You are on page 1/ 4

True experiments

A true experiment, often considered to be the “gold standard” in research designs, is thought
of as one of the most rigorous of all research designs. In this design, one or more independent
variables (as treatments) are manipulated by the researcher, subjects are randomly assigned
(i.e., random assignment) to different treatment levels, and the results of the treatments on
outcomes (dependent variables) are observed. The unique strength of experimental research is
its ability to increase internal validity and help establish causality through treatment
manipulation, while controlling for the effects of extraneous variables. As such they are best
suited for explanatory research questions.

In true experimental design, research subjects are assigned to either an experimental group,
which receives the treatment or intervention being investigated, or a control group, which
does not. Control groups may receive no treatment at all, the standard treatment (which is
called “treatment as usual” or TAU), or a treatment that entails some type of contact or
interaction without the characteristics of the intervention being investigated. For example,
the control group may participate in a support group while the experimental group is
receiving a new group-based therapeutic intervention consisting of education and cognitive
behavioral group therapy.

After determining the nature of the experimental and control groups, the next decision a
researcher must make is when they need to collect data during their experiment. Do they take
a baseline measurement and then a measurement after treatment, or just a measurement after
treatment, or do they handle data collection another way? Below, we’ll discuss three main
types of true experimental designs. There are sub-types of each of these designs, but here, we
just want to get you started with some of the basics.

Using a true experiment in social work research is often difficult and can be quite resource
intensive. True experiments work best with relatively large sample sizes, and random
assignment, a key criterion for a true experimental design, is hard (and unethical) to execute
in practice when you have people in dire need of an intervention. Nonetheless, some of the
strongest evidence bases are built on true experiments.

For the purposes of this section, let’s bring back the example of CBT for the treatment of
social anxiety. We have a group of 500 individuals who have agreed to participate in our
study, and we have randomly assigned them to the control and experimental groups. The
participants in the experimental group will receive CBT, while the participants in the control
group will receive a series of videos about social anxiety.

Classical experiments (pretest posttest


control group design)
The elements of a classical experiment are (1) random assignment of participants into an
experimental and control group, (2) a pretest to assess the outcome(s) of interest for each
group, (3) delivery of an intervention/treatment to the experimental group, and (4) a posttest
to both groups to assess potential change in the outcome(s).
When explaining experimental research designs, we often use diagrams with abbreviations to
visually represent the components of the experiment. Table 14.2 starts us off by laying out
what the abbreviations mean.

Table 14.2 Experimental research design notations


R Random assignment
O Observation (assessment of the dependent/outcome variable)
X Intervention or treatment
Xe Experimental condition (i.e., the treatment or intervention)
Xi Treatment as usual (sometimes denoted TAU)
A, B, C, etc. Denotes different groups (control/comparison and experimental)

Figure 14.1 depicts a classical experiment using our example of assessing the intervention of
CBT for social anxiety. In the figure, RA denotes random assignment to the experimental
group A and RB is random assignment to the control group B. O1 (observation 1) denotes the
pretest, Xe denotes the experimental intervention, and O2 (observation 2) denotes the posttest.

Figure 14.1 Pretest posttest control group design testing CBT an intervention

The more general, or universal, notation for classical experimental design is shown in Figure
14.2.

Figure 14.2 Classical


experimental (pretest posttest control group) design

In a situation where the control group received treatment as usual instead of no intervention,
the diagram would look this way (Figure 14.3), with Xi denoting treatment as usual:

Figure 14.3 Pretest


posttest control group design with treatment as usual instead of no treatment

Hopefully, these diagrams provide you a visualization of how this type of experiment
establishes temporality, a key component of a causal relationship. By administering the
pretest, researchers can assess if the change in the outcome occured after the intervention.
Assuming there is a change in the scores between the pretest and posttest, we would be able
to say that yes, the change did occur after the intervention.

Posttest only control group design


Posttest only control group design involves only giving participants a posttest, just like it
sounds. But why would you use this design instead of using a pretest posttest design? One
reason could be to avoid potential testing effects that can happen when research participants
take a pretest.

In research, the testing effect threatens internal validity when the pretest changes the way the
participants respond on the posttest or subsequent assessments (Flannelly, Flannelly, &
Jankowski, 2018).[1] A common example occurs when testing interventions for cognitive
impairment in older adults. By taking a cognitive assessment during the pretest, participants
get exposed to the items on the assessment and get to “practice” taking it (see for example,
Cooley et al., 2015).[2] They may perform better the second time they take it because they
have learned how to take the test, not because there have been changes in cognition. This
specific type of testing effect is called the practice effect.[3]

The testing effect isn’t always bad in practice—our initial assessments might help clients
identify or put into words feelings or experiences they are having when they haven’t been
able to do that before. In research, however, we might want to control its effects to isolate a
cleaner causal relationship between intervention and outcome. Going back to our CBT for
social anxiety example, we might be concerned that participants would learn about social
anxiety symptoms by virtue of taking a pretest. They might then identify that they have those
symptoms on the posttest, even though they are not new symptoms for them. That could
make our intervention look less effective than it actually is. To mitigate the influence of
testing effects, posttest only control group designs do not administer a pretest to participants.
Figure 14.4 depicts this.

Figure 14.4 Posttest only control group


design

A drawback to the posttest only control group design is that without a baseline measurement,
establishing causality can be more difficult. If we don’t know someone’s state of mind before
our intervention, how do we know our intervention did anything at all? Establishing time
order is thus a little more difficult. The posttest only control group design relies on the
random assignment to groups to create groups that are equivalent at baseline because, without
a pretest, researchers cannot assess whether the groups are equivalent before the intervention.
Researchers must balance this consideration with the benefits of this type of design.
Solomon four group design
One way we can possibly measure how much the testing effect threatens internal validity is
with the Solomon four group design. Basically, as part of this experiment, there are two
experimental groups and two control groups. The first pair of experimental/control groups
receives both a pretest and a posttest. The other pair receives only a posttest (Figure 14.5). In
addition to addressing testing effects, this design also addresses the problems of establishing
time order and equivalent groups in posttest only control group designs.

Figure 14.5
Solomon four-group design

For our CBT project, we would randomly assign people to four different groups instead of
just two. Groups A and B would take our pretest measures and our posttest measures, and
groups C and D would take only our posttest measures. We could then compare the results
among these groups and see if they’re significantly different between the folks in A and B,
and C and D. If they are, we may have identified some kind of testing effect, which enables
us to put our results into full context. We don’t want to draw a strong causal conclusion about
our intervention when we have major concerns about testing effects without trying to
determine the extent of those effects.

Solomon four group designs are less common in social work research, primarily because of
the logistics and resource needs involved. Nonetheless, this is an important experimental
design to consider when we want to address major concerns about testing effects.

 True experimental design is best suited for explanatory research questions.


 True experiments require random assignment of participants to control and
experimental groups.
 Pretest posttest research design involves two points of measurement—one pre-
intervention and one post-intervention.
 Posttest only research design involves only one point of measurement—after the
intervention or treatment. It is a useful design to minimize the effect of testing effects
on our results.
 Solomon four group research design involves both of the above types of designs,
using 2 pairs of control and experimental groups. One group receives both a pretest
and a posttest, while the other receives only a posttest. This can help uncover the
influence of testing effects

You might also like