Discourse Text1
Discourse Text1
The traditional concern of linguistic analysis was the construction of sentences but in the
years (1970s) there was an increasing interest in analysing the way sentences work in sequence to
Two main approaches developed. Discourse Analysis focuses on the structure of naturally
and speeches. Text Analysis focuses on the structure of written language, as found in such ‘texts’ as
essays, notices road signs, and chapters. But this distinction is not clear-cut, and there were many
other uses of these labels. In particular, both ‘discourse’ and ‘text’ can be used in a much broader
sense to include all language units with a definable communicative function, whether spoken or
written. Some scholars talk about ‘Spoken and Written Discourse’ and others about ‘Spoken and
Written text’. In Europe, the term Text Linguistics is often used for the study of the linguistic
The search for larger linguistic units and structures has been pursued by scholars from many
disciplines. Linguists investigate the features of language that bind sentences when they are used in
sequence. Ethnographers and sociologists study the structure of social interaction, especially as
manifested in the way people enter into dialogue. Anthologists analyse the structure of myths and
folk-tales. Psychologists carry out experiments on the mental process underlying comprehension and
further contributions have come from those concerned with artificial intelligence, rhetoric,
These approaches have a common concern: they stress the need to see language as a dynamic,
social interactive phenomenon whether between speaker and listener, or writer and reader. It is
argued that meaning is conveyed not by single sentences but by more complex exchanges, in which
the participants’ beliefs and expectation, the knowledge they share about each other and about the
world, and the situation in which they interact, play a crucial part.
Of the many types of communicative acts, most study has been devoted to conversation,
seen as the most fundamental and pervasive means of conducting human affairs. These very
characteristics, however, complicate any investigation. Because people interact linguistically in such a
wide range social situations, on such a variety of topics, and with such an unpredictable set of
participant, it has proved very difficult to determine the extent to which conversational behaviour is
There is now no doubt that such a system exists. Conversation turns out, upon analysis, to be
a highly structured activity, in which people tacitly operate with a set of basic conversation. A
comparison has even been drowned with games such as chess: conversations, it seem, can be
thought of as having an opening, a middle, and an endgame. The participants make their moves and
often seem to follow certain rules as the dialogue proceeds. But the analogy ends: there A successful
conversation is not a game: it is no more than a mutually satisfying linguistic exchange. Few rules are
ever stated explicitly;)some exceptions are ‘don’t interrupt’, and ‘look at me when I talk to you’(.
Furthermore apart from in certain types of argument and debate, there are no winners.
For a conversation to be successful, in most social contexts, the participants need to feel they
are contributing something to it and are getting something out of it. For this to happen, certain
conditions must apply. Everyone must have an opportunity to speak: no one should be monopolizing
or constantly interrupting. The participants need to make their roles clear especially if there are
They need to have to develop a mutual tolerance, to allow for speaking or staying silent: when to
stay aloof or become involved. They need to develop mutual tolerance, to allow for speaker
ambiguity and listener inattention: perfect expression and comprehension are rare, and the success
of a dialogue largely depends on people recognizing their communicative weaknesses, through the
There is a great deal of ritual in conversation, especially at the beginning and end, and when
topics change. For example, people cannot simply leave a conversation at any random point, unless
they wish to be considered socially inept or ill -mannered. They have to choose their point of
departure(such as the moment when a topic changes(or construct a special reason for leaving.
Routines for concluding a conversation are particularly complex, and cooperation if it is not to end
abruptly, or in an embarrassing silence, the parties may prepare for their departure a long way in
advance, such as by looking at their watches or giving a verbal early warning. A widespread
convention is for visitors to say they must leave sometime before they actually intend to depart and
for the hosts to ignore the remark. The second mention then permits both parties to act.
The topic of the conversation is also an important variable. In general it should be one with
which everyone feels at ease: ‘safe topics’ between strangers in English situations usually include the
weather, pets, children, and the local context(e.g. while waiting in a room or queuing (. ‘Unsafe
topics’ include religious and political beliefs and problem of health. There are some arbitrary
divisions: asking what someone does for a living is generally safe;asking how much they earn is not.
Cultural variations can cause problems: commenting about the cost of the furniture or the taste of a
practice, as there is so much cultural variation. Silence, for example, varies in status. It is in an
embarrassment in English conversations, unless there are special reason)such as in moments of grief
(. However, in some cultures (e.g.Laps, Danes, the Western Apache(,it is quite normal for participants
to become silent. Often, who speaks and how much is spoken, depends on the social status of the
participants – for example, those of lower rank maybe expected to stay silent if their seniors wish to
speak. Even the basic convention of ‘one person speaks at a time ’maybe broken. In Antigua, for
example, the phenomenon of several people speaking at once during a whole conversation is a
In recent years, the phrase “conversation analysis” has come to be used as the name of a
The emphasis in previous sociological research had been deductive and quantitative focusing
In conversation analysis, one data thus consist of tape recordings of natural conversation and their
associated transcriptions. These are then systematically analysed to determine what properties
govern the way in which a conversation proceeds. The approach emphasizes the need for empirical,
inductive work, and in this is sometimes contrasted with’ discourse analysis’, which has often been
more concerned with formal methods of analysis (such as the nature of the rules governing the
structure of texts).
I.1.3Conversational Maxims
The success of a conversation depends not only on what speakers say but on their whole
approach to the interaction. People adopt a ‘cooperative principle when they communicative: they
try to get along with each other by following certain conversational maxims that underlie the
efficient use of language. Four basic maxims were proposed after (H.P.Grice,1975)
The maxim of quality states that speakers’ contribution to a conversation ought to be true they
should not say what they believe to be false anything for which they lack adequate evidence.
The maxim of quantity states that the contribution should be as informative as is required for the
purposes of the conversation. One should say neither too little nor too much.
The maxim of relevance states that contributions should clearly relate to the purpose of the
exchange.
The maxim of manner states that the contribution should be perspicuous -in particular, that it should
Other maxims were also proposed, such as “politeness and consistency”. The principle of relevance is
currently attracting most attention, as it was proposed as a fundamental explanatory principle for a
theory of human communication. Listeners will normally assume that speakers are following these
criteria. Speakers may of course break or flout these maxims- for example, they may lie, be sarcastic,
try to be different, or clever- but conversation proceeds on the assumption that they are not doing
so.
Listeners may then draw inferences from what speakers have said(the literal meaning of the
utterance) concerning what they have not said (the implications or’implicatures of the utterance ).
For example:
A: I need a drink
If B is adhering to the cooperative principle, several implicatures arise out of this dialogue for
example the Bell must be place that sells drinks and is open (as far as B knows); it must be nearby, if
B is not being cooperative(e.g. if he knows that the Bell is closed, or is the name of a greengrocer’s),
conversational maxims- as would happen if B genuinely did not know that the Bell was closed, and
accidentally went on a wild goose chase. In every day conversation, misunderstandings often take
place as speakers make assumptions about what their listeners know, or need to know that turn out
to be wrong. At such points, the conversation can break down and may need to be repaired, with the
participants questioning, clarifying, and cross-checking. The repairs are quickly made in the following
extract, through the use of such pointers as ‘told you’ and sorry.
But it is quite common for participants not to realize that there has been a breakdown and to
I.1.4Conversational Turns
Probably the most widely recognized conversational convention is that people take turns to
Some rules must be present; otherwise conversations would be continually breaking down
into a disorganized jumble of interruptions and simultaneous talk. In many formal situations, such as
committee meetings and debates, there are often explicit markers showing that a speaker is about to
yield the floor, and indicating who should speak next (‘I think Mr Smith will know the answer to that
question’). This can happen in informal situation too (‘what do you think, John’ ?), but there the turn-
The clues may be semantic (‘So anyway….’,‘Last but not least…..’); but more commonly the
speech itself can be modified to show that a turn is about to end – typically, by lowering its pitch,
loudness, or speed. Body movements and patterns of eye contact are especially important. While
speaking, we look at and away from our listener in about equal proportions; but as we approach the
Similarly, when talking to a group of people, we often look more steadily at a particular person to
Listeners are not passive in all of this. Here too, there are several ways of signalling that
someone wants to talk next. Most obviously, the first person in a group actually starts speaking, after
the completion of a turn, will usually be allowed to hold the floor. More subtly, we can signal that we
want to speak next by an observable increase in body tension- by leaning forward, or producing an
interrupt- a strategy which may be tolerated, if the purpose is to clarify what the speaker is saying,
I.1.5 Exchanges
generally begins by breaking an interaction down into the smallest possible units, then examining the
way these units are used in sequences. The units have been called ‘exchanges’, and in their minimal
form consist simply of an initiating utterance (I) followed by a response utterance (R), as in:
R: Two o’clock.
Two- part exchanges (sometimes called’ adjacency pairs’) are common place, being used in such
exchanges are also important; where the response is followed by an element of feedback (F).Such
What is of particular interest is to work out of the constraints that apply to sequences of
this kind. The teacher –feedback sequence would be inappropriate in many everyday situations:
On the other hand, with ingenuity it is often possible to imagine situations where such a
sequence could occur (e.g., if B were staring out of the window at the time). Discourse analysts are
always on the lookout for unexpected, but perfectly acceptable, sequences in context, such as:
A: goodbye.
B: Hello.
(used, for example, as A is leaving an office, passing B on his way in ). Many jokes too break discourse
A: Yes, I can.
(B made a challenge since he/she hears A saying I can, so he/she told him/her can you predict the
future).
Openings
Lovely day!
Got a match?
On-going checks
By the speaker:
Do you see?
By the listener:
Changing topic
Incidentally ……………
Where was I?
Concluding a topic:
So it goes
That’s life.
Ending
I.1.7 Misunderstandings
An important aim of discourse analysis is to find out why conversations are not always
Participants often operate with difference rules and expectations about the way in which the
conversation should proceed, something that is particularly evident when people of different cultural
backgrounds interact. But even within a cultural, different ‘rules of interpretation’ may exist.
It has been suggested, for example, that there are different rules governing the way in
which men and women participate in a conversation. A common source of misunderstanding is the
way both parties use head nods and ’mhm’noises while the other is speaking –something that
women do much more frequently than men. Some analysts have suggested that the two sexes mean
different things by this behaviour. When a woman does it she is simply indicating that she is listening
and encouraging the speaker to continue. But the male interprets it to mean that she is agreeing with
everything he is saying. By contrast, when a man does he is signalling that he does not necessarily
agree, whereas the woman interprets it to mean that he is not always listening. Such interpretations
are plausible, it is argued, because they explain two of the most widely reported reaction from
participants in cross-sex conversations-the male reaction of ‘It’s impossible to say what a woman
really thinks, and the female reaction of ‘you never listen to a world I say. (Maltz&Borker, 1982)
To call a sequence of sentences a ‘text’ is to imply that the sentences display some kind of
mutual dependence: they are not occurring at random. Sometimes the internal structure of text is
demonstrated, as in the network of relationships that enter into a literary work. In all cases, the task
of textual analysis is to identify the linguistic features that cause the sentence sequence to ‘cohere’-
something that happens whenever the interpretation of one future is dependent upon another
elsewhere in the sequence. The ties that bind a text together are often referred to under the heading
of cohesion (Halliday&Hasan, 1976). Several types of cohesive factor have been recognized:
Conjunctive Relation: What is about to be said is explicitly related to what has been said before,
Coreference: Features that cannot be semantically interpreted without referring to some other
feature in the text. Two types of relationship are recognized: anaphoric relation look backwards for
their interpretation, and cataphoric relations look forwards. Several people approached. They
seemed angry.
Ellipsis: A piece of structure is omitted and can be understood only from the preceding discourse.
Lexical Relationships: One lexical item enters into a structural relationships whit another: the rovers
Cohesive links go a long way towards explaining how the sentences of a text hang together,
but they do not tell the whole story. It is possible to invent a sentence sequence that is highly
Cats have four legs. The cat is on the mat. Mat has three letters.
A text plainly has to be coherent as well as cohesive, in that the concepts and relationships expressed
should be relevant to each other, thus enabling us to mark plausible inferences about the underlying
meaning.
Coherence
The textual world (what the text is about) is made up of concepts and relations. Coherence
concerns the way in which concepts and relations are mutually accessible and relevant. In other
words, a coherent text is one which is easy for us to understand because it is easy for us to make a
mental representation of it. Remember that it is possible for a text to be cohesive but not coherent.
Paragraphs are often highly cohesive entities. The cohesive ties can stand out very clearly if the
sentences are shuffled into a random order. It may even be possible to reconstitute the original
sequence solely by considering the nature of these ties, as in the following case:
5. Could it be a fox?
We can use graphological devices to indicate the paragraph of James Joyce’s shout story’ A
Painful Case. the sequences of pronouns, the anaphoric definite articles, and the repeated phrases
are the main cohesive features between the clauses and sentences.
Several of course refer back to previous parts of the story, these making this paragraph, out context,
impossible to understand.
He turned back the way he had come, the rhythm of the engine pounding in his ears. He
began to doubt the reality of what memory told him. He halted under a tree and allowed the
rhythmto die away. He could not feel hear near him in the DARNESS nor her voice touch
his ear. He waited for some minutes listening. He could hear NOTHING: the NIGHT was
Perfectly silent. He listened again: perfectly silent. He felt that: he was ALONE.
I.2.2Macrostructures
Not all textual analysis starts with small units and works from the bottom up. Some
approaches aim to make very general statements about the macrostructure of a text. In psychology,
for example, attempts have been made to analyses narratives into schematic outlines that represent
the elements in a story that readers remember. These schemata have been called story- grammars
setting, theme, plot, and resolution. The setting has three components: the characters, a location,
and a time. The theme consists of an event and a goal. The plot consists of various episodes, each
with its own goal and outcome, Using distinctions of this kind, simple stories are analysed into these
components, to see whether the same kinds of structure can be found in each. Certain similarities do
quickly emerge, but when complex narratives are studied, it proves difficult to devise more detailed
categories that are capable of generalization, and analysis becomes increasingly arbitrary.
Exercises
Maxims
Contestant: (silence)
(7) A: Of the three friends you invited to your party, who turned up?
B: John did.
(9) [in a testimonial about a pupil who is a candidate for a philosophy job]
Dear Sir, Mr X’s command of English is excellent and his attendance at tutorials.
Compare these two texts and identify the linking devices in the second text.
Text 1
Bobby was a Skye Terrier. Bobby roamed the streets of Edinburgh. Bobby met John Grey in
the 1850s. Grey worked as a night watchman in the Edinburgh police. Bobby kept John Grey
company. The winters in Edinburgh can be very cold. Grey fell sick with tuberculosis. Tuberculosis
was a fatal disease back in the 1800s. On 15 February 1858, Grey died.
Bobby followed John Grey to his grave at GreyfriarsKirkyard in the old part of Edinburgh.
Bobby did not leave the grave except for when he was hungry. Bobby did not leave the grave except
People started to notice the dog in the churchyard. People started worrying about Bobby. The City of
Edinburgh had decided that ownerless dogs should be shot. The city council bought a licence for
Bobby. Bobby could keep on watching his master’s grave. Bobby survived his master by 14 years. He
died in 1872. He was buried just inside the gate of the churchyard. He could not be buried together
Text 2
Bobby was a Skye Terrier roaming the streets of Edinburgh in the 1850s until he met John
Grey. Grey worked as a night watchman in the Edinburgh police and Bobby kept him company. The
winters in Edinburgh can be very cold and one day Grey fell sick with tuberculosis. This was a fatal
Bobby followed him to his grave at GreyfriarsKirkyard in the old part of Edinburgh and he did
not leave the grave except for when he was hungry or very cold.
People started to notice the dog in the churchyard and they started worrying about Bobby
because the City of Edinburgh had decided that ownerless dogs should be shot. However, the city
council bought him a licence and he could keep on watching his master’s grave. Bobby survived his
master by 14 years, and when he died in 1872 he was buried just inside the gate of the churchyard.
He could not be buried together with his master, since church ground is sacred.
Linking words
He didn’t get the job _____________ he arrived late for the interview.
She wanted to spend the holiday in Norway _____________ could not afford the ticket.
The public cheered for their team ______________ it did not play very well.
At the foot of the mountain there was a small, picturesque village __________ I lived.
The book was very good _____________ the film version was better.
Logical order
“Why don’t you have a salad with your hamburger instead”, she suggested.