2019 - Measurement of Attention
2019 - Measurement of Attention
In
Caprì, T., Fabio, R.A., Towey, G.E., & Antonietti, A. (Eds). Psychology Research
Progress. New York: Nova Science Publisher. ISBN: 978-1-53615-441-2
Chapter 2
MEASUREMENT OF ATTENTION
INTRODUCTION
*
Corresponding Author’s Email: [email protected].
2. VISUAL ATTENTION
target stimuli and distractor stimuli. The difficulty of the task is strictly
related to the complexity of the perceptive stimuli used in the different
versions of the test.
The Deux Barraget Test, created by Feuerstein (1979), is composed of
matrices consisting of three stimuli-targets (squares with adjacent segments
coming out of the vertex or the sides) alternating with distractor stimuli
belonging to five different categories (fillers, squares with different
orientations of the segments with respect to the targets). Each matrix is
composed of sheets containing 144 targets and 336 distractors, arranged in
random order. At the top of each page of the matrix, there are three targets
to be identified within the matrix (Antonietti, Balconi, & Fabio, 2000;
Fabio, Caprì & Romano, in press). This instrument can be administered in
classic version, or in dual-task conditions. In the dual-task condition, while
the subjects are identifying the stimuli-targets they are exposed to a second
task that needs a response, for example, listening to the recording of the
reading of some passages. In the Deux-Barrage test, the scores are the total
number of correct responses (number of stimuli-targets identified) and the
number of omissions (number of stimuli-targets omitted).
In a continuous performance task, the individual is asked to respond to
signals, presented in a repeated occurrence (Mackworth, 1951, Casagrande,
Curcio, Tricarico, Ferrara, Porcù & Bertini, 2000). Two factors are
particularly relevant: the ability to keep the activity focused on specific
stimuli and, at the same time, the ability to resist the distraction exerted by
other stimuli (distracting stimuli). Moreover, in the case of prolonged
attentional tasks, the setting is more complex. Kahneman (1973) argued
that the characteristics of information processing over a long time are
related to the ability to allocate resources strategically.
From the analysis of the attentional curves, it is possible to detect
substantial differences in the performance of the subjects. Precisely, in the
Deux Barrage test, a monotonic performance trend was observed, with a
gradual improvement in both parameters (number of correct responses) and
accuracy (number of omissions) (Antonietti, Balconi, & Fabio, 2000).
These paradigms show how people deal with external stimuli, and that
spatial attention can be directed voluntarily in specify directions.
subject is asked to match the color of the square to the word that describes
the color and, depending on whether the correct word is on the right or left
side, he responds by pressing the mouse button relative to the position. The
“congruent color-word condition” represents the third block; as in the first
block, at the center of the screen a word is placed, which names the name
of a color, which must be combined with another word placed on the right
or left. In this condition, the central word is colored, you can find the word
RED, written in red that must be connected to the word RED, however,
written in black. The “incongruent color-word condition”, represents the
last block; the subject is presented a word, for example RED, but it will be
written in green. On the two sides of the screen there are two other words,
one on the right and one on the left, which in our case will be RED and
GREEN, the task of the subject is to link the color of the written word in
the center to the word, either on the right or left, which describes that
color.
The parameters are the reaction times (RT). When the task is to say the
name of the color, RTs are faster for congruent stimuli than non-congruent
ones (Stroop, 1935).
Some authors state that interference occurs because color naming and
word reading are processed in parallel, so that when only one of the
automatic processes can be articulated, interference occurs when
generating the output response (Cohen, 2014). RTs will be higher for the
color naming conditions, as word reading has been shown to proceed
faster.
As outlined above, there are different versions of the test. For example,
a different version created by Delis, Kaplan, and Kramer (2001), (Delise-
Kaplan Executive Function System (D-KEFS) Color-Word Interference
subtest) presents the particularity of an Inhibition/Switching condition.
This condition includes a contextual cue presented in the box which
indicates that participants need to “switch” to reading the word instead of
naming the color. In a recent study that analyzes cognitive flexibility in
Generalized Anxiety Disorder (GAD), the D-KEFS test was used, adapting
it so as to include valence stimuli (Emotional Stoop Test) (Lee & Orsillo,
2014).
The Stroop paradigm has always been considered the “gold standard”
measure for studying focused attention within a competitive response
situation. Even so, attentional processes are not involved in a spatial
selection, as for example in the Posner paradigm, but rather they focus on
specific features, such as the meaning of the word or the font color. Even
though there is an involvement of attentional processes, the situation in
which people are asked to respond to competitive stimulus creates an
interference of executive control. For this reason, the Stroop test is often
used by neuroscientists to evaluate executive control.
In several studies conducted using the Stroop paradigm, it has been
shown that intellectually disabled people suffer most from the phenomenon
of interference. This seems to be due to the fact that the intellectually
disabled subjects have less attention resources at their disposal; they
present the phenomenon of cognitive inertia, a typical characteristic of
intellectual disability understood as “the tendency to persevere on a certain
behavior even when this is no longer adaptive to the situation” (Fabio,
2005; 2007a; 2007b; Fabio, Oliva, & Murdaca, 2011; Fabio, 2012a; 2012b;
Fabio, Caprì, Campana, Buzzai, 2018).
Since early neuropsychological studies, it has been demonstrated that
there is activation of the cingulate cortex during the Stroop task. These
findings induced Cohen (2014) to propose that he activation of the anterior
cingulate cortex (ACC) is associated with conflict monitoring.
In the Simon effect, RTs are faster when the position of the stimulus
and the position of key responses coincide (both on the right or the left)
than when they do not coincide (one on the left and the other on the right).
The Simon effect shows that an irrelevant feature of the stimulus (its
position in space) has an effect on the rapidity of the response provided on
the basis of the relevant characteristic. Probably, the automatic orientation
of attention, caused by the appearance of the stimulus at the periphery of
the visual field, leads to the encoding of its spatial position.
Even though the Stroop effect and the Simon effect seem to be very
similar, Liu, Banich, Jacobson, & Tanabe (2004; 2016) studied the two
effects, and demonstrated that there are differences in the brain activation.
It is probable that the differences are associated with the characteristics of
the tasks: stimulus–stimulus (Stroop) vs. stimulus–response (Simon) (Liu,
Banich, Jacobson, & Tanabe, 2004; 2016). The authors concluded that the
ACC activation is much more complex and does not refer only to the
conflict monitoring system (Cohen, 2014). Indeed, the ACC is associated
with different cognitive processes, such as executive functions, including
focused attention and response intention (Cohen, 2014).
attention at the local level. The Navon effect can be interrupted into two
independent effects. When the relevant level is local, the presence of an
inconsistent letter at the global level (not relevant causes a sharp slowdown
of the RT. When the relevant level is global, the presence of inconsistent
letters at the local level (not relevant) produces a much smaller or even null
interference. The parameter is the reaction time. The Navon effect occurs
because there is an asymmetric interference effect. When the relevant level
is local, the presence of an inconsistent letter at the global level slows RTs
(Navon, 1977). This effect induced Navon to affirm that perception
proceeds from global analysis to a more general perception. Global
analysis is vertiginous as it allows for the economization of processing
resources, thus, studies have demonstrated that if the patterns are
manipulated in terms of stimuli density, there are advantages for local
features.
2.3. Priming
a) strong triplets, when all the stimulus words are strongly correlated
with each other (fork, knife, glass), Figure 5;
b) weak triplets in which the stimulus to be identified is not related to
the other two words, which instead belong to the same conceptual
category, Figure 6;
for letters or symbols among distracting stimuli that are arranged either
randomly or in organized rows and columns.
Mesulaum and Weintraub’s Symbol Cancellation Test (SCT;
Mesulam, 1985) is composed of four test forms: verbal and non-verbal
stimuli, presented as random and structured arrays. In the non-verbal part,
examinees are presented with a sheet in which there is a series of familiar
(e.g., circles, triangles, and stars) or non-familiar symbols (60 targets for
each sheet). Participants are asked to draw a line through all the target
shapes that they can see. In the random symbol version, 60 target stimuli
are presented in a page among many distracting stimuli. The performance
is evaluated according to the number of correct targets that have been
identified and the time to complete the task.
Even though the test is prevalently used in neuropsychological
assessment, it often needs qualitative analysis. For example, Mesulam
(1985) found that healthy adults carrying out the test usually proceed using
a systematic approach, from left to right or starting in the first row and
continuing from right to left for the subsequent row. This means that it is
important to evaluate the strategy used in completing the task. Moreover,
the number of omissions, if concentrated only on one side of the sheet, can
give information about neurological disorders, such as hemiattentional
neglect (Lowery, Ragland, Gur, & Gur, 2004).
Studies have demonstrated that visual search tasks are influenced by
different variables such as age and educational level (Lowery, Ragland,
Gur, & Gur, 2004; Ostrosky-Solis, Ardila, Rosselli, Lo ́pez-Arango, &
Uriel-Mendoza, 1998). While it seems that age does not influence the
number of errors, the elderly need greater time in completing the task
(Dozzi Brucki, Mansur, Carthery-Goulart & Nitrini, 2011; Lowery, et al.,
2004) while young adults complete the test in under 2 min.
Mesulam (19885) affirm that there are differences in the way people
use scanning strategies, with erratic search strategies mainly used when the
stimuli are organized in unstructured arrays, while structured arrays are
associated to systematic search. The effect of educational level has been
analyzed in a study with participants having zero schooling against 3 years
of education.
Figure 8. Sample of the Benton Visual Form Discrimination Test (VFD; Item 9).
We can say that there are different types of sustained attention, and
sustained attention tasks imply different components of attentional
processes. It is very different if subjects are asked to respond to rare events
or implement complicated motor responses in a continuous way.
Nevertheless, many of these tasks implicate different attentional skills,
such as inhibitory components (that is executive attention), focused
attention, and sensorimotor processing (in terms of selection). Even though
these subcomponents are implicated in the evaluation of sustained
attention, there is a major difference. In particular, they are not defined in
terms of “temporal inconsistency of performance” and the ability to
maintain attention over time is associated with those “neural mechanisms
that enable iterative or recursive processes” (Cohen, 2014).
There are many disturbances that affect sustained attention, for
example difficulties are correlated with learning, behavioral, and emotional
difficulties in adolescence, sub-groups of attentional deficit hyperactive
disorder (Fabio, 2005; 2017; Fabio & Antonietti; 2012; Fabio & Caprì,
2015; 2017; Fabio, Castriciano, & Rondanini; 2015; Fabio & Urso, 2014;
Fabio, Caprì, Mohammadhasani, Gangemi, Gagliano, & Martino, 2018;
Hawkins, Hillyard, Luck, Mouloua, Downing, & Woodward, 1990;
Martino, Caprì, Castriciano, & Fabio, 2017; Mohammadhasani, Fabio,
Fardanesh, & Hatami, 2015; Mohammadhasani, Fardanesh, Hatami,
Mozayani, & Fabio, 2018) and neurodevelpmental disorder (Gangemi et al,
2018; Fabio, Gangemi, Caprì, Budden, & Falzone, 2018).
The tests that are designed to evaluate sustained attention, and in
particular vigilance components, are usually characterized by monotonous
tasks that last for a long period of time; there are many versions, but in
general we refer to these as continuous performance tests (CPT). Despite
the presence of many forms of the task (Greenberg, 1994), there are two
primary versions. In both tasks, participants are required to maintain
vigilance for a long period of time in a boring task, in front of a monitor
that displays stimuli that may be letters or figures. In one version of the
test, participants have to respond pressing a key, only in the presence of the
target (e.g., the letter X; discrimination version), while in the “successive
discrimination version” the objective is to respond to the target only after a
warning signal, that may be another letter of the alphabet. The two versions
imply different cognitive processes; for example, successive discrimination
tasks need memory activation.
There are two outcomes that can be considered: 1) accuracy, that
considers omission (miss) and commission (false-alarm) errors. 2) RTs.
Accuracy does not give realistic information because of the absence of
variability among the population (ceiling effect).
One of the ways to evaluate the effect of automation with the clock test
is to administer it several times over a period of time. Fabio, Pravettoni,
and Antonietti (2007), used the clock test to investigate automatic
processes. The results indicated a decrease in the number of errors and an
increase in speed as the subjects perform the same task of visual attention;
the subjects that best automate have higher cognitive rigidity indices than
those that automate more slowly.
The results of the study by Fabio, Pravettoni, Antonietti (2007) are in
line with a study by Szymura, Slabosz and Orzechowski (2001) which
found higher levels of rigidity in those with higher automation rates.
presented with arrays of stimuli that they need to look at, analyze and
select across locations (Cohen, 2014). Auditory attention is much more
difficult to assess; firstly, because the auditory system is independent from
the body position: people listen to auditory stimuli independently from
body orientation and the position of the ears. Secondly, while it is possible
to search for stimuli in a visual way over a period of time, auditory
information requires moment-by-moment vigilance; after the selection
between relevant and irrelevant sounds occurs, the sounds (e.g., words) are
available in the memory system.
Colin Cherry (1953), throughout the so-called cocktail party effect,
described very clearly how selection occurs in the auditory system. There
are situations in which people are exposed to a variety of stimuli,
nevertheless a person can focus on a speaker while tuning out several other
stimuli. Those with a higher priority receive attention.
Researchers have examined auditory attention components (e.g.,
orienting, detection and focus) using different tasks. The most elementary
form of evaluation requires listening to sounds and responding to specific
ones. These tasks have demonstrated that people can detect specific
frequencies, and when asked to orient attention to others, they can re-orient
attention and filter out only the frequencies of interest. Similar auditory
paradigms are based on the capacity of detecting particular sounds within a
noisy environment. This capacity emerges when, for example, we
concentrate attention on the sound of a car, and isolate this from the
background.
Auditory selective attention has been analyzed within other known
paradigms, such as Posner’s cuing paradigm; this test allows us to evaluate
the orienting component, and thus the auditory location. Many studies have
demonstrated that for valid and invalid cuing, auditory location does not
have significant effects (Driver, & Spence, 1994). Other studies that have
used different paradigms confirm that, for auditory attention, detection
based on location is not important. These results demonstrate that spatial
allocation of auditory attention is not linked to detection, as for visual
attention; nevertheless, if the task is cognitively more challenging, as in a
series of letters of the alphabet, about 120, randomly ordered, are presented
through the aid of a recorder. The subject is asked to raise his hand
whenever he hears, for example, the letter A (present 12 times) and then
lower it immediately afterwards. The measurement parameters are:
CPT in all its forms is often used for the diagnosis and treatment of
children and adults with ADHD (Riccio, Reynolds & Lowe, 2001).
Recently, there has been an increase of interest in the auditory form of the
CPT, as authors assume that it is a better measure of the concentration
defect and may give greater discriminating power over visual tests.
Moreover, event related potential studies have demonstrated differences
between ADHD and normal children, as a sign of auditory selective
attention deficits (Fabio, Caprì, Mohammadhasani, Gangemi, Gagliano, &
Martino, 2018; Mohammadhasani, Fardanesh, Hatami, Mozayani, &
Fabio, 2018).
Considering the lack of research that also takes into account the
auditory component of attention in subjects with ADHD, a study by Fabio,
Piran, and Antonietti (2005) investigated deficits in the inhibition of
responses using the CPT paradigm, in conditions of absence and presence
of interference. Through the use of a recorder, a series of letters of the
alphabet is presented in random order. One way to vary the difficulty of the
task is to have the subjects perform a double task; half of the subjects had
to listen to a series of 900 stimuli (consisting of a letter of the alphabet and
a numerical figure) in random order for a total of 30 minutes. The subjects
responded by raising their hands each time they heard the target. At a later
time, the subjects repeated the same test with the elements of the inverted
target (first the numerical digit and then the letter of the alphabet), while at
The results showed that subjects with ADHD experience falls in the
auditory surveillance test and become less accurate in the presence of
interference. Some authors (Shallice, Marzocchi, Coser, Del Savio,
Meuter, & Ruminati, 2002) argue that it is possible that CPT performance
is mainly influenced by impulsivity factors and the inhibition deficiency of
the answer.
ACPT can also be applied with distractor auditory stimuli. For
example, Broadbent (1958) studied auditory attention in the normal
population by analyzing what happens to auditory selective attention in the
presence of an irrelevant sound. In these cases, we speak of loss of
efficiency when an irrelevant sound is introduced during a task that
involves cognitive work, such as a task of attention, and a decrease in
performance is recorded.
CONCLUSION