The Dev of Imp and Exp Mem
The Dev of Imp and Exp Mem
com
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
EDITOR
Maxim I. Stamenov
(Bulgarian Academy of Sciences)
EDITORIAL BOARD
David Chalmers (University of Arizona)
Gordon G. Globus (University of California at Irvine)
Ray Jackendoff (Brandeis University)
Christof Koch (California Institute of Technology)
Stephen Kosslyn (Harvard University)
Earl Mac Cormac (Duke University)
George Mandler (University of California at San Diego)
John R. Searle (University of California at Berkeley)
Petra Stoerig (Universität Düsseldorf)
Francisco Varela (C.R.E.A., Ecole Polytechnique, Paris)
Volume 24
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
THE DEVELOPMENT
OF IMPLICIT AND
EXPLICIT MEMORY
CAROLYN ROVEE-COLLIER
Rutgers University
HARLENE HAYNE
University of Otago
MICHAEL COLOMBO
University of Otago
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
Table of Contents
Preface ix
CHAPTER 1
Background of the Problem 1
CHAPTER 2
Distinctions between Implicit and Explicit Memory 7
CHAPTER 3
Neuroanatomical Basis of Implicit and Explicit Memory 29
CHAPTER 4
The Jacksonian Principle and Memory Development 65
CHAPTER 5
Development of Implicit and Explicit Memory in Nonhuman Primates 83
CHAPTER 6
Development of Implicit and Explicit Memory in Human Infants 97
CHAPTER 7
Memory Dissociations in Human Infants 127
CHAPTER 8
Structural and Processing Accounts of Memory Dissociations 189
CHAPTER 9
Interactions between Implicit and Explicit Memories in Infants 231
CHAPTER 10
Epilogue 247
References 253
Author Index 291
Subject Index 303
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
Preface
This book evolved out of our concern that researchers who study memory in
human adults have constructed a scenario of the ontogeny of memory in
human infants without reference to empirical studies of infants themselves.
This scenario is largely based on studies of brain-damaged adults and brain-
lesioned animals and, because it fits neatly into a simple theoretical frame-
work, is intuitively appealing — but it is wrong.
At the core of this scenario are two assumptions — the assumption that
ontogeny recapitulates phylogeny and the assumption that the Jacksonian
first-in, last-out principle applies to the development and dissolution of
memory systems. In the first instance, evolutionary biologists have long
recognized the error of designating species as “higher” or “lower” when the
phylogenetic scale is tree-like and branching rather than linear. In the present
context, this error has translated into the flawed conclusion that human
newborns mirror lower animals and, over the course of development, come
increasingly to resemble higher animals, namely, adult (or verbal) humans. In
the second instance, studies with animals and human infants have failed to
find support for the Jacksonian principle. Moreover, predicting the sequence
of the normal dissolution of neural function from the sequence of its normal
development is different from predicting the sequence of the normal develop-
ment of neural function from the sequence of its normal dissolution. Taking
this one step further, predicting the sequence of the normal development of
neural function from the sequence of its abnormal dissolution due to localized
brain damage is radically — and conceptually — different. In fact, some have
even questioned whether the abnormal aging brain is an adequate model of the
abnormal developing brain.
Finally, those of us who study infants for a living have come to realize the
risk inherent in proclaiming that infants cannot do this or that. The study of
human infants is itself in its infancy. It has become increasingly apparent that
infants may do tomorrow what they seemed unable to do yesterday. Our new
insights have resulted from the time-honored principle of “building a better
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
x THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
CHAPTER 1
This chapter reviews the origins of the popular notion that two, functionally distinct
memory systems mature at different rates during the infancy period. Young infants are
thought to share with amnesics an early memory system which supports implicit memory;
the late memory system, which is damaged in amnesia and supports explicit memory, is
thought to mature late in the first year. Recent evidence from preverbal infants, however, is
inconsistent with this view.
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
2 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
The notion that adults possess multiple memory systems is not new. As long ago
as the late 1800s, philosophers, psychologists, psychiatrists, and neurologists
proposed different typologies of long-term memory (for a listing, see Hermann
1982). What is new, however, is the notion that these memory systems develop
hierarchically during the infancy period (Naito 1990; Schacter & Moscovitch
1984; Tulving 1983). How did this notion come about?
The origins of this belief are two-fold. For one thing, it seemed intuitively
unlikely that organisms as immature and helpless as infants could have
memories that in any way resemble our own. Over the years, this intuitive
belief has been reiterated in countless general psychology and child develop-
ment texts (e.g., Bjorklund, 2000). Secondly, the initial studies of infant
memory suggested that young infants were incapable of retention longer than
only a few seconds or minutes at most (e.g., Kagan & Hamburg 1981; Werner
& Perlmutter 1979). These findings, however, were derived from measures of
young infants’ looking behavior in a task that is an analog of the delayed-
matching-to-sample paradigm — the paradigm that researchers traditionally
used to study short-term memory in rats (Roberts 1972a, 1974), pigeons
(Grant & Roberts 1973; Roberts & Grant 1976; Shimp & Moffitt 1974;
Zentall 1973), and monkeys (D’Amato 1973; Jarrard & Moise 1971; Jarvik,
Goldfarb & Carley 1969). Findings that older infants can remember for longer
periods of time came from motorically more demanding tasks, such as object
search tasks, that very young infants are physically incapable of performing.
In the mid-1980s, the disparity between the memory performance of
younger and older infants was attributed to two different and functionally
distinct memory systems — the early memory system and the late memory
system — that were hypothesized to mature at different rates during the
infancy period (Schacter & Moscovitch 1984). The early memory system was
thought to mediate implicit memory — a primitive form of memory for
procedures or skills that is long-lasting and does not require conscious recol-
lection or intentional retrieval, whereas the late memory system was thought
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
BACKGROUND OF THE PROBLEM 3
The notion that two, functionally distinct memory systems underlie memory
performance initially arose when researchers found that amnesic and Korsakoff
patients exhibited excellent performance on some memory tests but were
severely impaired on others (Milner, Corkin & Teuber 1968; Scoville & Milner
1957; Warrington & Weiskrantz 1968, 1970). Subsequently, Tulving (1972,
1983) proposed that there were two types of memory systems — the semantic
memory system and the episodic memory system, citing data from amnesics in
support of this dichotomy. He characterized the semantic memory system as
appearing early in development and the episodic memory system as appearing
late in development. Schacter and Moscovitch (1984), however, were the first
to draw a direct parallel between the memory abilities of infants and amnesics,
suggesting that both possess only an early memory system. They assumed that
the memory system that was lost in amnesia matured late in the first year of life.
One year later, Graf and Schacter (1985) introduced the terms implicit and
explicit memory, which were then applied to the forms of memory supported by
the early and late memory systems, respectively. Since that time, the poor
memory performance of brain-damaged amnesics has generally been accepted
as the evidence that implicit and explicit memory emerge sequentially in infancy
during the course of normal development.
Ironically, researchers have only recently begun to study memory devel-
opment directly in infants. Two approaches are commonly taken to study the
development of implicit and explicit memory during the infancy period. The
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
4 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
BACKGROUND OF THE PROBLEM 5
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
CHAPTER 2
Distinctions between
Implicit and Explicit Memory
In this chapter, we review the characteristics that distinguish implicit from explicit memory
and describe the various tasks that have been used to measure them. Memory retrieval that
is accompanied by conscious awareness of having previously experienced an event is
widely regarded as the defining characteristic of explicit memory. Because animals and
very young children are thought to lack the capacity for conscious recollection, they are also
thought to be incapable of explicit memory. Therefore, we have focused particularly on the
potential contributions of conscious and unconscious processes to memory performance,
the means by which their contributions might be differentiated, and the significance of
consciousness for memory research with infants. Finally, we consider whether implicit
memory is a meaningful construct.
Popular support for two, functionally distinct memory systems initially arose
from empirical dissociations in the memory performance of amnesic adults
(Warrington & Weiskrantz 1968, 1970). Subsequently, similar dissociations
were observed in the memory performance of normal adults who were admin-
istered different instructional sets (Graf, Mandler & Haden 1982; Graf &
Schacter 1985). These memory dissociations have led to a number of impor-
tant inferences about functional dissociations in the cognitive processes and
neural mechanisms that might underlie them (see Chapter 3).
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
8 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
Implicit and explicit memory are descriptive concepts that originally referred
to different forms of memory as well as to the memory tests that were used to
measure them (Graf & Schacter 1985). The type of memory tapped by priming
tests was referred to as implicit memory, whereas the type of memory tapped
by recall and recognition tests was referred to as explicit memory. Graf and
Schacter cited three lines of evidence supporting a distinction between im-
plicit and explicit memory. First, healthy adults performed differently on
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
DISTINCTIONS BETWEEN IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY 9
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
10 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
Table 2.1. Frequently Cited Distinctions Between Implicit and Explicit Memory
Implicit Memory Explicit Memory
phylogenetically primitive phylogenetically more advanced
early maturing late maturing
spared in amnesia impaired in amnesia
unconscious conscious
nonepisodic episodic
general highly specific
abstract concrete
automatic controlled
involuntary voluntary
direct access indirect access
fast access slow access
all-or-none retrieval partial retrieval
no capacity demand limited capacity
weighted for object form weighted for object function
perceptually-based conceptually-based
context-free context-dependent
incidental intentional
nonassociative associative
temporally persistent time-limited
inflexible flexible
know remember
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
DISTINCTIONS BETWEEN IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY 11
The terms direct and indirect were introduced by Johnson and Hasher (1987)
to refer to different types of memory tasks and are often used interchangeably
with the terms explicit and implicit. Both explicit and direct tasks are generally
thought to require conscious recollection of a prior experience, whereas both
implicit and indirect tasks do not. Whereas memory performance on both
explicit and direct tasks is impaired in amnesia, memory performance on both
implicit and indirect tasks is not. The terms explicit and implicit, however, also
refer to different forms of memory, whereas the terms direct and indirect do not.
The tasks that are used in studies of explicit memory are familiar to
students of memory — recall, cued-recall, and recognition — and presumably
require the explicit remembering of a specific, prior study episode. According
to Mandler (1990), for example:
“Recall is defined as accessing (bringing to awareness) information about
something that is not perceptually present. By definition, recall is a conscious
product” (p. 486).
The various tasks that are used in studies of implicit memory are less familiar
(see Table 2.2). The implicit tasks that have figured prominently in studies of
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
12 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
Table 2.2. Implicit Memory Tasks Used with Amnesics and Normal (Instructed) Adults
Task Study
fragmented picture identification Warrington & Weiskrantz 1968
word completion Warrington & Weiskrantz 1970
short-term memory Baddeley & Warrington 1970
free association of related information Schacter 1986; Shimamura & Squire 1984
preference judgments Kunst-Wilson & Zajonc 1980
perceptual identification (words) Jacoby & Dallas 1981
word-stem completion Graf, Mandler & Haden 1982
lexical decision Moscovitch 1982
homophone spelling Jacoby & Witherspoon 1982
shadow-face identification Tulving 1984
anagram solution Dominowski & Ekstrand 1967
word completion with new associates Graf & Schacter 1985
serial reaction time Nissen & Bullemer 1987
classical conditioning Woodruff-Pak 1993
skill learning Schwartz & Hashtroudi 1991
perceptual identification (objects) Biederman & Cooper 1991, 1992
object decision Cooper, Schacter, Ballesteros & Moore 1992
precuing serial order Clayton, Habibi & Bendele 1995
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
DISTINCTIONS BETWEEN IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY 13
In these tasks, adults are asked to study a list of words and are then presented
with either word stems composed of the first three letters at the beginning of
the words (ass-) or word fragments with several letters missing (a - - a - - - -).
Their task is to complete the word stem or word fragment with the first word
that comes to mind. Some or all of the word stems can be completed with
words that previously appeared on the study list (assassin) or with a number of
other words (assiduous, assail, asset), but each word fragment has a unique
solution and can be completed only with a word from the prior study list.
Subjects tend to use words from the previously studied list to complete both
the stems or fragments.
These tasks are like the word-stem or word-fragment completion tasks except
that participants are tested with nonverbal materials. In these tasks, subjects
study a set of pictures and then are asked to perceptually identify them from
either fragmented or otherwise degraded versions of the same picture. In some
studies, researchers present picture fragments at a number of different levels
of perceptual completion, from most fragmented to most complete, via an
ascending method of limits until the fragment is correctly identified (e.g.,
Mitchell 1993; Snodgrass & Feenan 1990). The priming effect is measured as
the difference in accuracy of identifying old and new pictures.
In the object decision task, adults are shown line drawings of unfamiliar
objects, some that can actually exist in three-dimensional space (possible
objects), and some that cannot (impossible objects). During the initial expo-
sure phase, subjects are asked to judge the direction that each object faces.
During testing with some old and some new objects, they must decide as
rapidly as possible whether the test objects are possible or impossible. Pos-
sible objects that have been seen before show priming effects, but impossible
objects do not (Cooper, Schacter, Ballesteros & Moore 1992). The lexical
decision task, an older verbal version of this task, is the same except that the
study list contains either words or nonwords, and subjects must decide during
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
14 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
This task was introduced by Nissen and Bullemer (1987) and has since been
used with both normal and amnesic adults (for review, see Curran 1998) and
children (Meulemans, Van der Linden & Perruchet 1998). In this task, a cue
light is illumined on each trial under one of four keys that are arranged
horizontally. The subject’s task is to press the key above the light as rapidly as
possible. When the response key is pressed, the cue light goes off and, after a
brief delay, another cue light is illumined under a different key. The cue lights
are illumined in either a repeating or a random sequence, but subjects are not
informed when the sequence of lights is repeating. The RTs of subjects tested
on a repeating sequence decrease over successive trials, suggesting that sub-
jects are learning the sequence and anticipating which key will be signaled
next. However, a decrease in RT per se is not sufficient for inferring sequence
learning because subjects become practiced with nonsequential aspects of the
task as well (e.g., mapping the response key to the corresponding stimulus).
Therefore, the difference in RT on adjacent trial blocks on which the sequence
is repeating (S) and random (R) is usually taken as the primary measure of
sequence learning (R-S).
The clearest evidence that the SRT task measures implicit memory comes
from a within-subjects study in which students were given scopolamine, a
drug that induces temporary amnesia. Although the drug did not impair their
retention of the repeating-sequence SRT task, they were unable to press the
correct key when asked where the next light would appear — an explicit
memory task (Nissen, Knopman & Schacter 1987).
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
DISTINCTIONS BETWEEN IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY 15
as the unlearned reflex that the UCS had originally elicited. The percent of
trials on which a CR occurred indexes how well the subject learned that the CS
predicts the UCS. The development of the CR during acquisition can be
measured by either its occurrence on successive UCS-omission trials (e.g., on
every fifth trial, the CS is presented without the UCS) or its anticipatory
production in the interval between the CS and the UCS over successive trials.
In the latter case, the CR initially occurs immediately after the CS and then its
latency becomes progressively longer until it immediately precedes and par-
tially overlaps with the UCS. The gradual increase in the occurrence of the
anticipatory CR over trials is usually classified as an instance of procedural
memory or implicit memory. Retention is usually expressed in terms of
savings during reacquisition. Because the two events that become associated
need not be verbal, and verbal instructions are unnecessary for performing the
task, classical conditioning can be used with nonverbal subjects.
Woodruff-Pak (1993) found that the amnesic H.M. exhibited savings in
classical eyeblink conditioning to a tone 2 years after his original condition-
ing, reaching criterion in one-tenth the number of original acquisition trials,
even though he could not remember them.
Consciousness
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
16 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
DISTINCTIONS BETWEEN IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY 17
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
18 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
DISTINCTIONS BETWEEN IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY 19
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
20 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
complete the stems with words that were not presented earlier, however,
would produce the opposite result. To the extent that responding was under
their conscious control, Jacoby et al. assumed that subjects would select
against old words that they could actually recollect. Should they respond with
an old word, then their response would be attributed to unconscious influ-
ences, and the difference between responding with old and new words would
be taken as the measure of conscious recollection. The probability of using a
particular word as a completion for a stem when that word was not presented
during study serves as a baseline against which the effects of prior study can
be assessed. This baseline was subtracted from the calculated probability that
a word would automatically come to mind.
Although this procedure has revealed that recollection and familiarity
make statistically independent contributions to memory performance, there
are at least two problems with using the process-dissociation procedure to
assess the role of consciousness in memory processing. First, as Ratcliff and
colleagues pointed out:
“Process dissociation equations have two parameters (the probability of
recollection and the probability of familiarity being above a threshold), and
the equations are applied to only two data points in each experimental
condition. This means that the method will always produce estimates of two
components, even if the data were actually generated from a single process …
Furthermore, the interpretations of the components are valid only under the
process dissociation assumptions….Therefore, what is learned about con-
scious versus unconscious processes [in recognition] is theory dependent, and
process dissociation does not provide a theory-independent means of examin-
ing memory processes” (Ratcliff, Van Zandt & McKoon 1995, p. 359).
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
DISTINCTIONS BETWEEN IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY 21
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
22 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
Like Searle, Griffin proposed that mental experiences are directly linked to
neurophysiological processes which, he noted, are highly similar in all multi-
cellular animals.
Independence
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
DISTINCTIONS BETWEEN IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY 23
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
24 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
DISTINCTIONS BETWEEN IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY 25
Other researchers have expressed concern about the grab-bag nature of implicit
memory (Johnson & Hirst 1993; Roediger 1990a; Roediger, Srinivas & Weldon
1989a; Schwartz & Hashtroudi 1991; Willingham & Preuss 1995). A major
source of concern, for example, is the repeated finding of parallels in memory
performance on tests of implicit memory and explicit memory and functional
dissociations in memory performance on different tests of implicit memory. As
reviewed earlier, theorists have attributed the parallels to the contribution of
conscious recollection on implicit memory tests and to the contribution of
unconscious influences on explicit memory tests. The finding of dissociations
between tasks of implicit memory, however, is particularly problematic for the
construct validity of implicit memory. On the surface, for example, word-
fragment completion and perceptual word-identification tasks would seem to be
highly related, but researchers have found that memory performance on these
two tasks is not only uncorrelated (Perruchet & Baveux 1989) but is stochasti-
cally independent (Hayman & Tulving 1989; Witherspoon & Moscovitch
1989).
Some of the clearest evidence that different implicit tasks are functionally
independent has surfaced in studies comparing performance on the serial
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
26 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
reaction time (SRT) task with performance on other tasks of implicit memory.
Healthy adults exhibit a decrease in RTs over successive trials when trained
with a repeating sequence (Willingham, Nissen & Bullemer 1989), as do
patients with Alzheimer’s disease (Knopman & Nissen 1987) and Korsakoff
syndrome (Nissen & Bullemer 1987). Like healthy adults, Korsakoff patients
also retain their memory of the repeating sequence for 1 week (Nissen,
Willingham & Hartman 1989). In contrast to their normal learning and
memory on the repeating-sequence SRT task, however, Korsakoff patients’
ability to learn a tactual style maze in which all blind alleys are blocked is
significantly impaired (Nissen et al. 1989). Because both the maze task and
the SRT task require the learning of a spatial sequence and, in normal adults,
performance on these tasks is facilitated by prior training, both are thought to
measure implicit memory. These data, then, reveal a memory dissociation
between two tasks of implicit memory.
Schwartz and Hashtroudi (1991) similarly found a dissociation between
implicit memory tasks. In Experiment 1 of their study, college students
exhibited skill learning in partial-word identification and inverted reading
tasks but not in a word-fragment completion task, but their amount of priming
in all tasks was the same. In Experiment 2, they tested priming and skill
learning with degraded words in the partial-word identification task. The
amount of priming was the same whether students had acquired the skill of
identifying degraded words or not. Importantly, the correlation between skill
improvement over trials and priming performance was negligible whether
subjects were tested immediately (r = .01) or after 1 week (r = .06). In
Experiment 3 of their study, they hypothesized that students might be more
skilled in learning to identify degraded words to which they had been
preexperimentally exposed more often (high-frequency words) than words
that occurred in language less often. If priming were unrelated to skill learn-
ing, however, then the frequency of occurrence of the words should not affect
it. They found that skill learning was significantly affected by word frequency,
but the amount of priming was not.
The large number of implicit memory tests that were functionally inde-
pendent of each other prompted researchers to elaborate different mechanisms
for explaining them. Schacter (1990), for example, proposed that priming
effects in different implicit memory tasks are best understood in terms of a
class of distinct memory modules or subsystems — the visual word form
system, the structural descriptions system — comprising a perceptual repre-
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
DISTINCTIONS BETWEEN IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY 27
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
28 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
Conclusions
Despite the large number of recent books devoted to implicit memory, this
form of memory is still poorly understood. Willingham and Preuss (1995)
argued that because implicit memories are not supported by common brain
structures and do not use common processes and representations, implicit
memory is not a coherent system. Moreover, there is no common attribute that
links different implicit memory tasks such as priming, motor skill learning,
classical conditioning, mirror reading, perceptual identification, and so forth.
Johnson and Hirst (1993) made the same observation about procedural
memory tasks, namely, that they share no obvious common characteristics
except that amnesics cannot perform them. Changing the procedural/declara-
tive nomenclature to nondeclarative/declarative (Squire 1992a) did not alter
this basic problem. Nondeclarative memory remains a miscellaneous collec-
tion of tasks that are linked simply by the fact that they can be performed
without the neural structures that are required to perform declarative memory
tasks. In other words, nondeclarative tasks are, simply, not declarative.
Although the absence of conscious awareness is thought to characterize
performance in implicit memory tasks, there is no agreement about how to
define it. Moreover, because the function of conscious awareness is unknown,
it is impossible to specify how a memory system that is based on conscious
awareness differs functionally from one that is not (Willingham & Preuss
1995). As a result, any classification scheme that differentiates memory
systems on the basis of the presence or absence of conscious awareness fails
Sherry and Schacter’s (1987) criterion, the functional incompatibility of op-
erations (see Chapter 8), for establishing a new memory system.
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
CHAPTER 3
We begin this chapter with an overview of the early studies of human amnesia that gave rise
to the notion of two, dissociable memory systems. We review similar evidence for two
dissociable memory systems in nonhuman animals — primarily, monkeys and rats. We then
discuss evidence for the neural substrates that are believed to underlie implicit and explicit
memory. We note that the concept of multiple memory systems emerged predominantly
from studies in which damage to the medial temporal lobe impaired explicit memory but not
implicit memory. Therefore, we present recent evidence that damage to the striatal system
impairs implicit memory but not explicit memory and consider the implications of this
evidence for multiple memory systems.
For quite some time, damage to the medial temporal lobes (MTL) in humans
has been known to produce severe memory impairments. The classic and
frequently cited example of the effects of such lesions is the patient H.M., who
underwent bilateral resection of the MTL (hippocampus, amygdala, and sur-
rounding tissue) in an effort to relieve intractable epilepsy that did not respond
to conventional anticonvulsive medication (Scoville & Milner 1957). Al-
though H.M.’s seizures were somewhat alleviated by the operations, he was
left with a profound memory impairment that persists to this date (Corkin
1984; Gabrieli, Cohen & Corkin 1988; Milner 1972; Milner et al. 1968).
In the original 1957 paper, most of H.M.’s memory impairments were
described anecdotally in terms of his postoperative failures to give accurate
dates, his age, or to remember that he had been talking with a particular
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
30 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
individual just minutes earlier. For example, H.M. was operated upon in
September, 1953, when he was 27 years old. When asked during a psychologi-
cal examination 19 months later (in April, 1955) for the current date and his
current age, H.M. responded that the current date was March, 1953, and that
he was 27 years old. His response suggested that he had no memory for events
subsequent to his operation. In fact, so serious were his memory impairments
that after the operation,
“this young man could no longer recognize the hospital staff nor find his way
to the bathroom, and he seemed to recall nothing of the day-to-day events of
his hospital life” (Scoville & Milner 1957, p. 14).
According to Scoville and Milner, H.M. and patients with similar extensive
damage to the medial temporal lobe appear to “forget the incidents of their
daily life as fast as they occur” (p. 15).
Despite his profound memory impairment, H.M. performed normally on
tests of intelligence, reasoning, and perceptual abilities (Milner et al. 1968). He
was also able to remember a list of numbers and words if he was not distracted
during the retention interval (Scoville & Milner 1957). Although his memory
for events of his childhood appeared to be quite normal, H.M. did have difficulty
remembering information that occurred within the 3 years prior to his surgery.
In current terminologies, his memory deficits were characterized both by a
severe anterograde amnesia (difficulty in forming new memories) and a limited
retrograde amnesia (memory impairments for events prior to the operation).
The characteristics of H.M.’s memory impairments have since been observed
in many other patients with medial temporal lobe damage (Cummings,
Tomiyasu, Read & Benson 1984; Victor & Agamanolis 1990; Victor,
Angevine, Mancall & Fisher 1961). According to the convention we have
adopted in this book, H.M. is said to suffer from impairments in explicit memory.
It soon became apparent that damage to the MTL did not produce a global
amnesia because some memories were affected by the damage, but others
were not. In particular, tasks that required certain motor skills were unaffected
by MTL lesions. Milner (1962) made the first observation of one such spared
memory. She noted that H.M. showed a steady improvement on a mirror-
drawing task which required drawing within the outline of a double star while
looking at a reflection of the star and his hand in a mirror (see Figure 3.1A).
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
NEUROANATOMICAL BASIS OF MEMORY SYSTEMS 31
Not only did H.M. show improvement within a session, but he also showed
retention across sessions, beginning each day’s session at roughly the level he
had finished on the previous day. In spite of near-perfect retention that
spanned 24 hours, he had no recollection of having performed this task on the
preceding day.
The finding of spared memory with respect to the mirror-drawing task
was the first of a series of such descriptions that ultimately formed the
foundation for the idea of dissociable memory systems. Other motor skill tasks
were soon described that also appeared unaffected by MTL lesions. For
example, H.M. showed both improvement within a session and retention
across sessions in the acquisition of a rotary pursuit task (see Figure 3.1B) in
which he had to maintain a metal stylus pen in contact with a rotating metal
disc (Corkin 1968). One advantage of this study over the mirror-drawing
study was that H.M.’s performance was compared to that of a control group of
normal subjects. Although his time-on-target score did improve steadily, his
overall performance was inferior to that of the normal control group. Similar
findings were reported for acquisition and retention of a bimanual tracking
task and a bimanual tapping task, although his performance in the tapping task
more closely approximated that of the normal control group (Corkin 1968).
Based on her early findings, Milner (1965) suggested that the types of
memories spared by MTL damage were those that involved learning a motor
skill. Subsequent studies, however, showed that MTL damage also spared
performance on tasks in which a motor component was not the defining
feature. Warrington and Weiskrantz (1968), for example, showed that patients
with alcoholic Korsakoff’s amnesia, a form of amnesia that produces memory
impairments roughly similar to MTL amnesia, were able to learn the Gollin
incomplete-pictures task. This task consists of a series of five pictures ranging
from very fragmented to complete. Subjects are first shown the most frag-
mented rendition and then progressively less fragmented renditions until they
are able to identify the picture. On the first exposure to the task, correct
identification is likely to occur only when the complete rendition is shown.
With practice, however, the subject is able to identify the picture from increas-
ingly fragmented renditions. In line with the findings from motor skill tasks,
amnesics improved their performance within a session and exhibited near-
perfect retention across sessions (see Figure 3.1C). Also in line with their
performance on other skill-based tasks, their overall error rate was signifi-
cantly higher than that of the control group across the 3 days of testing.
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
32 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
Figure 3.1. Spared memories in patients with medial temporal lobe damage or alcoholic
Korsakoff’s syndrome. (A) Mirror drawing task. The subject had to draw within the outline
of the figure of a star (redrawn from Milner 1965). (B) Rotary pursuit task. The subject had
to keep a metal pen in contact with a moving metal target (redrawn from Corkin 1968).
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
NEUROANATOMICAL BASIS OF MEMORY SYSTEMS 33
Figure 3.1 (continued) (C) Gollin incomplete-pictures task. The subject saw a series of five
pictures that ranged from degraded to complete. The most degraded picture was presented
first, followed by progressively less degraded pictures, until the subject could identify the
picture (redrawn from Warrington and Weiskrantz 1968). (D) Mirror reading task. The
subject read a list of three mirror-drawn words. Some words appeared more than once
within a session (repeated word triads), whereas others did not (nonrepeated word triads)
(redrawn from Cohen and Squire 1980).
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
34 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
NEUROANATOMICAL BASIS OF MEMORY SYSTEMS 35
tion was critical to the outcome of the experiment. On the word-stem comple-
tion test, amnesics performed like normal controls, whereas on the cued-recall
test, amnesics’ performance was impaired relative to that of normal controls.
In summary, patients with MTL lesions and patients with disorders and
injuries that damage structures related to the MTL suffer from severe memory
impairments. Despite these impairments, they display a variety of forms of
spared memories, ranging from normal motor skills to intact performance on
perceptual and even some cognitive tasks. This simple dissociation led Cohen
and Squire (1980) to distinguish between knowing that, which is impaired
following MTL damage, and knowing how, which is spared following MTL
damage. Over the years, this distinction has taken on many different labels.
These are reviewed in Chapter 8. For our present purposes and for ease of
exposition, we refer to knowing that as explicit memory and knowing how as
implicit memory.
Shortly after Scoville and Milner (1957) reported severe memory impairments
in humans with MTL damage, attempts were made to develop an animal
model of MTL amnesia. The main problem in this endeavor was expressed
cogently by Orbach, Milner and Rasmussen (1960) when they stated:
“the requirements of the tests applied to man are difficult to adapt to behavior
that is within the repertoire of the monkey” (p. 248).
Nevertheless, over the years, numerous tasks have been used to study the
effects of MTL lesions in nonhuman animals. Because later sections of this
chapter rely heavily on an understanding of these tasks, a brief description of
them is in order. What follows is not an exhaustive list of the tasks that have
been used to assess the effects of brain damage in animals but, rather, descrip-
tions of the tasks used most frequently.
Visual Discrimination. In a visual discrimination task, the animal must
learn which of the two stimuli is associated with reward (see Figure 3.2A). In
a typical session consisting of numerous trials, the same pair of stimuli is
presented on each trial, and the same stimulus always serves as the rewarded
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
36 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
Figure 3.2. The three primary tasks that have been used with nonhuman animals to study
the effects of damage to the medial temporal lobe on memory performance. (A) Visual
discrimination. The same two stimuli are presented on every trial. The animal must respond
to the same stimulus (A) to obtain a reward (+). Responses to the nonrewarded stimulus (B)
result in punishment (–). (B) Visual delayed nonmatching-to-sample (DNMS). Each trial
consists of two parts. The sample stimulus is presented and then is removed from view for
a delay period. At the end of the delay period, two comparison stimuli are presented. The
animal must respond to the novel stimulus to obtain a reward. Responses to the familiar
stimulus are punished. (C) Visual concurrent discrimination (CD). The animal is trained
with eight pairs of stimuli. Within each pair, the same stimulus is always rewarded, whereas
the other stimulus is not. The animal must learn which stimulus of each pair is the rewarded
stimulus. Each of the eight pairs of stimuli is presented a number of times within each
session.
(positive) stimulus. Of all the tasks used in lesion studies, the discrimination
task is the easiest for animals to learn. Although making the physical identity
of the stimuli more similar increases both the difficulty of the task and the
length of time required to solve it, the acquisition of discrimination tasks is
typically measured in terms of days.
Visual Delayed Nonmatching-to-Sample (DNMS). In a visual DNMS
task, the animal must remember which stimulus it saw most recently. The
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
NEUROANATOMICAL BASIS OF MEMORY SYSTEMS 37
DNMS task has become a standard test of recognition memory in animals and
is perhaps the most frequently used task in studies examining the effects of
brain lesions on memory in monkeys. The procedure is quite simple (see
Figure 3.2B). At the start of a trial, a stimulus is presented to an animal and
then is removed from view for the duration of a delay interval. At the end of
the delay, two stimuli are presented to the animal — one that is the same as the
sample and one that is different. The animal must choose the stimulus that is
different from the sample stimulus, that is, the nonmatching stimulus. Each
session consists of numerous trials in this format. Although the task can also
be structured so that the animal is required to choose the matching stimulus,
the nonmatching task is the preferred version in lesion studies, most likely
because it taps the monkey’s predisposition for novelty (Mishkin & Delacour
1975).
Acquisition of the DNMS task takes considerably longer than acquisition
of a visual discrimination task and is typically measured on a scale of weeks
rather than days. The acquisition of the DNMS task, however, is not of
primary interest in brain lesion experiments. The power of the DNMS proce-
dure is that the delay between the sample and comparison stimuli can be
varied, thereby allowing the experimenter to measure the effects of brain
lesions on short-term retention. This is the context in which the DNMS
procedure is used most often. With training, monkeys can eventually perform
at above-chance levels with delays as long as 40 min between the sample and
the comparison stimuli.
Visual Concurrent Discrimination (CD). The CD task is a complex
version of the visual discrimination task described earlier. Recall that in a
visual discrimination task, the animal obtains a reward by responding to the
positive stimulus of one pair of stimuli that are presented repeatedly within a
session. In the visual CD task, eight pairs of stimuli are presented repeatedly
within a session, and the animal must learn which stimulus in each pair is
associated with reward (see Figure 3.2C). Naturally, given so many pairs of
stimuli, acquisition of a CD task takes longer than acquisition of a visual
discrimination task and also longer than the acquisition of a DNMS task.
Radial Arm Maze. The radial arm maze task is one of the most frequently
used procedures to study the neural basis of learning and memory in rats.
Developed by Olton and Samuelson (1976), the task taps the amazing spatial
abilities of rats. The maze consists of a central arena from which radiate eight
arms (see Figure 3.3A). The rat is placed inside the arena and allowed to
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
38 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
collect a piece of food placed at the end of each arm. The optimal strategy is to
enter an arm once and then not revisit that arm. Revisiting an arm that has
already been entered is scored as an error. Each session consists of a single
trial in which the rat attempts to retrieve all eight pieces of food in the maze.
Rats quickly learn to retrieve all the food after entering only eight (no errors)
or nine (1 error) arms. They can achieve such levels of performance within 1
to 2 weeks.
Morris Water Maze. Like the radial arm maze, the Morris water maze
(Morris 1981, 1984) also taps the spatial abilities of rats. Rats are placed in a
circular pool filled with water rendered opaque by the addition of milk (see
Figure 3.3B). Just below the water level in a constant position in the tank is a
hidden platform. The rats are placed in the water in various quadrants of the
tank and, in a short period, learn to locate and escape to the platform. A session
consists of approximately 10 trials. Although rats will reach asymptotic levels
of performance within a few days of testing, considerable learning occurs over
the first 10 trials of the first session.
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
NEUROANATOMICAL BASIS OF MEMORY SYSTEMS 39
Figure 3.3. The two primary tasks that have been used with rats to study the effects of
damage to the hippocampus on memory performance. (A) Radial arm maze. The apparatus
consists of a central arena from which radiate eight arms. The rat is placed within the central
arena and allowed to forage for food that is placed in a recessed well (small circles) at the
end of each arm. The optimal strategy is to enter an arm once and then not return to that arm.
(B) Morris water maze. The apparatus is a tank that contains water that has been rendered
opaque by the addition of milk. Rats are placed in the tank at different positions against the
wall and learn to escape to a hidden platform (small circle) that is submerged just below the
water level.
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
40 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
NEUROANATOMICAL BASIS OF MEMORY SYSTEMS 41
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
42 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
Figure 3.4. The effects of damage to components of the medial temporal lobe on antero-
grade and retrograde memory in monkeys. (A) Anterograde memory impairments. The
effects of damage to the amygdala (A), hippocampus (H), or combined amygdala-hippo-
campus (A-H) damage on delayed nonmatching-to-sample performance in monkeys. The
dotted line represents chance levels of performance (redrawn from Mishkin, 1978). (B)
Retrograde memory impairments. The effects of combined amygdala-hippocampus damage
on retention of visual discriminations that were learned prior to surgery (redrawn from
Zola-Morgan and Squire, 1990).
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
NEUROANATOMICAL BASIS OF MEMORY SYSTEMS 43
hand). Like humans, however, monkeys with MTL damage showed the oppo-
site effect, with greater forgetting of discriminations that had been learned in
the recent past. Similar temporally-graded retrograde amnesia curves have
been obtained in studies with rats (Bolhuis, Stewart & Forrest 1994; Cho,
Kesner & Brodale 1995; Kim & Fanselow 1992; Ramos 1998; Winocur
1990). In summary, both anterograde and retrograde memory impairments
that have been noted in humans with MTL damage have been replicated in
monkeys and rats with similar lesions.
Recall that humans with MTL damage, although densely amnesic, are never-
theless able to learn certain motor, perceptual, and even problem-solving
tasks. The same appears to be true for monkeys with MTL lesions. Malamut,
Saunders and Mishkin (1984) showed that monkeys with combined damage to
the hippocampus and amygdala were not impaired in the acquisition of a 24-
hour CD task but were impaired on a DNMS task. The claim of spared
performance on the 24-hour CD task was repeated frequently in numerous
books and articles that followed the publication of this paper.
Recall that monkeys with MTL damage are impaired in the acquisition of
an eight-pair CD task. The 24-hour CD task is procedurally almost identical to
the eight-pair CD task with two main exceptions: First, in the 24-hour CD
task, monkeys are trained with 20 pairs of stimuli instead of just eight. Second,
in the 24-hour CD task, each pair of stimuli is presented only once in a session,
whereas in the eight-pair CD task, the pairs of stimuli are presented numerous
times within a session.
In the Malamut et al. (1984) study, seven monkeys with combined
damage to the hippocampus, amygdala, and adjacent tissue and four unoper-
ated controls were trained with 20 pairs of stimuli until they satisfied a
criterion of 90 correct responses in 100 trials in five consecutive sessions.
After they completed training with the first set of 20 pairs of stimuli (set A),
they were trained in a similar fashion with a second set of 20 pairs of stimuli
(set B). The lesioned monkeys were mildly impaired on the acquisition of the
24-hour CD task on set A but not on set B. Moreover, according Malamut et
al., the monkeys who showed an impairment on either set A or set B had
incidental damage to the visual association areas of the brain (area TE), which
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
44 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
led the authors to discount damage to the amygdala and hippocampus as the
source of the impairment.
Several aspects of this paper argue against the conclusion that MTL
damage did not affect performance on the 24-hour CD task. Figure 3.5 pre-
sents data for the unoperated control monkeys on the left, for the operated
monkeys that sustained no damage to area TE in the middle, and for the
operated monkeys that sustained additional damage to area TE on the right. In
Figure 3.5, data for monkeys that sustained damage to area TE are arranged
from left to right in order of the increasing amount of damage to this area.
Consider the acquisition of set A (see Figure 3.5A). Although monkey L2–3,
who had the greatest damage to the visual areas, took the longest to learn the
task, a closer inspection of this figure reveals that monkeys L2–2 and L3–2,
who sustained no damage to the visual areas, also performed quite poorly.
Furthermore, monkey L3–1, who learned faster than any other animal (includ-
ing the unoperated animals), had considerable sparing of the amygdala and,
most likely, of the tissue adjacent to the amygdala. In fact, researchers now
recognize that the tissue adjacent to the amygdala — the perirhinal cortex — is
critical for the successful performance of a number of visual memory tasks.
Therefore, monkey L3–1, for whom this critical region was probably spared,
should not be included with the animals that sustained combined damage to
the amygdala and hippocampus.
Despite these problems, it is fair to say that the impairment in animals
with amygdala-hippocampal damage that did not sustain damage to area TE
was relatively mild, and they were eventually able to relearn the task to
criterion. Furthermore, as the data for set B clearly show (see Figure 3.5B),
there was very little difference between operated and control animals in terms
of the number of sessions to learn the task. Therefore, this study has been used
as evidence of spared memory following damage to the MTL in monkeys,
especially because the same operated monkeys were seriously impaired on a
DNMS task.
A second point, however, is also worth noting, namely, that the 24-hour
CD task is easier for monkeys to learn than the eight-pair CD task. On
average, monkeys require almost three times as many trials to learn an eight-
pair CD task (i.e., 550 trials) than to learn a 24-hour CD task (i.e., 195 trials).
The difficulty is even more apparent when one considers how many pairs the
animal is required to learn in each task; monkeys require seven times as many
trials to learn each pair in the eight-pair CD task (i.e., 69 trials) compared to
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
NEUROANATOMICAL BASIS OF MEMORY SYSTEMS 45
Figure 3.5. The effects of combined damage to the amygdala and hippocampus (A-H) on
the 24-hour concurrent discrimination (CD) task. Animals with damage to the visual cortex
are arranged from left to right in terms of increasing damage to the visual cortex (redrawn
from Malamut, Saunders and Mishkin 1984). (A) Set A: Performance with the first set of 20
pairs of stimuli. (B) Set B: Performance with the second set of 20 pairs of stimuli.
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
46 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
the 24-hour CD task (i.e., 9.8 trials). For whatever reason, because the eight-
pair CD task is more difficult for monkeys to learn than the 24-hour CD task,
damage to the MTL might reasonably be expected to impair their performance
on the eight-pair CD task before impairing it on the 24-hour CD task.
The effects of MTL lesions on monkeys’ performance on motor tasks are
more similar to the spared performance that has been seen following MTL
lesions in humans, and they are certainly more convincing than the effects of
MTL lesions on monkeys’ acquisition of the 24-hour CD task. Zola-Morgan
and Squire (1984) trained monkeys with medial temporal lobe lesions on both
a Barrier motor-skill task and a Lifesaver task. In the Barrier task, the mon-
keys had to move a breadstick through three rows of a vertically arranged
wooden barrier. In the Lifesaver task, monkeys had to remove a Lifesaver
candy from a horizontal metal rod that contained a 90-deg bend. Monkeys
with MTL lesions showed normal acquisition of both tasks (see Figure 3.6).
Furthermore, in the case of the Lifesaver task, both control and lesioned
animals showed similar levels of retention of the task following a 1-month
pause in testing.
Thus, monkeys, like humans, do show some spared memories following
damage to the MTL.
Humans have been tested on most of the tasks that are sensitive to MTL
lesions in monkeys, and the results are generally consistent with the monkey
data. Amnesic patients, for example, are impaired in performing the DNMS
task and show the same accelerated delay-dependent decay rates as monkeys
with similar damage (Sidman, Stoddard & Mohr 1968; Squire, Zola-Morgan
& Chen 1988). Human amnesics are also impaired in the acquisition of an
eight-pair CD task (Oscar-Berman & Bonner 1985; Oscar-Berman & Zola-
Morgan 1980; Squire et al. 1988). The only inconsistency between the human
and nonhuman data is in the acquisition of a 24-hour CD task: Monkeys are
generally not impaired on this task, whereas humans are.
Problems with Tasks Used with Nonhumans. The primary problem with
the tasks that are used with animals is that they tend to be defined as either
explicit or implicit according to whether they are impaired by MTL lesions or
not. The DNMS task, for example, is defined as an explicit memory task
because it is sensitive to MTL damage, whereas the 24-hour CD task is defined
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
NEUROANATOMICAL BASIS OF MEMORY SYSTEMS 47
Figure 3.6. The effects of combined damage to the amygdala and hippocampus (A-H) on
two motor skill tasks (redrawn from Zola-Morgan and Squire, 1984). (A) Barrier task. The
monkeys were required to maneuver a breadstick through three rows of vertically arranged
wooden pegs. (B) Lifesaver task. The monkeys were required to maneuver a Lifesaver
candy off a metal rod that contained a 90-deg bend.
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
48 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
Mishkin’s Model. Although the MTL may be critical for normal memory, it is
clear that memories are not stored in this area of the brain. This conclusion
follows from the fact that H.M. and other patients with similar damage to the
MTL, while suffering from severe anterograde amnesia and perhaps some
retrograde amnesia, have some preserved memories for events that occurred
prior to the operation or damage. What exactly, then, does the MTL contribute
to the memory process? And how does it accomplish this feat? Mishkin (1982)
proposed that recognition memory (i.e., explicit memory) is mediated by a
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
NEUROANATOMICAL BASIS OF MEMORY SYSTEMS 49
Figure 3.7. Neural substrates of explicit memory. (A) Mishkin (1982) model. In this model,
both the amygdala and the hippocampus contribute equally to visual recognition memory
(redrawn from Mishkin, 1982). (B) Petri and Mishkin (1994) model. In this revised model,
the rhinal cortex is critical for visual recognition memory (redrawn from Petri and Mishkin,
1994).
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
50 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
in what today is referred to as the ventral visual stream. Each of these cortical
areas (OB, OA, and TEO) is important for extracting and processing some
aspect of a visual scene, such as color or form. The neurally most veridical
version of visual information is represented and stored in the terminal regions
of the ventral visual stream in the inferior temporal cortex, or area TE. Before
that information can be stored in area TE, however, it must engage the limbic
and diencephalic areas of the brain. By doing so, the information can then be
stamped in to area TE.
According to the model, the memory is initially stored in area TE in a
limbic-dependent fashion, that is, retrieval of the information requires the
participation of the limbic system. Over time, however, the memory in area
TE becomes so well established that it can be retrieved without the assistance
of the limbic regions. At this stage, damage to the limbic system would not
impair information that is already stored in area TE but would impair the
future storage of information in area TE. This is believed to be what occurs in
the case of MTL amnesia: Individuals with MTL damage are no longer able to
form new memories because the limbic regions responsible for stamping in
the memory are damaged. On the other hand, information that has already
been stamped in, and is therefore limbic-independent, is spared following
MTL damage.
Although this model was originally developed for visual memories, one of
its strong features is its applicability to memories in any of the sensory systems
by simply substituting the appropriate higher-order sensory area. For example,
the same model would apply for the auditory system, with the exception that
the auditory cortical areas TC→TB→TA would be substituted for the visual
cortical areas OC→OB→OA→TEO→TE. Such substitution is justified on the
grounds that the visual and auditory systems of the brain are anatomically
similar in both their intrinsic and extrinsic projection patterns (Jones & Powell
1970; Pandya & Kuypers 1969). Furthermore, the behavioral consequences of
damage to the auditory association cortex are virtually identical to the behav-
ioral consequences of damage to the visual association cortex (Colombo,
D’Amato, Rodman & Gross 1990; Colombo, Rodman & Gross 1996).
Modifications of the Model. Mishkin’s basic scheme has remained essen-
tially unchallenged since its inception, but its exact components and their
contributions to memory have changed considerably (see Figure 3.7B). In his
original formulation, both the hippocampus and the amygdala contributed
equally to recognition memory performance, and both had to be damaged to
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
NEUROANATOMICAL BASIS OF MEMORY SYSTEMS 51
Figure 3.8. The spatial arrangement of the critical structures of the limbic system (adapted
from Murray, 1996).
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
52 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
NEUROANATOMICAL BASIS OF MEMORY SYSTEMS 53
Figure 3.9. The effects of damage to the hippocampus (H), hippocampus plus perirhinal
and parahippocampal cortex (H++), or only the perirhinal cortex and parahippocampal
gyrus (++) on visual memory in monkeys (control animals: redrawn from Zola-Morgan,
Squire, Rempel, Clower and Amaral, 1992; H animals: redrawn from Alvarez, Zola-
Morgan and Squire, 1995; H++ animals: redrawn from Zola-Morgan, Squire, Clower and
Rempel, 1993; ++ animals: redrawn from Suzuki, Zola-Morgan, Squire and Amaral, 1993).
all impaired in learning the eight-pair CD task. Recall that monkeys with
hippocampal lesions that extended to the adjacent tissue were impaired in
learning the eight-pair CD task. These findings suggest, therefore, that the
visual memory deficit that follows damage to the hippocampus is transient at
best. The same appears to be true in the case of rats (Mumby & Pinel 1994;
Mumby, Wood & Pinel 1992).
Although these findings suggest that the hippocampus may not be a
component of the explicit memory circuit, it is important to note that failures
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
54 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
Although the neural substrates of explicit memory are generally well defined,
the neural substrates of implicit memory are not. In much the same way that
the behavioral mechanisms of nondeclarative (implicit) memory are defined
“not by any positive feature so much as by the fact that they are not declarative
[explicit]” (Squire 1994, p. 233), the neural substrates of implicit memory
seem to be defined as those areas of the brain that are not the neural substrates
of explicit memory. According to Squire (1994),
“the memory abilities that are not declarative are not of a single type and are
not subserved by a single brain system [but] embrace several kinds of
memory and depend on multiple brain systems” (p. 233).
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
NEUROANATOMICAL BASIS OF MEMORY SYSTEMS 55
Figure 3.10. Neural substrates of implicit memory (redrawn from Petri and Mishkin 1994).
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
56 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
Most of the evidence implicating the striatal system in implicit memory comes
from patients with Huntington’s disease — a genetically transmitted disorder
that results in atrophy of the striatal system, in particular, of the caudate
nucleus. Patients with Huntington’s disease are impaired on tests of implicit
memory, such as rotary pursuit (Heindel, Butters & Salmon 1988), mirror
reading (Martone, Butters, Payne, Becker & Sax 1984), and the Tower of
Hanoi (Saint-Cyr, Taylor & Lang 1988), but they perform normally on tests of
explicit memory. Evidence of the latter, however, is not at all convincing, and
on some tests, they perform as poorly as patients with MTL-type amnesia
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
NEUROANATOMICAL BASIS OF MEMORY SYSTEMS 57
Figure 3.11. Diagram of the divisions of memory, the behaviors that underlie each division,
and the candidate brain structures that support each behavior (redrawn from Milner, Squire
and Kandel 1998).
(Butters, Wolf, Martone, Granholm & Cermak 1985). Furthermore, they can
perform the implicit memory tasks, but they simply do so at a degraded level
— a point that was also stressed by Wise (1996):
“In the major clinical papers one can find evidence of procedural [implicit]
learning that has often been neglected in favor of emphasizing statistically
significant deficits” (p. 41).
Most of what we know about the neural basis of memory is based on lesion
studies. The problem, of course, is that lesions rarely abide by functional
boundaries. Even when they do, the consequences of a lesion can extend well
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
58 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
The two most popular imaging techniques are positron emission tomography
(PET) and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). Neither technique
directly measures neural activity. Rather, both rely on the fact that those areas
of the brain that are neurally active are also those areas of the brain that are
metabolically active. PET and fMRI are designed to detect changes in brain
metabolism that follow naturally as a function of neural activity. Because the
brain is always active, however, both techniques rely on the method of
subtraction, in which activity during a baseline condition is subtracted from
activity during an experimental condition.
Positron Emission Tomography (PET). PET produces a picture of the
brain by measuring blood flow, oxygen consumption, or glucose metabolism.
In all three cases, the subject is injected with a small amount of a lightly
radioactive substance that decays and emits positively charged particles called
positrons. When these positrons collide with electrons, which are negatively
charged, they annihilate each other and produce energy in the form of two
photons. The PET scanner records the level and origin of the photon activity.
This information is then converted into a color-coded picture with a spatial
resolution of approximately 5 mm and a temporal resolution of 1 min.
Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI). Like PET, fMRI mea-
sures brain metabolism in the form of blood oxygenation. In contrast to PET,
which relies on the decay of an ingested radioactive substance, fMRI mea-
sures the magnetic properties of hemoglobin. Areas of the brain that are
neurally very active are also undergoing greater amounts of blood oxygen-
ation. Because oxygenated blood has more magnetic properties than deoxy-
genated blood, the fMRI scanner can detect this difference and convert this
information into a color-coded picture of neural activity with a spatial resolu-
tion of 1 to 2 mm and a temporal resolution of 2 to 8 s (Ungerleider 1995).
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
NEUROANATOMICAL BASIS OF MEMORY SYSTEMS 59
Imaging Studies of Memory. Studies using PET and fMRI are being used
with increasing frequency to chart the neural structures that are activated during
memory performance (for reviews, see Gabrieli, Brewer & Poldrack 1998;
Schacter 1997; Ungerleider 1995). Although a number of studies have imaged
the brain during memory performance, only recently have studies begun to
address the dissociation between explicit and implicit memory. One of the first
studies of this type was conducted by Squire, Ojeman, Miezin, Petersen, Videen
and Raichle (1992), who asked subjects to study a list of words. To ensure that
they attended to each word, the experimenter asked them to rate how much they
liked each word on a scale of 1 to 5. Their brains were then scanned using PET
while they performed either a word-stem completion (implicit memory) task or
a cued-recall (explicit memory) task. The explicit memory task activated the
hippocampus and parahippocampal gyrus, whereas the implicit memory task
led to a decrease in activity in the right posterior visual cortex. Unfortunately,
the implicit memory task also activated the hippocampus and parahippocampal
gyrus, although to a lesser extent.
The Squire et al. (1992) study fell short of providing the required double
dissociation between memory task (explicit versus implicit) and neural struc-
ture (hippocampus versus posterior visual cortex) that would support the
notion of multiple memory systems. The authors noted, however, that success
on the word-stem completion task was uncharacteristically high (71.5%) and,
more importantly, not that different from success on the cued-recall task
(72.4%). It is possible, they argued, that performance on the word-stem
completion task may have been contaminated by explicit memory influences
(see also Chapter 2), and that the hippocampal activation that was seen during
word-stem completion was a consequence of this contamination.
To address this possibility, Schacter, Alpert, Savage, Rauch and Albert
(1996) repeated the word-stem completion task. Subjects were shown a list of
words, but instead of rating them on a pleasantness scale, they counted the
number of T-junctions in each word. This manipulation deemphasized the
semantic nature of the task and encouraged them to process the words in a
more incidental fashion. The experiment yielded the desired results: Subjects
still showed priming effects, but they completed many fewer word stems than
subjects in the Squire et al. (1992) study. Under these conditions, PET scans
again revealed a decrease in activity in the posterior visual cortex. In contrast
to the findings of Squire et al. (1992), however, no activation was seen in the
hippocampus or parahippocampal gyrus.
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
60 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
NEUROANATOMICAL BASIS OF MEMORY SYSTEMS 61
the task, rats with damage to the caudate nucleus were impaired relative to
unoperated controls, but rats with damage to the fornix performed better than
controls. (Note, however, that rats with caudate lesions were able to learn the
task to above-chance levels.) In a follow-up study, Packard and White (1991)
found that intracaudate injections of the dopamine (DA) agonist d-amphet-
amine improved performance on the win-stay version of the radial arm maze
task, but intrahippocampal injections had no effect. Conversely, intrahippo-
campal injections improved performance on the win-shift version of the radial
arm maze task, but intracaudal injections had little effect. These studies, then,
revealed a double dissociation of function with respect to both the effects of
lesions and dopaminergic activation.
Packard and McGaugh (1992), who trained rats on two versions of the
two-platform Morris water maze task, provided more evidence of a double
dissociation. In this task, one platform is stable and will support the rat,
whereas the second platform is unstable and sinks when the rat stands on it. In
the spatial (explicit) version of the task, the stable platform always resided in
the same quadrant of the water maze, whereas the unstable platform shifted
between the remaining three quadrants across trials. In the visual (implicit)
version of the task, one visual cue indicated the correct platform, and another
indicated the incorrect platform. Again, rats with fornix lesions were impaired
on the explicit version of the two-platform task, but animals with caudate
lesions performed as well as unoperated controls. Conversely, on the implicit
version of the task, rats with caudate lesions were impaired in learning the
task, but rats with fornix lesions performed no differently from unoperated
controls.
McDonald and White (1993) uncovered evidence of a triple dissociation
of function across the hippocampus, amygdala, and striatum. Unoperated
control animals, as well as animals with damage to the amygdala, were trained
on the win-shift and win-stay versions of the radial arm maze (see above) as
well as on a radial arm maze version of a conditioned cue preference (CCP)
task. In the CCP task, which used only two of the eight arms in the radial arm
maze, one arm was lighted, and the second arm was not. For half of the
animals, the lighted arm signaled reward, and the dark arm signaled no
reward; for the remaining half, the cue-reward associations were reversed.
During testing, rats’ preference for the rewarded arm was measured. The
experiments revealed a triple dissociation: Rats with damage to the fornix
were impaired on the win-shift version but not on the win-stay or CCP
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
62 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
versions; rats with dorsal striatum lesions were impaired on the win-stay
version but not on the win-shift or CCP versions; and rats with amygdala
lesions were impaired on the CCP version but not on the win-shift or win-stay
versions.
Double Dissociations: Human Studies. Bechara et al. (1995) reported
data from three patients — one with damage to the amygdala, one with
damage to the hippocampus, and one with damage to both the hippocampus
and amygdala. The patients were trained in a classical conditioning procedure
in which the CS was either pictures or sounds, and the US was a very loud
noise. The dependent measure was the skin conductance response. The patient
with lesions of the amygdala showed no conditioning but could describe facts
about the task, such as the stimuli. The patient with hippocampal damage
exhibited conditioning but was unable to describe facts about the task. The
patient with damage to both the hippocampus and amygdala showed neither
conditioning nor the ability to describe facts about the task. Thus, this study
shows a double dissociation between conditioning (implicit memory) perfor-
mance and declarative (explicit memory) knowledge with respect to the func-
tions of the amygdala and the hippocampus.
A second study demonstrating a double dissociation was conducted by
Gabrieli, Fleischman, Keane, Reminger and Morrell (1995). In this study, a
patient with damage to the right occipital lobe received a perceptual identifica-
tion (implicit memory) test with 24 old and 24 new words. Initially, the stimuli
were presented very briefly, and then the duration of each word presentation
increased until the subject correctly identified the word. In the recognition
test, the patient was presented with 48 words and asked to judge if each word
had been seen before. The patient was no faster at identifying old words than
new words but performed as well as the controls on the test of recognition
memory. According to the authors, this patient, along with amnesics who
showed the opposite pattern of results,
“provides the first neuropsychological evidence for [a double dissociation]
that illuminate[s] the functional neural architecture of human memory: A
memory system mediating visual implicit memory…is separable from the
memory systems mediating explicit memory for words” (p. 81).
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
NEUROANATOMICAL BASIS OF MEMORY SYSTEMS 63
Conclusions
In this chapter, we have reviewed the effects of damage to the medial temporal
lobe on explicit and implicit memory performance in humans and nonhuman
animals. We have also reviewed the neural substrates of explicit and implicit
memory. Finally, we have tackled the question of whether this evidence points
to a single memory system or a multiple memory system.
There is ample evidence to suggest that explicit and implicit memory are
mediated by two distinct neural systems — one that taps structures in the
medial temporal lobe, and the other that taps structures in the striatal system.
This view, however, is not without problems. It is hard to escape the fact, for
example, that the vast majority of studies have reported impairments in
explicit but not implicit memory performance. Furthermore, although a few
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
64 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
studies have noted impairments in implicit but not explicit memory perfor-
mance after damage to structures that are believed to support implicit memory,
it is usually the case that some learning on implicit tasks is still possible. For
that matter, some learning on explicit tasks is also possible after damage to
structures that are believed to support explicit memory. With respect to
multiple memory systems, there seems to be ample support for the soft
definition of a double dissociation in memory performance but very little
support for the hard definition of a double dissociation.
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
CHAPTER 4
The Jacksonian principle states that the nervous system is organized hierarchically and that
the development and dissolution of function follow a first-in, last-out sequence. The general
finding that implicit memory is spared in brain-damaged human amnesics but explicit
memory is not, encouraged the application of the Jacksonian principle to the development
and dissolution of memory systems. This principle attributes implicit memory to a primitive
memory system that matures quite early in development and explicit memory to a late-
maturing memory system. This chapter reviews the Jacksonian principle and evidence of its
applicability to the hierarchical development and dissolution of memory in animals and
children.
In 1884, John Hughlings Jackson introduced the concept that the nervous
system — and the behavior that it controlled — was vertically and hierarchi-
cally organized. By this account, the neural functions that appeared first in
evolution are also those that appear first in development and disappear last in
disease. This progression reflects the fact that earlier-appearing neural ele-
ments are overlaid or suppressed by new functions under higher levels of
neural control as the nervous system continues to develop. The later-appearing
neural elements are increasingly more fragile or susceptible to disruption by
disease or other challenges. As a result, adverse conditions can reverse the
stages of evolution and the development of the nervous system such that the
neural control of behavior reverts to that of a more primitive stage. This
account is referred to as the Jacksonian principle of the development and
dissolution of function. In brief, the Jacksonian principle states that the last
functions to develop are the first to disappear after brain damage from injury,
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
66 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
disease, or aging, whereas the first functions to develop are the last to be
affected when the organism undergoes demise.
Classic examples of the Jacksonian principle come from studies of reflex-
ive development. The first objective reports of fetal reflexive development
were by Hooker (1952), who filmed exteriorized, prevital foetuses in a tem-
perature-controlled water bath as they underwent anoxia. Hooker reported that
the first foetal response to tactile stimulation at 7½ postmenstrual weeks was a
contralateral reflex in which the head and upper trunk bent away from the site
of stimulation. Contralateral reflexes are referred to as avoiding reactions
because they move the stimulated region away from the stimulating source
(Angulo y Gonzales 1932; Coghill 1916, 1929). Beginning at 11½ post-
menstrual weeks, contralateral reflexes began to be replaced by ipsilateral
reflexes. The earliest appearing reflexes were total-pattern reflexes. Later
appearing reflexes were increasingly specific to the site of stimulation and
progressed cephalocaudally with age: Stimulated head movements preceded
stimulated hand movements, which preceded stimulated plantar movements,
which preceded integrated hand-mouth reflexes (Humphrey 1969). This same
sequence was subsequently observed in spontaneous activity by de Vries,
Visser and Prechtl (1984), who recorded foetal activity longitudinally over the
course of gestation.
Critical support for the Jacksonian principle was Hooker’s finding that as
the foetus progressively underwent anoxia, stimulated reflexes disappeared in
the reverse order of their original appearance, with the most primitive reflexes
being preserved the longest. As one example, ipsilateral reflexes were replaced
by contralateral reflexes. A reversion in the sequence of reflexes accompany-
ing anoxia was also reported by Angulo y Gonzales (1932). Hooker attributed
this reversion to the fact that the reflexes that appeared first in development
required less oxygen to perform:
“The low oxygen capacity of the early embryo is evidently without untoward
effect upon the activity of the primary reflex arcs. They have developed in a
still lower oxygen environment and the greater supply at the time of their
beginning function is entirely adequate. This is not the case, however, with
newer neural mechanisms which become active later. It is particularly not the
case in connection with those centers which have their synapses at higher
levels of the central nervous system…all newer neural components which
develop find a decreasing increment in oxygen supply….As a result, there is a
tendency for newer neural mechanisms at higher levels to succumb relatively
early to the effects of anoxia and asphyxia. This causes a reversion in the
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
THE JACKSONIAN PRINCIPLE AND MEMORY DEVELOPMENT 67
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
68 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
this sequence to the developing rat, finding that the thermal and chemical
(olfaction, gustation) senses were the last to develop prenatally — after
vestibular and tactile sensation; audition and vision began functioning postna-
tally. In the course of normal aging in humans, these functions (olfaction,
thermal sensitivity, audition, and vision) decline in the reverse order of their
onset.
One of the first attempts to apply the Jacksonian principle to the development
and dissolution of memory function in animals was undertaken by Campbell,
Sananes and Gaddy (1984, 1985). Given evidence that developing animals
typically exhibit poorer memory than adults (Campbell & Spear 1972), these
investigators asked if the Jacksonian principle describes the sequential appear-
ance of various memory abilities in infant animals and the sequential decline
of the same memory abilities in aging animals. To answer this question,
Campbell et al. (1984) conducted a major review of the studies of memory
performance on tasks that had been used in common with infant, young adult,
and aged rats. This section reviews their basic assumptions, the relevant
findings, and their final conclusions.
The Basic Assumptions. In order to provide a compelling test of the
Jacksonian principle, Campbell et al.’s analysis was based on four assump-
tions. First, they required that the tasks that were used with normal adult and
aged rats had to be solved sequentially during development by the prewean-
ling rat. Three representative tasks for which infants exhibited sequential
development of long-term memory were conditioned taste aversion, which
infants learn at the youngest age; the auditory conditioned emotional re-
sponse, which infants learn when they are slightly older; and visually-based
passive avoidance, which infants learn when they are older yet. The rate at
which rat pups acquired these three tasks mirrored the sequential development
of the sensory systems on which they depended (Alberts 1984) — gustation,
audition, and vision, respectively. A fourth task, spatial learning, also relies
heavily on visual cues and appears quite late in infancy. Animals’ perfor-
mance on this task will be considered separately below (“Final Conclusions”).
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
THE JACKSONIAN PRINCIPLE AND MEMORY DEVELOPMENT 69
Table 4.1. Studies that Assessed the Memory Performance of Infant, Young Adult, and Aged
Adult Rats on Conditioned Taste Aversion, Auditory Conditioned Emotional Response, and
Passive-Avoidance Tasks (Cited in Campbell, Sananes & Gaddy 1984)
Task x Age Study
Conditioned Taste Aversion
Infants Campbell & Alberts, 1979; Gregg et al., 1978; Schweitzer
& Green, 1982
Young Adults Campbell & Alberts, 1979; Cooper et al., 1980;
Guanowsky & Misanin, 1983; Klein et al., 1977;
Steinert et al., 1980
Aged Adults Cooper et al., 1980; Guanowsky & Misanin, 1983
Passive Avoidance
Infants Schulenberg et al., 1981; Stehouwer & Campbell, 1980
Young Adults Bartus et al., 1980; Campbell et al., 1980; Dean et al., 1981;
Gold et al., 1981; Jensen et al., 1980; Schulenberg et al.,
1981
Aged Adults Bartus et al., 1980; Campbell et al., 1980; Dean et al., 1981;
Gold et al., 1981; Jensen et al., 1980
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
70 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
Figure 4.1. Scatterplots of transformed retention data from differently-aged rats in condi-
tioned taste aversion tasks (top panel), auditory conditioned emotional response tasks
(middle panel), and passive-avoidance tasks (bottom panel). From left to right, panels
contain data from infants, young adults, and aged adults, respectively. Each point represents
an independent observation from one of the studies listed in Table 4.1.
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
THE JACKSONIAN PRINCIPLE AND MEMORY DEVELOPMENT 71
adults (in the middle panel of each figure) reveal that they exhibited little or no
forgetting of any task after any retention interval, thereby meeting Campbell et
al.’s third requirement. The sole exception occurred in a passive-avoidance
study in which the researchers had used a single training trial, a weak shock,
and a very long retention interval (Gold, McGaugh, Hankins, Rose & Vasquez
1981). The data of old adults are presented in the right panel of each figure.
As shown in Figure 4.1, the pattern of retention across tasks reveals no
consistent relation between forgetting by the aged rats and forgetting by the
infants. Whereas infants exhibited rapid forgetting in the conditioned taste
aversion and auditory conditioned emotional response tasks, for example,
aged rats exhibited almost perfect retention in the same tasks over the same
delays. Recall, however, that the passive-avoidance task is the last task to be
learned during development, and only in some studies using this task did the
aged adults forget as rapidly as infants — in several other studies, they
exhibited near-perfect retention after very long delays.
Only one task provided support for the Jacksonian hypothesis. Prewean-
lings exhibited no spatial learning prior to 20 days of age whether they were
tested in an eight-arm radial maze (Rauch & Raskin 1984) or in a Morris water
maze (Schenk, Inglin & Morris 1983). Likewise, old adults failed to attain the
final level of performance of young adults in the eight-arm radial maze even
after extensive training (Davis, Idowu & Gibson 1983; Wallace, Krauter &
Campbell 1980); similar results were found in the Morris water maze when the
hidden platform was moved (Gage, Dunnett & Bjorklund 1984). These data,
then, yield the inverted U-shaped function of acquisition and retention that is
predicted by the Jacksonian hypothesis. (The extent to which age-related
deficits in perception, motivation, or motoric competence contributed to these
results is not known.)
Final Conclusions. Campbell et al. asked whether the Jacksonian hypoth-
esis predicts the development and dissolution of memory in animals. They
found that rats’ memories for the two tasks that were acquired sequentially
early in ontogeny (conditioned taste aversion, the auditory conditioned emo-
tional response) were completely spared in aged rats. Aged rats exhibited no
evidence of sequential loss. Although memory for the spatial learning task,
which is acquired late in ontogeny, was consistently impaired in aged rats,
their memory for the passive-avoidance task, which also is acquired late in
ontogeny — and after the taste aversion and conditioned emotional response
tasks, was not. In fact, in some passive-avoidance studies, aged rats exhibited
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
72 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
THE JACKSONIAN PRINCIPLE AND MEMORY DEVELOPMENT 73
Many scientists have argued that the order in which memory systems fail in
elderly and amnesic adults predicts the order in which they appear in infancy
(Nadel 1990; Naito & Komatsu 1993; Parkin 1993; Schacter & Moscovitch
1984; Tulving & Schacter 1990). Nadel (1990), for example, proposed that the
hippocampal system is essential for the formation of cognitive maps and that,
during development, an increasingly greater proportion of it becomes func-
tional until a threshold level of connectivity is passed and the structure as a
whole can function. Invoking the Jacksonian principle, he wrote:
“[This view] seems to provide a handle not only on the up-side of life
(development), but also on the down-side (senescence, or brain damage). It is
well known that many brain functions undergo what ‘connectionist’ modelers
have come to call ‘graceful degradation’ with aging/damage. That is, the
progressive removal of functional elements from the system has little impact
until some critical extent is reached, at which point the system rapidly
deteriorates. This phase transition is really the mirror image of what is seen
during early life [italics ours]…” (p. 621).
In fact, that part of the Jacksonian principle that applies to the hierarchical
dissolution of multiple memory systems has met with some success. Most
research on the effects of age on the memory performance of healthy individu-
als has found that elderly adults are inferior to young adults on explicit
memory tasks but perform as well as young adults on implicit memory tasks
(for reviews, see Graf 1990; Light & Lavoie 1993). In particular, age-related
memory deficits have been found on tests of recognition and cued recall, but
age differences on various single-item priming tests have been small or absent
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
74 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
altogether (e.g., Isingrini, Vazoiu & Leroy 1995; Java & Gardiner 1991; Light
& Albertson 1989; Light & Singh 1987; Mitchell, Brown & Murphy 1990).
Mitchell and Schmitt (1992, cited in Mitchell 1993), for example, com-
pared the memory performance of young adults, healthy older adults, and older
patients with Alzheimer’s disease on two explicit memory tasks (cued recall
and free recall) and an implicit memory task (priming). They found that
performance on the free-recall task was worse for both groups of older adults
than for the younger adults, performance on the cued-recall task was the same
for the younger and healthy older adults but was impaired for older adults with
Alzheimer’s disease; in contrast, performance on the priming task (picture-
naming latencies) was equally excellent for all groups. They concluded that the
brain damage associated with Alzheimer’s disease hastened the dissolution of
the memory system responsible for performance on the free-recall task but
spared the memory system responsible for performance on the priming task.
Developmental changes in healthy young and older adults’ ability to
recognize specific item, location, and color information in isolation was
examined by Chalfonte and Johnson (1996). Participants studied a particular
feature of a set of line-drawn objects (from Snodgrass & Vanderwart 1980)
that were arranged on a grid and were not in their original color. They then
received a recognition test for one type of feature. For the item recognition
test, they saw old and new black and white objects; for the location recogni-
tion test, they saw black Xs in old and new locations on the grid; and for the
color recognition test, they saw old and new colors on rectangles. Young and
older adults recognized item and color information equivalently, but older
adults’ recognition of location information was impaired.
Although healthy elderly adults perform more poorly than young adults
on tasks that require them to retrieve factual knowledge that was presented
earlier (Radvansky, Zacks & Hasher 1996), they perform as well as or better
than young adults on tasks that require subjects to exhibit more global levels
of understanding, for example, the ability to understand what is being referred
to by the presented information (Radvansky 1999). Thus, although the decline
of memory functioning in elderly adults is consistent with the Jacksonian
principle, their excellent-to-superior performance on some higher-level cogni-
tive tasks is not.
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
THE JACKSONIAN PRINCIPLE AND MEMORY DEVELOPMENT 75
That part of the Jacksonian principle that applies to the hierarchical develop-
ment of multiple memory systems in childhood has yielded findings that, on
the surface, appear to be consistent with the Jacksonian hypothesis. When
analyzed more carefully, however, the findings appear to be more inconclu-
sive. The issue is whether or not implicit memory performance, as measured in
these studies, remains stable over age. Most research on the effects of age on
the performance of children on explicit and implicit memory tasks has re-
ported that performance on recall and recognition tests improves with age,
while performance on priming tests remains stable (e.g., Carroll, Byrne &
Kirsner 1985; DiGiulio, Seidenberg, O’Leary & Raz 1994; Greenbaum &
Graf 1989; Naito 1990; Parkin 1989; Parkin & Streete 1988). Based on this
pattern, most researchers concluded that implicit memory develops earlier
than explicit memory in childhood. Parkin (1993), for example, attributed the
earlier appearance of implicit memory to phylogenetic influences. In doing so,
he invoked Jackson’s (1880) argument that the neural systems that are evolu-
tionarily more primitive (e.g., those that mediate the organism’s perceptually-
based responding) are more localized than the neural systems that appeared
later in evolution (e.g., those that mediate higher-level neural functions) and
are more widely distributed throughout the brain. Assuming that ontogeny
recapitulates phylogeny — an assumption that the noted evolutionary biolo-
gist Steven Jay Gould has vigorously disputed (Gould 1998), Parkin con-
cluded that the phylogenetic system that is more primitive and mediates
perceptually-based memory performance (i.e., implicit memory) appears ear-
lier in development and is less susceptible to insult in old age than the
phylogenetic system that mediates higher-level memory performance (i.e.,
explicit memory).
Studies of children’s memory performance on implicit and explicit tests
have increased in frequency over the last decade. Their general finding is that
memory performance on implicit tests is invariant over age, whereas memory
performance on explicit tests increases with age. A pervasive interpretative
problem in studies of children’s memory development, however, is the ready
tendency of researchers to attribute age-related changes in memory perfor-
mance to maturational changes in the brain. Although such changes are just as
likely to reflect age-related differences in experience or language develop-
ment, this possibility is rarely considered.
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
76 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
THE JACKSONIAN PRINCIPLE AND MEMORY DEVELOPMENT 77
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
78 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
her colleagues found that the relative measure provided a better fit to the data
(Snodgrass, Smith, Feenan & Corwin 1987).
The particular savings measure that is used has major consequences for
conclusions about whether priming changes with age. Mitchell’s meta-analy-
sis, for example, included a study by Parkin and Streete (1988) with subjects
ranging from 3 years of age to young adults. When they expressed savings as
the absolute difference in correct identifications of old and new items, they
found that priming increased with age, but when they calculated the difference
proportionally to take baseline differences into consideration (i.e., the number
of items correctly identified on trial 2 minus the number of items correctly
identified on trial 1/the number of items correctly identified on trial 2), they
found that priming was age-invariant (see also Graf 1990). When Parkin
(1993) recalculated the amount of priming in the original data set using
Snodgrass’s (1989) measure of relative difference, however, he again found
that priming increased significantly with age.
In a subsequent study using pictorial stimuli, Russo et al. (1995) at-
tempted to exclude potential explicit memory influences from the final esti-
mate of implicit memory (for discussion, see Jacoby et al. 1993; Russo &
Parkin 1993) and to eliminate age differences in baseline responding. In an
initial experiment, preschoolers between 4 and 6 years of age and a group of
young adults participated in two tasks — a picture-completion (implicit
memory) task and a cued-recall (explicit memory) task. Two sets of pictures
were used for the implicit task and a third set was added for the explicit task.
During the study phase of both tasks, children were asked to name the objects
that appeared in 12 pictures.
In the picture-completion test, children were shown up to eight versions
each of the old pictures and 12 new ones and were asked to identify the objects
in the pictures as quickly as possible. The most fragmented version of all
objects was presented first, followed by the next most fragmented version of
all objects, and so forth until the whole object was presented. Once an object
was correctly identified, it was not tested further. Scoring was based on the
level of fragmentation at which an object was correctly identified. For the
cued-recall test, children were shown the fragmented versions of the old
objects and asked to use them as cues to try and remember what they had seen
during the study phase. The two ages did not perform differently on the
priming test, but 6-year-olds were superior on the cued-recall test.
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
THE JACKSONIAN PRINCIPLE AND MEMORY DEVELOPMENT 79
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
80 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
One week later, RTs were significantly faster at the outset of session 2 than
they had been at the end of session 1 for both 6- and 10-year-olds but not for
adults, whose RTs could not decrease further. Although adults responded that
they had seen a test sequence before more often than children whether it was old
or new, the most frequent response was “I don’t know,” and performance on the
recognition test was unrelated to performance during the sequence-learning
task. These data, then, are consistent with prior evidence that performance on
implicit tasks does not change over development and is statistically indepen-
dent of performance on explicit tests of the same information.
Pictures have also been used to assess the basic features of an object that
children of different ages recognize. Using exactly the same line-drawn stimuli
that had been used with healthy adults (Chalfonte & Johnson 1996), Gulya,
Figure 4.2. Mean corrected recognition scores for 5-, 7-, and 10-year-old children on three
nonverbal feature recognition tests.
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
THE JACKSONIAN PRINCIPLE AND MEMORY DEVELOPMENT 81
Figure 4.3. A comparison of the mean corrected recognition scores of 10-year-old children
19-year-old (young) adults, and 70-year-old (elderly) adults. An identical pencil-and-paper
version of the feature recognition task with identical study and test stimuli was used with
children and adults.
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
82 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
adults’ memory for location had significantly worsened, and their color recog-
nition had begun to decline as well, predictive of an even greater decline at
older ages. Nonetheless, elderly adults’ item recognition was excellent and at
the same high level as that of the youngest children. This inverted U-shaped
function is consistent with Jackson’s first-in, last-out principle and suggests
that the Jacksonian principle describes the development and dissolution of
memory for individual features.
Conclusions
The Jacksonian principle states that the development and dissolution of func-
tion follow a first-in, last-out sequence. The present chapter reviewed evi-
dence that this principle also describes the development and dissolution of
memory. Studies of memory function with infant, young adult, and aged
animals failed to support the Jacksonian principle. In all but one instance, the
tasks that were acquired earliest in ontogeny were not learned or remembered
better at other ages than the tasks that were acquired later in ontogeny. Studies
with young adults and healthy elderly adults, however, found that normal
memory performance declines late in adulthood on explicit but not implicit
tests, and studies that were free of age confounds in verbal competence or base
rates of responding found that memory performance improves during child-
hood on explicit tests but not implicit tests. The combined results of these
studies portray explicit memory performance as an inverted-U shaped func-
tion of age and implicit memory performance as age-invariant over the major
portion of the life span.
These studies, however, assessed developmental changes in implicit and
explicit memory performance in individuals for whom both types of memory
were already functional. They did not ask whether the onset of implicit and
explicit memory function is hierarchical, which is the first half of the Jackso-
nian principle. To answer this question, we next will consider studies of
implicit and explicit memory in infants (Chapters 5, 6, and 7).
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
CHAPTER 5
In this chapter, we review the evidence for a developmental dissociation between implicit
and explicit memory in nonhuman primates. We begin with the classic study conducted by
Bachevalier and Mishkin (1984) in which monkeys were shown to master one memory task
substantially earlier than another during development. We then describe the neural mecha-
nisms that are thought to underpin this age-related change in memory performance. Finally,
we reconsider the notion that there is a developmental dissociation between two memory
systems in nonhuman primates.
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
84 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
discuss the revisions to this claim that are required in light of recent findings.
In Chapters 6 and 7, we address the issue of a developmental dissociation in
the memory performance of human infants.
Historical background
Figure 5.1. Acquisition of the 24-hour concurrent discrimination (CD) task by 3-month-old
and 3-year-old monkeys. The monkeys were trained first on set A and then on set B. Each
set contained 20 different pairs of stimuli (redrawn from Bachevalier and Mishkin 1984).
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
MEMORY DEVELOPMENT IN NONHUMAN PRIMATES 85
the 24-hour concurrent discrimination (CD) task (e.g., Malamut, Saunders &
Mishkin 1984). Based on these findings, Bachevalier and Mishkin (1984)
hypothesized that monkeys’ ability to solve these tasks might emerge inde-
pendently during development if the neural systems that supported perfor-
mance on each task matured at different rates.
To test their hypothesis, Bachevalier and Mishkin (1984) compared the
performance of 3- and 36-month-old monkeys on the 24-hour CD task and the
performance of 3-, 6-, 12-, and 36-month-old monkeys on the DNMS task. In
the CD task, monkeys were trained successively with two sets of 20 pairs of
stimuli (set A, set B). On both sets, one stimulus of each pair was designated as
correct, and choosing that stimulus was rewarded. The other stimulus in the
pair was designated as incorrect; choosing that stimulus was not rewarded.
Within each daily session, a pair of stimuli was presented only once. Acquisi-
tion of the CD task is shown in Figure 5.1. Although 3-month-olds were
slower than 3-year-olds to learn the task when trained with set A (see Fig-
Figure 5.2. Number of days required by 3-, 6-, 12-, and 36-month-old monkeys to learn the
delayed-nonmatching-to-sample (DNMS) task. The dashed line represents criterial levels
of performance (redrawn from Bachevalier and Mishkin 1984).
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
86 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
ure 5.1, left panel), there was no age-related difference in the number of
sessions required to learn set B (see Figure 5.1, right panel).
In contrast to performance on the CD task, Bachevalier and Mishkin
(1984) documented a significant age-related difference in the number of days
required for monkeys to learn the DNMS task. Three-year-old monkeys
reached criterion (90% correct) on the DNMS task after only 8 days of
training, whereas 3-month-olds required 36 days of training to achieve the
same level of performance (see Figure 5.2). The performance of 6- and 12-
month-olds was intermediate between these two age extremes. When tested
after a 15-day retention interval, there was an inverse relation between age and
the number of trials required for animals to relearn the task (see Figure 5.3).
Furthermore, younger animals were more impaired than older animals when
the delay between the sample and the test stimuli was increased from 10 to
120 s (see Figure 5.4) and when the list length was increased.
Figure 5.3. Number of trials required by 3-, 6-, 12-, and 36-month-old monkeys to learn
(open columns) the delayed-nonmatching-to-sample (DNMS) task and to relearn (shaded
columns) the same task after a 15-day rest period (redrawn from Bachevalier and Mishkin
1984).
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
MEMORY DEVELOPMENT IN NONHUMAN PRIMATES 87
On the basis of these findings, Bachevalier and Mishkin (1984) drew two
conclusions. First, given that the monkeys succeeded on the CD task long
before they succeeded on the DNMS task, Bachevalier and Mishkin con-
cluded that one neural system, which they called the habit system, emerged
earlier in development than the other neural system, which they called the
memory system. Using the terminology adopted in the present book, the habit
system is roughly equivalent to implicit memory, and the memory system is
roughly equivalent to explicit memory. Second, given that adult monkeys with
lesions of the medial temporal lobe (including the hippocampus) are impaired
when tested on the DNMS task but not on the 24-hour CD task, Bachevalier
and Mishkin (1984) attributed the delayed development of DNMS perfor-
mance to the delayed maturation of the hippocampus.
Figure 5.4. Performance of 3-, 6-, 12-, and 36-month-old monkeys on the delayed-non-
matching-to-sample (DNMS) task with delays of 10, 30, 60, and 120 s (redrawn from
Bachevalier and Mishkin 1984).
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
88 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
Bachevalier and Mishkin (1984) originally assumed that the slow emergence
of DNMS performance was due to the late development of the hippocampus.
Subsequent research, however, has shown that the time-table for hippocampal
maturation does not match the time-table for mastery on the DNMS task in
nonhuman primates. Bachevalier and Mishkin’s original assumption that the
hippocampus matured late in ontogeny was based primarily on anatomical
studies that had been conducted with rodents (Nadel & Zola-Morgan 1984).
Similar studies with nonhuman primates, however, have revealed that
the hippocampus matures relatively early in development (Bachevalier,
Ungerleider, O’Neill & Friedman 1986; Diamond 1990a). Although only 15%
of the neurons in the dentate gyrus (a component of the hippocampus) are
present at birth in the rat, 80% of these neurons are present at birth in the
monkey (Rakic & Nowakowski 1981). Furthermore, the opiate and muscar-
inic receptor-binding sites in the hippocampus of the nonhuman primate brain
at birth are virtually identical to those found in the adult brain (Bachevalier et
al. 1986; O’Neill, Friedman, Bachevalier & Ungerleider 1986).
Studies using lesion techniques have also challenged links between the
development of DNMS performance and hippocampal maturation. The ration-
ale behind these studies was that lesions of the hippocampus during infancy
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
MEMORY DEVELOPMENT IN NONHUMAN PRIMATES 89
should be less disruptive than the same lesions performed during adulthood if
the hippocampus did not mature until late in ontogeny. Contrary to Bachevalier
and Mishkin’s original hypothesis, however, neonatal lesions of the medial
temporal lobe have been found to severely impair performance on the DNMS
task (Bachevalier & Mishkin 1994; Malkova, Mishkin & Bachevalier 1995).
As shown in Figure 5.5, infants who received bilateral lesions of the medial
temporal lobe during the first month of life were impaired on the DNMS task
when they were tested for the first time at 10 months of age (Bachevalier &
Mishkin 1994), and they continued to be impaired when they were retested at
4 to 5 years of age (Malkova et al. 1995). In fact, the effect of neonatal lesions
on DNMS performance is virtually identical to the effect of adult lesions on
performance in the same task (Mishkin 1978; Mishkin & Philips 1990).
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
90 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
MEMORY DEVELOPMENT IN NONHUMAN PRIMATES 91
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
92 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
Figure 5.6. Mean percent fixation time to the novel test stimulus in younger (Young: 1 to 2
weeks old), intermediate-aged (Middle: 4 weeks old), and older (Old: 13 weeks old) infant
pigtailed macaques tested in a visual paired-comparison task (redrawn from Gunderson and
Sackett 1984).
familiar and the novel objects during the test was recorded.
The novelty-preference scores (percent time spent looking at the novel
stimulus) obtained by Bachevalier (1990) are shown in Figure 5.7 as a func-
tion of age and gender. Clearly, novelty preference increased as a function of
age. Although 5-day-old monkeys did not exhibit a reliable preference for the
novel stimulus during the test, 30-day-old monkeys did. Furthermore, 15-day-
old females — but not 15-day-old males — also exhibited a preference for the
novel stimulus during the test.
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
Figure 5.7. Mean percent fixation time to the novel test stimulus by rhesus monkeys tested
in a visual paired-comparison (VPC) task as a function of age and gender (redrawn from
Bachevalier 1990).
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
94 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
MEMORY DEVELOPMENT IN NONHUMAN PRIMATES 95
Bachevalier and Mishkin’s (1984) seminal paper set the stage for more than a
decade of research on the developmental dissociation of memory systems.
More recent research has challenged both the underlying neural mechanism
thought to be responsible for their findings as well as the notion of the
developmental dissociation per se. In fact, Bachevalier and Mishkin’s original
data foreshadowed questions regarding a developmental dissociation between
memory systems.
Consider first the original Bachevalier and Mishkin (1984) finding that
younger animals were not impaired relative to older animals in the acquisition
of a 24-hour CD task. Recall that the monkeys were actually trained in
succession on two 24-hour CD tasks, each using a different set of stimuli (set
A, set B). Although there was no age-related difference in the speed with
which the monkeys learned set B, younger monkeys learned set A more
slowly. This procedure may have contributed to early success on the CD task.
With this in mind, it is possible that had the younger animals also been given a
second exposure to the DNMS task (with new stimuli), they might have
learned the second task as quickly as adult monkeys.
In addition, although the older monkeys clearly learned the DNMS task
faster than the younger monkeys (see Figure 5.2), it is important to keep in
mind that even the youngest monkeys eventually learned the DNMS task to
the same 90% level of performance. Although the authors argue that the
infants were not able to learn the task until they were at least 4 months old,
even at 3 months of age, infants performed above chance and showed a steady
improvement in performance across their first month of training. Furthermore,
although the younger animals required more trials than older animals to
relearn the DNMS task after a 15-day retention interval (see Figure 5.3), even
the youngest animals exhibited substantial savings. Finally, although perfor-
mance by the infants was inferior to that of the 3-year-olds when the delay
between the sample and the test was increased, even the youngest animals
were approximately 90% correct when tested after a 120-s delay (see Fig-
ure 5.4). Taken together, these findings demonstrate that although infants’
performance on the DNMS task was statistically poorer than that of adults, by
all accounts, they still performed remarkably well.
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
96 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
Conclusions
The hypothesis that two or more memory systems develop independently and
at different rates in nonhuman primates is no longer tenable. When monkeys
were tested on the VPC task, for example, they exhibited long-term (i.e., 24-
hour) recognition memory as early as 1 month of age — well before they could
even be tested in either the CD task or the DNMS task. Although monkeys
clearly master some tasks earlier in life than others, these differential rates of
mastery do not reflect the emergence of different memory systems, but the
emergence of different processes other than memory that are required to
perform the tasks. Although successful performance on the 24-hour CD and
DNMS tasks requires the same degree of perception, attention, and motivation
as well as the same target response (i.e., reaching), the two tasks differ on a
number of other dimensions that may contribute to infants’ differential rate of
success on them. One process that is likely to contribute to the protracted
development of DNMS performance is inhibition. When infants are not
required to inhibit responding to a previously rewarded object (as, for ex-
ample, in the VPC task), visual recognition (explicit) memory emerges very
early in development.
Perhaps the best test of the hypothesis that age-related changes in inhibi-
tion rather than in memory underlie the late emergence of successful perfor-
mance on the DNMS task would be to compare performance on the DNMS
task with performance on the delayed-matching-to-sample (DMS) task. In the
DMS task, monkeys are rewarded for choosing the test stimulus that is the
same as the sample stimulus. In other respects, however, the DMS task is
identical to the DNMS task with the exception that monkeys are not required
to inhibit responding to a previously rewarded stimulus. If inhibition is the
basis for the late emergence of successful DNMS performance, then monkeys
should succeed on the DMS task considerably earlier than on the DNMS task.
We are currently exploring this possibility.
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
CHAPTER 6
In this chapter, we review some of the experimental procedures that have been used to study
memory in human infants. We then outline the classic claims that have been made regarding
the similarity of memory in human infants and adult amnesics and consider the challenges
that recent data have presented for these claims. Finally, we evaluate the adequacy of the
analogy between infants and amnesics by discussing the problems associated with inferring
conscious recollection on the part of preverbal infants.
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
98 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
Below we describe some of the tasks that have been used in studies of memory
with preverbal infants. Our review is selective (for a complete review, see
Rovee-Collier & Hayne, 2000) and focuses on tasks with direct implications
for the development of implicit and explicit memory.
Response-to-novelty tasks
The two classic response-to-novelty tasks that have been used to study infant
visual recognition memory are the paired-comparison task and the habitua-
tion task, which exploit the young infant’s propensity to look at novel stimuli.
Conceptually, both tasks are based on Sokolov’s (1963) model of habituation
of the orienting reflex. In this model, each time a stimulus is encountered, an
internal representation or engram of it is established. Over successive encoun-
ters with the same stimulus, the initial representation is gradually fleshed out
by new information that the subject notices about the stimulus. As the repre-
sentation becomes progressively more complete, subjects attend to the exter-
nal stimulus less and less; once the representation is complete (i.e., no new
information remains to be added to it), then subjects no longer orient to it. As
the representation decays over time (i.e., forgetting), subjects renew orienting
to the extent that the internal representation and the external stimulus no
longer match. In most response-to-novelty studies conducted with human
infants, the primary dependent variable is visual attention. The measure of
retention of a previously encountered visual stimulus is inferred from the
extent to which an infant does not look at it but directs attention elsewhere,
exhibiting a novelty preference.
Paired-Comparison Task. Fagan (1970) was the first to study infant
memory using a novelty-preference procedure, which he adapted from Fantz’s
(1956) original pioneering work on infants’ visual preferences. In this proce-
dure, infants are initially exposed to a particular stimulus for a fixed duration
or until they have accumulated a specified amount of time looking at it.
Immediately afterward, infants are tested with two stimuli presented simulta-
neously for two trials (counterbalancing for the side of the novel stimulus) (see
Figure 6.1). One stimulus is the previously exposed one (i.e., familiar), and
one is novel.
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
MEMORY DEVELOPMENT IN HUMAN INFANTS 99
Figure 6.1. The paired-comparison response-to-novelty task with a 6-month-old infant. The
infant was previously shown two identical pictures (e.g., of the woman’s face). Immediately
afterward, the infant received a paired-comparison test, shown here, with the previously
exposed picture (the woman’s face) and a novel one (the man’s face). Proportionally longer
looking at the novel picture — a novelty preference — is taken as evidence that the infant
recognized the preexposed one. (Test photographs courtesy of J.F. Fagan, III.)
The proportion of time that infants spend looking at the novel member of the
test pair is statistically compared with chance (50% of total looking time).
Proportionally longer looking at a novel stimulus than at the preexposed one is
taken as evidence that infants recognize the preexposed stimulus. Some
researchers have argued, however, that proportionally longer looking at the
preexposed stimulus also indicates that infants recognize that stimulus because
a familiarity preference represents nonchance attention (Cohen & Gelber
1975; Colombo & Bundy 1983; Hunter & Ames 1988; Weizmann, Cohen &
Pratt 1971). For infants of all ages, proportionally equivalent looking at the
novel and the preexposed stimulus is taken as evidence of forgetting.
Habituation Task. In the habituation task, a stimulus is repeatedly pre-
sented until infants’ attention to it has declined to some absolute (e.g., McCall,
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
100 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
Kennedy & Dodds 1977) or relative (e.g., Cohen, DeLoache & Pearl 1977)
level. At this point, some infants are presented with a novel stimulus, whereas
others are again presented with the original one. Greater looking at the novel
stimulus is again taken as evidence that infants remember the preexposed
(habituation) stimulus. As the delay increases between the last habituation
trial and the test with the original stimulus, infants increasingly respond to it.
At some point, the infant’s responding returns to the level that was seen at the
outset of the habituation trials. Because the original stimulus is physically
absent when infants are tested with the novel one, the habituation procedure
incurs a greater memory load than the paired-comparison procedure.
Infants’ response to novelty in paired-comparison and habituation tasks is
usually measured within a single brief session, and the novel stimulus is
presented within seconds of infants’ prior exposure to the other stimulus.
Under these conditions, the task provides a measure of short-term memory.
When familiarization with the preexposed stimulus occurs over multiple daily
sessions (Fagan 1970, 1973) or when testing occurs across sessions that are
separated by days or weeks (Bahrick & Pickens 1995; Bahrick, Hernandez-
Reif & Pickens 1997; Pascalis & DeSchonen 1994), then long-term memory is
implicated.
Long-Term Familiarization Task. Long-term familiarization is procedur-
ally similar to habituation except that infants are preexposed to a stimulus over
a period of 1 or more days before the final test. During the initial preexposure
phase, the experimenter does not record responding and often is not even
present. In a sense, the long-term familiarization procedure provides a degree
of ecological validity to formal laboratory studies in which stimulus exposure
is highly controlled and typically very brief.
Hunt and his colleagues (Hunt 1970; Hunt & Uzgiris 1964; Kaplan 1967;
Uzgiris & Hunt 1970), for example, used a long-term familiarization task with
infants in their classic studies of the development of visual attention. Kaplan,
for example, exposed infants twice daily between 8 and 14 weeks of age to a
nonmoving mobile, a continuously moving mobile, or a mobile that moved in
response to their own activity. Every 2 weeks, she gave them a 4-min paired-
comparison test with the familiar and a novel mobile (both nonmoving) 24
hours after their last exposure. Until infants were 12 weeks old, they showed
no differential fixations to novel and familiar mobiles, when they fixated
novel mobiles longer. However, the type of movement infants saw during
familiarization interacted with stimulus novelty: Infants familiarized with
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
MEMORY DEVELOPMENT IN HUMAN INFANTS 101
The standard DNMS task that has been used with human infants is procedur-
ally identical to the standard DNMS procedure that has been used with
nonhuman primates (see Chapter 3 and 5). Briefly, infants participate in two
kinds of trials. On sample trials, the sample object is placed over a well that
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
102 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
contains a reward. After the infant removes the sample and finds the reward, a
screen is lowered between the infant and the sample object. After a delay, the
screen is raised revealing the sample and a novel object. On these test trials,
the reward is always hidden under the novel object. If the infant responds
correctly by displacing the novel object, then he or she is allowed to retrieve
the reward. If the infant responds incorrectly, then the experimenter reveals
the reward under the novel object but does not let the infant have it. Typically,
a new pair of objects is used on every trial (i.e., trial-unique stimuli). The
maximum delay after which an infant responds correctly is the measure of
memory.
Human infants do not succeed on the standard version of the DNMS task
until midway through their second year of life. In most studies, they do not
reliably choose the novel stimulus until approximately 15 to 21 months of age,
even when the delay between the sample trial and the test trial is only 5 to 10 s
(Diamond, Towle & Boyer 1994; Overman, Bachevalier, Turner & Peuster
1992). Even after younger infants have mastered the task at these short delays,
their performance is still disrupted by delays of 30 s and longer.
The prolonged time course for the development of DNMS during the
infancy period is surprising in view of the procedural similarity between this
task and the visual paired-comparison task. In both tasks, the infant is required
to respond to the novel stimulus during the test. Subsequent research has
shown that minor changes in the behavioral requirements of the DNMS task
can dramatically alter its developmental time course (Diamond 1995; Dia-
mond et al. 1999). In the traditional version of the DNMS task, for example,
infants are required to displace the novel test stimulus in order to retrieve a
reward that is placed beneath it. Alternatively, when the infant’s reward is
simply the opportunity to play with the novel test stimulus (i.e., stimulus =
reward), infants solve the task at 6 months of age and perform significantly
above chance after delays as long as 10 min (Diamond 1995). Their perfor-
mance on the modified version of the DNMS task is virtually identical to their
performance on the standard response-to-novelty paired-comparison task.
Similarly, infants’ performance on the task is also enhanced when verbal
praise of the correct choice is used as a reward (Diamond et al. 1999). Taken
together, these findings have led Diamond (1990b, 1995; Diamond et al.
1999) to conclude that some ability other than memory contributes to the
developmental lag in DNMS performance on the standard task.
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
MEMORY DEVELOPMENT IN HUMAN INFANTS 103
The A-not-B search task was originally introduced by Piaget (1954) and has
resurfaced as another method for studying memory development in infants
and young children (Diamond 1985; Fox, Kagan & Weiskopf 1979; Gratch &
Landers 1971). In the standard version of the task, the infant is seated between
two identical wells (A, B) where an object can be hidden. The experimenter
shows the infant an object, places it in well A as the infant watches, and
simultaneously covers both wells. After a delay, the infant is permitted to
reach. Infants reaching to the empty well are shown the object in the other well
but are not given it. The experimenter continues to hide the object in well A
until criterion is achieved (e.g., a correct response on two consecutive trials),
at which point the procedure is repeated with the object hidden in well B. After
the hiding well is reversed, infants often continue to search in well A, despite
having watched the experimenter place it in well B. This behavior is called the
A-not-B error.
When infants are tested in this version of the object search task, the
frequency of the A-not-B error increases as a function of delay, and older
infants tolerate increasingly longer delays between hiding and reaching. Dia-
mond (1985), for example, found that infants who were tested cross-section-
ally tolerated delays of 1 to 2 s at 7 months, 5 s at 9 months, and 10 s at 12
months, whereas infants tested longitudinally tolerated slightly longer delays.
Irrespective of the experimental design, however, the delay at which the A-
not-B error occurred increased gradually and continuously at the rate of
approximately 2 s per month.
As in DNMS studies, seemingly minor variations in the task parameters
produce dramatically different estimates of retention (for review, see Diamond
1990a). These variations have included the use of multiple wells (Diamond,
Cruttenden & Niederman 1989) and the substitution of a different object for the
object that infants either saw hidden or heard named (Ramsay & Campos 1978).
Considered in the light of similar findings obtained with the DNMS task, these
results suggest that developmental improvement on object search tasks is due
not only to age-related changes in memory ability but to nonmemorial factors
as well.
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
104 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
MEMORY DEVELOPMENT IN HUMAN INFANTS 105
Figure 6.2. Developmental progression in the delay that human infants can tolerate in the
delayed response and A-not-B tasks (redrawn from Diamond 1990b).
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
106 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
search errors significantly more often than controls who had practiced reach-
ing to midline in the light.
Most of the early studies of classical conditioning (Pavlov 1927) with infants
were single-subject studies, and those that tracked an individual’s condition-
ing performance over substantial periods of time provided evidence of long-
term memory. In a hallmark study of this kind, Watson and Rayner (1920)
sounded a loud gong (the unconditioned stimulus, or UCS) which produced
crying and withdrawal (the unconditioned response, or UCR) when an 11-
month-old infant named Albert touched a white rat (the conditioned stimulus,
or CS). This procedure occurred twice. One week later, Albert still withdrew
his hand when tested with the rat (the conditioned response, or CR); in this
session, he also received five more CS-UCS pairings. Five days later, Albert
exhibited CRs when tested with the rat as well as with previously neutral
stimuli that resembled it in some way (a rabbit, a dog, a fur coat, a Santa Claus
mask, and cotton wool) but not to perceptually different stimuli (wooden
blocks). Ten days later, his CR to the rat had become so muted that it was
freshened with another CS-UCS pairing. Also at this time, the UCS was
explicitly paired once each with the rabbit and the dog. One month later,
Albert still exhibited strong CRs to the rat, dog, mask, and fur coat.
In another early study of classical conditioning, Jones (1930) exposed a
7-month-old infant to repeated pairings of a tapping sound (the CS) and an
electrotactual stimulus (the UCS) for five sessions over consecutive days. The
CR, an anticipatory galvanic skin reflex, was established midway through
session 1. Jones found that the infant still exhibited the CR 6 weeks later
despite receiving no additional conditioning trials in the interim. Moreover,
the CR had not completely disappeared 7 weeks after training was over. Later,
Kantrow (1937) reported that an infant less than 10 days old still exhibited a
conditioned leg flexion (CR) to a tone (CS) 18 days after the final conditioning
session.
The most widely studied response in the classical conditioning literature,
irrespective of species or age, is the eyeblink response — a protective reflex.
When the UCS is a corneal air puff, the CR that immediately anticipates the
UCS is also a functional avoidance response. Many of the early failures to
establish a conditioned eyelid reflex in infants undoubtedly resulted from use
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
MEMORY DEVELOPMENT IN HUMAN INFANTS 107
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
108 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
Figure 6.3. Percentage of responses on CS-UCS trials by infants who were initially
conditioned at 10, 20, or 30 days of age (left panels) to blink their eyes to a tone (CS) that
was paired with an air puff (UCS) and were retrained in a second session 10 days later (right
panels). The CS and UCS were separated by either a 1500-ms or 500-ms interstimulus
interval (ISI).
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
MEMORY DEVELOPMENT IN HUMAN INFANTS 109
level of conditioning than the youngest group. In addition, infants who were
trained at 20 days of age and tested at 30 days of age performed significantly
better than 30-day-olds being trained for the first time. The finding that infants
exhibited significant retention after 10 days is particularly impressive in view
of the small number of reinforced presentations (50), the small percentage of
reinforced trials (71%), the brevity of the training session (20 min), and the age
at which the youngest infants were trained (10 days).
Solomon, Groccia-Ellison, Levine, Blanchard and Pendlebury (1990)
found that the optimal CS-UCS interval increases during early to late adult-
hood. Whereas young adults conditioned more rapidly with an ISI of 400 ms
than with ISIs of 650 and 900 ms, elderly adults conditioned more slowly with
the 400-ms ISI. Their rate of conditioning was equivalent to that of young
adults who were trained with a 900-ms ISI. Solomon et al. concluded that their
facilitated conditioning at the longest ISI was not a result of age differences in
sensory acuity or motor function but probably reflected age differences in
synaptic efficacy. Taken together, the infant and adult data reveal that the
temporal relationships in classical conditioning are an inverted U-shaped
function of age (see also Chapter 4).
The heart-rate response was successfully conditioned in premature
hydraencephalic and decerebrate infants via a UCS-omission procedure
(Berntson, Tuber, Ronca & Bachman 1983; Tuber, Berntson, Bachman & Allen
1980). Omitting the UCS when it was expected to occur elicited a what-is-it
reflex (Pavlov 1927), which was reflected in the heart-rate CR. The preceding
result is consistent with evidence that amnesics (Warrington & Weiskrantz
1979; Woodruff-Pak 1993), hippocampally-damaged adult animals (O’Keefe
& Nadel 1978), and even Aplysia (Kandel & Hawkins 1992) can be classically
conditioned. Because of such data, many scientists have concluded that all
conditioning tasks — both classical and operant — require only a primitive
memory system, regardless of whether they take a multiple-memory-systems
approach (Mandler 1984, 1998; Schacter & Moscovitch 1984) or a processing
approach (Roediger et al. 1990; Roediger & Srinivas 1993).
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
110 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
MEMORY DEVELOPMENT IN HUMAN INFANTS 111
Figure 6.4. The mobile task. (a) The experimental arrangement during baseline and the
delayed recognition test. Here, the ankle ribbon is attached to an empty stand while the
mobile is suspended from a second stand. In this arrangement, the infant’s kicks cannot
move the mobile. (b) The experimental arrangement during acquisition. Shown here is a 3-
month-old who is moving the mobile via a ribbon that is strung from his ankle to the same
stand as the mobile.
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
112 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
Figure 6.5. The train task. Shown here is a 6-month-old pressing the lever below the
Plexiglas window to move the train inside the box during an acquisition phase. During the
delayed recognition test, the lever is deactivated so that lever presses do not move the train.
recognize the novel mobile, failing to respond above their baseline level. Their
behavior was taken as evidence that they discriminated the test mobile as
different from the training mobile they had seen 24 hours earlier.
Subsequent studies using the mobile and train tasks have found that
retention increases linearly between 2 and 18 months of age (Hartshorn, Rovee-
Collier, Gerhardstein, Bhatt, Wondoloski et al. 1998b; see Figure 6.6). When
infants are tested in the mobile conjugate reinforcement paradigm, for example,
2-month-olds exhibit retention for 1 or 2 days, 3-month-olds exhibit retention
for 5 or 6 days, and 6-month-olds exhibit retention for 14 or 15 days. When 6-
month-olds are tested in the operant train task, their maximum retention is
identical to their maximum retention in the mobile conjugate reinforcement
task. Furthermore, as in the mobile task, retention in the train task increases with
age: 6-month-olds exhibit retention for 2 weeks, 9-month-olds exhibit retention
for 6 weeks, 12-month-olds exhibit retention for 8 weeks, 15-month-olds
exhibit retention for 10 weeks, and 18-month-olds exhibit retention for 13
weeks.
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
MEMORY DEVELOPMENT IN HUMAN INFANTS 113
Figure 6.6. The maximum duration (in weeks) of delayed recognition of independent
groups of infants who were trained and tested in the mobile task (2 to 6 months of age) and
the train task (6 to 18 months of age). At 6 months of age, infants were trained and tested in
both tasks, and their long-term retention in both tasks was the same. Also shown is the
maximum duration (in weeks) of retention of independent groups of infants who were
tested in the puppet task (6 to 18 months of age). Although the absolute duration of retention
in the operant conditioning and deferred imitation paradigms is different, the pattern of
retention in the two paradigms is identical.
Deferred imitation is another task that can be used to study memory during
infancy and early childhood. Simply put, this task incorporates a “monkey see,
monkey do” procedure in which an adult models a behavior, and the infant is
then given the opportunity to imitate it after a delay. Deferred imitation played
an important role in Piaget’s (1962) theory of the emergence of mental
representation during infancy. In tasks that are based on Piaget’s original
conception of deferred imitation, infants do not practice the target behavior(s)
prior to the test. In this way, their memory performance is based exclusively
on a representation of the originally modeled event and not on a memory of
their own prior actions.
According to Piaget (1962), deferred imitation does not emerge until
infants are approximately 18 to 24 months of age. Although a number of
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
114 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
Figure 6.7. The experimental arrangement used with 6- to 24-month-old infants in deferred
imitation studies. Shown here is a 6-month-old removing a mitten from the puppet’s hand
— an action the experimenter had modeled 24 hours earlier.
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
MEMORY DEVELOPMENT IN HUMAN INFANTS 115
mitten. The infant’s ability to imitate these target actions is then assessed after
a delay (see Figure 6.7). Six-month-olds can imitate these actions 24 hours
later if the demonstration lasts 60 s but not if it lasts only 30 s (Barr et al.
1996). This latter result suggests that failures to document deferred imitation
by young infants are as likely to reflect the choice of an inappropriate task or
task parameters as the infants’ fundamental inability to imitate what they saw
an adult do earlier.
In a modified version of the deferred imitation procedure, infants are
allowed to practice the target behaviors prior to the retention interval (e.g.,
Bauer, Hertsgaard & Dow 1994). Using this procedure, Bauer and Shore
(1987) reported that 1- to 2-year-olds exhibited retention after 6 weeks.
Infants’ imitation is also influenced by the structure of the target event (Bauer
& Mandler 1989; Bauer & Shore 1987). Without exception, infants’ imitation
of actions that can only be performed in a specific temporal order (causal or
enabling events; e.g., making a rattle by placing a ball in a container, putting a
lid on it, and shaking it) is superior to their imitation of actions that can be
performed in any order (arbitrary events; e.g., dressing a teddy bear by putting
a ring on its finger, a scarf around its neck, and a cap on its head). The finding
occurs even when the enabling and arbitrary events have been matched on the
basis of target actions and event goals (Barr & Hayne 1996) and is identical to
findings with older children (Fivush, Kuebli & Clubb 1992) and adults
(Ratner, Smith & Dion 1986).
The range of behaviors that infants can imitate after a delay expands with
age from facial and body movements (Meltzoff & Moore 1994), to specific
actions on a specific object and then on similar objects (Barr et al. 1996;
Hayne, MacDonald & Barr 1997; Hayne et al. 2000a), to intended actions and
social goals (Meltzoff 1995). This developmental progression is thought to
reflect developmental increases in infants’ motor competence and cognitive
abilities as well as age changes in their social niche.
Schacter and Moscovitch (1984) were the first to suggest a direct analogy
between the memory performance of infants younger than 9 months of age
and the memory performance of amnesic adults. In their view, infants and
amnesics possess only an early memory system that supports unconscious or
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
116 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
implicit memory and lacked the late memory system that supports conscious
recollection or explicit memory. These conclusions were based on the extant
literature on infant memory development.
A closer review of Schacter and Moscovitch’s (1984) arguments, how-
ever, indicates that many of their conclusions were not supported either by the
data that they originally reviewed or by data that have been collected since
their seminal chapter was published. Below we outline four of the parallels
that they drew between the memory performance of amnesics and the memory
performance of human infants younger than 9 months of age. In addition, we
review the evidence for and against each claim.
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
MEMORY DEVELOPMENT IN HUMAN INFANTS 117
Rose 1981). Even when infants exhibit retention across sessions, however,
their memory performance is delay-dependent (Bahrick et al. 1997; Bahrick &
Pickens 1995).
Delayed-nonmatching-to-sample
A-not-B errors
Schacter and Moscovitch (1984) also cited data from object search tasks to
argue that the performance of infants and amnesics is the same. The data they
cited for amnesics originated in a study with six severely amnesic patients and
six control patients who exhibited mild cognitive deficits but no significant
memory loss and who did not differ from the amnesics in age, education, or
IQ. When tested in an object search task analogous to the traditional tasks that
had been used with infants, the amnesic patients — but not the controls —
committed the A-not-B error. Amnesic patients successfully found the object
when it was hidden at location B if they were tested after no delay, but they
unsuccessfully searched at location A — where the object was previously
found — after a delay.
The corresponding data that were cited for infants came from a study
conducted by Fox et al. (1979). In that study, infants were tested in a standard
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
118 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
object search task in which they first learned to search at location A, and then
the object was hidden at location B. Although 8- and 9-month-old infants
searched successfully at B when there was no delay between hiding and
searching, they committed the A-not-B error when there was a 7-s delay. In
contrast, 10-month-old infants successfully searched at B irrespective of
whether the delay was 0 or 7 s. On the basis of these data, Schacter and
Moscovitch (1984) concluded that the search performance of 8- and 9-month-
olds and adult amnesics was analogous.
More recent data, however, have refuted this analogy. Subsequent re-
search using the standard task has shown that the search performance of
infants of all ages declines as the delay between hiding and finding increases
and that the maximum delay that infants will tolerate increases as a function of
age (see Figure 6.2). As such, conclusions regarding the stage-like effect of
age on A-not-B errors from the Fox et al. (1979) study cited by Schacter and
Moscovitch (1984) are limited by the fact that the maximum delay after which
infants were tested was only 7 s. Furthermore, age-related gains in the maxi-
mum delay that infants tolerate increases at a relatively constant rate between
7½ and 12 months of age (Diamond 1985). This latter finding does not support
the view that infants suddenly gain access to the late memory system after 9
months of age. Finally, the finding that infants can solve object search tasks
much earlier in development when looking rather than searching manually
also suggests that their failure on the standard task is due to some process other
than memory (Baillargeon & Graber 1988; Baillargeon et al. 1989).
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
MEMORY DEVELOPMENT IN HUMAN INFANTS 119
actions during a single session, and the infant’s ability to reproduce those
actions is then assessed after a delay. In this task, infants can only respond to
the test stimuli on the basis of their memory for the single demonstration
episode. Given that infants as young as 6 months have been shown to exhibit
deferred imitation, we would conclude that, at least by this age, infants can
recall a single prior episode.
Deferred imitation is not the only task in which infants have exhibited
memory for a single prior episode. Research using the mobile conjugate
reinforcement paradigm has shown that at least by 3 months of age, infants
exhibit retention of an event that occurred on a single prior occasion (Greco et
al. 1990; Hayne, Greco-Vigorito & Rovee-Collier 1993; Rovee-Collier et al.
1993b). In a prototypic study, 3-month-old infants learned to kick to activate a
series of different yellow-block mobiles displaying alphanumeric characters
on each block (see Figure 6.8, bottom) and were subsequently provided with
information that a stained-glass and metal windchime (see Figure 6.8, top)
was movable. This information was provided to the infant during a 3-min
passive-exposure procedure during which the experimenter hung the wind-
chime from the mobile suspension hook and jiggled it while the infant merely
watched. This event occurred on only a single occasion 4 days after training.
When infants were shown the stationary windchime during a delayed recogni-
tion test 1 day later (5 days after training), they kicked vigorously, apparently
attempting to move it by using the response that they had previously used to
move the yellow-block mobiles. The infants kicked, however, only if they had
been provided with information that the windchime could move; infants who
had previously viewed the windchime while it was motionless did not attempt
to move it during the test (Greco et al. 1990; Rovee-Collier et al. 1993b). Also,
because infants had not moved the windchime themselves on that occasion,
they obviously had neither practiced kicking to move it nor been reinforced
for kicking to move it.
In another study demonstrating memory for a single prior episode, 3-
month-olds were trained for three sessions with a series of yellow-block
mobiles (Hayne et al. 1993). During session 3 only, a blue- and red-striped
cloth was draped around the sides of the crib. At the end of session 3, the ankle
ribbon was detached from the overhead suspension hook, the mobile was
removed, and the stationary windchime was suspended over the infant for 3
min in the presence of the blue- and red-striped context. The next day, infants
received a delayed recognition test with the stationary windchime. Even
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
120 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
Figure 6.8. (Top) The stained-glass and metal butterfly windchime to which 3-month-olds
were briefly exposed on a single occasion. (Bottom) One of the three mobiles with which
infants were initially trained; the blocks on all mobiles were yellow and displayed the same
figure (A or 2) in the same color, but the color of the figures differed from one mobile to the
next.
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
MEMORY DEVELOPMENT IN HUMAN INFANTS 121
though the blue- and red-striped context was not present during the test,
infants kicked robustly, apparently trying to move the windchime — even
though they had never before seen it move. A control group was treated
exactly like the experimental group except that the distinctive blue- and red-
striped cloth was not present either during their third session or when they saw
the stationary windchime immediately afterward. This group did not kick
during the delayed recognition test. These data indicated that the shared
session-3 context had linked the representation of the windchime with the
representation of the training event. This study offers unambiguous evidence
that the delayed recognition performance of the experimental group during the
24-hour test resulted from the retrieval of their memory for a single prior
episode — session 3. Moreover, although the training event and the stationary
windchime had been integrated by means of the common context, like a
catalyst, that context was unnecessary for infants’ subsequent test perfor-
mance once the integration had occurred.
Unfortunately, many researchers have summarily dismissed infants’
memory performance in the mobile conjugate reinforcement task without
understanding how the task has actually been exploited to study infants’
memory abilities (e.g., Nelson 1995; Mandler 1998; Schacter & Moscovitch
1984). Because the initial response in this procedure is established by condi-
tioning, for example, they think that studies using this procedure tap only
implicit memory that is acquired gradually or incrementally through rein-
forced practice (e.g., Bauer 1995, 1997; Bauer & Hertsgaard 1993; Mandler
1984, 1990, 1998; Schacter & Moscovitch 1984).
On the contrary, the experimental procedures described above are re-
markably similar to the procedures commonly used in deferred imitation tasks
— tasks that these same individuals argue tap explicit memory (Bauer 1995,
1997; Bauer & Hertsgaard 1993; Mandler 1984, 1990; Nelson 1995, see
below). As in deferred imitation, for example, the target action and its conse-
quence were not physically present at the time of the delayed recognition test
with the windchime in the mobile task. Also, in both the deferred imitation and
mobile tasks, the experimenter provides the subject with a prop (e.g., a
mobile) in lieu of instructions, thereby creating an occasion for cued recall, but
the specific action to be performed is not externally cued. Finally, as in studies
of deferred imitation, infants acquired information about the windchime
through observation alone; during the test, infants merely applied an action
that was already in their behavioral repertoire upon the test object in order to
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
122 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
Although the analogy between infants and amnesics originally put forward by
Schacter and Moscovitch has not withstood empirical scrutiny, their argument
has been reiterated by others who still contend that the memory processing by
infants and amnesics is the same. In fact, the current strategy used by these
investigators resembles that of Schacter and Moscovitch (1984) in fundamen-
tal ways. Consistent with Schacter and Moscovitch, a common approach to
infant memory development has been to find tasks that amnesics fail and
then determine whether or not infants fail the same tasks (Mandler 1990;
McDonough et al. 1995; McKee & Squire 1993). Although the outcome of
this process is often used to support the analogy between infants and amnesics,
recent empirical work in at least two new domains — deferred imitation and
response-to-novelty tasks — indicates that no such analogy exists.
Adult amnesics have recently been shown to fail on two other nonverbal tasks
that are typically used with infants — deferred imitation and response-to-
novelty tasks (McDonough et al. 1995; McKee & Squire 1993). McDonough
et al. (1995), for example, found that human adults with amnesia did not
exhibit retention in a deferred imitation task when tested after a 24-hour delay.
In their study, an experimenter demonstrated a series of three-step actions with
objects. Healthy adults, patients with frontal-lobe damage, and patients with
amnesia were then given the opportunity to reproduce those actions 24 hours
later. Although healthy adults and patients with frontal lobe damage repro-
duced more of the actions during the test than they had prior to the demonstra-
tion (i.e., during baseline), patients with amnesia did not.
Although imitation, like DNMS, has traditionally been viewed as a
relatively late developmental milestone (McCall et al. 1977; Piaget 1962;
Uzgiris 1981), more recent research has shown that at some point during the first
year of life, infants are able to exhibit imitation even when tested for the first
time after a delay (i.e., deferred imitation). In the first experiment of this kind,
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
MEMORY DEVELOPMENT IN HUMAN INFANTS 123
Response-to-novelty tasks
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
124 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
MEMORY DEVELOPMENT IN HUMAN INFANTS 125
Conclusions
The assumption that the Jacksonian principle extends to the normal develop-
ment and dissolution of memory systems is intuitively appealing, but evidence
on the development of memory in human infants and its dissolution in amne-
sics has not supported this assumption. This result is hardly surprising given
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
126 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
that the demise of memory function in adults with severe brain damage does
not even parallel the loss of memory function in healthy, aging adults
(Campbell et al. 1984). In fact, brain-damaged adults do not even provide a
suitable model for understanding many developmental disorders (Karmiloff-
Smith 1998).
The empirical evidence reviewed in this chapter provides no basis for the
analogy between the development of memory in infants and the dissolution of
memory in amnesic adults. First, across experimental tasks, even very young
infants exhibit retention in tasks that adult amnesics do not (see Table 6.1).
Second, age-related changes in infants’ performance within a given task give
no hint of the stage-like transition in memory performance that would be
expected if a fundamentally different memory system, such as the one that is
subsequently lost in amnesia, were to emerge de novo at some point during
development (see Figure 6.2 and Figure 6.6). Third, the memory of preverbal
infants differs in a number of other ways from the memory of adult amnesics.
Not only have infants yet to develop language, for example, but also they have
yet to develop the vast networks of associations that adult amnesics acquired
over a lifetime. Finally, the poor memory that is exhibited by developing
organisms is not a pathological condition, whereas the poor memory that is
exhibited by brain-damaged amnesics clearly is (Campbell et al. 1984). Given
the preceding considerations, the fact that some scientists still take the decline
in memory performance during amnesia as evidence of how memory normally
develops is surprising.
Table 6.1 . The Success of Adults and Human Infants < 9 Months of Age on the DNMS,
Deferred Imitation, and Novelty-Preference Tasks
Group Memory Task
DNMS Deferred Imitation Novelty Preference
Adult Amnesics No No No
Infants < 9 months Yes Yes Yes
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
CHAPTER 7
The ontogenetic hierarchy in which implicit memory develops before explicit memory was
originally inferred from the excellent memory performance of amnesic adults on priming
tests and their poor memory performance on recall/recognition tests. Similar functional
memory dissociations have been produced in normal adults by means of different test
instructions. This chapter reviews evidence that very young infants exhibit memory disso-
ciations exactly like those exhibited by normal adults on priming and recognition tests, and
they do so in response to manipulations of all of the same independent variables.
Recall that infants and amnesics were commonly assumed to possess only
implicit memory. To determine when explicit memory develops, therefore,
some researchers attempted to find tasks that amnesics fail, but normal adults
can solve. Once such a task was found, they then asked at what age preverbal
infants could first solve them. Studies in which researchers took this approach
were discussed in Chapter 6. A second approach to the development of
explicit memory, however, is to ask at what age infants exhibit the same
functional memory dissociations as adults. Studies that have addressed this
question are the focus of Chapter 7.
Teuber (1955) was the first to propose the idea of a double dissociation in
memory performance. Tulving (1983) described the rule of experimental
dissociation as follows:
“Dissociation is said to have occurred if it is found that the manipulated
variable affects subjects’ performance in one of two tasks, but not in the other,
or affects the performance in different directions in the two tasks. Thus,
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
128 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
MEMORY DISSOCIATIONS IN INFANTS 129
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
130 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
Figure 7.1. (A) Schematic of the delayed recognition task showing training and the long-
term retention test (i.e., the delayed recognition test). (B) Schematic of the reactivation task,
showing training, the reactivation treatment, and the long-term retention test. The only
difference between these tasks is the brief reactivation (priming) treatment prior to the long-
term retention test. The memory probe in (A) serves as the reactivation stimulus or memory
prime in (B); the long-term retention test is usually with the original training stimulus.
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
MEMORY DISSOCIATIONS IN INFANTS 131
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
132 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
(A) (B)
(C)
(D)
Figure 7.2. Stimuli that have served as memory primes for preverbal infants. (A) the
photograph that 5-month-olds had viewed during the familiarization phase of a response-to-
novelty task (photograph courtesy of J.F. Fagan, III.); (B) the mobile that had been used
during operant conditioning with 2-, 3-, and 6-month-olds; (C) the training context that had
been present during mobile conditioning with 3-month-olds; (D) the hand-puppet on which
target actions had been demonstrated to 6-month-olds in the presence of the train (the bell is
pinned on the puppet’s back instead of inside its mitten).
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
MEMORY DISSOCIATIONS IN INFANTS 133
Figure 7.3. The forgetting and reforgetting functions of the original memory and the
reactivated memory, respectively, as a function of the number of days after training or
priming when retention testing occurred. The memory prime was presented either 13 or 27
days after training was over (priming), when forgetting was complete. Each data point
represents a different group of 3-month-old infants.
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
134 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
prime reactivated the memory and restored infants’ retention to the same level
that it was immediately after training. Note also that the reactivated memory
was not transient but was reforgotten at approximately the same rate as the
original memory.
Examples of Delayed Recognition and Reactivation Tasks. The distinc-
tion between the delayed recognition and the reactivation tasks is well-
illustrated in a series of experiments with 3-month-olds in which we asked if
infants are sensitive to the same primitive perceptual units as adults (Rovee-
Collier, Hankins & Bhatt 1992a). These units, called textons (Julesz 1984) or
critical features (Treisman & Gelade 1980), are thought to be the building
blocks of perception. In the delayed recognition task, 3-month-olds failed to
discriminate between a pair of horizontal and vertical bars (two textons) that
were in different spatial relations (L versus T) after 24 hours, but they did
discriminate +s from both Ls and Ts. Presumably, the latter discrimination
occurred because a + contains an additional texton — a line crossing. In the
reactivation task, however, 3-month-olds discriminated between pairs of hori-
zontal and vertical black bars in all combinations. The procedural details of
these two studies are described below.
The delayed recognition task was used in Experiment 1. Here, indepen-
dent groups of infants were trained in their home cribs for 2 days (9 min per
day) with a five-object, pink-block mobile displaying two computer-gener-
ated black bars arranged as an L, T, or + on all sides of each block. The mobile
was suspended from one of two flexible stands that were clamped to opposing
crib rails (in studies with 6-month-olds, infants are situated in a sling seat
inside a playpen, and the mobile is suspended from one of two horizontal bars
on floor microphone stands that are placed on opposite sides of the playpen.)
During training, infants lay supine in their home cribs, and a white satin ribbon
was connected without slack from one ankle to the overhead mobile suspen-
sion bar (see Figure 6.4b). In this arrangement, each footkick moved the
overhead mobile with a rate and intensity proportional to the rate and vigor of
kicking (conjugate reinforcement: Rovee & Rovee 1969).
Each training session began and ended with a 3-min nonreinforcement
period when the ankle ribbon was attached to an empty stand. At the outset of
session 1, this 3-min period was the baseline phase when the infant’s un-
learned kick rate was measured; at the end of session 2, this 3-min period was
an immediate retention test when the infant’s final level of learning and
retention after zero delay were measured. The 3-min long-term retention test
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
MEMORY DISSOCIATIONS IN INFANTS 135
was identical to the baseline phase and immediate retention test and occurred
24 hours after training was over. At this time, either the same mobile (i.e., a
mobile displaying Ls for infants who were trained with Ls) or a different one
(i.e., a mobile displaying Ts or +s for infants who were trained with Ls) was
hung over each infant (see Figure 6.4a).
Retention was indexed by two individual measures of relative respond-
ing. The primary measure, the baseline ratio (response rate during the long-
term test divided by the same infant’s baseline rate), indicated the extent to
which test responding exceeded an infant’s pretraining baseline. Thus, if a
group kicked significantly above its pretraining baseline (i.e., M baseline ratio
significantly > 1.00), then this was taken as evidence of retention (if the test
mobile was the same as the training mobile) or generalization (if the test
mobile was different). If a group did not kick significantly above baseline (i.e.,
M baseline ratio not significantly > 1.00), then this was taken as evidence of
no retention or discrimination, respectively. Convergent evidence of no reten-
tion/discrimination was provided by the retention ratio (response rate during
the immediate retention test divided by the same infant’s response rate during
the long-term test). Thus, if a group kicked significantly less during the long-
term test than it had immediately after training (i.e., M retention ratio signifi-
cantly < 1.00), then this was taken as evidence of discrimination (if the test
mobile was different from the training mobile) or forgetting (if, for example,
the training and test mobiles were the same).
In Experiment 1, the no-change control group exhibited excellent reten-
tion on the 24-hour delayed recognition test, as did infants who were trained
with Ls and tested with Ts (or vice versa). Infants who were trained with either
Ls or Ts and were tested with +s (and vice versa), however, failed to respond
above baseline during the 24-hour test, indicating that they discriminated the
test stimulus from the training one.
The reactivation task was used in Experiment 2. Here, independent
groups of infants were tested exactly like the no-change group in Experiment
1 but were allowed 2 weeks to forget the original training memory first.
Twenty-four hours before the long-term test (13 days after training), these
infants were primed for 3 min with either the original training mobile or a
different one (see Figure 7.4). During the reactivation treatment, the end of the
ribbon was not attached to the infant’s ankle but was drawn and released by
the experimenter at the same rate that each infant had kicked during the final 3
min of training. In this way, each infant saw the mobile moving in a way that
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
136 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
was phenomenologically equivalent to what that same infant had seen at the
end of acquisition. During a standard delayed recognition test with the original
training mobile 24 hours after the reactivation treatment (14 days after train-
ing), we tested infants’ retention to determine whether or not the prime had
actually reactivated their forgotten memory. We also tested two control
groups that are requisite in studies of reactivation — a reactivation control
group and a forgetting control group. The reactivation control group was not
originally trained but was primed with the training mobile. This group ensured
that the reactivation treatment per se did not induce new learning that might be
misconstrued as retention during the long-term test. The forgetting control
group was originally trained but was not primed. This group ensured that
forgetting was complete by the time of the long-term retention test.
Figure 7.4. The experimental arrangement during the reactivation or priming treatment
with 3-month-olds. The ribbon was not connected to the infant’s ankle but was held by the
experimenter, who pulled it to move the mobile at the same rate that the infant had moved it
by kicking during the last few training minutes. The infant seat minimized spontaneous
kicking during the reactivation treatment.
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
MEMORY DISSOCIATIONS IN INFANTS 137
The no-change group, which was primed with the original training mo-
bile, exhibited excellent retention, but the forgetting control group and the
reactivation control group exhibited none. Infants who were trained with Ls
and primed with either Ts or +s also exhibited no retention during the long-
term test. The fact that Ls were not effective primes for infants trained with Ts
and vice versa suggested that infants in Experiment 1 had originally encoded
the difference between Ls and Ts but had forgotten this distinction by the time
of the 24-hour retention test with the other mobile. This suggestion was
subsequently confirmed: Infants who received a delayed recognition test only
1 hour after the end of training discriminated Ls from Ts and vice versa (Adler
& Rovee-Collier 1994).
The preceding experiments illustrate an important distinction between
reactivation and delayed recognition tasks. Recall that in a delayed recogni-
tion task, infants are tested with a stimulus that is either the same as or
different from the training one, whereas in a reactivation task, infants are
reactivated with a stimulus that is either the same as or different from the
training one. In a delayed recognition task, infants may fail to exhibit retention
(i.e., they discriminate) if the test stimulus differs from the training one; in the
reactivation task, however, they fail to exhibit retention because the reactiva-
tion stimulus differs from the training stimulus — not because the reactivation
stimulus differs from the test stimulus. In other words, they exhibit no reten-
tion if their forgotten memory was never reactivated in the first place (Hayne
& Rovee-Collier 1995; for review, see Rovee-Collier & Hayne 1987).
The delayed recognition and reactivation tasks that have been used with infants
correspond to two of the prototypic tasks that have been used in tests of implicit
and explicit memory with adults. The delayed recognition task is analogous to
the standard yes/no (e.g., Dorfman, Kihlstrom, Cork & Misiaszek 1995) and
old/new recognition tests (e.g., Mitchell & Brown 1988; Musen & Treisman
1990) that have been used in studies of explicit memory with adults. As in the
adult studies, the retrieval cue is presented at the time of the retention test
instead of before it (see Table 7.1). The reactivation task is analogous to
repetition priming tasks that researchers use to study implicit memory with
adults (see Graf & Schacter 1985; Nelson 1995). In studies with infants, the
reactivation stimulus (i.e., the memory prime) is a fractional component of the
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
138 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
Table 7.1. Tasks That Produce Implicit/Explicit Memory Dissociations in Adults and
Corresponding Tasks Used With Infants
Memory System Adult Task Infant Task
Implicit Priming Reactivation
Explicit Recognition Delayed Recognition
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
MEMORY DISSOCIATIONS IN INFANTS 139
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
140 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
MEMORY DISSOCIATIONS IN INFANTS 141
same; rather, the prior-cuing procedure and the particular instructions that
precede the test are the distinguishing factors. Twelve other independent
variables that also produce parallel memory dissociations for adults and
infants on recognition and priming tests are reviewed below.
Some researchers have objected to the analogy between reactivation tasks that
are used with infants and priming tasks that are used with adults. Their
objection has been based on initial evidence that the latency of response to a
memory prime by 3-month-olds (Fagen & Rovee-Collier 1983) and 6-month-
olds (Boller, Rovee-Collier, Borovsky, O’Connor & Shyi 1990) in a reactiva-
tion task is so slow — from 1 to 24 hours, whereas in adults, the response
latency to a word fragment or word stem is essentially instantaneous. Hildreth
and Rovee-Collier (1999a), however, found that the latency of responding to a
reactivation stimulus (i.e., a memory prime) decreases linearly over the first
year of life. By 9 months of age, infants respond to a prime within 1 min, and
by 12 months of age, they respond instantaneously, like adults.
These data are summarized in Figure 7.5, which also includes the latency
data previously collected from 3- and 6-month-olds. As can be seen, the
latency of response to a memory prime decreases linearly over the entire first
year of life. This result is particularly impressive when one considers that the
time between forgetting and priming in all experiments was held constant over
age; that is, at all ages, the prime was always presented 1 week after infants
had last remembered the task. Because infants take progressively longer to
forget the original training memory over the first year of life, the prime was
necessarily presented after increasingly longer delays with age. Thus, the
prime was presented 13 days after the original event to 3-month-olds, but it
was presented 2½ months after the original event to 12-month-olds. Despite
this, the latency of priming decreased linearly between 3 and 12 months of
age. In contrast, priming has never been studied in normal adults after forget-
ting is complete (i.e., when subjects cannot recognize the stimulus) but only in
amnesics.
The preceding results suggest that a maturation-based factor is respon-
sible for the age-related decrease in priming latency. This suggestion is
consistent with prior evidence that experimental manipulations that accelerate
or retard CNS maturation in mice produce corresponding effects on long-term
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
142 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
Figure 7.5. The decrease in the latency (log s) of responding to a memory prime over the
first year of life. Three-month-olds were trained, primed, and tested in the mobile task (open
circles), whereas 9- and 12-month-olds were trained, primed, and tested in the train task
(filled circles); 6-month-olds were studied in both tasks.
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
MEMORY DISSOCIATIONS IN INFANTS 143
Figure 7.6. The decrease in 3-month-olds’ latency of responding after a second memory
prime. Independent groups of infants were given one (striped columns) or two (dark
columns) reactivation treatments and then were tested from 1 to 24 hours later (14 days after
training). The no-reminder control group (open column), which received no reactivation
treatment, exhibited no retention during the 14-day test.
to a single memory prime 13 days after mobile training, they did not exhibit
renewed retention until 24 hours later. When they were exposed to two primes
— the first on day 7 and the second on day 13, they exhibited renewed
retention only 1 hour later (see Figure 7.6). The preceding data reveal that
prior retrieval experience is also a significant factor in the latency of response
to a memory prime. The latter result is reminiscent of Tulving’s (1983)
description of a reduction in access time to information in semantic memory as
a consequence of repeated retrievals in adults. He characterized this reduction
as an improvement in retrieval skill with practice.
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
144 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
Table 7.2. Independent Variables and Their Effects on Adult Performance in Implicit and
Explicit Memory Tasks
Independent Variable Effect on Implicit Tasks Effect on Explicit Tasks
age* none large
amnesia* none large
retention interval none large
vulnerability (interference) limited great
number of study trials small large
study (exposure) time none large
number of items studied none large
level of processing none large
spacing effects none large
affect* important less important
affect none large
serial position transient or none persisistent primacy effect
studied size none large
memory load none large
context dependency less pronounced more pronounced
*Subject variable
and normal adults. These same variables produce the same dissociations in the
memory performance of very young infants, who are thought to lack explicit
memory, on reactivation and delayed recognition tasks. In this section, we
present data from adults that illustrate the memory dissociation produced by
each independent variable on implicit and explicit tasks and data from infants
that illustrate the corresponding memory dissociation produced by the same
independent variable on delayed recognition and reactivation tasks. The bulk
of the infant data was compiled from mobile studies with infants, but infant
data that are available from other paradigms are also presented.
1. Age. As Tulving put it, “The neural pathways that subserve episodic
remembering, maturing late in childhood and deteriorating early in old age,
are not necessary for priming” (Tulving 1991, p. 16). In fact, tests of explicit
memory with 3- to 11-year-olds and young adults (Carroll, Byrne & Kirsner
1985; Greenbaum & Graf 1989; Mitchell 1993; Naito 1990) have found that
recall and recognition improve with age, but the amount of priming remains
stable across age. Performance also improves with age on imitation tests of
explicit memory. The number of logically ordered steps that toddlers success-
fully imitated on an immediate cued-recall test increases between 13 and 20
months of age (Bauer 1996), and the level of deferred imitation of a three-step
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
MEMORY DISSOCIATIONS IN INFANTS 145
sequence also increases between 6 and 18 months of age (Hayne et al. 2000a;
Hayne 1997).
Memory performance in mobile studies similarly improves with increas-
ing age over the first half year of life in the delayed recognition task (the
explicit memory task) but not in the memory reactivation task (the implicit
memory task). Gekoski, Fagen, and Pearlman (1984) compared the memory
performance of premature infants who were born between 31.1 and 39.7
weeks of gestational age on a delayed recognition test with that of full-term 3-
month-olds. The mean conceptional age of the preterm and full-term infants at
the time of testing did not differ (51.3 and 53.4 weeks, respectively), but their
mean chronological age did (156.4 and 81.7 days, respectively). All infants
were trained in the mobile task and were tested for recognition of the training
mobile 1 week later. During the 1-week test, only full-term infants exhibited
significant retention. This difference in retention resulted despite the fact that
the preterm infants were of equivalent conceptional age and almost 3 months
older in chronological age than the full-term infants. The memory perfor-
mance of the premature group paralleled that of full-term 2-month-olds, who
also fail to exhibit significant recognition after a 1-week delay.
Figure 7.7 shows that the magnitude of infants’ retention in a delayed
recognition test 1 week after training increases as a function of age despite the
fact that the final levels of acquisition were equivalent (Hayne 1990; Hill,
Borovsky & Rovee-Collier 1988; Rovee-Collier 1984; Sullivan, Rovee-
Collier & Tynes 1979). The same figure also shows that the magnitude of
retention following a reactivation treatment that was administered 3 weeks
after training is not affected by age (Davis & Rovee-Collier 1983; Hayne
1990; Hill et al. 1988; Rovee-Collier 1984; Rovee-Collier et al. 1980; Vander
Linde, Morrongiello & Rovee-Collier 1985).
The age-related increase in infants’ memory performance on delayed
recognition tasks is not unique to the mobile procedure. Hartshorn et al.
(1998b) trained infants between 6 and 18 months of age in the train procedure
(Hartshorn & Rovee-Collier 1997; see Chapter 6) and tested their delayed
recognition after delays that incremented in 1-week steps until they failed to
exhibit significant retention for 2 consecutive weeks. These data were then
combined with delayed recognition data previously obtained from infants
between 2 and 6 months of age who were identically trained and tested in the
mobile procedure. Figure 7.8 summarizes the resulting age changes in reten-
tion over the first year-and-a-half of life. This figure presents infants’ mean
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
146 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
Figure 7.7. Memory performance of 2-, 3-, and 6-month-olds on a delayed recognition test
(striped columns) 1 week after training and on a reactivation test (filled columns) 3 weeks
after training. (A retention ratio = 1.00 indicates no forgetting from the immediate to the
long-term test.) Asterisks mark groups that displayed retention (M baseline ratio signifi-
cantly > 1.00).
retention ratios after a 0-day retention interval, after the longest retention
interval at which infants of a given age performed significantly above base-
line, and after the longest retention interval at which infants of a given age
were tested (i.e., when they no longer performed significantly above baseline).
As can be seen, all infants exhibited equivalent retention after short test
delays, but infants remembered after progressively longer delays with age,
irrespective of task.
These findings are identical to those originally obtained by Campbell and
Campbell (1962) and in subsequent animal studies of memory development
with a variety of different species (for reviews, see Campbell & Coulter 1976;
Coulter 1979; Nagy 1979). In those studies, animals ranging from prewean-
lings to adults were required to learn a simple response-reinforcement contin-
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
MEMORY DISSOCIATIONS IN INFANTS 147
Figure 7.8. Mean retention ratios of independent groups of infants between 2 and 18
months of age who were trained for 2 consecutive days in the mobile task (2 to 6 months,
filled symbols) or the train task (6 to 18 months, open symbols) and received an immediate
retention test after the conclusion of acquisition on day 2. Six-month-olds were trained and
tested in both tasks. Infants of each age received a delayed recognition test after different
retention intervals until, as shown in the last point on each curve, they exhibited no retention
(i.e., M baseline ratio not significantly > 1.00).
gency that was well within the sensorimotor capacity of all ages studied. With
the exception of preweanlings, who occasionally responded at slightly lower
rates and reached the learning asymptote more slowly than more mature
subjects, animals showed no age differences in acquisition, reached the learn-
ing asymptote at the same rate, and exhibited similar levels of retention shortly
after learning. Despite this, younger animals consistently forgot more rapidly.
The maximum duration of retention that was exhibited by infants be-
tween 2 and 18 months of age in the mobile task (Hartshorn et al. 1998b) and
between 6 and 18 months of age in the deferred imitation task (Hayne &
Campbell 1997; Herbert & Hayne 2000) was described in Chapter 6 (see
Figure 6.6). These functions reveal that long-term memory improves mono-
tonically as a function of age over the first 2 years of life. There is no
suggestion that long-term memory is suddenly enhanced at 8 months of age, as
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
148 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
was postulated by Kagan (1984; Kagan & Hamburg 1981), or that delayed
recognition changes abruptly at 8 or 9 months of age, when the late-maturing
memory system was thought to become functionally mature (Nadel & Zola-
Morgan 1984; Schacter & Moscovitch 1984), or when the transition from
implicit to pre-explicit memory was hypothesized to occur (Nelson 1995,
1997). Nor is there any indication that memory changes qualitatively with the
emergence of language in the second year of life (Best 1995; Herbert & Hayne
2000; Nelson 1990).
These age differences in retention (see Figure 7.8 and Figure 6.6) are not
due to differences in the baseline levels of activity or the original levels of
learning in either the conditioning or imitation tasks — there were no age
differences. Analyses revealed that infants’ operant levels and degree of
original learning did not change with age whether they were studied in the
mobile or train task over the period of 2 to 18 months (Hartshorn et al., 1998b)
or in the imitation task between 6 and 24 months of age (Herbert & Hayne
2000). These results are particularly striking when one considers the major
physical, behavioral, and cognitive changes that occur over the course of the
infancy period (see Figure 7.9), including marked changes in motoric and
linguistic competence.
The preceding findings demonstrate that immature infants are as capable
of encoding memories as more mature infants. We conclude, therefore, that
age differences in explicit memory must arise from maturational differences in
the neural mechanisms that are responsible for either maintaining access to the
Figure 7.9. Left to right: Infants 2, 3, 6, 9, 12, 15, or 18 months of age. Note the dramatic
physical and behavioral differences between younger and older infants over the first year-
and-a-half of life.
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
MEMORY DISSOCIATIONS IN INFANTS 149
stored memory over time or retrieving it. We suspect that these neural mecha-
nisms involve the perceptual mechanisms that are associated with selective
attention. Younger infants would have leaner memories, for example, if they
selectively attended to fewer elements in a given situation. As a result, fewer
effective retrieval cues would be available for them to reperceive at the time of
testing (Treisman 1992), and their retention would suffer. We have previously
obtained evidence from 2- and 3-month-olds that supports this possibility
(Rovee-Collier, Earley & Stafford 1989). At 2 months of age, infants do not
appear to process information about the environmental context within which
mobile training occurs, but at 3 months of age, they do. In a related vein,
Gordon (1979) proposed that age differences in retention might reflect differ-
ences in the content of what is encoded at different ages. By this account,
infants of different ages acquire different kinds of information in the same
learning situation — perhaps because they have different learning histories or
find different components of the learning situation salient. Evidence of onto-
genetic differences in stimulus selection in animals has also been reported by
Spear and Kucharski (1984).
Additional support for the proposition that age differences in selective
attention affect age differences in retention comes from the finding that at 3
months of age, the duration for which infants can recognize a particular
stimulus can be increased or decreased relative to control performance simply
by increasing or decreasing their attention to that stimulus during original
training (Adler, Gerhardstein & Rovee-Collier 1998; see Level of Processing,
below).
A dissociation in memory performance on cued-recall and reactivation
tasks was also found for infants of different ages in a study that used a
reenactment procedure (Sheffield & Hudson 1994). In this study, 14- and 18-
month-olds engaged in a different activity at each of six different stations in a
laboratory playroom during a single visit. Each activity consisted of a defined
set of target actions (e.g., get fish food from the cabinet, feed the fish, put the
food back in the cabinet). On the cued-recall test, children returned to the
playroom and produced as many of the target actions at each station as they
could. The 14-month-olds remembered the initial activities for 8 weeks,
whereas the 18-month-olds remembered them for 10 weeks. After forgetting
was complete, new groups at both ages again engaged in the original activi-
ties. After their original memory was presumably forgotten, they received a
reactivation treatment in which they watched an experimenter model half of
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
150 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
the original activities. When tested for retention of the remaining half of the
activities 24 hours later, infants of both ages exhibited the same magnitude of
reactivation (i.e., the number of nonmodeled activities performed). The de-
layed recognition/reactivation dissociation found in this study was identical to
the dissociation that was obtained in mobile studies with younger infants,
despite the fact that the experimental paradigms were very different. Whereas
the younger infants had learned to produce the target response (kicking) via a
conditioning procedure, infants in the preceding study were initially shown
how to find and use the materials required for each activity and then reenacted
that target response.
In the response-to-novelty task, infants who were familiarized with the
training stimulus in a single session also remembered progressively longer
with age on a explicit (recognition) test. Rose (1981) found that 6- and 9-
month-olds exhibited an equivalent novelty preference when given a paired-
comparison test with four different stimuli immediately after familiarization.
After delays ranging from 75 to 150 s, however, 6-month-olds exhibited a
novelty preference only for a facial stimulus, but 9-month-olds continued to
show a novelty preference for all of the stimuli that they had preferred on the
immediate response-to-novelty test. Greco and Daehler (1985) familiarized
even older infants — 2-year-olds — with exemplars of basic-level categories
and found that they still exhibited a novelty preference after 1 week. (Data
from a reactivation task were not collected.)
2. Retention Interval. A basic observation of human memory is that
retention on standard recognition, cued-recall, and free-recall tests declines
with the passage of time (Wickelgren 1972; Woodworth 1938). In contrast,
priming effects are long-lasting in both normal adults and children (Cave
1997; Komatsu & Ohta 1984; Mitchell & Brown 1988; Roediger & Blaxton
1987; Tulving et al. 1982) and amnesics (Cave & Squire 1992; Schacter, Chiu
& Ochsner 1993a; Tulving, Hayman & Macdonald 1991). Tulving et al.
(1982), for example, primed subjects with words and found that their memory
performance on a word-fragment completion test was stable over a period of 1
week, but their performance on a yes/no recognition test declined over the
same delay. Similar data were reported by Naito (1990, Experiment 3), who
assessed the proportion of correct word-fragment completion and recognition
scores for old and new items in Japanese first graders, sixth graders, and
college students after delays of 7 min and 6 days. As in Tulving et al.,
recognition memory scores declined over the retention interval for all ages,
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
MEMORY DISSOCIATIONS IN INFANTS 151
but word-fragment completion did not change over the same delay.
Musen and Treisman (1990), using visual patterns with adults, also found
a perceptual priming effect that remained stable across delays ranging from a
few hours to 1 week, but pattern recognition declined significantly across the
same period. In the perceptual priming task, subjects were briefly exposed to a
series of visual patterns that they had previously studied or that were novel.
After each exposure, they attempted to draw the pattern just seen, and the
difference in accuracy between subjects’ reproductions of previously studied
and novel patterns was taken as the measure of implicit memory. For the
measure of explicit memory, subjects were asked to select which pattern of
four alternatives they remembered having studied. Using a picture-naming
paradigm, Cave and Squire (1992) found that amnesics demonstrated a repeti-
tion priming effect that was of normal magnitude 1 week after study, yet
amnesics cannot recognize pictures (an explicit task) after even a short delay.
In adults, repetition priming effects can be quite long-lasting. Mitchell
and Brown (1988) obtained a robust priming effect on a picture-naming task
that remained stable across delays ranging from 1 to 6 weeks, and naming
times were not affected by whether or not the picture was recognized. In
contrast, performance on an old/new picture recognition task declined over the
same period. Cave (1997) replicated their results and found in addition that
adults still exhibited significant repetition priming after delays as long as 48
weeks in the same task. Similarly, 9-month-olds still exhibit a significant
priming effect almost 6 weeks after reactivation in the operant train procedure,
and 12-month-olds still exhibit a significant priming effect at least 8 weeks
afterward (Hildreth & Rovee-Collier 1999b).
The same pattern of memory dissociations was obtained in studies of
deferred imitation with infants. Bauer, Hertsgaard and Wewerka (1995) re-
ported that the number of correctly ordered pairs of actions produced by 14-
month-olds immediately following a verbal prime was approximately
equivalent whether children were tested after 1 week or after 1 month, but
children’s imitation performance on a cued-recall test (i.e., an explicit
memory test) decreased as the retention interval increased from 1 week to 1
month. Hayne and her colleagues have likewise reported that retention in a
deferred imitation task varies both as a function of the retention interval and
age. In all studies, older infants remembered longer (Barr & Hayne, 2000;
Herbert & Hayne 2000)
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
152 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
A similar memory dissociation was found for 14- and 18-month-olds who
were trained to perform six distinct activities in a single session in a multiple-
activities paradigm. The magnitude of retention in the delayed recognition task
progressively declined as the retention interval between training and testing
increased to 8 and 10 weeks, respectively (Hudson 1994), but the magnitude of
retention following a reactivation treatment was the same whether the delay
between training and priming was 8 weeks or 10 weeks (Sheffield & Hudson
1994).
In mobile studies with infants ranging in age from 2 through 6 months,
performance on delayed recognition tests is also affected by the retention
interval, but performance on priming tests is not. Figure 7.10, for example,
shows that the memory performance of 3-month-olds on a delayed recognition
test declined steadily with increases in the training-test delay (Hayne 1990;
Rovee-Collier 1984; Sullivan et al. 1979), but the magnitude of their retention
24 hours after a reactivation treatment was the same whether the interval
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
MEMORY DISSOCIATIONS IN INFANTS 153
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
154 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
obtained when infants were trained with the same mobile in different contexts
(distinctively colored-and-patterned crib or playpen liners) in each of two
sessions and were then tested for recognition of the mobile in the session-1
context. At both 3 months (Rovee-Collier & DuFault 1991) and 6 months
(Amabile & Rovee-Collier 1991), training in a different context in session 2
impaired infants’ ability to recognize the mobile in the original context during
the 24-hour delayed recognition test. Retroactive interference also occurred
when both 3-month-olds (Rossi-George & Rovee-Collier 1999) and 6-month-
olds (Boller & Rovee-Collier 1992) were trained in the same context for 2
days and then were simply allowed to view a novel context immediately after
training. Infants could no longer recognize the training mobile in the original
context 24 hours later; instead, they could recognize it only in the new context.
Figure 7.11. Magnitude of delayed recognition (left panel) 1 day after training and magni-
tude of reactivation (right panel) 3 weeks after training at 6 months of age. After reactiva-
tion in the original context, groups of 6-month-olds were exposed to a novel context
between training or priming and testing (Interference) or were not exposed to an interpo-
lated context (None) and were tested in the original training context 1 day later. (A retention
ratio = 1.00 indicates no forgetting from the immediate to the long-term retention test).
Asterisks mark groups that displayed retention (M baseline ratio significantly > 1.00).
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
MEMORY DISSOCIATIONS IN INFANTS 155
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
156 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
lag 0 and lag 4 but not at lag 15; after five trials, however, repetition facilitated
recognition, as measured in both reaction time and accuracy, after all lags
(Experiment 3). In addition, although the repetition effect in a recognition
(explicit memory) task was bigger in the five- than in the one-trial learning
condition, in a structural (implicit memory) face/nonface discrimination task,
it was independent of the number of study trials (Experiment 4).
In deferred imitation studies with infants, the benefit of additional study
trials has also been demonstrated. Bauer et al. (1995) gave 14-month-olds
either one or three trials and measured their ability to reproduce a series of
ordered events either 1 week or, for the three-trial group only, 1 month later.
Infants who received only a single trial exhibited excellent cued-recall after 1
week but poor cued-recall after 1 month; infants who received three trials,
however, performed as well after 1 month as infants who received only one
trial performed after 1 week. Likewise, Barr et al. (1996) found that 6-month-
olds could imitate target actions on a hand-puppet 24 hours after watching six
but not three demonstrations of the actions.
In mobile studies with 3-month-olds, the number of training trials also
affects memory performance on delayed recognition tests but not on reactiva-
tion tests. Figure 7.12 shows that increasing the number of study trials from
one to three significantly extended the delayed recognition of 3-month-olds
(Ohr, Fagen, Rovee-Collier, Vander Linde & Hayne 1989) but had no effect
on the magnitude of their retention after a reactivation treatment (Greco,
Hayne & Rovee-Collier 1990; Rovee-Collier et al. 1980). Here, each addi-
tional trial prolonged recognition by an additional week, but both two and
three successive training trials produced exactly the same magnitude of reten-
tion 1 day following a reactivation treatment that was administered 3 weeks
after the end of training.
This result was not age-specific: At 2 months of age, infants trained for 18
min exhibited excellent retention 1 week later, but infants trained for 6 or 12
min exhibited none (Rovee-Collier 1984; Vander Linde et al. 1985). Simi-
larly, at 6 months of age, two training trials also produced poorer 24-hour
delayed recognition than three (Merriman & Rovee-Collier 1995), but two
training trials produced the same magnitude of retention 1 day after a reactiva-
tion treatment (Borovsky & Rovee-Collier 1990) as three (Gulya, Sweeney &
Rovee-Collier 1999) or four (Timmons 1994) training trials. In our experi-
ence, however, an operant task that was acquired in only a single training trial
cannot be reactivated between 2 and 6 months of age, although actions that
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
MEMORY DISSOCIATIONS IN INFANTS 157
Figure 7.12. The effect of number of training sessions at 3 months on the magnitude of
delayed recognition (left panel) after different delays and the magnitude of reactivation
(right panel) 3 weeks after training. Asterisks mark groups that exhibited retention (M
baseline ratio significantly > 1.00).
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
158 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
Figure 7.13. The magnitude of reactivation 3 weeks after training for 3-month-old infants
who had received one, two, or three reactivations. In each case, retention was tested 1 day
after the final reactivation treatment.
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
MEMORY DISSOCIATIONS IN INFANTS 159
of time since the last retrieval. In a classic study (Landauer & Bjork 1978),
adults learned a number of names or name-picture associations during a single
study session in which each item was presented once. They then received three
or four cued-recall tests, but the intervals between succeeding tests were filled
with different numbers of names. Retention was poorest when test items were
massed (no intervening names), better when four or five items intervened
between successive tests, and best when the number of items (hence the time)
between successive tests progressively increased. They concluded that reten-
tion was facilitated because the expanding series of intertrial intervals permitted
adults to practice retrieval under increasingly difficult conditions. Subse-
quently, Rea and Modigliani (1985) found that children’s ability to recall
multiplication facts and spelling lists was significantly enhanced when they
used an expanding series of practice tests.
Both 2-month-olds (Vander Linde et al. 1985) and 3-month-olds (Rovee-
Collier, Evancio & Earley 1995) also exhibited significantly better delayed
recognition a week later when the interval between training sessions was
increased from 1 to 2 days. When the intersession interval was increased to 4
days for the 3-month-olds, however, the infants exhibited no retention whatso-
ever. This result was interpreted in terms of the “time window” construct
(Rovee-Collier 1995), which holds that there is a limited period of time
following an initial event (i.e., session 1) within which subsequent information
(i.e., session 2) can be integrated with the memory representation of the initial
event; if the new information is encountered after the time window has shut,
then it is not integrated with the initial event. In the Rovee-Collier et al. (1995)
study, the time window for integrating the effects of successive training
sessions apparently shut after 3 days (see Figure 7.14). When 3-month-olds
were trained and tested in a reactivation paradigm, however, the magnitude of
retention 1 day after priming was the same whether the sessions were spaced by
1 or 2 days; for infants whose second session was 4 days after their first (outside
the time window), however, the reactivation treatment was ineffective (Rovee-
Collier et al. 1995).
In another study (Rovee-Collier, Greco-Vigorito & Hayne 1993b), 3-
month-olds were trained for three sessions and then were shown a novel
moving object either immediately after training was over (when the time
window opened) or 4 days later (at the end of the time window, which closes
after 4 days with three training sessions). Infants who were exposed to the
object at the beginning of the time window recognized it for only 4 days, but
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
160 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
Figure 7.14. The effect of spacing between successive training sessions (session 1 = day 0;
session 2 = day 1, 2, or 4) on the magnitude of delayed recognition (left panel) 8 days after
session 1 and the magnitude of reactivation (right panel) 15 days after session 1 at 3 months
of age. Asterisks mark groups that displayed retention (M baseline ratio significantly >
1.00). The magnitude of recognition of group 0–2 is significantly greater than that of group
0–1, but the magnitude of reactivation of the two asterisked groups does not differ. The
second training session on day 4 was not integrated with the first training session on day 0;
that is, day 4 fell outside of the time window.
infants who were exposed to the object at the end of the time window
recognized it for 10 additional days. Although the delay between training and
exposure to the novel object affected infants’ magnitude of delayed recogni-
tion after a retention interval of 1 day, it had no effect on their magnitude of
reactivation after a retention interval of 3 weeks (see Figure 7.15; Greco et al.
1990; Rovee-Collier et al. 1993b).
In studies with 3-month-olds (Greco et al. 1990; Hayne, Rovee-Collier &
Perris 1987; Ohr et al. 1989), infants trained for three sessions on successive
days (days 0–1–2) had exhibited significant retention after 2 weeks but not
after 3 weeks. Hartshorn, Wilk, Muller and Rovee-Collier (1998c) replicated
this finding. In addition, they trained other groups for three sessions but
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
MEMORY DISSOCIATIONS IN INFANTS 161
Figure 7.15. The effect of the timing of a brief posttraining exposure to a novel object on
the magnitude of delayed recognition of that object (left panel) 1 day after the exposure and
on the magnitude of reactivation (right panel) 3 weeks after training at 3 months of age. (A
retention ratio = 1.00 indicates no forgetting.) Asterisks indicate that all groups displayed
retention (M baseline ratio significantly > 1.00).
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
162 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
MEMORY DISSOCIATIONS IN INFANTS 163
Figure 7.16. The effect of study time (session duration) on the magnitude of delayed
recognition of independent groups of 3-month-olds tested 7, 14, or 21 days later. Asterisks
mark groups that displayed retention (M baseline ratio significantly > 1.00).
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
164 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
MEMORY DISSOCIATIONS IN INFANTS 165
Figure 7.17. The effect of the number of different objects (studied items) on the training
mobile on the magnitude of delayed recognition 1 day after training (left panel) and the
magnitude of reactivation (right panel) 3 weeks after training at 2 and 3 months of age. (A
retention ratio = 1.00 indicates no forgetting.) Asterisks mark groups that displayed
retention (M baseline ratio significantly > 1.00).
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
166 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
In fact, infants who were trained with two sets of features discriminated
the recombinations (Bhatt & Rovee-Collier 1994, 1996), but infants who were
trained with three sets of features did not (see Figure 7.18, left panel), al-
though they did discriminate mobiles on which one of the test features was
novel (Bhatt & Rovee-Collier 1997). Even though they had not discriminated
a feature recombination during the delayed recognition test, however, only the
original mobile reactivated the forgotten memory 2 weeks later; a mobile
displaying feature recombinations did not (see Figure 7.18, right panel). This
is yet another instance of an infant memory dissociation on recognition and
reactivation tests.
Finally, Gulya et al. (1998) trained 6-month-olds to kick using a serial list
composed of three different mobiles. Infants successively viewed each mobile
for 2 min per session for three sessions. Infants subsequently recognized the
Figure 7.18. Left panel: The 24-hour delayed recognition performance of 3-month-olds
who were tested with a mobile displaying three sets of features either in their original
combinations (no change) or in new combinations (3-recomb). Right panel: The magnitude
of reactivation following priming with a mobile displaying three sets of features either in
their original combinations (no change) or in new combinations (3-recomb). Asterisks
mark groups that displayed retention (M baseline ratio significantly > 1.00).
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
MEMORY DISSOCIATIONS IN INFANTS 167
test mobile from serial position 1 but not the mobiles from serial positions 2
and 3 during a 24-hour delayed recognition test — a classic primacy effect
(see Figure 7.19, left panel). When the number of mobiles on the study list was
increased from three to five, however, the primacy effect disappeared during
the 24-hour test. Instead, infants recognized mobiles from all serial positions
but failed to recognize a mobile that had not been on the study list (see
Figure 7.19, right panel). Here, the impairing effect of increasing the number
of studied items on infants’ memory for serial order was manifested as a
paradoxical increase in test responding. This increase, however, reflected the
fact that infants still recognized all of the items on the study list but, as
evidenced by elimination of the primacy effect, no longer remembered their
original serial order. A similar result was obtained with adults by Murdock
(1962), who found that increasing the number of words on the study list
impaired adults’ memory for order but not item information.
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
168 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
MEMORY DISSOCIATIONS IN INFANTS 169
higher for semantic questions and questions requiring a “yes” response; on the
perceptual identification test, however, neither question type nor whether the
question required a “yes” or “no” answer affected the probability of a hit.
Although there was no effect of LOP, presentation of a word in the first phase
did produce a significant priming effect in the second phase: Perceptual
identification was significantly enhanced when test items were old; moreover,
the amount of priming was the same whether recognition memory was very
poor or near-perfect.
Examples of the dissociation produced by LOP manipulations have also
been reported in the developmental literature. Carroll et al. (1985) presented a
list composed of pictures to 5-, 7-, and 10-year-olds and a group of adults,
instructing participants either to search for pictures marked with a cross (the
shallow encoding condition) or to judge the weight of the objects that were
pictured (the deep encoding condition). At each age, performance was mea-
sured on both a recognition test (explicit memory task) and a naming test
(implicit memory task). As expected, recognition improved with age. In
addition, however, picture recognition was better in the deep than in the
shallow encoding condition, but the priming measure was unaffected by either
age or encoding condition. Lindberg (1980, Experiment 1) found that explicit
memory performance (free recall) significantly increased from kindergarten to
adulthood following elaborative encoding but not after physical encoding.
Naito (1990) replicated this result and extended the analysis to implicit memory
performance. She found no effect of encoding condition (orthographic or
elaborative encoding) on performance in the implicit word-completion test for
6-, 8-, and 11-year-olds and college students. Memory performance on the
explicit free-recall test, however, was significantly better with elaborative
encoding at all but the youngest age (see Chapter 4).
Adler et al. (1998) obtained a robust effect of LOP on 3-month-old
infants’ memory performance in a delayed recognition task. Infants’ attention
during encoding was enhanced by training them with a pop-out display
composed of a single target (L or +) amidst six distractors (+s or Ls, respec-
tively). This display is thought to enhance attention to the target at the expense
of attention to the distractors (see Figure 7.20).
In Experiment 1, infants who were tested with a mobile composed
entirely of targets or distractors recognized both the original target and the
original distractors during a 24-hour delayed recognition test, confirming that
both were encoded during training. In Experiment 2, infants who were trained
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
170 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
Figure 7.20. One of the pop-out mobiles that was used with 3-month-olds to enhance
attention during training. The single target (+) amidst six distractors (Ls) stood out from the
display and captured infants’ attention. As a result, infants tested with a mobile displaying
seven +s remembered it longer than infants whose training mobile displayed seven +s in the
first place (see Figure 6.4a).
with the single L target amidst six + distractors recognized a test mobile
displaying seven Ls after delays more than twice as long as infants who were
originally trained with a mobile composed of seven Ls; conversely, the delay
after which infants trained with the pop-out mobile recognized a test mobile
displaying seven +s was less than half as long as that of controls who were
originally trained with a mobile composed of seven +s (Adler & Rovee-
Collier 1994). Finally, Experiment 3 demonstrated that the retention advan-
tage gained by enhancing infants’ attention with a pop-out mobile was not
specific to the particular character that had been used as the target during
encoding. Experiment 2 was replicated even when the characters that served
as target and distractors were reversed. These results were consistent with the
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
MEMORY DISSOCIATIONS IN INFANTS 171
adult findings that increasing attention to an item during encoding increases its
depth of processing and thereby protracts its retention on an explicit memory
task (see Figure 7.21).
Training and priming 3-month-olds with a pop-out mobile, however, did
not differentially affect the magnitude of their retention following a reactiva-
tion treatment. Rather, the magnitude of reactivation was the same as when
they were trained and primed with a homogeneous mobile--results consistent
with the effect of LOP on implicit memory tasks used with adults.
Figure 7.21. The LOP effect at 3 months of age shown as the protracted recognition over
days of +s (left panel) and Ls (right panel) when they were the target (white columns) on the
pop-out training mobile. Also shown is the maximum duration of retention of control
groups (striped columns) who were trained with a homogeneous + or L mobile, respec-
tively, and the diminished recognition over days of +s and Ls when they were the distractors
(dark columns) on the L or + pop-out training mobile, respectively. The corresponding
control group for the distractors was trained with a homogeneous mobile displaying the
same character as the distractor. (A retention ratio = 1.00 indicates no forgetting). Asterisks
mark groups that displayed retention (M baseline ratio significantly > 1.00).
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
172 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
MEMORY DISSOCIATIONS IN INFANTS 173
the test mobiles after a 1-day retention interval but not after a 1-week delay
(see Figure 7.22, left panel; Fagen, Ohr, Singer & Klein 1989). Despite the
impaired performance of criers on the 1-week delayed recognition test, when a
reactivation treatment was administered 3 weeks after the conclusion of
training, all infants exhibited the same magnitude of reactivation 1 day later
whether they had cried after a mobile shift or not (see Figure 7.22, right panel;
Fagen, Ohr, Fleckenstein & Ribner 1985).
Singer and Fagen (1992) repeated this procedure but used a measure of
infants’ facial expressions of affect (AFFEX; Izard, Dougherty & Hembree
1980) in addition to crying. Following the shift to the 2-object mobile, criers
exhibited decreasing amounts of interest expressions and increasing amounts
of anger and sadness expressions. During the 1-day recognition test, criers
again displayed significantly more anger expressions than noncriers, but
during the 7-day recognition test, they displayed none. In addition to provid-
Figure 7.22. The effect of crying on the magnitude of delayed recognition 1 day or 7 days
after training (left panel) and the magnitude of reactivation (right panel) 3 weeks after
training at 3 months of age. Asterisks mark groups that displayed retention (M baseline ratio
significantly > 1.00).
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
174 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
ing a convergent measure of affect, these data confirmed that the poor perfor-
mance of criers during the 1-week test was not simply a result of their refusal
to participate in the task but an instance of true forgetting (Fagen & Prigot
1993).
Finally, the interval between training with the preferred (preshift) mobile
and training with the nonpreferred (postshift) mobile is an important determi-
nant of the degree of impairment in infants’ delayed recognition performance
(Fagen et al. 1989). When a delay of 0, 2, 5, or 15 min was interpolated
between infants’ exposure to the pre- and post-shift mobiles, criers again
failed to recognize the test mobile 1 week later; when the interpolated delay
was increased to 30 min, however, their performance on the delayed recogni-
tion test was excellent. The performance of noncriers on the 1-week test, by
comparison, was excellent after all pre- to post-shift delays.
The preceding studies with infants were based on between-subjects mea-
sures, as in the Hertel and Hardin (1990) study with adults. For both age
groups, affect differentially affected the magnitude of recognition but not the
magnitude of reactivation (priming). Within-subjects measures of preference
yield the same result but in a slightly different form. In studies with adults,
implicit preference is associated with familiarity: Individuals prefer items that
were on the original study list even though they cannot recognize them. Engen
and Ross (1973), for example, asked college students to indicate whether
different odors were familiar or unfamiliar and whether they liked, disliked, or
were indifferent to each odor. On this implicit test, students were more likely
to like the odors that they had said were familiar and to either dislike or be
indifferent to the odors that they had said were unfamiliar. Of the familiar
odors, for example, 46% were rated like, whereas of the unfamiliar odors, only
11% were. After delays of 1, 7, or 30 days, they gave students a forced-choice
(explicit) recognition test in which odors from the original study list were
pitted against new odors. Students’ accuracy of recognition was unrelated to
whether they had initially classified the odors as familiar. Using a slightly
different procedure, Kunst-Wilson and Zajonc (1980) similarly found that
adults preferred visual stimuli that they had previously seen relative to new
ones, even though the stimuli were exposed for durations too brief to be
recognized.
Using a between-subjects design, Wilk, Klein and Rovee-Collier (2000)
obtained a similar result. They trained 3-month-olds with a particular mobile
and gave them a delayed recognition test in which the original mobile and a
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
MEMORY DISSOCIATIONS IN INFANTS 175
novel one were simultaneously presented either 1 day or 4 days later. During
testing, they measured both kicking and looking. Typically, 3-month-olds
exhibit no retention if tested exclusively with a novel mobile 1 day after
training, but they exhibit near-perfect retention if tested exclusively with the
training mobile after the same delay (Hayne, Greco, Earley, Griesler &
Rovee-Collier 1986). As the interval between training and testing increases,
however, 3-month-olds increasingly respond to a novel test mobile, and by 4
days after training, they kick as robustly to a novel mobile as to the training
one (Rovee-Collier & Sullivan 1980). The latter data are taken as evidence
that infants gradually forget the specific details of their training mobile until,
after 4 days, they cannot discriminate between them. In the Wilk et al. study,
as expected, infants exhibited significant recognition 4 days after training.
Despite prior evidence that they cannot discriminate between these test mo-
biles after 4 days on the kicking measure, however, infants also looked
proportionally longer at the original mobile than at the new one after both test
delays. Thus, as in the Kunst-Wilson and Zajonc (1980) study with adults,
although infants do not discriminate the new mobile from the old one after 4
days on the kicking measure, they do discriminate them on the looking
measure, implicitly preferring to look at the familiar mobile.
10. Serial Position. The serial position of items on a study list has long
been known to produce strong and persistent primacy and/or recency effects
on immediate tests of free recall, cued recall, and recognition memory (e.g.,
Bousfield, Whitmarsh & Esterson 1958; Gershberg & Shimamura 1994;
Murdock 1962; Wright et al. 1985). A handful of researchers have found
recency effects on implicit tests of both word-stem completion (McKenzie &
Humphries 1991; Rybash & Osborne 1991) and word-fragment completion
(Sloman et al. 1988), but Brooks (1994) observed that the instructions and
procedures in these reports were either suspect or reported in insufficient
detail.
Two laboratories have recently used the same study instructions and test
stimuli but different instructions for the implicit and explicit tests. Gershberg
and Shimamura (1994) obtained inconsistent evidence for primacy and recency
effects across three experiments. Implicit word-stem completion tests, for
example, yielded a primacy effect in two experiments and a recency effect in
one, and these effects were highly transient when they did occur. Different
explicit tests (word-stem cued recall, free recall) also yielded inconsistent
primacy and/or recency effects across experiments. Using word-stem comple-
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
176 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
tion and word-stem cued-recall tests with normal adults, Brooks (1994) found
primacy effects on an explicit test in two experiments and, when the last items
studied were the first ones tested, a recency effect in the second of these. In
neither experiment, however, did serial position affect performance on the
implicit test. Taken together, the data indicated that the serial order of the items
on the study list had no effect on performance in implicit tests but produced a
primacy effect in explicit tests. Finally, in two experiments with Korsakoff and
nonKorsakoff amnesic patients, both patient groups performed more poorly
than controls on tests requiring the recall, recognition, and sequencing of words
and facts (Shimamura, Janowsky & Squire 1990).
Delayed recognition tests with both 3- and 6-month-olds also reveal a
strong and persisting primacy effect (Gulya et al. 1998; Merriman, Rovee-
Collier & Wilk 1997). Independent groups of infants were trained with a list
composed of three different mobiles for 2 min each for 3 successive days and
were given a recognition test 24 hours later with either a mobile from one of
the three serial positions or a completely novel mobile. Infants of both ages
responded only to the test mobile in serial position 1–a strong primacy effect
(see Figure 7.23, left panel). In a follow-up study (see Number of Studied
Items), when the number of mobiles on the original list was increased from
three to five, the primacy effect was eliminated, and infants recognized all of
the mobiles on the 24-hour delayed recognition test. Even so, they still
discriminated a novel test mobile that had not been on the study list (Gulya et
al. 1999).
Gulya et al. (1998) conjectured that infants who had been trained with the
three-mobile list in the initial study had failed to recognize the test mobiles
from serial positions 2 and 3 because those mobiles were out of order when
they had been presented singly to independent groups of infants at the time of
testing. (Recall that infants had always seen the mobile from serial position 1
first at the outset of each daily training session.) In addition, they observed that
a single mobile is not a retrieval cue for serial order, which entails relational
information and hence requires two cues (i.e., this-before-that or this-after-
that). Therefore, they primed 3- and 6-month-old infants who had been trained
on a three-mobile list with the mobile from the preceding serial position
immediately before the 24-hour test. This priming task was procedurally
identical to word-stem and word-fragment completion tasks used in priming
studies with adults except that the materials were not verbal. Although they
had failed to recognize the mobile from serial position 2 on the delayed
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
MEMORY DISSOCIATIONS IN INFANTS 177
Figure 7.23. The effect of the serial position of mobiles on a three-mobile list on the
magnitude of delayed recognition (left panel) and the magnitude of reactivation (right
panel) 24 hours after training at 3 months of age. The delayed recognition function (left
panel) shows a classic primacy effect which was eliminated when mobiles in serial
positions 2 and 3 were primed with mobiles from the preceding serial position(s) immedi-
ately before the test (right panel). Asterisks mark groups that displayed retention (M
baseline ratio significantly > 1.00).
recognition test, this time infants of both ages responded to the test mobile
from serial position 2 on the 24-hour test when they were initially primed with
the mobile from serial position 1–the familiar explicit-implicit dissociation
(see Figure 7.23, right panel). If infants were primed with the mobile from
serial position 3, however, they still did not recognize it. In addition, even
though infants of both ages had failed to recognize the mobile from serial
position 3 after 24 hours, when they were successively primed immediately
before the test with the mobiles from serial positions 1 and 2 for 1 min each,
they did respond to the mobile from serial position 3–again, the familiar
explicit-implicit dissociation. When primed for 2 min with the mobile from
serial position 1, however, they failed to respond to the test mobile from serial
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
178 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
position 3. These data demonstrated that only valid order cues were effective
memory primes at both 3 and 6 months of age, whereas invalid order cues
were not. This result confirmed that even though infants of both ages had
previously failed to recognize the same mobiles, they responded selectively to
a valid order prime on the basis of their prior training experience.
Using a response-to-novelty task, Cornell and Bergstrom (1983) familiar-
ized independent groups of 7-month-olds during multiple study periods with a
serial list composed of three photographs of adult female faces that were
spaced by 1 s. Infants received a paired-comparison test with a preexposed and
a novel face after delays of 5 s, 1 min, or 5 min. As in recognition studies with
adults (Crowder 1976; Wright et al. 1985), Cornell and Bergstrom obtained a
classic U-shaped serial position effect with both primacy and recency effects
after the shortest delay (5 s) and a persisting primacy effect after the longest
delay (5 min). This result resembles that of Merriman et al. (1997) and Gulya
et al. (1998), who also found only a primacy effect after a long delay (24
hours) when 3- and 6-month-olds were trained and tested on a three-item list
in the mobile procedure.
11. Studied Size. Ungerleider and Mishkin (1982) conjectured that the
memory system represents objects in two separable representations — one that
is size-sensitive and one that is size-invariant — which function in different
situations. In studies with normal adults and amnesic patients, changes in the
studied size of an object at the time of testing impair performance on recogni-
tion memory tasks (Biederman & Cooper 1992; Jolicoeur 1987; Milliken &
Jolicoeur 1992; Schacter, Cooper & Treadwell 1993b) and object decision
(Cooper, Schacter, Ballesteros & Moore 1992; Schacter et al. 1991a) tasks, but
these same study-to-test size changes do not affect priming on either object
naming tasks (Biederman & Cooper 1991, 1992) or object decision tasks
(Schacter, Cooper, Tharan & Rubens 1991b; Schacter et al. 1993b), whether
the objects are novel or familiar.
Biederman and Cooper (1992), for example, presented adults with a set of
line drawings of common objects and asked them to identify the objects.
Following their first exposure, adults who performed a recognition task (same/
different judgment) were slower to recognize previously viewed objects at a
new size than previously viewed objects that were the same size as in the first
exposure. When asked simply to name the objects on their second exposure,
however, the amount by which their naming latency was facilitated by the
prior exposure was the same whether the objects’ size was changed between
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
MEMORY DISSOCIATIONS IN INFANTS 179
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
180 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
stage, where binocular suppression occurs, and during which relatively ab-
stract object representations are registered (Cave et al. 1996; Tulving &
Schacter 1990). Because priming occurs for visual stimuli that never enter
conscious awareness (Kihlstrom, Schacter, Cork, Hurt & Behr 1990; Kunst-
Wilson & Zajonc 1980; Mandler, Nakamura & Van Zandt 1987), this stage of
processing must be intermediate between the stage that mediates suppression
and the stage that supports conscious visual awareness.
The same dissociation — impaired recognition but preserved priming —
has been obtained with 3-month-olds in delayed recognition and reactivation
tests, respectively, following transformations in studied size. In an initial
study, infants who were trained with a mobile composed of seven pink blocks
with a computer-generated black L on each side exhibited excellent retention
in a 24-hour delayed recognition test with the same characters (the control
group); when tested with Ls that were either reduced or increased in size by
25%, however, infants’ performance on the delayed recognition test was
significantly impaired (Adler & Rovee-Collier 1994). In a follow-up study,
Gerhardstein et al. (2000) asked whether 3-month-old infants show a memory
dissociation on delayed recognition and reactivation tests analogous to the
dissociation exhibited by adults on size-sensitive (explicit) and size-invariant
(implicit) memory tests. They found that the 24-hour delayed recognition
performance of 3-month-olds who were trained with a mobile displaying +s of
a particular size was significantly impaired when they were tested with +s that
were either reduced or increased in size by 33% (see Figure 7.24, left panel).
In contrast, their magnitude of retention following a reactivation treatment
with mobiles displaying identically size-transformed +s was unaffected by
whether the +s used to prime the memory were larger or smaller than the +s
they saw during training (see Figure 7.24, right panel). In each case, controls
who were trained and either tested 24 hours later or primed 2 weeks later with
a mobile displaying characters of the same size exhibited excellent retention.
The finding that 3-month-olds, like normal adults, show preserved prim-
ing across size transformations and, also like normal adults, are sensitive to
size transformations on same/different recognition tests suggests that the same
memory systems mediate their memory performance.
12. Memory Load. The term memory load has been used with reference to
both the length of the retention interval (i.e., longer test delays creating a
greater memory load; Quinn & Eimas 1996) and the amount of information that
must be retained (Mandler 1985). Both usages were considered earlier (see
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
MEMORY DISSOCIATIONS IN INFANTS 181
Figure 7.24. The effect of changing the size of the training character by 33% on the
magnitude of delayed recognition 24 hours after training (left panel) and the magnitude of
reactivation (right panel) 3 weeks after training at 3 months of age. The performance of no-
change control groups who were trained and either tested or primed with the original
character is also shown. Asterisks mark groups that displayed retention (M baseline ratio
significantly > 1.00).
Retention Interval and Number of Studied Items). Recall that in both instances,
increasing the memory load impaired memory performance on recognition
tests but not on priming tests. A third factor that affects the memory load is the
nature of the to-be-remembered information. For infants as for adults, for
example, relational information creates a greater memory load than absolute
information. Adler and Rovee-Collier (1994) reported that 3-month-old infants
could discriminate horizontal and vertical line segments (two textons) in
different spatial relations (L versus T) during a 1-hour delayed recognition test
but not during a 24-hour test. However, they could discriminate +s, which
contain a line crossing (a third texton), from both Ls and Ts during a 7-day test.
In addition, infants recognized Ls and Ts for only 3 days, but they recognized
+s for 7 days. These data illustrate the predicted memory dissociation: Abso-
lute information (texton number) was remembered longer than relational
information (spatial arrangement) on delayed recognition tests. Infants’ mag-
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
182 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
nitude of reactivation, however, was equivalent when exactly the same stimuli
were used as memory primes with 3-month-olds (Rovee-Collier et al. 1992a)
and 6-month-olds (Bhatt, Rovee-Collier & Weiner 1994).
In the following studies, increasing the memory load also selectively
impaired infants’ retention of relational information on a delayed recognition
task but not on a reactivation task. In a sequel to the feature combinations
study described earlier (Bhatt & Rovee-Collier 1997; see Number of Studied
Items), 3-month-olds differentially forgot the correlations between different
training features (relational information) as the retention interval increased
from l day to 3 days, but they still recognized the individual features (absolute
information) and discriminated them from novel features on a delayed recog-
nition test after a 4-day retention interval (Bhatt & Rovee-Collier 1996).
When the number of feature sets on the training mobile was increased from
two to three, infants recognized the feature relations on the two-set mobile but
not on the three-set mobile during a 24-hour delayed recognition test. In a
reactivation task, however, the three-set mobile was an effective memory
prime 2 weeks later if it displayed the original feature combinations but not if
it displayed feature recombinations (Bhatt & Rovee-Collier 1997).
Finally, recall that 6-month-olds who were trained with a three-mobile
list had recognized only the mobile from serial position 1 — a classic primacy
effect — during a 24-hour delayed recognition test (see Figure 7.19, left
panel). When the length of the list was increased from three mobiles to five
mobiles, however, the primacy effect on the delayed recognition test disap-
peared (Figure 7.19, right panel; Gulya et al. 1999). In other words, increasing
the memory load impaired infants’ retention of order (relational) information
but not their retention of item-identity (absolute) information. Murdock
(1962) had similarly found that the primacy effect disappeared when the
number of words that were presented to adults on a study list was increased.
13. Context. The role of context in retention has been of interest to
psychologists for more than 2 decades, yet the term has been used in many
different ways (for review, see Clark & Carlson 1981). Broadly speaking, the
context refers to global aspects of the experimental environment, including
both external (the room, the experimenter, and the apparatus) and internal (the
inner state of the subject, including the pharmacological context, the mood or
affective state, or the point in the circadian cycle) conditions. However, the
context can also refer to the visual stimuli that surround or precede a target
object or the items that accompany the target item on each study trial, also
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
MEMORY DISSOCIATIONS IN INFANTS 183
called the local context. In the latter situations, the context facilitates the
interpretation of the target stimulus. As a rule, retention is better when the
encoding and retrieval contexts are the same than when they differ (Cohen
1985; Spear 1978).
Tulving (1983) claimed that episodic memories included spatiotemporal
details, whereas semantic memories did not. Mandler (1985) rejected a mul-
tiple-memory-systems approach but similarly considered non-automatic (i.e.,
explicit) memories to be context-dependent and automatic (i.e., implicit)
memories to be context free. Neely (1989; Neely & Durgunoglu 1985),
however, concluded that the evidence for these distinctions was ambiguous.
Although a change in context between study and test has been shown to impair
adults’ memory performance on recognition and recall tests (Thomson &
Tulving 1970; Underwood & Humphreys 1979), the effect of a context change
on memory performance on implicit tests is less clear. Richardson-Klavehn
and Bjork (1988), for example, observed that small but persistent context
effects appeared over a wide range of perceptual identification (implicit
memory or priming) studies, even though the effects within a particular study
were often not significant.
Jacoby (1983) asked college students to read a list of words and then gave
them a perceptual identification test containing different proportions of words
from the previously read list. Their perceptual identification was facilitated by
the proportional overlap between the study and the test list. Likewise, Graf
and Schacter (1985) found a larger priming effect on a word-fragment
completion task when college students and amnesic patients were primed with
a word from a word pair on a prior study list (same context condition) than
with other words (different context condition), but this difference emerged
only when the study task required elaborative processing. More recently,
however, Allen and Jacoby (1990) determined that the priming effect on a
perceptual identification test was equivalent whether test words had been
generated during study (i.e., were easily recognized) or had only been read
(i.e., were poorly recognized). Changes in the modality of presentation
(Roediger & Blaxton 1987), the visual display of the target stimuli (Jacoby &
Dallas 1981), the environmental setting of encoding and retrieval (Graf 1988;
Smith, Heath & Vela 1990), the perceived sense or meaning of a word
(Lewandowsky, Kirsner & Bainbridge 1989), and mood (Macaulay et al.
1993) have also been reported to disrupt memory performance on implicit
tests.
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
184 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
For many years, studies of context effects with young infants were
relatively sparse, probably because the brain structures responsible for repre-
senting information about the environmental context in adults were thought to
be functionally immature in infants younger than 8 or 9 months of age.
Research with both infant rats (Richardson, Riccio & McKenney 1988) and
human infants, however, subsequently challenged this belief. For 3-month-old
human infants, changing the context at the time of testing has no impact on
retention after a retention interval of 1 day (Butler & Rovee-Collier 1989;
Hayne, Rovee-Collier & Borza 1991) but impairs retention after longer delays
(Butler & Rovee-Collier 1989; Rovee-Collier, Griesler & Earley 1985a). At 6
months, a context change impairs retention after both 1 day and 3 days but has
no effect on retention after longer delays (Borovsky & Rovee-Collier 1990).
Training in one room and testing in another one has the same impairing effect
as training in the presence of a distinctive cloth panel and testing in the
presence of another one (Hartshorn & Rovee-Collier 1997).
In addition, memory reactivation is impaired by a context change at both
3 months (Butler & Rovee-Collier 1989; Hayne et al. 1991) and 6 months
(Borovsky & Rovee-Collier 1990; Hartshorn & Rovee-Collier 1997; Shields
& Rovee-Collier 1992). Two-month-olds have not been tested in an altered
context, but attempts have been made to enhance their long-term retention by
both training and testing them in a highly distinctive context (Rovee-Collier et
al. 1989). Although this manipulation facilitates retention in Korsakoff pa-
tients (Winocur & Kinsbourne 1978) and 3-month-olds (Butler & Rovee-
Collier 1989; Hayne & Rovee-Collier 1995), it does not facilitate retention in
2-month-olds.
In an initial study, 3-month-olds recognized the original mobile 1 week
after training in the original context but not in a different one, and the training
context alone — without the mobile — could reactivate the memory 2 weeks
after training, but a novel context could not (Rovee-Collier et al. 1985a).
Hayne and Findlay (1995) replicated the context-alone reactivation result after
delays of 3 and 4 weeks. If the original context, by itself, is an effective
retrieval cue, then it must have been represented in infants’ memory. Butler
and Rovee-Collier (1989) subsequently tested 3-month-olds after delays rang-
ing from 1 to 5 days with all combinations of cues and contexts that were the
same or different from training to testing. This study yielded three important
results. First, infants did not treat the cue and context as a stimulus configura-
tion. Had they done so, then changing either the cue or the context would have
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
MEMORY DISSOCIATIONS IN INFANTS 185
impaired retention, but a context change did not impair retention during the 1-
day test. Second, the focal cue, an otherwise effective memory prime, was
rendered ineffective in a novel context. This result has been replicated many
times with both 3- and 6-month-olds. And third, a highly distinctive training
and test context facilitated discrimination of a novel test mobile from the
training mobile after delays when generalization to a novel mobile was seen in
its absence. This result reflects the disambiguating function of context when
the memory of the original cue is fuzzy (Bouton & Bolles 1985).
Butler and Rovee-Collier (1989) proposed a hierarchical, attention-gat-
ing model of memory retrieval, adapted from Reeves and Sperling’s (1986)
model of retrieval from visual short-term memory, to account for the role of
context in memory retrieval. According to their model, the memory represen-
tation of an event is organized hierarchically, with contextual information
occupying a relatively high node. Thus, attention to potential retrieval cues is
first filtered or screened at the level of the context, and perceptual identifica-
tion (an automatic or parallel process) of appropriate retrieval cues in the
context permits attention to flow to the next level (the focal cue). A mismatch
between the contextual information present during testing and that represented
in memory (i.e., a failure of perceptual identification), however, prevents the
attention gate from opening and blocks the flow of attention to the cue. As a
result, the retrieval process is terminated, and infants fail to recognize the cue
during the test. Like Reeves and Sperling, Butler and Rovee-Collier also
assumed that the attention gate might be partially opened under some circum-
stances, for example, if a partial match is made or if a sufficient number or
kind of critical retrieval cues are detected. The latter assumption was sup-
ported by findings that infants exhibit recognition if the test context contains
certain critical features that were present during initial encoding (Earley, Bhatt
& Rovee-Collier 1995; Rovee-Collier, Schechter, Shyi & Shields 1992b).
Context effects have now been studied in the mobile and train tasks with
infants over the entire first year of life (Hartshorn, Rovee-Collier, Gerhard-
stein, Bhatt, Klein et al. 1998a). These data revealed that a context change
impairs retention only at the end of the forgetting function at all ages except 6
months (Borovsky & Rovee-Collier 1990), when it impairs recognition for
only a few days after training only. In deferred imitation studies with 12- to
18-month-olds, infants similarly generalize across widely varying training-
test contexts after delays from 3 min to 4 weeks (Hanna & Meltzoff 1993;
Hayne et al. 2000a; Klein & Meltzoff 1999). At 6 months, however, a context
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
186 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
Conclusions
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
MEMORY DISSOCIATIONS IN INFANTS 187
Both Bauer and McDonough take the position that deferred imitation is an
explicit memory task. Their approach to the study of memory development in
infants is to find a task that amnesics cannot solve, but normal adults can —
and use conscious recollection to do so (see Chapter 1). Because preverbal
infants can also solve a deferred imitation task, these investigators infer that
they must use conscious recollection to solve it as well. Deferred imitation per
se, however, is neither an explicit nor an implicit task; rather, the type of
memory task (either recognition/cued-recall or priming) in which it is used is
what determines the form of memory it yields. To date, however, only two
studies of deferred imitation have used a reactivation or priming (implicit
memory) task (Barr 1997; Barr & Vieira 1999); all others have used only
cued-recall (explicit memory) tasks.
In retrospect, the mobile and deferred imitation procedures are more
similar than they are different, as the form of the retention function obtained
with each procedure shows. Demonstrating target actions on a particular
object(s) is simply a way of introducing infants to the target information and
providing them with a nonverbal means of indicating whether they recognize
the specific test cue. Likewise, putting critical stimulus information on the
sides of the mobile blocks and reinforcing a distinctive response (a footkick)
in their presence is simply a way of introducing infants to the target informa-
tion and providing them with a nonverbal means of indicating whether they
recognize the specific test cue.
Because operant training in mobile studies (or exposure to the demonstra-
tion in deferred imitation studies) occurs prior to the introduction of the
manipulations that differentiate reactivation from delayed recognition tasks,
any subsequent performance differences in these tasks cannot be attributed to
how the response got into the infant’s behavioral repertoire in the first place.
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
188 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
Consider, for example, that even verbally proficient children and adults ini-
tially had to learn the verbal labels for the objects and patterns with which they
are subsequently tested in implicit and explicit memory tasks. Yet, thought is
rarely given to the specific manner in which these verbal labels were actually
acquired in the first place. Why? Because that information is unimportant in
accounting for adults’ functional dissociations in implicit and explicit memory
tasks. As this chapter documents, that same information is equally unimpor-
tant in accounting for infants’ functional dissociations in implicit and explicit
memory tasks.
We conclude that the functional memory dissociations that are exhibited
by preverbal infants, normal children and adults, and amnesics on priming and
recognition tasks are the same whether the target response was initially
acquired by operant conditioning, modeling (deferred imitation), familiariza-
tion (response-to-novelty), or verbal instructions (“study this list of nonsense
syllables”). Without exception, evidence from infant memory dissociations
reveals that the development of explicit and implicit memory is not hierarchi-
cal during the infancy period, hence is not described by the Jacksonian
principle. In terms of multiple memory systems, these data reveal that the
memory systems that support implicit and explicit memory are both present
from early in infancy.
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
CHAPTER 8
In this chapter, we consider some of the major theoretical accounts that have been advanced
to explain the functional memory dissociations that are exhibited by brain-damaged and
normal (instructed) adults. Whereas accounts based on multiple memory systems empha-
size the cognitive architecture and neural structures that underlie different kinds of memory
performance, processing accounts focus on the cognitive operations that are required to
perform different kinds of memory tasks. Our position is that a satisfactory account must
also explain the memory dissociations that are exhibited by very young infants. Therefore,
we briefly review the major theoretical accounts and consider their implications for the
development of implicit and explicit memory.
Throughout the preceding chapters, we have seen that tasks have been desig-
nated as either explicit or implicit on the basis of whether or not amnesics can
succeed on them, and the common usage of those terms was extended to refer
to the underlying brain systems that were presumed to be responsible for that
performance. In response to this situation, Gardiner and Java (1993) com-
plained that the science of memory had become obfuscated by “terminological
confusion and excess.” They were referring to the fact that the same terms —
implicit and explicit — were used to signify different memory tasks, different
memory systems or processes, and different states of awareness. Their concern
was that such labeling presupposed an “identity between the task and the system
or process, between the system or process and the state of awareness, [and]
between the state of awareness and the task” (Gardiner & Java 1993, p. 163).
Making matters worse, a variety of different labels have been given to
similar memory systems, and increasing numbers of new memory systems and
subsystems have begun to flood the taxonomy of memory classification. Many
of these new systems have resulted from the stochastic and functional indepen-
dence that has been found within tasks of implicit memory (see Chapter 2).
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
190 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
Within just a few years of the introduction of implicit and explicit memory, for
example, Roediger (1990a) had counted as many as 25 memory systems (for a
cursory list, see Table 8.1), and that number has continued to grow. Noting the
proliferating number of memory systems, Roediger, Rajaram and Srinivas
(1990, p. 585) mused, “Although parsimony is not a pristine scientific virtue
and must be abandoned when the data demand it, we wonder if that point has
been reached yet.”
Perhaps the most fundamental question left to answer is whether multiple
memory systems even exist (e.g., Anderson & Ross 1980; Baddeley 1984;
Craik 1983; Hintzman 1984; Jacoby 1984; Johnson 1992; McKoon, Ratcliff &
Dell 1986; Roediger 1990a; Roediger & Blaxton 1987). Because the multiple-
memory-system view has become so widely accepted in the fields of develop-
mental psychology, cognitive psychology, and behavioral neuroscience, we
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
STRUCTURAL AND PROCESSING ACCOUNTS 191
sometimes forget that not all memory researchers share this view. An alterna-
tive to the multiple-memory-system view is that memory is the result of a
unitary process and that experimental dissociations between particular tasks
merely reflect the processing operations that are required by different memory
tasks at the time of study and testing (e.g., Jacoby 1983; Johnson 1983; Mandler
1980; Roediger & Blaxton 1987). Some of the major multiple-memory-
systems and processing accounts are briefly reviewed below. Each account is
accompanied by a brief analysis of the role of conscious awareness, its
implications for implicit/explicit memory and memory development, and its
relation to memory data from human infants.
Multiple-memory-systems accounts
One of the critical issues associated with the proliferation of memory systems
concerns the rules for inferring them (Johnson & Hasher 1987; Roediger et al.
1990; Sherry & Schacter 1987; Tulving & Bower 1974; Weiskrantz 1990), but
some attempts at establishing these rules have been less successful than others.
In some of the earlier studies of memory, for example, researchers commonly
varied the types of materials that were presented to subjects. Paivio (1971), for
example, reported that college students who saw either a list of pictures or a
list of the names of the same pictures remembered the pictures better. More-
over, they remembered pictures of concrete objects better than pictures of
abstract ones. Data such as these led him to propose that there are two different
representational systems — the imagery system and the verbal system. This
distinction found some support in neuropsychological evidence that verbal
and nonverbal functions are lateralized in the cerebral hemispheres (e.g.,
Gazzaniga 1972).
Tulving and Bower (1974) reacted to Pavio’s proposition by questioning
if every dissociation that resulted from using a different set of experimental
materials would justify the creation of another memory system:
“It is not clear how one gets from this evidence to two memory systems or two
memory stores. It seems to us that the evidence for a ‘visual imagery’ store is
no better than that for a ‘tactual store,’ ‘olfactory store,’ ‘proprioceptive
store,’ ‘color store,’ and the like. The question is whether we should postulate
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
192 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
a distinct memory system for every discriminable stimulus variable and for
every variation of events along values of that variable that produces differ-
ences in memory for those events. If we did, we would soon have more
memory systems or memory stores than we could name. If we did not, it is
necessary to spell out the rules for such postulation when it is deemed
appropriate or necessary. In this connection, it is also interesting to note that it
has not yet been made clear by anyone how the task of explaining memory
phenomena is materially aided by the hypothesized existence of different
memory stores and systems…” (Tulving & Bower 1974, p. 273).
In fact, the same question could be raised today (e.g., Roediger et al. 1990).
A number of different criteria for inferring new memory systems from
performance on explicit and implicit tests have been proposed by different
researchers (for review, see Roediger et al. 1990). These criteria include
stochastic independence (Tulving et al. 1982), functional independence
(Tulving 1985), different underlying neural structures (Squire 1986; Zola-
Morgan & Squire 1990b), functional incompatibility of operations (Sherry &
Schacter 1987), and multiple tests (Tulving 1987). The criteria of stochastic
and functional independence were previously considered in Chapter 2; the
remaining criteria are considered below.
Different Underlying Neural Structures. One of the most successful
arguments for postulating different memory systems rests on the assumption
that different neural processes underlie performance on implicit and explicit
tasks. Most of the data in support of this view have come from studies of brain-
damaged humans and from monkeys with experimentally induced lesions
(see Chapter 3). On the basis of these data, it is currently assumed that the
cerebellum mediates performance on classical conditioning tasks, the medial
temporal-diencephalic and frontal lobe structures mediate event memory, the
temporal and parietal neocortex mediate knowledge systems and representa-
tions, and the cerebellum and/or basal ganglia mediate(s) perceptual-motor
skills (Weiskrantz 1990, p. 106).
Although developmental dissociations in memory performance that are
ascribed to maturation of the brain are also often used to support the multiple-
memory-system view, these developmental dissociations do not actually occur
— as least, not in primates (see Chapters 5 and 6). In one particularly
unsuccessful attempt to draw a one-to-one relation between maturation of the
brain and infants’ memory performance, Nelson (1995, 1997) reviewed neuro-
logical evidence that related different parts of the nervous system to perfor-
mance on different kinds of memory tasks during early development. Using the
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
STRUCTURAL AND PROCESSING ACCOUNTS 193
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
194 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
performance on one task (e.g., recall) are also implicated in a second task (e.g.,
recognition), but not in a third one (e.g., word-stem completion). In this
example, disrupting one component by lesioning would impair memory per-
formance on the two tasks for which it was a critical link but would not impair
it on the third task. Importantly, however, this dissociation would reveal
nothing about how many or what other neural components the two affected
tasks might share; rather, it would only reveal that the lesioned component did
not contribute to memory performance on the third task.
Functional Incompatibility of Operations. Another criterion for postulat-
ing more than one memory system grew out of a consideration of the evolution
of multiple memory systems. Sherry and Schacter (1987) argued that unique,
specialized memory systems evolved to handle new environmental demands
that existing memory systems could not. They suggested that different special-
ized memory systems for song, imprinting, and spatial location in birds, for
example, may have evolved to subserve the different functional demands
associated with mating, maternal care, species recognition, food caching, and
navigation. Presumably, each specialized memory system was uniquely con-
strained by temporal factors associated with the encoding (e.g., critical peri-
ods) or retrieval (e.g., seasonal migration) of information admitted into it.
Thus, remembering the prior location of a food cache, a nest, or a predator
would require the storage and retrieval of details of a particular episode. When
and where a given food source is accessible, such as when worms can be
found in the farmer’s field or when a truck arrives at the garbage tip
(McFarland 1977), however, could change weekly, monthly, or seasonally.
Thus, that memory system would have to be sufficiently flexible that details
about time or place could be updated. From an evolutionary perspective, then,
particular memory systems can be found in species whose adaptations reflect
the same functional demands.
Many psychologists, however, have assumed that the appearance of
different memory systems during ontogeny reflects their order of appearance
during phylogeny and have given little or no consideration to the particular
specializations that different species evolved in the course of adapting to
different niches. Analyses of the hierarchical development of multiple
memory systems that are based on the simple notion that ontogeny recapitu-
lates phylogeny make two fundamental errors. First, new memory systems,
such as the system that is implicated in seed-caching by nutcrackers, are
species-specific and reflect an organism’s specialized adaptation to its niche.
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
STRUCTURAL AND PROCESSING ACCOUNTS 195
Human infants, who occupy a very different niche (or series of niches), do not
require these same memory systems. Second, ontogeny is linear, but phylog-
eny is not. Evidence gathered over the last half-century now shows that
phylogeny is tree-like and branching rather than ladder-like and linear. In
short, there is no simple relation between the appearance of a capacity in
phylogeny and its appearance in ontogeny.
Multiple Tasks. Tulving (1987) argued that the inference of multiple
memory systems requires that the same dissociation be exhibited on several
tasks, each of which presumably taps the same underlying memory systems.
Thus, if a particular independent variable affects performance on the battery
of tasks thought to tap one memory system differently from the way it affects
performance on the battery of tasks thought to tap a different system, then it is
safe to conclude that the tasks within each battery share something not shared
with the tasks in the other battery. Although some would use such findings to
argue that what is shared by these tasks are common memory systems, it is just
as reasonable to argue that what is shared are common processing operations.
In fact, most studies with adults that have revealed memory dissociations
on one task but not on another have used only two tasks, each presumably
tapping a different underlying memory system. The same criticism — the use
of only two tasks to illustrate an experimental dissociation in memory perfor-
mance — can be levied against most of the infant studies cited in Chapter 7.
Unlike the adult studies in which the materials and temporal parameters of
study and test vary from report to report, however, all of the mobile studies
with infants used exactly the same two tasks, the same task parameters, and
infants from the same population. Thus, instead of manipulating a single
independent variable and producing the same dissociation across multiple
implicit and explicit tasks as in adult studies, the infant studies manipulated
multiple independent variables and produced the same dissociation on a single
implicit and explicit task. In doing so, however, the infant studies have
amassed a large amount of convergent evidence of functional memory disso-
ciations.
Although we have focused on the explicit/implicit distinction in this book,
a number of multiple memory systems have been proposed over the years that
meet one or more of the criteria outlined above. Below, we consider in more
detail the two major classifications of multiple memory systems that have been
proposed, episodic and semantic memory and declarative and procedural
(nondeclarative) memory, and their implications for memory development.
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
196 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
Tulving (1972) was the first to distinguish episodic and semantic memory as
two, functionally distinct memory systems and to differentiate them in terms
of the content of the memories they stored. The episodic system stored
autobiographical information about personally experienced events (episodes)
that were specified by spatiotemporal details. The semantic system, in con-
trast, stored the individual’s general (organized) knowledge. Although re-
trieval from the episodic system altered its content (e.g., by supplementing or
modifying it), retrieval from the semantic system did not. Tulving did not
explicitly specify the relation of episodic and semantic memory to the tradi-
tional distinction between long-term and short-term memory, but by describ-
ing episodic memory as what psychologists studied in laboratory tasks, he
implied that episodic memories were of shorter duration than semantic ones.
In fact, the question of whether development proceeds from the specific to the
general or from the general to the specific has long occupied developmental
psychologists. Some early researchers viewed development as a process of
increasing generalization. This view is reflected in Watson’s (1930) account
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
STRUCTURAL AND PROCESSING ACCOUNTS 197
The latter view, however, does not characterize the development of infant
memory. A large number of studies using different paradigms have now
documented that infants’ early memories are initially highly specific and
become increasingly generalized with age (Hartshorn et al. 1998a; Hayne et
al. 1997), as originally argued by Nelson and Brown (1978). In mobile studies,
for example, even 2-month-olds can detect if their test mobile differs ever so
slightly from the highly detailed, five-object mobile with which they were
trained 24 hours earlier. If more than a single object on the test mobile differs
from what appeared on the training mobile, then they cannot recognize it;
otherwise, their 24-hour recognition is excellent (Hayne et al. 1986). Simi-
larly, if 3-month-olds are trained with a block mobile displaying As on all
sides in a single color (red, green, black, or blue) and are tested 24 hours later
with an identical mobile that displays As (the same form) in a different color,
they cannot recognize it. Nor can they recognize a test mobile that displays 2s
(a different form) in the original training color (Hayne et al. 1987). In other
words, even 3-month-olds can discriminate a difference in just a single at-
tribute after a 24-hour delay.
In other studies, Gerhardstein et al. (2000) found that 3-month-olds could
not recognize a test mobile 24 hours later if the characters it displayed (Ls or
+s) were 33% smaller or larger than the characters on their training mobiles
(see Chapter 7, Studied Size), and Adler and Rovee-Collier (1994) found that
3-month-olds could not recognize a test mobile 24 hours later if it displayed
characters that were 25% smaller. Finally, recall that after 3-month-olds were
trained with a six-block mobile displaying two different sets of feature combi-
nations (red As on black blocks and green 2s on yellow blocks), they discrimi-
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
198 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
STRUCTURAL AND PROCESSING ACCOUNTS 199
Figure 8.1. Mean baseline ratios of independent groups of infants between 2 and 12 months
of age who were trained for two sessions and tested with a different cue in the original
context after common relative retention intervals that corresponded to the shortest (Early)
or longest (Late) test delays or to the midpoint (Middle) of the forgetting function for each
age group. An asterisk indicates that a group exhibited significant retention (i.e., M baseline
ratio significantly > 1.00). Vertical bars indicate + 1 SE.
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
200 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
Nelson and Brown (1978) also complained that Tulving’s choice of the
term semantic to refer to words and word meanings reflected the fact that
researchers always used verbal materials in studies with adults — a practice
that was extended to studies with children, although pictures or objects were
substituted for words in studies with very young children. But, they argued,
children’s nonverbal memories for actions, spatial locations, cognitive maps,
scripts, and so forth, were neither clearly semantic nor clearly episodic:
“How such an organized semantic language-based system emerges is at least
one important and central developmental question. But if all real world
knowledge — language based or not, categorically structured or not — is
termed “semantic memory,” the question cannot even be asked using an
episodic-semantic distinction” (p. 240).
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
STRUCTURAL AND PROCESSING ACCOUNTS 201
The finding that functional dissociations were found within both explicit and
implicit tests revealed that a single memory system could not underlie memory
performance on each type of test. As a result, theorists were motivated to
expand their original dichotomies. Thus, for example, Squire (1987) expanded
the procedural/declarative dichotomy (Squire 1986) to include priming,
simple classical conditioning, and skill learning as subcategories of proce-
dural memory and episodic (working) and semantic (reference) memory as
subcategories of declarative memory. Likewise, Tulving (1985) added proce-
dural memory to the semantic/episodic dichotomy, viewing episodic memory
as roughly corresponding to explicit memory and procedural and semantic
memory as roughly corresponding to implicit memory (Tulving 1987). He
characterized the three-system arrangement as monohierarchical, with epi-
sodic memory being a specialized subsystem that evolved out of semantic
memory, and semantic memory being a specialized subsystem that evolved
out of procedural memory. By this account, the procedural memory system
appeared first in phylogeny and ontogeny, and succeeding memory systems
appeared later:
“Semantic memory develops earlier in childhood than episodic memory:
Children are capable of learning facts of the world before they can remember
their own past experiences” (Tulving 1993, p. 68).
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
202 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
STRUCTURAL AND PROCESSING ACCOUNTS 203
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
204 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
imitate what they had seen modeled on one puppet when they were tested with
the other, even though both puppets were equally familiar. These data reveal
that infants subsequently transferred the memory of the modeled actions to the
second puppet only if they had previously formed an association between it
and the puppet on which the actions were modeled and remembered it.
Boller (1997) obtained a similar result when she repeatedly exposed 6-
month-olds to two distinctively colored and patterned cloth panels side-by-
side for 1 hour per day for 1 week and then subsequently trained them to kick
to move a mobile in the presence of one of the panels. When she tested them
with the mobile in the presence of the other context 1 day after training, they
recognized it. When she preexposed infants to the two panels for an equal
amount of time prior to training but at different times of day, however, they
subsequently did not recognize the mobile when tested with it in the presence
of the other panel, again revealing that infants transferred the training memory
they had acquired in the presence of one panel to the other panel by virtue of
the memory of the association between the two panels that they had previously
formed. If infants were equally familiar with the two panels but had no
opportunity to form an association between them, then the memory that
infants had subsequently acquired in the presence of one panel did not transfer
to the other.
In both of the preceding studies, infants learned that two discriminably
different objects were associated in the course of repeatedly seeing those
objects appear together, irrespective of the experimental paradigm within
which their memory of that association was tested. In both studies, the
memory of the association that infants had picked up by merely looking was
revealed only when they were subsequently given an opportunity to demon-
strate that knowledge through their direct actions.
Evidence for associations between memories in young infants. Finally, if
infants only possess the structural descriptions system, then they should not be
able to form associations between independent memories. However, they can
do this as well. Timmons (1994) obtained evidence that infants form associa-
tions between independent memories by 6 months of age. In an initial study,
she trained 6-month-olds either to move a mobile suspended over the playpen
or to turn on a music box affixed to the front of the playpen by either arm-
pulling or footkicking — an analog of the traditional paired-associate task
used in studies of verbal learning and memory. Infants were trained in a
distinctive context for 2 days on one cue-response pair and, 3 days later,
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
STRUCTURAL AND PROCESSING ACCOUNTS 205
learned the other cue-response pair in the same context. Three days after
learning the second pair, infants were tested with each of the cues in a delayed
recognition paradigm. During testing, infants produced only the particular
response (kicking or arm-pulling) that had originally been associated with a
given test cue (mobile or music box), regardless of which response they had
learned last. This result confirmed that infants’ memory of each cue-response
pair had been stored independently and that test responding was highly
specific to the retrieval cue that was presented.
In a second experiment, Timmons trained infants as before, but this time
she waited until both of the training memories had been forgotten (i.e., 3
weeks) and then exposed infants to either the mobile or to the music box as a
memory prime in a reactivation paradigm. On the following day, she adminis-
tered a long-term retention test to all infants using only the mobile as the
retrieval cue. As expected, infants who were both primed and tested with the
mobile exhibited the mobile-appropriate response to the mobile retrieval cue
— the typical finding in memory reactivation studies. Surprisingly, however,
infants who were primed with the music box also exhibited the mobile-
appropriate response (and no other) to the mobile test cue (see Figure 8.2).
Recall that the mobile memory was not directly primed with the mobile
and that it was forgotten at the time the music-box memory was primed.
Timmons assumed, therefore, that the music-box prime had activated the
memory node representing the music box paired-associate and that this activa-
tion had spread to the memory node representing the mobile paired-associate,
thereby enabling infants to perform the mobile-appropriate response to the
mobile cue during the subsequent test. Thus, even though the memory of each
task was independent and highly specific, the two memories were associa-
tively linked — possibly via the common context in which both tasks had
originally been acquired — in a common mnemonic network. As a result,
priming one memory brought to mind the memory of the other.
A similar phenomenon was reported by Barr and Vieira (1999), who
taught 6-month-old infants to associate the operant train procedure with an
imitation procedure. Drawing from Timmons’ (1994) finding that one
memory can be associated with another if the two memories were acquired in
a common context, they asked if 6-month-olds’ memory of the puppet task
would be associated with a memory of the train task if they acquired the two
memories in the same context. Because 6-month-olds remember the puppet
task for only 1 day but remember the train task for 2 weeks (see Figure 8.3),
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
206 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
they hypothesized that the longer-lived train memory might “carry” the asso-
ciated memory of the short-lived puppet task along with it and protract it
longer than otherwise possible. If so, then retrieving the train memory 1 or 2
weeks later might also retrieve infants’ memory of the puppet task, with the
result that infants would imitate the adult’s actions on the puppet after the
longer delay.
To test this, Barr and Vieira trained 6-month-olds in the train task.
Immediately after training was over, the mother — her infant on her lap —
turned her chair sideways so that the train was still in view to the infant’s side.
The experimenter then knelt in front of the infant and demonstrated a series of
three actions on a hand puppet (removing a mitten from the puppet’s hand,
shaking it to ring a bell hidden inside, and replacing the mitten). During the
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
STRUCTURAL AND PROCESSING ACCOUNTS 207
initial imitation test that followed immediately after the demonstration and
either 1 or 2 weeks later, infants were allowed 120 s from when they first
touched the puppet to imitate the experimenter’s actions. The hypothesis was
confirmed: Infants imitated the target actions after both delays — but only if
they were tested first with the train, not if they were tested first with the puppet
(see Figure 8.4). In other words, retrieving the train memory was necessary in
order to activate infants’ memory of the puppet task as well. Infants in a
baseline control group did not see the actions modeled originally, and they did
not spontaneously produce them during the test. We have recently found that
the puppet task can be associated with the memory of the train task even when
2 weeks have elapsed between learning the train task and initially viewing the
puppet task. The association is formed if the target actions on the puppet task
are modeled in the presence of the nonmoving train. These data clearly
demonstrate that a short-lived memory (the puppet demonstration) that is
associated with a longer-lived memory (the train task) can become longer-
lasting as well.
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
208 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
Figure 8.4. The duration of retention of 6-month-olds’ independent memory of the puppet
task (left panel, Exp. 1) and their memory of the puppet task after it had been associated with
the train task (right panel, Exp. 2). Asterisks indicate significant retention; vertical bars
indicate ±1 SE.
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
STRUCTURAL AND PROCESSING ACCOUNTS 209
train reactivated infants’ forgotten memory of the puppet task. One day later,
infants exhibited significant imitation if they were first primed for 2 min with
the train, either moving or nonmoving (see Figure 8.5, left panel). These
infants had seen the puppet demonstration only once and only briefly 3 weeks
earlier. The reactivation control group was not originally exposed to either
task but received the train reactivation treatment on day 20 and was primed
with the moving train immediately before the puppet test on day 21. This
group exhibited no retention when tested with the puppet.
A number of theorists have proposed that memories are represented in a
network and are connected to each other by links (e.g., Nelson & Kosslyn
1975). According to spreading-activation models (Collins & Loftus 1975;
Collins & Quillian 1969), the presentation of a retrieval cue activates a memory
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
210 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
STRUCTURAL AND PROCESSING ACCOUNTS 211
These memory systems were proposed by Squire (1986, 1987) and, like
Tulving’s (1972, 1983) memory systems, are distinguished by their content
and its accessibility to conscious recollection. The declarative memory system
stores information about facts and personally experienced events that can be
consciously recollected as a proposition or image, whereas the procedural
memory system (subsequently called the nondeclarative memory system;
Squire 1992a) stores acquired information that is embedded in procedures or
skills and cannot be consciously recollected (i.e., implicit memory). The
different types of information in the two systems have been distinguished as
knowing that and knowing how, respectively (Cohen & Squire 1980). In
Squire’s formulation, however, episodic memory and semantic memory are
subdivisions of the declarative system, and both are accessible to conscious-
ness. Episodic memory represents autobiographical information, whereas se-
mantic memory represents world knowledge. Likewise, episodic memory
represents information that is temporally dated, whereas semantic memory
represents information that is not.
Whereas Tulving’s systems were distinguished in an attempt to create a
taxonomy of memory, that is, for purposes of classification, Squire’s systems
were created for the purpose of developing a “biologically useful” level of
analysis of brain function, that is, an account of “how the brain itself actually
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
212 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
STRUCTURAL AND PROCESSING ACCOUNTS 213
Squire (1987) suggested that declarative memory may have evolved relatively
late in phylogeny and attained its maximum level of development in mam-
mals. Similarly, he suggested that declarative memory develops later than
procedural memory. This suggestion was based on Bachevalier and Mishkin’s
(1984) evidence from infant monkeys that the memory system develops later
than the habit system (see Chapter 5) and is consistent with other suggestions
that two memory systems that support comparable functions develop sequen-
tially (Schacter & Moscovitch 1984; Tulving 1983). As we have argued in
previous chapteres, a developmental dissociation between these two memory
systems is an article of faith rather than an empirical fact. For this reason, the
questions raised earlier about the developmental relationship between seman-
tic memory to episodic memory are also pertinent here.
Processing accounts
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
214 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
accounts (see Table 8.2). These accounts emphasize different cognitive opera-
tions within a unitary memory system instead of the different memory content
of different memory systems. The two major processing accounts that have
been proposed, transfer-appropriate processing and the multiple-entry modu-
lar model, are considered below.
Transfer-appropriate processing
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
STRUCTURAL AND PROCESSING ACCOUNTS 215
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
216 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
By this account, tasks that tap the same memory system would not be
expected to yield the same dissociations in response to every independent
variable; rather, the prediction of a dissociation would depend on the process-
ing requirements during encoding and testing in each instance. Although
transfer-appropriate processing can account for functional dissociations on
implicit and explicit tests by normal adults, it fares less well in accounting for
the memory dissociations of clinical populations who have experienced neural
insult, such as amnesics and Korsakoff patients (Roediger et al. 1989a).
A number of researchers have argued that implicit memories are less
flexible than explicit memories in transferring or generalizing to similar tasks
(Cohen & Eichenbaum 1993; Dienes & Berry 1997; Squire 1992b, 1994).
Willingham (1997, 1998b), however, argued that this distinction was based on
studies in which the structural features of the stimuli were changed at the time
of testing. He presented evidence from other studies that implicit and explicit
memories transfer equally well if the processes engaged in during encoding
and retrieval match and that both forms of memory transfer equally poorly if
the processes during encoding and retrieval differ. By this argument, process-
ing differences in flexibility can be understood in terms of the transfer-
appropriate processing account (Roediger & Blaxton 1987).
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
STRUCTURAL AND PROCESSING ACCOUNTS 217
might be applicable; however, given that the effect of priming was measured
during a recognition test and we cannot stipulate the kind of processing in
which infants initially engaged, this aspect of the processing analysis is
strained.
Although nothing in the infant’s behavior allows us to say whether
memory retrieval on a delayed recognition test is conscious or intentional, the
fact that infants do not recognize the reactivation stimulus at the time it is
presented as a memory prime allows us to conclude with confidence that
memory retrieval during reactivation does not result from a deliberate or
intentional search process and thus cannot be accompanied by conscious
awareness. For this reason, we attribute the retrieval that results from reactiva-
tion or priming to an all-or-none automatic, perceptual-identification process.
If the prime is able to reactivate the memory, then we can assume that its
structural or perceptual characteristics were represented in the infant’s
memory at the time of encoding — an assumption consistent with the transfer-
appropriate processing account.
The fact that manipulations such as interference or no interference,
shallow or deep level of processing, longer or shorter study time, and so forth
affect the magnitude of delayed recognition differently than they affect the
magnitude of reactivation, however, acceptance of the transfer-appropriate
processing account would force us to conclude that the operations required to
process these variables at the time of encoding and testing also differ. Yet,
processing differences between the delayed recognition and reactivation tasks
occur only at the time of retrieval. Thus, the onus for matching the input is
placed squarely on the conditions that prevail at the time of retrieval. In studies
with adults, differentiating the operations at the time of encoding is easier
because they can be instructed to attend to different aspects of the study
materials (i.e., engage in different types of processing), and at the time of
testing, the experimenter can provide material that either matched or not what
subjects were instructed to study.
Treisman (1992) referred to encoding and retrieval as perceiving and re-
perceiving, respectively. If an object had not originally been perceived, of
course, it could not be re-perceived. We have found it particularly useful to
focus on the type of processing in which infants and adults engage at the time
of retrieval. There are two fundamental kinds of visual processing — serial
and parallel processing (Treisman & Gelade 1980). Parallel processing is
automatic, preattentive, and rapid and occurs early in the visual system,
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
218 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
STRUCTURAL AND PROCESSING ACCOUNTS 219
Figure 8.6. A pop-out mobile that was used during reactivation with 6-month-olds. This
pop-out mobile contained a single target (L) amidst eight distractors (+s). All infants were
trained and tested with a mobile containing nine blocks. For infants in the novel pop-out
reactivation condition, all blocks on the training and test mobile displayed +s; for infants in
the familiar pop-out reactivation condition, all blocks on the training and test mobile
displayed Ls.
argument, the magnitude of pop-out was the same irrespective of the number
of distractors on the reactivation mobile (see Figure 8.7). Only when the
reactivation mobile contained 13 objects were the predictions reversed — and
for an uninteresting reason: Because the single object was centered in the
display, the largest number of objects on the reactivation mobile obscured it.
In the delayed recognition task at 6 months of age, Bhatt et al. (1994)
found evidence that a top-down serial process can override the automatic
parallel process. Whereas 3-month-olds exhibited significant 24-hour reten-
tion on a delayed recognition test if the test mobile displayed the training
character as the target (pop-out) stimulus but not if it displayed a novel one, 6-
month-olds exhibited significant 24-hour retention whether the single target
was the training character or it was novel and the six distractors were training
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
220 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
Figure 8.7. Mean baseline ratios as a function of the number of blocks on the reactivation
mobile (set size). A unique novel (open square) or familiar (filled square) block was
embedded amidst either familiar or novel distractor blocks, respectively, on the reactivation
mobile. Whether the reactivation mobile was an effective memory prime was assessed 24
hours later. An asterisk indicates that the memory was reactivated (M baseline ratio
significantly > 1.00). In both the novel and familiar reactivation conditions, test perfor-
mance over set sizes from 5 to 11 blocks did not differ.
characters. Because the single familiar target popped out, the single novel one
must have popped out as well. In the latter instance, however, infants must
have engaged in an attention-demanding serial (element-by-element) search
or they would not have located the predictive cue(s) that enabled them to
exhibit retention. Thus, although there are age-related changes in perceptual
processing, we argue that these are changes in processing speed — not in the
fundamental processes (or systems) that underlie memory performance. Here,
between 3 and 6 months, infants processed the test stimulus more rapidly, just
as they process the prime more rapidly at 6 than at 3 months of age (Hildreth &
Rovee-Collier 1999a).
Musen and Treisman (1990) related parallel and serial processing to
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
STRUCTURAL AND PROCESSING ACCOUNTS 221
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
222 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
Figure 8.8. A multiple-entry modular memory system (MEM). Top panel: Two reflective
subsystems (R-1, R-2) and two perceptual subsystems (P-1, P-2). Reflective and perceptual
subsystems can interact through a control (the supervisor) and monitoring (the executive)
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
STRUCTURAL AND PROCESSING ACCOUNTS 223
process of R-1 and R-2, respectively. These processes are the means by which the percep-
tion and reflection subsystems interact. They are depicted as cones passing through the
planes that represent the different subsystems. The size of the ellipses at the intersections of
the cones and planes indicates the extent to which each of these processes is involved in the
activities of each subsystem. Thus, the executive process has greater access to the reflection
system than to the perception system and greater access to P-2 than to P-1. Middle panel:
The component processes of R-1 and R-2. Bottom panel: The component processes of P-1
and P-2. ©1991, Plenum Publishing Corporation; adopted with permission. From M.K.
Johnson, Reflection, reality monitoring, and the self. In R. Kunzendorf (Ed.), Mental
imagery: Proceedings of the Eleventh Annual Conference of the American Association for
the Study of Mental Imagery (pp. 3–16). New York: Plenum.
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
224 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
processes interact (see Figure 8.8, top panel) and can be as simple as a task
demand (“identify the pronouns”) or as complex as a script that specifies the
combination of component processes and the order in which they must be
engaged. Whereas supervisor processes (R-1) are more holistic and global,
executive processes (R-2) are more deliberate and analytic (Johnson & Hirst
1993).
Earlier, Johnson and Raye (1981) argued that conscious recollection (i.e.,
experiencing a sense of pastness in remembering) is the consequence of a
reality-monitoring process in which a person decides whether a memory
originated in an actual experience (the perception system) or in imagination
(the reflection system) on the basis of its attributes. Presumably, the memory
of an actually experienced event is more vivid, highly detailed, contains
spatiotemporal details, and generally consists of more sensory or perceptual
attributes than a memory of an imagined event, which arises in the reflection
system. In addition, it is consistent with related memories. On the other hand,
an inability to discriminate between the attributes of actually perceived and
self-generated events (i.e., a break-down of reality-monitoring) can lead to
confabulation, when the individual treats an entry in the reflection system as if
it originated in the perception system (Johnson 1991a).
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
STRUCTURAL AND PROCESSING ACCOUNTS 225
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
226 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
STRUCTURAL AND PROCESSING ACCOUNTS 227
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
228 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
As an example that P-1 develops before P-2, Johnson and Hirst (1993)
noted that infants engage in visual tracking before they can recognize some-
thing as familiar. Evidence for this example and others that one might think of,
however, is either absent or contradictory. Although newborn infants can
track a visual stimulus, they can also distinguish their mother’s voice from that
of a stranger — and prefer it (DeCasper & Fifer 1980). Moreover, newborns
tested 33 hours after birth can distinguish a tape-recording of their mother
reading a familiar Dr. Seuss passage (i.e., one that she had read aloud for 20
min a day during the last 6 weeks of gestation) from a tape-recording of their
mother reading a novel Dr. Seuss passage (DeCasper & Spence 1986). Fur-
ther, newborns prefer the familiar passage.
In the same chapter, however, Johnson and Hirst acknowledged that a
particular memory subsystem or process should not be associated with a
particular age:
“it would be a mistake simply to characterize a human infant as ‘having’ only
P-1 and P-2 subsystems, or to characterize a young child as ‘getting’ R-2
processes at some particular age. For example, some aspects of reflective
processes (e.g., reactivation) may operate from quite early on (Rovee-Collier
1990). Furthermore, specific learning occurs in all subsystems throughout the
lifespan. Acquisition of new information in any particular subsystem depends
on acquisition of prior information. Consequently, differences in ‘sophistica-
tion’ of the subsystems at a given age are partly the consequence of what has
already been learned by the different subsystems. One might have a know-
ledgeable P-1 system and a less educated R-2 system or vice versa” (Johnson
& Hirst 1993, pp. 252-253).
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
STRUCTURAL AND PROCESSING ACCOUNTS 229
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
230 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
Rovee-Collier et al. 1985a). This example also exactly parallels one of the
examples of R-1 reactivating that was cited in Johnson and Chalfonte (1994)
and confirms that the R-1 subsystem is active by at least 3 months of age. In
the preceding examples, we cannot say whether infants’ memories were
indirectly reactivated in response to internally-generated cues, as is required
of reactivating in MEM. However, we can say that reactivation in examples 2
and 3 was indirect and mediated by a prior association.
If reactivating in MEM and reactivation in infants and animals are the
same, then the proposed appearance of the four memory subsystems in phy-
logeny must be rethought. Johnson and Chalfonte (1994) cited evidence of
reactivating (MEM) in rats, but reactivation effects have been documented in
organisms at all phyletic levels — from the sea snail Hermissenda (Matzel,
Collin & Alkon 1992) to humans (see Chapter 7).
Conclusions
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
CHAPTER 9
This chapter considers the fate of implicit memories after they have been primed, the effect
of priming when a memory is active versus when it is not, and the effect of priming with a
novel stimulus after delays when a memory can still be explicitly retrieved. The data in this
chapter were obtained exclusively from infants, but they have major implications for
everyday memory in individuals of all ages.
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
232 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
than R-1 processes alone, the product of these subprocesses should also differ.
In some ways, however, the distinction between explicit and implicit
memories is not so clear-cut. Adults’ memories that were originally explicit,
for example, can eventually become automatic — a defining characteristic of
implicit memories (Mandler 1985) — if they are repeatedly practiced or
retrieved (Logan 1988, 1990, 1992; Logan & Klapp 1991; Willingham &
Goedert-Eschmann 1999). Logan (1988), for example, proposed that specific
instances of past encounters with a given stimulus and the response that was
made to it are stored as episodic memories that are retrieved when the stimulus
is presented on a future occasion. Over repeated retrievals of prior processing
episodes involving the same stimulus and response (i.e., practice), memory
performance becomes faster and more automatized. Thus, learning to drive a
car with a standard transmission initially requires a great deal of focused
attention, and the dos-and-don’ts from specific prior training episodes are
recalled and rehearsed during later practice sessions. Eventually, however,
clutching, shifting, accelerating, braking, turning on the turn signals or wind-
shield wipers, and steering are performed automatically and effortlessly in a
coordinated manner.
Although some researchers might classify learning to drive as a skill or
procedural memory (Squire 1992a), more complex behavioral acts that defy
simple classification as procedural memories also can become automatic. A
common experience such as driving the car to work — which may require
more than an hour, traverse many miles, and require negotiating all manner of
traffic and hazards, can become frighteningly automatized. Although we
arrive safely at the office, we have no recollection of getting there or what we
saw or did en route.
In everyday life, implicit and explicit memories presumably guide our
normal commerce with the environment and influence our understanding.
Although the long-term effects of retrieving implicit and explicit memories
are clearly important for our future behavior, memory studies with adults have
given little consideration to whether these effects are the same or different.
Whereas instructing normal adults to respond with the first word that comes to
mind (an implicit memory task) yields immediate memory performance that is
similar to that of amnesics, the long-term effect of priming on the subsequent
memory performance of normal and abnormal populations surely differs.
When this question has been asked of normal adults, it has been framed only in
terms of the effect of retrieving explicit memories on future retrieval. In
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
EXPLICIT-IMPLICIT INTERACTIONS 233
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
234 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
may be an ideal population for answering some of the questions that are less
amenable to experimental study with adults. Because 3-month-olds forget
within only 1 week, and 6-month-olds forget within only 3 weeks, their
subsequent retention following administration of a prime after the original
memory was forgotten can be assessed within a reasonable time frame. In
addition, the functional significance of priming when the memory is active
versus when it is not, as well as the effect of priming with a novel stimulus
when the memory is active, can be studied with relative ease. The results of
studies with infants that addressed some of these questions are reviewed
below.
Despite the numerous functional dissociations that have been found between
implicit and explicit memories in infants (see Chapter 7), other aspects of their
implicit and explicit memories are very similar.
Magnitude of retention
Duration of retention
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
EXPLICIT-IMPLICIT INTERACTIONS 235
Figure 9.1. The duration of retention between 3 and 12 months of age after original training
(dark bars) and after reactivation (striped bars). In both cases, the duration of retention
increases linearly with age.
Hayne et al. 1997; Herbert & Hayne 2000), and the duration of retention of
implicit (reactivated) memories does as well. As can be seen in Figure 9.1, the
duration of a reactivated memory increased linearly between 3 and 12 months
of age in both the mobile and train tasks (Hildreth & Rovee-Collier 1999b).
Rate of forgetting
Since the time of Ebbinghaus (1885), forgetting has been recognized as the
most ubiquitous characteristic of adult memory. This characteristic describes
infant memory as well. Over the first year of life, infants gradually forget
explicit memories over the interval between training and the delayed recogni-
tion test, and they gradually forget implicit (reactivated) memories over the
interval between priming and testing as well. Figure 9.2 shows that the rate of
forgetting explicit and implicit memories was the same over the first year of
life, whether infants were studied with mobiles or trains (Hildreth & Rovee-
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
236 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
Figure 9.2. The rate at which infants between 3 and 12 months of age forget the original
memory (open squares) and the reactivated memory (filled squares). At each age, the
memory prime was administered 1 week after infants of a given age last remembered the
task. Asterisks indicate no retention (M baseline ratio not significantly > 1.00).
Collier 1999b). This common pattern occurred despite the fact that both
explicit and implicit memories were forgotten over increasingly longer peri-
ods of time with age. A cursory inspection of Figure 9.2 offers no clue as to
which function represents infants’ forgetting of the reactivated memory.
The preceding results are particularly impressive when one considers that
the forgotten memory was always reactivated 1 week after infants of a given
age last remembered the task. When this 1-week delay is taken into account,
we see that exposing infants to a single memory prime at least doubles the
accessibility of the memory over the entire first year of life (see Figure 9.3).
The robustness of this effect encourages us that it probably occurs for older
individuals as well.
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
EXPLICIT-IMPLICIT INTERACTIONS 237
Original Memory
Figure 9.3. The retention benefit of a single reactivation over the first year of life. The
maximum duration of retention (weeks) of the training memory is shown before reactiva-
tion (solid line; see Figure 6.6) and after reactivation (dashed line). The single memory
prime was given 1 week after infants of each age last remembered the task.
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
238 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
instance, retention was still seen 6 weeks after infants were initially trained —
a period almost half again their life-time. After being primed three times,
however, infants’ retention was no longer than when they were primed only
once.
Although Hayne found that three reactivations were not as effective as
two, we have found that whether additional reactivations can protract reten-
tion even longer depends on their timing. In a subsequent mobile study with
infants who were only 2 months old when they were trained, we gave infants
the same reactivation treatments as Hayne but spaced them every 3 weeks
through 26 weeks of age (Rovee-Collier et al. 1999). Immediately preceding
each reactivation treatment, we administered a 3-min delayed recognition test
in order to determine whether or not infants exhibited retention. After three to
five reactivations, these younger infants eventually exhibited significant re-
tention 3 weeks after their last reactivation.
Taken together, these data reveal that the durations of explicit and im-
plicit memories differ after a memory has been multiply primed.
Recall that the specific details of both the original mobile and the encoding
context are represented in infants’ training memory. As the interval between
training and testing increases, infants progressively forget the specific details
of the original mobile but continue to remember its general features or gist,
thereby generalizing to novel test mobiles (Rovee-Collier & Sullivan 1980). If
infants are trained and tested in a highly distinctive context, however, the
context primes these forgotten details via a serial attention-gating mechanism.
As a result, infants are able to discriminate the original mobile from a novel
one after longer delays, when they can still recognize the context (Butler &
Rovee-Collier 1989). Likewise, at both 3 and 6 months of age, an implicit
memory is also highly specific to the details of the original cue and context
(Hartshorn & Rovee-Collier 1997; Hayne & Rovee-Collier 1995; Shields &
Rovee-Collier 1992). In this respect, then, explicit and implicit memories are
similar.
After the implicit memory has been forgotten, however, the memory
attributes that represented the details of the encoding context are apparently
lost, because the second time the memory is reactivated, the context can be
different. Moreover, after the twice-reactivated memory has been forgotten,
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
EXPLICIT-IMPLICIT INTERACTIONS 239
the memory attributes that represented the details of the original mobile
apparently are lost, because the twice-reactivated and twice-forgotten memory
can be reactivated a third time by a different cue as well as in a different
context (Hitchcock & Rovee-Collier 1996). These data reveal that explicit and
implicit memories differ in the accessibility of their different attributes after
they have been multiply primed, perhaps because multiply primed memories
are older, and older memories are more fragile.
In 1972, Tulving proposed that information is more readily lost from the
episodic than from the semantic memory system. Furlong (1951, cited in
Tulving 1983, p. 17) had similarly hypothesized that retrospective [i.e., epi-
sodic] memory became nonretrospective [i.e., semantic] memory as the con-
text faded. These statements are consistent with the preceding evidence that
contextual information becomes lost from infants’ memories over the course
of repeated reactivations. If an episodic memory eventually becomes context
free because its details pertaining to the time and place of its origin have been
lost, however, then this raises questions about the usefulness of distinguishing
different memory systems in the first place. Does, for example, an episodic
memory become a semantic one, shifting memory systems, after this informa-
tion has been lost? Or does context-specific information become lost because
a memory has been transferred from the episodic to the semantic memory
system? Or does such a transfer even occur? The loss of context specificity
seems to be better accommodated by envisioning memory as a single system
that is characterized by different degrees of specificity, different routes of
access, and so forth (e.g., Mandler 1985).
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
240 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
treatment — a point when the twice-reactivated memory was still active. But
why would the state of the memory make a difference?
The answer was suggested a number of years ago by a study in which
researchers used memory primes of different durations. Gordon, Smith and
Katz (1979) reported that exposing adult rats to a memory prime for 15 s
reactivated a memory of active avoidance, whereas exposing rats to the prime
for 75 s extinguished it. Subsequently, Arnold and Spear (1993) obtained a
similar result with preweanling rat pups: Exposing pups to a memory prime
for 5 s or 15 s reactivated the forgotten training memory, but exposing them to
the prime for 30 s did not. Given evidence that a memory can be modified only
when it is active (Lewis 1979), Gordon (1981) had hypothesized that reactiva-
tion of a memory begins when a prime is initially exposed and that, after the
memory has been reactivated, the continued presence of the prime can lead to
new learning that can modify it. By this account, then, a lengthy prime
provides an opportunity for the reactivated memory to be modified counter-
acting the original effectiveness of the prime.
Figure 9.4. The duration of retention (days) following one, two, or three reactivation
treatments at 3 months of age. An asterisk indicates significant retention.
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
EXPLICIT-IMPLICIT INTERACTIONS 241
In fact, Hayne (1990) observed this result in a control group that was
included to determine whether the retention benefit of two reactivations
resulted because the memory prime was presented twice or because the total
duration of priming was twice as long. When Hayne had exposed infants to
two 3-min primes — each when the memory was inactive, their forgotten
mobile memory had not only been reactivated but had been protracted for 2
weeks. When she exposed infants to one 6-min prime — also when the
memory was inactive, however, infants exhibited no retention at all only 24
hours later. By Gordon’s (1981) account, the 6-min prime initially reactivated
the inactive memory, but its continued exposure enabled new learning that
either competed with or subtracted from what infants had learned before.
If a lengthy exposure to the prime once the memory is inactive can impair
retention, then a shorter exposure to the prime when the memory is already
active should have a similar effect. In fact, when Hayne (1990) gave infants a
third 3-min reactivation treatment only 1 week after the second reactivation
treatment — a time when the twice-reactivated memory was still highly
accessible, infants exhibited excellent retention 1 day later but none 1 week
later — when the benefit of two reactivations had still been seen. Apparently,
presenting the 3-min prime when the memory was still accessible was long
enough to partially counteract the retention benefit produced by the second
reactivation but not so long that it eliminated the effectiveness of the second
reactivation altogether.
As another test of the effect of priming on an accessible memory, Adler et
al. (2000) gave 3-month-olds an initial 3-min reactivation treatment either
immediately (0 days) or 3 days after training — when the original memory
was still relatively accessible — and asked how it affected infants’ subsequent
retention. Although 3-month-olds can remember an explicit memory for 5 but
not 6 days after training is over (Hayne 1990), infants who were primed for 3
min immediately after training exhibited significant retention 6 but not 7 days
later; and infants who were primed 3 days after training exhibited significant
retention 7 but not 9 days after training. Thus, priming an accessible memory
provided a slight memory boost that increased with the delay between training
and priming. Priming when the original memory was active, however, did not
boost retention nearly as long as priming when it was not (e.g., Hayne 1990;
Hildreth & Rovee-Collier 1999b).
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
242 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
In the preceding study, Adler et al. (2000) presented the prime 3 days after
training. Because 3-month-old infants have forgotten the specific details of
their original training mobile after this delay, we speculated that the effect of
the prime on the active memory might be different if it were novel. Previously,
we found that priming with a novel mobile for 3 min immediately after
training (day 0), when the memory does not have to be retrieved at all,
retroactively interfered with infants’ recognition of the original mobile the
next day, but this effect was temporary and disappeared within 2 days (Rossi-
George & Rovee-Collier 1999). The same interference phenomenon had
occurred when infants were primed with a novel mobile 3 days after training
and tested with the original mobile the next day (Rovee-Collier et al. 1994).
This phenomenon was also temporary: Although infants who were primed
with a novel mobile 3 days after training had failed to recognize the training
mobile 1 day later, Adler et al. found that they did recognize it 7 days but not
9 days afterward. Thus, as before, a 3-min exposure to a novel prime tempo-
rarily interfered with the original memory. Because priming the active
memory with either a novel mobile or the original mobile 3 days after training
protracted retention for the same duration (i.e., for an additional 2 days), Adler
et al. concluded that the common retention boost was mediated by the general
features that the novel mobile shared with the original one. (Recall that
priming occurred at a time when infants could not behaviorally differentiate a
novel mobile from the training one.) In other words, the prime in Adler et al.
did not protract retention because it was novel but because it was effectively
the same as the training mobile.
Because the original memory becomes progressively less accessible over
time until it finally cannot be retrieved on a delayed recognition test at all, we
thought that what infants actually learned about a novel prime might be
analyzed in more depth if it were exposed at more points along the original
forgetting function. To do this, we had to turn to older infants, who remember
longer. Therefore, we trained 6-month-olds in the mobile task and primed
them with a novel mobile (mobile B) after delays ranging from 1 to 13 days,
testing them with either the training mobile (mobile A) or mobile B 24 hours
later (Muzzio & Rovee-Collier 1996).
Previously, Boller et al. (1995) had primed 6-month-olds with a novel
mobile immediately after training (day 0) and had found that they recognized
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
EXPLICIT-IMPLICIT INTERACTIONS 243
both mobile A and mobile B 24 hours later. In the present study, we found that
if 6-month-olds were primed with a novel mobile after delays ranging from 1
to 8 days, then they subsequently recognized only mobile B but not mobile A;
but if they were primed with a novel mobile after delays ranging from 9 to 13
days, then they subsequently recognized neither mobile A nor mobile B (see
Figure 9.5). Because unprimed infants of this age can recognize mobile A for
14 days and can also discriminate it from mobile B after that same delay, we
concluded that priming with the novel mobile after delays longer than 1 day
had interfered with their recognition of mobile A whether they had recognized
mobile B 24 hours later or not.
In addition, both mobile A and mobile B reactivated the training memory
3 weeks later if the novel prime was exposed only 1 day after training, but only
mobile B reactivated the memory if the novel prime was exposed 13 days after
Figure 9.5. The time line for the onset (+) of three qualitatively different consequences of
priming an active memory — memory impairment, source confusion (memory facilitation),
and categorization. After training in the mobile task for 2 days, 6-month-olds were primed
for 2 min with a novel mobile after different delays (exposure delay) between 1 and 14 days
and were tested 1 day later.
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
244 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
training. These data are intriguing because infants who were primed with
mobile B after either delay had not recognized mobile A 24 hours later. This
result suggested that mobile A was still represented in the training memory
after the 1-day novel prime but was not after the 13-day novel prime. Because
other attempts to demonstrate that mobile A was still represented in the
training memory after being primed with mobile B after a long delay also
failed, we concluded that its details had probably been replaced in the training
memory by mobile B. These data, like the data of Hitchock and Rovee-Collier
(1996), suggest that older memories are more fragile.
The preceding results reveal that priming with a novel mobile when
infants’ training memory is still relatively accessible can produce either
memory facilitation (i.e., facilitated recognition of the novel prime) or memory
impairment (i.e., impaired recognition of original mobile), depending on the
timing of the prime in relation to the original training event. However, a third
effect of priming a relatively accessible memory with a novel stimulus can
also occur — a categorization effect. Exposing adults to at least two different
exemplars of a category facilitates their subsequent responding to novel
category members (e.g., Flannagan, Fried & Holyoak 1986; Fried & Holyoak
1984), and exposing infants to at least two different exemplars of a category
has the same effect. After being trained with mobile A in one session and
mobile B in another, for example, both 3- and 6-month-olds recognize a novel
category member (mobile C) 1 day later (Hayne et al. 1987; Shields & Rovee-
Collier 1992). The same result occurs when 3-month-olds are primed with a
novel mobile (mobile B) immediately after being trained for 2 days with
mobile A. Apparently, the prior memory of training with mobile A that is
retrieved at the outset of session 2 is recoded to include mobile B. As a result,
infants subsequently behave to mobile C as if they had actually been trained
with two different mobiles in the first place (Rovee-Collier et al. 1993a).
Otherwise, infants trained for 2 days with the same mobile would have
discriminated the novel test mobile (mobile C).
Muzzio and Rovee-Collier (1996) found a similar categorization effect,
but it too depended on when the prime was presented in relation to original
training. When 6-month-olds were primed with mobile B after delays of 1, 3,
6, or 9 days, they responded to mobile C after the two longest priming delays
(6 and 9 days) but not after the two shortest ones (see Figure 9.5). The fact that
infants responded to a completely novel test mobile means that their memory
of mobile A was still intact after 9 days (i.e., the interference by mobile B after
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
EXPLICIT-IMPLICIT INTERACTIONS 245
delays up to this point had been temporary) and distinguishes their memory
performance as a genuine categorization effect (recall that categorization
requires exposure to two different category members — A and B).
An important finding of this study is that failing to observe an effect of
recently encountered information or observing one effect (e.g., memory im-
pairment) but not another (e.g., memory facilitation or categorization) cannot
be taken as evidence that priming an accessible memory with a novel stimulus
has no other effects. Here, after priming delays when infants subsequently
recognized neither mobile A nor mobile B (i.e., 9 days), they still recognized
mobile C. Had they not previously been primed with mobiles A and B, then
they would not have responded to mobile C. Likewise, after priming delays
when they recognized neither mobile A nor mobile C, they recognized mobile
B. This study clearly demonstrates that the effect of a prime when the memory
is accessible is strictly determined by the point on the forgetting function at
which it is presented.
Conclusions
Studies with infants have answered several questions about the relation be-
tween implicit and explicit memories that are difficult to address with adults.
They have revealed that implicit memories assume a number of the character-
istics of explicit memories. Over the first year of life, for example, implicit
memories that were forgotten before they were primed are remembered for as
long as explicit memories, and their pattern of forgetting is also the same.
Memories that are still accessible at the time of priming are not remembered as
long as memories that are forgotten. Nonetheless, the extent to which an
already accessible memory is protracted by priming varies with the delay
between training and priming as it does with the delay between training and
recognition testing, even though the duration of retention after priming is not
as great when the memory is accessible. Finally, we previously found that
implicit and explicit memories enter into the same functional relationships
with the context at 3 and 6 months of age (Borovsky & Rovee-Collier 1990;
Butler & Rovee-Collier 1989; Hartshorn & Rovee-Collier 1997; Hayne &
Rovee-Collier 1995; Shields & Rovee-Collier 1992).
Taken together, these findings seem inconsistent with the notion that
explicit and implicit memories are distinctly different memories that are
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
246 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
CHAPTER 10
Epilogue
In this chapter, we summarize our basic conclusions and reflect on some of the popular
notions that are based on earlier assumptions about the development of implicit and explicit
memory. Finally, we consider the implications of recent memory research with infants for
theory and research on memory in general.
This practice is nowhere more apparent than in the field of memory develop-
ment, where the central problem is to specify how the superior memory of
adults evolves from the memory abilities of infants. Where do we stand today
in terms of its solution? In one proposed solution, adult memory is cast as the
product of multiple memory systems that develop hierarchically during the
infancy period (Bachevalier & Mishkin 1984; Schacter & Moscovitch 1984;
Squire 1987, 1992a; Tulving 1983, 1993). In the preceding chapters, however,
we reviewed evidence that this solution is not correct for either human or
animal infants, primate or nonprimate. Even if adults possess multiple memory
systems that decline sequentially, those systems most certainly do not develop
hierarchically.
In the face of mounting evidence against it, some scientists have contin-
ued to defend the hierarchical development hypothesis. Why? We can only
speculate that they are either philosophically committed to this solution, have
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
248 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
ignored the overwhelming evidence against it, or have not understood it. One
clear source of misunderstanding has centered on the kinds of memory that are
measured in studies of mobile conjugate reinforcement. Nelson (1995), for
example, likened infants’ conditioned footkicks during delayed recognition
tests to the conditioned limb flexion response of the cat. Given that limb
flexion is reflexively elicited by electrical shock, this analogy is obviously
incorrect. Operant kicks are not elicited, and their magnitude obviously cannot
depend on the parameters of the eliciting stimulation. Unlike conditioned limb
reflexes, whether the infant kicks above baseline during a delayed recognition
test depends on the informational content of the test stimulus and the test
context. Moreover, prior to 1 year of age, young infants do not immediately
respond to a priming stimulus, whereas elicited reflexes occur within millisec-
onds. In fact, an infant’s conditioned kicking or lever-pressing is no different
from an adult’s pressing a response key, touching a video screen, circling a
word on a piece of paper, or verbalizing “yes” or “no” as an index of
recognition.
For others, whether infants are capable of explicit memory or behave like
amnesics seems to boil down to the question of whether they can consciously
recollect having seen a stimulus before — a question that cannot be directly
answered. This stance is reflected in the following quote from Eacott (1999, p.
47):
“…a behavioral measure of recall, such as lever pressing, presents a problem,
as all memory abilities may not be alike. An ability to recall facts and events
has been contrasted to the ability to learn and remember how to use skills and
strategies. Memories of facts and events can be brought to mind and be made
consciously available, whereas skills and strategies are often implicit and not
accessible to consciousness. These two aspects of memory are called declara-
tive and nondeclarative (procedural) memory, respectively. The long-term
memory abilities demonstrated in infants may be an example of nondeclara-
tive memory. If so, it may be that they do not have…a late-developing
declarative memory system (Bachevalier & Mishkin 1984).”
In the preceding quote, Eacott classified infants’ kicks and lever presses on
retention tests as skills. Then, asserting that memory for skills does not require
conscious recollection, she concluded that infants’ memory performance —
like that of amnesics — lacks conscious awareness. Bjorklund (2000) ex-
pressed the same view in his text on children’s thinking:
“…conditioning experiments with infants do not seem to require conscious
awareness. That is, unlike most of the memory we will discuss in this chapter,
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
EPILOGUE 249
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
250 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
Studies with infants have yielded novel findings about basic memory phenom-
ena that are difficult to study with older children and impractical to study with
adults. These studies have revealed, for example, that implicit and explicit
memories “behave” identically after they have been retrieved. This finding
suggests that implicit and explicit memories are not different memories from
different systems (Squire 1992a) nor even different forms of memory (Graf &
Schacter 1985) but are the same memory that has simply been retrieved via
different routes — either implicitly or explicitly (Crowder 1988; Mandler
1985). Another finding from studies with infants pertains to the functional
significance of retrieving implicit and explicit memories. Memories that have
been primed remain accessible for as long as they were originally, being
forgotten only gradually and at the same rate. Priming the memory when it is
active, however, has different consequences than priming the memory when it
is not, and refreshing the memory has different consequences yet. All prolong
the life of a memory, but some conditions are more likely to facilitate its
distortion. These findings have major implications for whether and for how
long we can access and use that memory in the future. Finally, if a memory is
repeatedly retrieved either explicitly or implicitly, its contents will change.
New information can be added, and old information can be lost — and some
kinds of information are added and lost more readily. These changes distort
both the specificity and accuracy of the memory that was originally encoded
and thereby will impact what we remember and understand in the future.
These and other findings from studies of infant memory speak directly to
the three major phenomena that, according to Estes (1997), any satisfactory
theory of memory must address — memory retrieval, forgetting, and memory
distortions. Memory theories that have been formulated solely from adult data
and that do not accommodate new findings from infants are incomplete and
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
EPILOGUE 251
inadequate. In the past, memory data from infants have been put through a
sieve that lets pass only those fragments that are commensurate with preestab-
lished notions of how memory works at different ages or what different tasks
or procedures can reveal. This is wrong — and it is bad Science. Data from
critically controlled studies with infants must be taken at face value as evi-
dence of what organisms can and cannot do and how their prior experiences
affect their current behavior.
Let us be clear: We do not seek more satisfactory theories of memory
development — such theories inevitably turn out to be theories of language
development. If not, then, what? We seek more satisfactory theories of
memory. Although major aspects of both the brain and behavior change
dramatically over ontogeny, the basic processes of learning and memory and
the general principles that describe them do not. Only by drawing on a
complete data base will memory theorists be able to test the generality and
affirm the validity of their predictions. Only from such a rich data base will
psychologists be able to construct a truly satisfactory theory of memory.
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
References
Abravanel, E., Levan-Goldschmidt, E., & Stevenson, M.B. 1976. Action imitation: The
early phase of infancy. Child Development, 47, 1032–1044.
Adler, S.A., Gerhardstein, P., & Rovee-Collier, C. 1998. Levels-of-processing effects in
infant memory? Child Development, 69, 280–294.
Adler, S.A., & Rovee-Collier, C. 1994. The memorability and discriminability of primitive
perceptual units in infancy. Vision Research, 34, 449–459.
Adler, S.A., Wilk, A., & Rovee-Collier, C. 2000. Effects of reinstatement and reactivation
on active memory in infants, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 75, 93–115.
Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., Damasio, H., & Damasio, A.R. 1994. Impaired recognition of
emotion in facial expressions following bilateral damage to the human amygdala.
Nature, 372, 669–672.
Agayoff, J.L., Sheffield, E.G., & Hudson, J.A. 1999, April. Effects of video reminders on
young children’s long-term recall. Symposium paper presented at the meeting of the
Society for Research in Child Development, Albuquerque, NM.
Ajuriaguerra, J. de, Rey-Bellet-Muller, M., & Tissot, R. 1964. A propos de quelques
problemes poses par le deficit operatoire des vieillards atteints de demence degenerative
en debut d’evolution. Cortex, 1, 232–256.
Alberts, J.R. 1984. Sensory-perceptual development in the Norway rat: A view toward
comparative studies. In R. Kail & N.E. Spear (Eds.), Comparative perspectives on the
development of memory (pp. 65–101). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Allen, S.W., & Jacoby, L.L. 1990. Reinstating study context produces unconscious influ-
ences of memory. Memory & Cognition, 18, 270–278.
Alvarez, P., Zola-Morgan, S., & Squire, L.R. 1994. The animal model of human amnesia:
Long-term memory impaired and short-term memory intact. Proceedings of the Na-
tional Academy of Sciences, (USA), 91, 5637–5641.
Alvarez, P., Zola-Morgan, S., & Squire, L.R. 1995. Damage limited to the hippocampal
region produces long-lasting memory impairment in monkeys. Journal of Neuro-
science, 15, 3796–3807.
Alvarez-Royo, P., Clower, R.P., Zola-Morgan, S.M., & Squire, L.R. 1991. Stereotaxic
lesions of the hippocampus in monkeys: Determination of surgical coordinates and
analysis of lesions using magnetic resonance imaging. Journal of Neuroscience Meth-
ods, 38, 223–232.
Amabile, T.A., & Rovee-Collier, C. 1991. Contextual variation and memory retrieval at six
months. Child Development, 62, 1155–1166.
Anderson, J.R., & Ross, B.H. 1980. Evidence against a semantic-episodic distinction.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 6, 441–465.
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
254 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
Anderson, M.C., Bjork, R.A., & Bjork, E.L. 1994. Remembering can cause forgetting:
Retrieval dynamics in long-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learn-
ing, Memory, and Cognition, 20, 1063–1087.
Angeli, S.J., Murray, E.A., & Mishkin, M.M. 1993. Hippocampectomized monkeys can
remember one place but not two. Neuropsychologia, 31, 1021–1030.
Anglin, J.M. 1977. Word, object, and conceptual development. New York: W.W. Norton.
Angulo Y Gonzales, A.W. 1932. The prenatal development of behavior in the albino rat.
Journal of Comparative Neurology, 55, 395–442.
Arnold, H.M., & Spear, N.E. 1993. Order and duration of stimuli are important determi-
nants of reactivation. Animal Learning and Behavior, 21, 391–398.
Atkinson, R.C., & Joula, J.F. 1973. Factors influencing speed and accuracy of word
recognition. In S. Kornblum (Ed.), Attention and performance (Vol. 4, pp. 583–612).
New York, NY: Academic Press.
Babkin, P.S. 1960. The establishment of reflex activity in early postnatal life. Fiziologii
Zhurnal USSR, 44(10), 922–927. [The establishment of reflex activity in early postnatal
life. In The central nervous system and behavior, pp. 24–41, trans. by the Russian
Scientific Translation Program, National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, MD.]
Bachevalier, J. 1990. Ontogenetic development of habit and memory formation in primates.
In A. Diamond (Ed.), The development and neural bases of higher cognitive functions
(Vol. 608, pp. 457–477), Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. New York: New
York Academy of Sciences.
Bachevalier, J. 1992. Cortical versus limbic immaturity: Relationship to infantile amnesia.
In M.R. Gunnar & C.A. Nelson (Eds.), Developmental behavioral neuroscience (pp.
129–153). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Bachevalier, J., Beauregard, M., & Alvarado, M.C. 1999. Long-term effects of neonatal
damage to the hippocampal formation and amygdaloid complex on object discrimina-
tion and object recognition in rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). Behavioral Neuro-
science, 113, 1127–1151.
Bachevalier, J., Brickson, M., & Hagger, C. 1993. Limbic-dependent recognition memory
in monkeys develops early in infancy. NeuroReport, 4, 77–80.
Bachevalier, J., Brickson, M., Hagger, C., & Mishkin, M.M. 1990. Age and sex differences
in the effects of selective temporal lobe lesions on the formation of visual discrimination
habits in rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). Behavioral Neuroscience, 104, 885–899.
Bachevalier, J., Malkova, L., & Beauregard, M. 1996. Multiple memory systems: A
neuropsychological and developmental perspective. In L.G. Reid & N.E. Krasnegor
(Eds.), Attention, memory, and executive function (pp. 185–198). Baltimore, MD: Paul
H. Brookes Publishing Co.
Bachevalier, J., & Mishkin, M.M. 1984. An early and a late developing system for learning
and retention in infant monkeys. Behavioral Neuroscience, 98, 770–778.
Bachevalier, J., & Mishkin, M.M. 1994. Effects of selective neonatal temporal lobe lesions
on visual recognition memory in rhesus monkeys. Journal of Neuroscience, 14, 2128–
2139.
Bachevalier, J., Ungerleider, L.G., O’Neill, J.B., & Friedman, D.P. 1986. Regional distribu-
tion of [3H]naloxone binding in the brain of a newborn rhesus monkey. Developmental
Brain Research, 25, 302–308.
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
REFERENCES 255
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
256 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
Bauer, P.J., & Shore, C.M. 1987. Making a memorable event: Effects of familiarity and
organization on young children’s recall of action sequences. Cognitive Development, 2,
327–338.
Bechara, A., Tranel, D., Damasio, H., Adolphs, R., Rockland, C., & Damasio, A.R. 1995.
Double dissociation of conditioning and declarative knowledge relative to the amygdala
and hippocampus in humans. Science, 269, 1115–1118.
Beck, A.T., Ward, C.H., Mendelson, M., Mock, J., & Erbaugh, J. 1961. An inventory for
measuring depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 4, 561–571.
Bentin, S., & Moscovitch, M. 1988. The time course of repetition effects for words and
unfamiliar faces. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 117, 148–160.
Berntson, C.G., Tuber, D.S., Ronca, A.E., & Bachman, D.S. 1983. The decerebrate human:
Associative learning. Experimental Neurology, 81, 77–88.
Besson, M., Fischler, I., Boaz, T., & Raney, G. 1992. Effects of automatic associative
activation on explicit and implicit memory tests. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18, 89–105.
Best, C.T. 1995. Learning to perceive the sound pattern of English. In C. Rovee-Collier &
L.P. Lipsitt (Eds.), Advances in infancy research (Vol. 9, pp. 217–304). Norwood, NJ:
Ablex Publishing Corporation.
Bhatt, R.S. 1997, April. Determinants of memory modification in infancy: The strength of
the interfering experience. Paper presented at the meeting of the Society for Research in
Child Development, Washington, D.C.
Bhatt, R.S., & Rovee-Collier, C. 1994. Perception and 24-hour retention of feature relations
in infancy. Developmental Psychology, 30, 142–150.
Bhatt, R.S., & Rovee-Collier, C. 1996. Infants’ forgetting of correlated attributes and object
recognition. Child Development, 67, 172–187.
Bhatt, R.S., & Rovee-Collier, C. 1997. Dissociation between features and feature relations
in infant memory: Effect of memory load. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,
67, 69–89.
Bhatt, R.S., Rovee-Collier, C., & Weiner, S. 1994. Developmental changes in the interface
between perception and memory retrieval. Developmental Psychology, 30, 151–162.
Biederman, I., & Cooper, E.E. 1991. Evidence for complete translational and reflectional
invariance in visual object priming. Perception, 20, 585–593.
Biederman, I., & Cooper, E.E. 1992. Size invariance in visual object priming. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 121–133.
Bjork, R.A. 1975. Retrieval as a memory modifier: An interpretation of negative recency
and related phenomena. In R.L. Solso (Ed.), Information processing and cognition: The
Loyola symposium (pp. 123–144). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Bjorklund, D.F. 2000. Children’s thinking: Developmental function and individual differ-
ences. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.
Blaney, P.H. 1986. Affect and memory: A review. Psychological Bulletin, 99, 229–246.
Blaxton, T.A. 1989. Investigating dissociations among memory measures: Support for a
transfer-appropriate processing framework. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15, 657–668.
Bolhuis, J.J. Stewart, C.A., & Forrest, E.M. 1994. Retrograde amnesia and memory
reactivation in rats with ibotenate lesions to the hippocampus and subiculum. Quarterly
Journal of Experimental Psychology, 47B, 129–150.
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
REFERENCES 257
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
258 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
Campbell, B.A., & Coulter, X. 1976. Neural and psychological processes underlying the
development of learning and memory. In T.J. Tighe & R.N. Leaton (Eds.), Habituation
(pp. 129–157). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Campbell, B.A., Krauter, E.E., & Wallace, J.E. 1980. Animal models of aging: Sensory-
motor and cognitive function in the aged rat. In D.G. Stein (Ed.), The psychobiology of
aging: Problems and perspectives (pp. 201–226). North Holland: Elsevier Science
Publishers.
Campbell, B.A., Misanin, J.R., White, B.C., & Lytle, L.D. 1974. Species differences in
ontogeny of memory: Indirect support for neural maturation as a determinant of
forgetting. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 87, 193–202.
Campbell, B.A., Sananes, C.B., & Gaddy, J.R. 1984. Animal models of infantile amnesia,
benign senescent forgetfulness, and senile dementia. Neurobehavioral Toxicology and
Teratology, 6, 467–471.
Campbell, B.A., Sananes, C.B., & Gaddy, J.R. 1985. Animal models of Jacksonian dissolu-
tion of memory in the aged. In J. Traber & W.H. Gispen (Eds.), Senile dementia of the
Alzheimer type (pp. 283–291). Berlin: Springer-Verlag.
Campbell, B.A., & Spear, N.E. 1972. Ontogeny of memory. Psychological Review, 79,
215–236.
Carroll, M., Byrne, B., & Kirsner, K. 1985. Autobiographical memory and perceptual
learning: A developmental study using picture recognition, naming latency, and percep-
tual identification. Memory & Cognition, 13, 273–279.
Cave, C.B. 1997. Very long-lasting priming in picture naming. Psychological Science, 8,
322–325.
Cave, C.B., Blake, R., & McNamara, T.P. 1998. Binocular rivalry disrupts visual priming.
Psychological Science, 9, 299–302.
Cave, C.B., Bost, P.R., & Cobb, R.I. 1996. Effects of color and pattern on implicit and
explicit picture memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and
Cognition, 22, 639–653.
Cave, C.B., & Squire, L.R. 1992. Intact and long-lasting repetition priming in amnesia.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18, 509–520.
Cermak, L.S. 1984. The episodic-semantic distinction in amnesia. In L.S. Squire & N.
Butters (Eds.), Neuropsychology of memory (pp. 55–62). New York: Guilford Press.
Chalfonte, B.L., & Johnson, M.K. 1996. Feature memory and binding in young and old
adults. Memory & Cognition, 24, 403–416.
Chalfonte, B.L., Verfaellie, M., Johnson, M.K., & Reiss, L. 1996. Spatial location memory
in amnesia: Binding item and location information under incidental and intentional
encoding conditions. Memory, 4, 591–614.
Cho, Y.H., Kesner, R.P., & Brodale, S. 1995. Retrograde and anterograde amnesia for
spatial discrimination in rats: Role of hippocampus, entorhinal cortex, and parietal
cortex. Psychobiology, 23, 185–194.
Clark, H.H., & Carlson, T.B. 1981. Context for comprehension. In J. Long & A. Baddeley
(Eds.), Attention and performance IX (pp. 313–330). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates.
Clayton, K., Habibi, A., & Bendele, M.S. 1995. Recognition priming effects following
serial learning: Implications for episodic priming effects. American Journal of Psychol-
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
REFERENCES 259
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
260 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
Cooper, L.A., Schacter, D.L., Ballesteros, S., & Moore, C. 1992. Priming and recognition
of transformed three-dimensional objects: Effects of size and reflection. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18, 43–57.
Cooper, L.A., Schacter, D.L., & Moore, C. 1991, November. Orientation affects both
structural and episodic representations of three-dimensional objects. Paper presented at
the meeting of the Psychonomic Society, San Francisco, CA.
Cooper, R.L., McNamara, M.C., Thompson, W.C., & Marsh, G.R. 1980. Vasopressin
modulation of learning and memory in the rat. In L. Poon (Ed.), Aging in the 1980’s:
Psychological issues (pp. 201–211). Washington, D.C.: American Psychological Asso-
ciation.
Corkin, S. 1968. Acquisition of motor skill after bilateral medial temporal-lobe excision.
Neuropsychologia, 6, 255–265.
Corkin, S. 1984. Lasting consequences of bilateral medial temporal lobectomy: Clinical
course and experimental findings in H.M. Seminars in Neurology, 4, 249–259.
Cornell, E.H. 1974. Infants’ discrimination of photographs of faces following redundant
presentations. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 18, 98–106.
Cornell, E.H. 1979. Infants’ recognition memory, forgetting, and savings. Journal of
Experimental Child Psychology, 28, 359–374.
Cornell, E.H. 1980. Distributed study facilitates infants’ delayed recognition memory.
Memory & Cognition, 8, 539–542.
Cornell, E.H. 1984. Developmental continuity of memory mechanisms: Suggestive phe-
nomena. In R. Kail, Jr., & N.E. Spear (Eds.), Comparative perspectives on the develop-
ment of memory (pp. 287–316). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Cornell, E.H., & Bergstrom, L.I. 1983. Serial-position effects in infants’ recognition
memory. Memory & Cognition, 11, 494–499.
Correll, R.E., & Scoville, W.B. 1965. Performance on delayed match following lesions of
medial temporal lobe structures. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychol-
ogy, 60, 360–367.
Coulter, X. 1979. The determinants of infantile amnesia. In N.E. Spear & B.A. Campbell
(Eds.), Ontogeny of learning and memory (pp. 245–270). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates.
Craik, F.I.M. 1983. On the transfer of information from temporary to permanent memory.
Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, 302B, 341–359.
Craik, F.I.M., & Lockhart, R.S. 1972. Levels of processing: A framework for memory
research. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11, 671–684.
Craik, F.I.M., & Tulving, E. 1975. Depth of processing and the retention of words in
episodic memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 104, 268–294.
Crowder, R.G. 1976. Principles of learning and memory. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates.
Crowder, R.G. 1988. Modularity and dissociations in memory systems. In H.L. Roediger,
III, & F.I.M. Craik (Eds.), Varieties of memory and consciousness: Essays in honour of
Endel Tulving (pp. 271–294). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Cuddy, L.J., & Jacoby, L.L. 1982. When forgetting helps memory: An analysis of repetition
effects. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 21, 451–467.
Cummings, J.L., Tomiyasu, U., Read, S., Benson, D.F. 1984. Amnesia with hippocampal
lesions after cardiopulmonary arrest. Neurology, 34, 679–681.
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
REFERENCES 261
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
262 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
Diamond, A., Cruttenden, L, & Niederman, D. 1989. Why have studies found better
performance with multiple wells than with only two wells on AB? Society for Research
in Child Development Abstracts, 6, 227.
Diamond, A., & Doar, B. 1989. The performance of human infants on a measure of frontal
cortex function, the delayed response task. Developmental Psychobiology, 22, 271–294.
Diamond, A., Towle, C., & Boyer, K. 1994. Young children’s performance on a task
sensitive to the memory functions of the medial temporal lobe in adults: The delayed
nonmatching-to-sample task reveals problems that are due to non-memory-related task
demands. Behavioral Neuroscience, 108, 659–680.
Dienes, Z., & Berry, D. 1997. Implicit learning: Below the subjective threshold. Psy-
chonomic Bulletin & Review, 4, 3–23.
DiGiulio, D.V., Seidenberg, M., O’Leary, D.S., & Raz, N. 1994. Procedural and declarative
memory: A developmental study. Brain and Cognition, 25, 79–91.
Dominowski, R.L., & Ekstrand, B.R. 1967. Direct and associative priming in anagram
solving. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 74, 84–86.
Dorfman, J., Kihlstrom, J.F., Cork, R.C., & Misiaszek, J. 1995. Priming and recognition in
ECT-induced amnesia. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2, 244–248.
Dritschel, B.H., Williams, J.M.G., Baddeley, A.D., & Nimmo-Smith, I. 1992. Autobio-
graphical fluency: A method for the study of personal memory. Memory & Cognition,
20, 133–140.
Eacott, M.J. 1999. Memory for the events of early childhood. Current Directions in
Psychological Science, 8, 46–49.
Earley, L.A., Bhatt, R.S., & Rovee-Collier, C. 1995. Developmental changes in the
contextual control of recognition. Developmental Psychobiology, 28, 27–43.
Ebbinghaus, H. 1885/1964. Memory: A contribution to experimental psychology. (H.A.
Ruger & C.E. Bussenius, Trans.) New York: Dover.
Eich, E., Macaulay, D., & Ryan, L. 1994. Mood dependent memory for events of the
personal past. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 123, 201–215.
Eichenbaum, H., Otto, T., & Cohen, N.J. 1994. Two functional components of the hippoc-
ampal memory system. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 17, 449–518.
Engen, T., & Ross, B.M. 1973. Long-term memory of odors with and without verbal
descriptions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 100, 221–227.
Estes, W.K. 1997. Processes of memory loss, forgetting, and distortion. Psychological
Review, 104, 148–169.
Fagan, J.F., III. 1970. Memory in the infant. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 9,
217–226.
Fagan, J.F., III. 1973. Infants’ delayed recognition memory and forgetting. Journal of
Experimental Child Psychology, 16, 424–450.
Fagan, J.F., III. 1990. Discussion of J.M. Mandler, “Recall of events by preverbal chil-
dren.” In A. Diamond (Ed.), The development and neural bases of higher cognitive
functions (Vol. 608, pp. 503–516), Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. New
York: New York Academy of Sciences.
Fagen, J.W., Morrongiello, B.A., Rovee-Collier, C., & Gekoski, M.J. 1984. Expectancies
and memory retrieval in 3-month-old infants. Child Development, 55, 936–943.
Fagen, J.W., Ohr, P.S., Fleckenstein, L.K., & Ribner, D.R. 1985. The effect of crying on
long-term memory in infancy. Child Development, 56, 1584–1592.
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
REFERENCES 263
Fagen, J.W., Ohr, P.S., Singer, J.M., & Klein, S.J. 1989. Crying and retrograde amnesia in
young infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 12, 13–24.
Fagen, J.W., & Prigot, J.A. 1993. Negative affect and infant memory. In C. Rovee Collier &
L.P. Lipsitt (Eds.), Advances in infancy research (Vol. 8, pp. 169–216). Norwood, NJ:
Ablex Publishing Corporation.
Fagen, J.W., & Rovee-Collier, C. 1983. Memory retrieval: A time-locked process in
infancy. Science, 222, 1349–1351.
Fagen, J.W., Yengo, L.A., Rovee-Collier, C., & Enright, M.K. 1981. Reactivation of a
visual discrimination in early infancy. Developmental Psychology, 17, 266–274.
Fantz, R.L. 1956. A method for studying early visual development. Perceptual and Motor
Skills, 6, 13–15.
Field, T.M., Cohen, D., Garcia, R., & Greenberg, R. 1984. Mother-stranger face discrimina-
tion by the newborn. Infant Behavior and Development, 7, 19–25.
Feigenbaum, J.D., Polkey, C.E., & Morris, R.G. 1996. Deficits in spatial working memory
after unilateral temporal lobectomy in man. Neuropsychologia, 34, 163–176.
Fivush, R., Kuebli, J., & Clubb, P.A. 1992. The structure of events and event representa-
tion: A developmental analysis. Child Development, 63, 188–201.
Flannagan, M.J., Fried, L.S., & Holyoak, K.J. 1986. Distributional expectations and the
induction of category structure. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory,
and Cognition, 12, 241–256.
Fox, N.J., Kagan, J., & Weiskopf, S. 1979. The growth of memory during infancy. Genetic
Psychology Monographs, 99, 91–130.
Freud, S. 1935. A general introduction to psychoanalysis. New York: Clarion Books.
Fried, L.S., & Holyoak, K.J. 1984. Induction of category distributions: A framework for
classification learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and
Cognition, 10, 234–257.
Gabrieli, J.D.E., Brewer, J.B., & Poldrack, R.A. 1998. Images of medial temporal lobe
functions in human learning and memory. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 70,
275–283.
Gabrieli, J.D.E., Cohen, N.J., & Corkin, S. 1988. The impaired learning of semantic
knowledge following bilateral medial temporal-lobe resection. Brain and Cognition, 7,
157–177.
Gabrieli, J.D.E., Fleischman, D.A., Keane, M.M., Reminger, S.L., & Morrell, F. 1995.
Double dissociation between memory systems underlying explicit and implicit memory
in the human brain. Psychological Science, 6, 76–82.
Gage, F.J., Dunnett, S.B., & Bjorklund, A. 1984. Spatial learning and motor deficits in aged
rats. Neurobiology of Aging, 5, 43–48.
Gardiner, J.M., & Java, R.I. 1993. Recognising and remembering. In A.F. Collins, S.E.
Gathercole, M.A. Conway, & P.E. Morris (Eds.), Theories of memory (pp. 163–188).
Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Gazzaniga, M.S. 1972. One brain — two minds? American Scientist, 60, 311–317.
Gekoski, M.J., Fagen, J.W., & Pearlman, M.A. 1984. Early learning and memory in the
preterm infant. Infant Behavior and Development, 7, 267–276.
Gerhardstein, P., Adler, S.A., & Rovee-Collier, C. 2000. A dissociation in infants’ memory
for stimulus size: Evidence for the early development of multiple memory systems.
Developmental Psychobiology, 36, 123–135.
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
264 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
Gershberg, F.B., & Shimamura, A.P. 1994. Serial position effects in implicit and explicit
tests of memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cogni-
tion, 20, 1370–1378.
Gibson, E.J. 1969. Principles of perceptual learning and development. New York:
Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Gold, P.E., McGaugh, J.L., Hankins, L.L., Rose, R.P., & Vasquez, B.J. 1981. Age-
dependent changes in retention in rats. Experimental Aging Research, 8, 53–58.
Gordon, W.C. 1979. Age: Is it a constraint on memory content? In N.E. Spear & B.A.
Campbell (Eds.), Ontogeny of learning and memory (pp. 271–287). Hillsdale, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Gordon, W.C. 1981. Mechanisms of cue-induced retention enhancement. In N.E. Spear &
R.R. Miller (Eds.), Information processing in animals: Memory mechanisms (pp. 319–
339). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Gordon, W.C., Smith, G.J., & Katz, D.S. 1979. Dual effects of response blocking following
avoidance learning. Behavior Research and Therapy, 17, 479–487.
Gottlieb, G. 1971. Ontogenesis of sensory function in birds and mammals. In E. Tobach,
L.R. Aronson, & E. Shaw (Eds.), The biopsychology of development (pp. 67–128). New
York: Academic Press.
Goubet, N., & Clifton, R.K. 1998. Object and event representation in 61/2-month-old
infants. Developmental Psychology, 34, 63–76.
Gould, S.J. 1998, April. Ontogeny and phylogeny revisited, or why the child is not the
evolutionary father to the man. Address presented at the International Conference on
Infant Studies, Atlanta, GA.
Graf, P. 1988, November. Implicit and explicit remembering in same and different environ-
ments. Paper presented at the meeting of the Psychonomic Society, Chicago, IL.
Graf, P. 1990. Life-span changes in implicit and explicit memory. Bulletin of the Psy-
chonomic Society, 28, 353–358.
Graf, P., & Mandler, G. 1984. Activation makes words more accessible, but not necessarily
more retrievable. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 23, 553–568.
Graf, P., Mandler, G., & Haden, P. 1982. Simulating amnesic symptoms in normal subjects.
Science, 218, 1243–1244.
Graf, P., & Schacter, D.L. 1985. Implicit and explicit memory for new associations in
normal and amnesic patients. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory,
and Cognition, 11, 501–518.
Graf, P., & Schacter, D.L. 1987. Selective effects of interference on implicit and explicit
memory for new associations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory,
and Cognition, 13, 45–53.
Graf, P., Squire, L.R., & Mandler, G. 1984. The information that amnesic patients do not
forget. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 10,
164–178.
Gratch, G., & Landers, W.F. 1971. Stage IV of Piaget’s theory of infants’ object concepts:
A longitudinal study. Child Development, 42, 359–372.
Greco, C., & Daehler, M.W. 1985. Immediate and long-term retention of basic-level
categories in 24-month-olds. Infant Behavior and Development, 8, 459–474.
Greco, C., Hayne, H., & Rovee-Collier, C. 1990. The roles of function, reminding, and
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
REFERENCES 265
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
266 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
REFERENCES 267
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
268 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
REFERENCES 269
science, 4, 268–280.
Johnson, M.K., & Chalfonte, B.L. 1994. Binding complex memories: The role of reactiva-
tion and the hippocampus. In D.L. Schacter & E. Tulving (Eds.), Memory systems 1994
(pp. 311–350). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Johnson, M.K., & Hasher, L. 1987. Human learning and memory. Annual Review of
Psychology, 38, 631–668.
Johnson, M.K., & Hirst, W. 1991. Processing subsystems of memory. In R.G. Lister & H.J.
Weingartner (Eds.), Perspectives on cognitive neuroscience (pp. 197–217). New York:
Oxford University Press.
Johnson, M.K., & Hirst, W. 1993. MEM: Memory subsystems as processes. In A.F.
Collins, S.E. Gathercole, M.A. Conway, & P.E. Morris (Eds.), Theories of memory (pp.
241–286). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Johnson, M.K., Kounios, J., & Nolde, S.F. 1996. Electrophysiological brain activity and
memory source monitoring. NeuroReport, 7, 2929–2932.
Johnson, M.K., & Raye, C.L. 1981. Reality monitoring. Psychological Review, 88, 67–85.
Johnson, M.K., & Raye, C.L. 1998. False memories and confabulation. Trends in Cognitive
Sciences, 2, 137–145.
Jolicoeur, P. 1987. A size-congruency effect in memory for visual shape. Memory &
Cognition, 15, 531–543.
Jones, H.E. 1930. The retention of conditioned emotional reactions in infancy. Journal of
Genetic Psychology, 37, 485–498.
Jones, E.G., & Powell, T.P.S. 1970. An anatomical study of converging sensory pathways
within the cerebral cortex of the monkey. Brain, 93, 793–820.
Kagan, J. 1984. The nature of the child. New York: Basic Books.
Kagan, J., & Hamburg, M. 1981. The enhancement of memory in the first year. Journal of
Genetic Psychology, 138, 3–14.
Kandel, E.R., & Hawkins, R.D. 1992. The biological basis of learning and individuality.
Scientific American, 267(3), 79–86.
Kantrow, R.W. 1937. An investigation of conditioned feeding responses and concomitant
adaptive behavior in young infants. University of Iowa Studies in Child Welfare, 13, 3.
Kaplan, M.G. 1967. Infant visual preferences: The role of familiarity and responsiveness.
Unpublished master’s thesis, University of Illinois, Champaign, IL.
Karmiloff-Smith, A. 1998. Is atypical development necessarily a window on the normal
mind/brain? The case of William’s syndrome. Developmental Science, 1, 273–277.
Kesner, R.P. 1980. An attribute analysis of memory: The role of the hippocampus.
Physiological Psychology, 8, 189–197.
Kihlstrom, J.F., Schacter, D.L., Cork, R.C., Hurt, C.A., & Behr, S.E. 1990. Implicit and
explicit memory following surgical anesthesia. Psychological Science, 1, 303–306.
Kim, J.J., & Fanselow, M.S. 1992. Modality specific retrograde amnesia of fear. Science,
256, 675–677.
Kimble, G. (Ed.) 1961. Hilgard & Marquis’ conditioning and learning. New York:
Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Kinsbourne, M., & Wood, F. 1982. Theoretical considerations regarding the episodic-
semantic distinction. In L. Cermak (Ed.), Human memory and amnesia (pp. 195–218).
Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
270 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
REFERENCES 271
Logan, G.D. 1992. Repetition priming and automaticity: Common underlying mecha-
nisms? Cognitive Psychology, 22, 1–35.
Logan, G.D., & Klapp, S.T. 1991. Automatizing alphabet arithmetic: I. Is extended practice
necessary to produce automaticity? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning,
Memory, and Cognition, 17, 179–195.
Macaulay, D., Ryan, L., & Eich, E. 1993. Mood dependence in implicit and explicit
memory. In P. Graf & M.E.J. Masson (Eds.), Implicit memory: New directions in
cognition, development, and neuropsychology (pp. 75–94). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates.
Madigan, S. 1983. Picture memory. In J.C. Yuille (Ed.), Imagery, memory, and cognition:
Essays in honor of Allan Paivio (pp. 65–69). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associ-
ates.
Malamut, B.L., Saunders, R.C., & Mishkin, M.M. 1984. Monkeys with combined amygdalo-
hippocampal lesions succeed in object discrimination learning despite 24-hour intertrial
intervals. Behavioral Neuroscience, 98, 759–769.
Malkova, L., Bachevalier, J., Webster, M., & Mishkin, M.M. in press. Effects of neonatal
inferior prefrontal and medial temporal lesions on learning the rule for delayed nonmatch-
ing-to-sample. Developmental Neuropsychology.
Malkova, L., Mishkin, M.M., & Bachevalier, J. 1995. Long-term effects of selective
neonatal temporal lobe lesions on learning and memory in monkeys. Behavioral Neuro-
science, 109, 212–226.
Mandler, G. 1980. Recognizing: The judgment of previous occurrence. Psychological
Review, 87, 252–271.
Mandler, G. 1985. Cognitive psychology: An essay in cognitive science. Hillsdale, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Mandler, G. 1989. Memory: Conscious and unconscious. In P.R.Solomon, G.R. Goethals,
C.M. Kelley, & R.B. Stephens (Eds.), Perspectives on memory (pp. 84–106). New
York: Springer-Verlag.
Mandler, G., Nakamura, Y., & Van Zandt, B.J.S. 1987. Nonspecific effects of exposure on
stimuli that cannot be recognized. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning,
Memory, and Cognition, 13, 646–648.
Mandler, J.M. 1984. Representation and recall in infancy. In M. Moscovitch (Ed.), Ad-
vances in the study of communication and affect. Vol. 9: Infant memory (pp. 75–101).
New York: Plenum Publishing Corporation.
Mandler, J.M. 1986. The development of event memory. In F. Klix & H. Hagendorf (Eds.),
Human memory and cognitive capabilities: Mechanisms and performance (pp. 459–
467). New York: Elsevier Science Publishers.
Mandler, J.M. 1988. How to build a baby: On the development of an accessible representa-
tional system. Cognitive Development, 3, 113–136.
Mandler, J.M. 1990. Recall of events by preverbal children. In A. Diamond (Ed.), The
development and neural bases of higher cognitive functions (Vol. 608, pp. 485–503),
Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. New York: New York Academy of
Sciences.
Mandler, J.M. 1998. Representation. In W. Damon (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology:
Cognition, perception, & language (Vol. 2, pp. 255–307). New York: John Wiley &
Sons.
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
272 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
Marrott, H., Barr, R., & Rovee-Collier, C. 2000, March. Sensory preconditioning facilitates
deferred imitation by 6-month-olds. Paper presented at the meeting of the Eastern
Psychological Association, Baltimore, MD.
Martone, M., Butters, N., Payne, M., Becker, J. T., & Sax, D. S. 1984. Dissociations
between skill learning and verbal recognition in amnesia and dementia. Archives of
Neurology, 41, 965–970.
Matzel, L.D., Collin, C., & Alkon, D.L. 1992. Biophysical and behavioral correlates of
memory storage: Degradation and reactivation. Behavioral Neuroscience, 106, 954–
963.
McCall, R.B., Kennedy, C.B., & Dodds, C. 1977. The interfering effect of distracting
stimuli on the infant’s memory. Child Development, 48, 79–87.
McCall, R.B., Parke, R.D., & Kavanaugh, R.D. 1977. Imitation of live and televised models
by children one to three years of age. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child
Development, 42 (5, Serial No. 173).
McDonald, R.J., & White, N. M. 1993. A triple dissociation of memory systems: Hippo-
campus, amygdala, and dorsal striatum. Behavioral Neuroscience, 107, 3–22.
McDonough, L., Mandler, J.M., McKee, R.D., & Squire, L.R. 1995. The deferred imitation
task as a nonverbal measure of declarative memory. Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences (USA), 92, 7580–7584.
McDougall, W. 1923. Outline of psychology. New York: Scribner.
McFarland, D.J. 1977. Decision making in animals. Nature, 269, 15–21.
McKee, R.D., & Squire, L.R. 1993. On the development of declarative memory. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19, 397–404.
McKenzie, W.A., & Humphries, M.S. 1991. Recency effects in direct and indirect memory
tasks. Memory & Cognition, 19, 321–331.
McKoon, G., Ratcliff, R., & Dell, G.S. 1986. A critical evaluation of the semantic-episodic
distinction. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition,
12, 295–306.
Meltzoff, A.N. 1988a. Infant imitation and memory: Nine-month-olds in immediate and
deferred tests. Child Development, 59, 217–225.
Meltzoff, A.N. 1988b. Imitation of televised models by infants. Child Development, 59,
1221–1229.
Meltzoff, A.N. 1990. Towards a developmental cognitive science: The implications of
cross-modal matching and imitation for the development of representation and memory
in infancy. In A. Diamond (Ed.), The development and neural bases of higher cognitive
functions (Vol. 608, pp. 1–37), Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. New
York: New York Academy of Sciences.
Meltzoff, A.N. 1995a. What infant memory tells us about infantile amnesia: Long-term
recall and deferred imitation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 59, 497–515.
Meltzoff, A.N. 1995b. Understanding the intentions of others: Re-enactment of intended
acts by 18-month-old children. Developmental Psychology, 31, 838–850.
Meltzoff, A.N., & Moore, M.K. 1994. Imitation, memory, and the representation of
persons. Infant Behavior and Development, 17, 83–99.
Merriman, J., & Rovee-Collier, C. 1994, June. Developmental changes in infants’ sensitiv-
ity to temporal order. Paper presented at the International Conference on Infant Studies,
Paris, France.
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
REFERENCES 273
Merriman, J., Rovee-Collier, C., & Wilk, A. 1997. Exemplar spacing and infants’ memory
for category information. Infant Behavior and Development, 20, 219–232.
Meulemans, T., Van der Linden, M., & Perruchet, P. 1998. Implicit sequence learning in
children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 69, 199–221.
Milliken, B., & Jolicoeur, P. 1992. Size effects in visual recognition memory are deter-
mined by perceived size. Memory & Cognition, 20, 83–95.
Milner, B. 1962. Les troubles de la memoire accompagnant des lesions hippocampiques
bilaterales. In P. Passouant (Ed.), Physiologie de l’hippocampe (pp. 257–272). Paris:
Editions du Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique.
Milner, B. 1965. Memory disturbance after bilateral hippocampal lesions. In P.M. Milner &
S.E. Glickman (Eds.), Cognitive processes and the brain (pp. 97–111). Princeton, NJ:
D. Van Nostrand Company, Inc.
Milner, B. 1972. Disorders of learning and memory after temporal lobe lesions in man.
Clinical Neurosurgery, 19, 421–446.
Milner, B., Corkin, S., & Teuber, H.L. 1968. Further analysis of the hippocampal amnesic
syndrome: Fourteen year follow-up study of H.M. Neuropsychologia, 6, 215–234.
Milner, B., Squire, L.R., & Kandel, E.R. 1998. Cognitive neuroscience and the study of
memory. Neuron, 20, 445–468.
Mishkin, M.M. 1978. Memory in monkeys is severely impaired by combined but not by
separate removal of amygdala and hippocampus. Nature, 273, 297–298.
Mishkin, M.M. 1982. A memory system in the monkey. Philosophical Transactions of the
Royal Society of London, 298B, 85–92.
Mishkin, M.M., & Delacour, J. 1975. An analysis of short-term visual memory in the
monkey. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 1, 326–
334.
Mishkin, M.M., Malamut, B.L., & Bachevalier, J. 1984. Memories and habits: Two neural
systems. In G. Lynch, J.L. McGaugh, & N.M. Weinberger (Eds.), Neurobiology of
learning and memory (pp. 65–77). New York: Guilford Press.
Mishkin, M.M., & Petri, H.L. 1984. Memories and habits: Some implications for the
analysis of learning and retention. In L.R. Squire & N. Butters (Eds.), Neuropsychology
of memory (pp. 287–296). New York: Guilford Press.
Mishkin, M.M, & Phillips, R.R. 1990. A corticolimbic memory path revealed through its
disconnection. In C.B. Trevarthen (Ed.), Brain circuits and functions of the mind:
Essays in honor of Roger W. Sperry (pp. 196–210). New York, NY: Cambridge
University Press.
Mitchell, D.B. 1993. Implicit and explicit memory for pictures: Multiple views across the
lifespan. In P. Graf & M.E.J. Masson (Eds.), Implicit memory: New directions in
cognition, development, and neuropsychology (pp. 171–190). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates.
Mitchell, D.B., & Brown, A.S. 1988. Persistent repetition priming in picture naming and its
dissociation from recognition memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning,
Memory, and Cognition, 14, 213–222.
Mitchell, D.B., Brown, A.S., & Murphy, D.R. 1990. Dissociations between procedural and
episodic memory: Effects of time and aging. Psychology & Aging, 5, 264–276.
Morris, R. 1984. Development of a water-maze procedure for studying spatial learning in
the rat. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 11, 47–60.
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
274 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
Morris, R.G.M. 1981. Spatial localization does not require the presence of local cues.
Learning and Motivation, 12, 239–260.
Morris, R.G.M., Garrud, P., Rawlins, J.N.P., & O’Keefe, J. 1982. Place navigation im-
paired in rats with hippocampal lesions. Nature, 297, 681–683.
Moscovitch, M. 1982. A neuropsychological approach to perception and memory in normal
and pathological aging. In F.I.M. Craik & S. Trehub (Eds.), Aging and cognitive
processes (pp. 55–78). New York: Plenum Publishing Corporation.
Moscovitch, M. 1985. Memory from infancy to old age. Implications for theories of normal
and pathological memory. In D.S. Olton, E. Gamzy, & S. Corkin (Eds.), Memory
dysfunctions: An integration of animal and human research from preclinical and
clinical perspectives (Vol 444, pp. 78–96). Annals of the New York Academy of
Sciences. New York: New York Academy of Sciences.
Moscovitch, M. 1994. Memory and working with memory: Evaluation of a component
process model and comparisons with other models. In D.L. Schacter & E. Tulving
(Eds.), Memory systems 1994 (pp. 269–310). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Mulligan, N.W. 1997. Attention and implicit memory tests: The effects of varying
attentional load on conceptual priming. Memory & Cognition, 25, 11–17.
Mulligan, N.W., & Hartman, M. 1996. Divided attention and indirect memory tests.
Memory & Cognition, 24, 453–465.
Mumby, D.G., & Pinel, J.P.J. 1994. Rhinal cortex lesions and object recognition in rats.
Behavioral Neuroscience, 108, 11–18.
Mumby, D.G., Wood, E.R., & Pinel, J.P.J. 1992. Object-recognition memory is only mildly
impaired in rats with lesions of the hippocampus and amygdala. Psychobiology, 20, 18–
27.
Murdock, B.B. 1962. The serial position effect of free recall. Journal of Experimental
Psychology, 64, 482–488.
Murdock, B.B. 1988. The past, the present, and the future: Comments on Section 1. In H.L.
Roediger, III., & F.I.M. Craik (Eds.), Varieties of memory and consciousness: Essays in
honour of Endel Tulving (pp. 93–98). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Murray, E.A. 1996. What have ablation studies told us about the neural substrates of
stimulus memory? Seminars in the Neurosciences, 8, 13–22.
Murray, E.A., & Mishkin, M.M. 1998. Object recognition and location memory in mon-
keys with excitotoxic lesions of the amygdala and hippocampus. Journal of Neuro-
science, 18, 6568–6582.
Musen, G. 1991. Effects of verbal labeling and exposure duration on implicit memory for
visual patterns. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cogni-
tion, 17, 954–962.
Musen, G., & Treisman, A. 1990. Implicit and explicit memory for visual patterns. Journal
of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16, 127–137.
Muzzio, I.A., & Rovee-Collier, C. 1996. Timing effects of postevent information on infant
retention. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 63, 212–238.
Nadel, L. 1990. Varieties of spatial cognition: Psychological considerations. In A. Diamond
(Ed.), The development and neural bases of higher cognitive functions (Vol. 608, pp.
613–626), Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. New York: New York
Academy of Sciences.
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
REFERENCES 275
Nadel, L. 1992. Multiple memory systems: What and why. Journal of Cognitive Neuro-
science, 4, 179–188.
Nadel, L. 1994. Multiple memory systems: What and why, an update. In D.L. Schacter & E.
Tulving (Eds.), Memory systems 1994 (pp. 39–63). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Nadel, L., & Zola-Morgan, S.M. 1984. Infantile amnesia: A neurobiological perspective. In
M. Moscovitch (Ed.), Advances in the study of communication and affect. Vol. 9: Infant
memory (pp. 145–172). New York: Plenum Publishing Corporation.
Nagy, Z.M. 1979. Development of learning and memory processes in infant mice. In N.E.
Spear & B.A. Campbell (Eds.), Ontogeny of learning and memory (pp. 101–133).
Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Nagy, Z.M., Misanin, J.R., Newman, J.A., Olsen, P.L., & Hinderliter, C.F. 1972. Ontogeny
of memory in the neonatal mouse. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychol-
ogy, 81, 380–393.
Naito, M. 1990. Repetition priming in children and adults: Age-related dissociation be-
tween implicit and explicit memory. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 50,
462–484.
Naito, M., & Komatsu, S.I. 1993. Processes involved in childhood development of implicit
memory. In P. Graf & M.E.J. Masson (Eds.), Implicit memory: New directions in
cognition, development, and neuropsychology (pp. 231–260). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates.
Neely, J.H. 1989. Experimental dissociations and the episodic/semantic memory distinc-
tion. In H.L. Roediger, III., & F.I.M. Craik (Eds.), Varieties of memory and conscious-
ness: Essays in honour of Endel Tulving (pp. 229–270). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates.
Neely, J.H., & Durgunoglu, A. 1985. Dissociative episodic and semantic priming effects in
episodic recognition and lexical decision tasks. Journal of Memory and Language, 24,
466–489.
Neill, W.T., Beck, J.L., Bottalico, K.S., & Molloy, R.D. 1990. Effects of intentional versus
incidental learning on explicit and implicit tests of memory. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16, 457–463.
Nelson, C.A. 1995. The ontogeny of human memory: A cognitive neuroscience perspec-
tive. Developmental Psychology, 31, 723–738.
Nelson, C.A. 1997. The neurobiological basis of early memory development. In N. Cowan
(Ed.), The development of memory in early childhood (pp. 41–82). Hove East Sussex,
UK: Psychology Press.
Nelson, C.A. 1998. The nature of early memory. Preventive Medicine, 27, 172–179.
Nelson, D.L., Keelean, M.J., & Negrao, M. 1989. Word-fragment cuing: The lexical search
hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition,
15, 388–397.
Nelson, D.L., LaLomia, P.D., & Canas, J.J. 1991. Dissociative effects in different prime
domains. Memory & Cognition, 19, 44–62.
Nelson, K. 1984. The transition from infant to child memory. In M. Moscovitch (Ed.),
Advances in the study of communication and affect. Vol. 9: Infant memory (pp. 103–
130). New York: Plenum Publishing Corporation.
Nelson, K. 1990. Remembering, forgetting, and childhood amnesia. In R. Fivush & J.A.
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
276 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
Hudson (Eds.), Knowing and remembering in young children (pp. 301–316). Cam-
bridge: Cambridge University Press.
Nelson, K., & Brown, A.L. 1978. The semantic-episodic distinction in memory develop-
ment. In P. Ornstein (Ed.), Memory development in children (pp. 233–241). Hillsdale,
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Nelson, K., & Kosslyn, S. 1975. Semantic retrieval in children and adults. Developmental
Psychology, 11, 807–813.
Nissen, M.J., & Bullemer, P.T. 1987. Attentional requirements of learning: Evidence from
performance measures. Cognitive Psychology, 19, 1–32.
Nissen, M.J., Knopman, D.S., & Schacter, D.L. 1987. Neurochemical dissociation of
memory systems. Neurology, 37, 789–794.
Nissen, M.J., Willingham, D., & Hartman, M. 1989. Explicit and implicit remembering:
When is learning preserved in amnesia? Neuropsychologia, 27, 341–352.
Nolde, S.F., Johnson, M.K., & D’Esposito, M. 1998. Left prefrontal activation during
episodic remembering: An event-related fMRI study. Neuroreport, 9, 3509-3514.
Nolde, S.F., Johnson, M.K., & Raye, C.L. 1998. The role of prefrontal cortex during tests of
episodic memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2, 399–405.
Nyberg, L. 1994. A structural equation modeling approach to the multiple memory systems
question. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20,
485–491.
Oakley, D.A. 1983. The varieties of memory: A phylogenetic approach. In A. Mayes (Ed.),
Memory in animals and humans: Some comparisons and their theoretical implications
(pp. 20–82). Cambridge, UK: Van Nostrand Reinhold.
Ogilvie, J.C., Tulving, E., Paskowitcz, S., & Jones, G.V. 1980. Three-dimensional memory
traces: A model and its application to forgetting. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal
Behavior, 19, 405–415.
Ohr, P., Fagen, J.W., Rovee-Collier, C., Hayne, H., & Vander Linde, E. 1989. Amount of
training and retention by infants. Developmental Psychobiology, 22, 69–80.
O’Keefe, J., & Nadel, L. 1978. The hippocampus as a cognitive map. Oxford: Clarendon
Press.
Olson, G.M., & Strauss, M.S. 1984. The development of infant memory. In M. Moscovitch
(Ed.), Advances in the study of communication and affect. Vol. 9: Infant memory (pp.
29–48). New York: Plenum Publishing Corporation.
Olton, D.S. 1989. Inferring psychological dissociations from experimental dissociations:
The temporal context of episodic memory. In H.L. Roediger, III., & F.I.M. Craik (Eds.),
Varieties of memory and consciousness: Essays in honour of Endel Tulving (pp. 161–
177). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Olton, D.S., Becker, J.T., & Handelmann, G.E. 1979. Hippocampus, space, and memory.
Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 2, 313–365.
Olton, D.S., & Samuelson, R.J. 1976. Remembrance of places passed: Spatial memory in
rats. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 2, 97–116.
O’Neill, J.B., Friedman, J., Bachevalier, J., & Ungerleider, L.G. 1986. Distribution of
muscarinic cholinergic receptors in the brain of a newborn monkey. Society for Neuro-
science Abstracts, 12, 809.
Orbach, J., Milner, B., & Rasmussen, T. 1960. Learning and retention in monkeys after
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
REFERENCES 277
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
278 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
Paulson, G., & Gotlieb, G. 1968. Development of reflexes: The reappearance of foetal and
neonatal reflexes in aged patients. Brain, 91, 37–52.
Pavlov, I.P. 1927. Conditioned reflexes. (Translated by G.V. Anrep.) London: Oxford
University Press.
Perris, E.E., Myers, N.A., & Clifton, R.K. 1990. Long-term memory for a single infancy
experience. Child Development, 61, 1796–1807.
Perruchet, P. 1989. The effect of spaced practice on explicit and implicit memory. British
Journal of Psychology, 80, 113–130.
Perruchet, P., & Baveux, P. 1989. Correlational analyses of explicit and implicit memory
performance. Memory & Cognition, 17, 77–86.
Petri, H.L., & Mishkin, M. 1994. Behaviorism, cognitivism and the neuropsychology of
memory. American Scientist, 82, 30–37.
Piaget, J. 1952. The origins of intelligence in children. New York: International Universi-
ties Press.
Piaget, J. 1954. The construction of reality in the child. New York: Basic Books.
Piaget, J. 1962. Play, dreams and imitation in childhood. New York: Norton.
Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. 1973. Memory and intelligence. New York: Basic Books.
Postman, L., & Underwood, B. 1973. Critical issues in interference theory. Memory &
Cognition, 1, 19–40.
Preyer, W. 1885. Spezielle physiologie des embryo. Leipsig: Grieben.
Quinn, P.C., & Eimas, P.D. 1996. Perceptual organization and categorization in young
infants. In C. Rovee-Collier & L.P. Lipsitt (Eds.), Advances in infancy research (Vol.
10, pp. 1–36). Norwood, NJ: Ablex Publishing Corporation.
Radvansky, G.A. 1999. Aging, memory, and comprehension. Current Directions in Psy-
chological Science, 8, 49–53.
Radvansky, G.A., Zacks, R.T., & Hasher, L. 1996. Fact retrieval in younger and older
adults: The role of mental models. Psychology and Aging, 11, 258–271.
Rajaram, S. 1993. Remembering and knowing: Two means of access to the personal past.
Memory & Cognition, 21, 89–102.
Rakic, P., & Nowakowski, R.S. 1981. The time of origin of neurons in the hippocampal
region of the rhesus monkey. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 196, 99–128.
Ramos, J.M.J. 1998. Retrograde amnesia for spatial information: A dissociation between
intra- and extramaze cues following hippocampus lesions in rats. European Journal of
Neuroscience, 10, 3295–3301.
Ramsay, D.S., & Campos, J.J. 1978. The onset of representation and entry into Stage 6 of
object permanence development. Developmental Psychology, 14, 79–86.
Ratcliff, R., & McKoon, G. 1986. More on the distinction between episodic and semantic
memories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 12,
312–313.
Ratcliff, R., & Murdock, B.B., Jr. 1976. Retrieval processes in recognition memory.
Psychological Review, 83, 190–214.
Ratcliff, R., Van Zandt, T., & McKoon, G. 1995. Process dissociation, single-process
theories, and recognition memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124,
352–374.
Ratner, H., Smith, B., & Dion, S. 1986. Development of memory for events. Journal of
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
REFERENCES 279
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
280 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
Roediger, H.L., III., Rajaram, S., & Srinivas, K. 1990. Specifying criteria for postulating
memory systems. In A. Diamond (Ed.), The development and neural bases of higher
cognitive functions (Vol. 608, pp. 572–589), Annals of the New York Academy of
Sciences. New York: New York Academy of Sciences.
Roediger, H.L., III., & Srinivas, K. 1993. Specificity of operations in perceptual priming. In
P. Graf & M.E.J. Masson (Eds.), Implicit memory: New directions in cognition, devel-
opment, and neuropsychology (pp. 17–48). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associ-
ates.
Roediger, H.L., III., Srinivas, K., & Weldon, M.S. 1989a. Dissociations between implicit
measures of memory. In S. Lewandowsky, J.C. Dunn, & K. Kirsner (Eds.), Implicit
memory: Theoretical issues (pp. 67–84). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Roediger, H.L., III., & Weldon, M.S. 1987. Reversing the picture superiority effect. In
M.A. McDaniel & M. Pressley (Eds.), Imagery and related mnemonic processes:
Theories, individual differences, and applications (pp. 151–174). New York: Springer-
Verlag.
Roediger, H.L., III., Weldon, M.S., & Challis, B.H. 1989b. Explaining dissociations
between implicit and explicit measures of retention: A processing account. In H.L.
Roediger, III., & F.I.M. Craik (Eds.), Varieties of memory and consciousness: Essays in
honour of Endel Tulving (pp. 3–41). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Roediger, H.L., III., Weldon, M.S., Stadler, M.L., & Riegler, G L. 1992. Direct comparison
of two implicit memory tests: Word fragment and word stem completion. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18, 1251–1269.
Rose, S.A. 1981. Developmental changes in infants’ retention of visual stimuli. Child
Development, 52, 227–233.
Ross, B.H., & Bradshaw, G.L. 1994. Encoding effects of remindings. Memory & Cogni-
tion, 22, 591–605.
Rossi-George, A., & Rovee-Collier, C. 1999. Retroactive interference in 3-month-old
infants. Developmental Psychobiology, 35, 167–177.
Rovee, C.K., & Fagen, J.W. 1976. Extended conditioning and 24-hour retention in infants.
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 21, 1–11.
Rovee, C.K., & Rovee, D.T. 1969. Conjugate reinforcement of infant exploratory behavior.
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 8, 33–39.
Rovee-Collier, C. 1984. The ontogeny of learning and memory in human infancy. In R.
Kail, & N.E. Spear (Eds.), Comparative perspectives on the development of memory
(pp. 103–134). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Rovee-Collier, C. 1990. The “memory system” of prelinguistic infants. In A. Diamond
(Ed.), The development and neural bases of higher cognitive functions (Vol. 608, pp.
517–536), Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. New York: New York
Academy of Sciences.
Rovee-Collier, C. 1995. Time windows in cognitive development. Developmental Psychol-
ogy, 31, 147–169.
Rovee-Collier, C. 1997. Dissociations in infant memory: Rethinking the development of
implicit and explicit memory. Psychological Review, 104, 467–498.
Rovee-Collier, C., Adler, S.A., & Borza, M.A. 1994. Substituting new details for old?
Effects of delaying postevent information on infant memory. Memory & Cognition, 22,
644–656.
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
REFERENCES 281
Rovee-Collier, C., Bhatt, R.S., & Chazin, S. 1996. Set size, novelty, and visual pop-out in
infancy. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance,
22, 1178–1187.
Rovee-Collier, C., & Boller, K. 1995. Current theory and research on infant learning and
memory: Application to early intervention. Infants and Young Children, 7, 1–12.
Rovee-Collier, C., Borza, M.A., Adler, S.A., & Boller, K. 1993a. Infants’ eyewitness
testimony: Effects of postevent information on a prior memory representation. Memory
& Cognition, 21, 267–279.
Rovee-Collier, C., & DuFault, D. 1991. Multiple contexts and memory retrieval at 3
months. Developmental Psychobiology, 24, 39–49.
Rovee-Collier, C., Earley, L.A., & Stafford, S. 1989. Ontogeny of early event memory: III.
Attentional determinants of retrieval at 2 and 3 months. Infant Behavior and Develop-
ment, 12, 147–161.
Rovee-Collier, C., Enright, M.K., Lucas, D., Fagen, J.W., & Gekoski, M.J. 1981. The
forgetting of newly acquired and reactivated memories of 3-month-old infants. Infant
Behavior and Development, 4, 317–331.
Rovee-Collier, C., Evancio, S., & Earley, L.A. 1995. The time window hypothesis: Spacing
effects. Infant Behavior and Development, 18, 69–78.
Rovee-Collier, C., Greco-Vigorito, C., & Hayne, H. 1993b. The time-window hypothesis:
Implications for categorization and memory modification. Infant Behavior and Devel-
opment, 16, 149–176.
Rovee-Collier, C., Griesler, P.C., & Earley, L.A. 1985a. Contextual determinants of
retrieval in three-month-old infants. Learning and Motivation, 16, 139–157.
Rovee-Collier, C., Hankins, E., & Bhatt, R.S. 1992a. Textons, visual pop-out effects, and
object recognition in infancy. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 121, 435–
445.
Rovee-Collier, C., Hartshorn, K., & DiRubbo, M. 1999. Long-term maintenance of infant
memory. Developmental Psychobiology, 35, 91–102.
Rovee-Collier, C., & Hayne, H. 1987. Reactivation of infant memory: Implications for
cognitive development. In H.W. Reese (Ed.), Advances in child development and
behavior (Vol. 20, pp. 185–238). New York: Academic Press.
Rovee-Collier, C., & Hayne, H. 2000. Memory in infancy and early childhood. In E.
Tulving & F.I.M. Craik (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of memory. (pp. 267–282). New
York: Oxford University Press.
Rovee-Collier, C., Patterson, J., & Hayne, H. 1985b. Specificity in the reactivation of infant
memory. Developmental Psychobiology, 18, 559–574.
Rovee-Collier, C., Schechter, A., Shyi, G.C-W., & Shields, P. 1992b. Perceptual identifica-
tion of contextual attributes and infant memory retrieval. Developmental Psychology,
28, 307–318.
Rovee-Collier, C., & Sullivan, M.W. 1980. Organization of infant memory. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 6, 798–807.
Rovee-Collier, C., Sullivan, M.W., Enright, M.K., Lucas, D., & Fagen, J.W. 1980. Reacti-
vation of infant memory. Science, 208, 1159–1161.
Rozin, P. 1976. The psychobiological approach to human memory. In M.R. Rosenzweig &
E.L. Bennett (Eds.), Neural mechanisms of learning and memory (pp. 3–48). Cam-
bridge, MA: MIT Press.
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
282 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
Rudy, J.W. 1991. Elemental and configural associations, the hippocampus, and develop-
ment. Developmental Psychobiology, 24, 219–236.
Rudy, J.W., & Sutherland, R.J. 1989. The hippocampal formation is necessary for rats to
learn and remember configural discriminations. Behavioral Brain Research, 34, 97–
109.
Ruggiero, F.T., & Flagg, S.F. 1976. Do animals have memory? In D.L. Medin, W.A.
Roberts, & R.T. Davis (EDs.), Processes of animal memory (pp. 1–19). Hillsdale, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Russo, R., & Parkin, A.J. 1993. Age differences in implicit memory: More apparent than
real. Memory & Cognition, 21, 73–80.
Russo, R., Nichelli, P., Gibertoni, M., & Cornia, C. 1995. Developmental trends in implicit
and explicit memory: A picture completion study. Journal of Experimental Child
Psychology, 59, 566–578.
Rybash, J.M., & Osborne, J.L. 1991. Implicit memory, the serial position effect, and test
awareness. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 29, 327–330.
Saint-Cyr, J.A., Taylor, A.E., & Lang, A.E. 1988. Procedural learning and neostriatal
dysfunction in man. Brain, 111, 941–959.
Sameroff, A.J. 1968. The components of sucking in the human newborn. Journal of
Experimental Child Psychology, 6, 607–623.
Schacter, D.L. 1985. Priming of old and new knowledge in amnesic patients and normal
subjects. In D.S. Olton, E. Gamzu, & S. Corkin (Eds.), Memory dysfunctions: An
integration of animal and human research from preclinical and clinical perspectives
(vol 444, pp. 44–53). Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. New York: New
York Academy of Science..
Schacter, D.L. 1987. Implicit memory: History and current status. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 13, 501–518.
Schacter, D.L. 1989. On the relation between memory and consciousness: Dissociable
interactions and conscious experience. In H.L. Roediger, III., & F.I.M. Craik (Eds.),
Varieties of memory and consciousness: Essays in honour of Endel Tulving (pp. 355–
389). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Schacter, D.L. 1990. Perceptual representation systems and implicit memory: Toward a
resolution of the multiple memory systems debate. In A. Diamond (Ed.), The develop-
ment and neural bases of higher cognitive functions (Vol. 608, pp. 543–567), Annals of
the New York Academy of Sciences. New York: New York Academy of Sciences.
Schacter, D.L. 1992. Priming and multiple memory systems: Perceptual mechanisms of
implicit memory. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 4, 244–256.
Schacter, D.L. 1994. Priming and multiple memory systems: Perceptual mechanisms of
implicit memory. In D.L. Schacter & E. Tulving (Eds.), Memory systems 1994 (pp. 233–
268). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Schacter, D.L. 1997. The cognitive neuroscience of memory: Perspectives from neuroimag-
ing research. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, 352B, 1689–
1695.
Schacter, D.L., Alpert, N.M., Savage, C.R., Rauch, S.L., & Albert, M.S. 1996. Conscious
recollection and the human hippocampal formation: Evidence from positron emission
tomography. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (USA), 93, 321–325.
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
REFERENCES 283
Schacter, D.L., Bowers, J., & Booker, J. 1989. Intention, awareness, and implicit memory:
The retrieval intentionality criterion. In S. Lewandowsky, J.C. Dunn, & K. Kirsner
(Eds.), Implicit memory: Theoretical issues (pp. 47–65). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates.
Schacter, D.L., Chiu, C.Y.P., & Ochsner, K.N. 1993a. Implicit memory: A selective review.
Annual Review of Neuroscience, 16, 159–182.
Schacter, D.L., & Cooper, L.A. 1993. Implicit and explicit memory for novel visual objects:
Structure and function. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and
Cognition, 19, 995–1009.
Schacter, D.L., Cooper, L.A., & Delaney, S.M. 1990. Implicit memory for unfamiliar
objects depends on access to structural descriptions. Journal of Experimental Psychol-
ogy: General, 119, 5–24.
Schacter, D.L., Cooper, L.A., Delaney, S.M., Peterson, M.A., & Tharan, M. 1991a. Implicit
memory for possible and impossible objects: Constraints on the construction of struc-
tural descriptions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cogni-
tion, 17, 3–19.
Schacter, D.L., Cooper, L.A., Tharan, M., & Rubens, A.B. 1991b. Preserved priming of
novel objects in patients with memory disorders. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 3,
118–131.
Schacter, D.L., Cooper, L.A., & Treadwell, J. 1993b. Preserved priming of novel objects
across size transformation in amnesic patients. Psychological Science, 4, 331–335.
Schacter, D.L., & Moscovitch, M. 1984. Infants, amnesics, and dissociable memory
systems. In M. Moscovitch (Ed.), Advances in the study of communication and affect.
Vol. 9: Infant memory (pp. 173–216). New York: Plenum Publishing Corporation.
Schacter, D.L., & Tulving, E. 1994. What are the memory systems of 1994? In D.L.
Schacter & E. Tulving (Eds.), Memory systems 1994 (pp. 1–38). Cambridge, MA: MIT
Press.
Schenk, F., Inglin, F., & Morris, R.G.M. 1983. Place navigation in rats as a function of age.
Society of Neuroscience Abstracts, 9, 332.
Schmidt, R.A., & Bjork, R.A. 1992. New conceptualizations of practice: Common prin-
ciples in three paradigms suggest new concepts for training. Psychological Science, 3,
207–217.
Schulenberg, C.J., Riccio, D.C., & Stikes, E.S. 1971. Acquisition and retention of a
passive-avoidance response as a function of age in rats. Journal of Comparative and
Physiological Psychology, 74, 75–83.
Schwartz, B.L., & Hashtroudi, S. 1991. Priming is independent of skill learning. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17, 1177–1187.
Schweitzer, L., & Green, L. 1982. Acquisition and extended retention of a conditioned taste
aversion in preweanling rats. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology,
96, 791–806.
Scott, S.K., Young, A.W., Calder, A.J., Hellawell, D.J., Aggleton, J.P., & Johnson, M.
1997. Impaired auditory recognition of fear and anger following bilateral amygdala
lesions. Nature, 385, 254–257.
Scoville, W.B., & Milner, B. 1957. Loss of recent memory after bilateral hippocampal
lesions. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 20, 11–21.
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
284 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
Searle, J.R. 1983. Intentionality: An essay in the philosophy of mind. Cambridge, UK:
Cambridge University Press.
Searle, J.R. 1984. Minds, brains, and science. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
Searle, J.R. 1995. The mystery of consciousness. New York Review of Books, 42(17), 60–
66.
Searle, J.R. 1998. Mind, language, and society. New York: Basic Books.
Shallice, T. 1988. From neuropsychology to mental structure. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge
University Press.
Shapiro, M.L., & Olton, D.S. 1994. Hippocampal function and interference. In D.L.
Schacter & E. Tulving (Eds.), Memory systems 1994 (pp. 87–117). Cambridge, MA:
MIT Press.
Sheffield, E.G., & Hudson, J.A. 1994. Reactivation of toddlers’ event memory. Memory, 2,
447–465.
Sherry, D.F., & Schacter, D.L. 1987. The evolution of multiple memory systems. Psycho-
logical Review, 94, 439–454.
Shields, P.J., & Rovee-Collier, C. 1992. Long-term memory for context-specific category
information at 6 months. Child Development, 63, 245–259.
Shimamura, A.P. 1985. Problems with the finding of stochastic independence as evidence
for multiple memory systems. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 23, 506–508.
Shimamura, A.P. 1986. Priming effects in amnesia. Evidence for a dissociable memory
function. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38A, 619–644.
Shimamura, A.P. 1990. Forms of memory: Issues and directions. In J.L. McGaugh, N.M.
Weinberger, & G. Lynch (Eds.), Brain organization and memory: Cells, systems and
circuits (pp. 159–173). New York: Oxford University Press.
Shimamura, A.P., Janowsky, J.S., & Squire, L.R. 1991. What is the role of frontal lobe
damage in memory disorders? In H.S. Levin, H.M. Eisenberg, & A.L.Benton (Eds.),
Frontal lobe function and dysfunction (pp. 173–195). New York: Oxford University
Press.
Shimamura, A.P., & Squire, L.R. 1984. Paired-associate learning and priming effects in
amnesia: A neuropsychological study. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,
113, 556–570.
Shimp, C.P., & Moffitt, M. 1974. Short-term memory in the pigeon: Stimulus-response
associations. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 22, 507–512.
Sidman, M., Stoddard, L.T., & Mohr, J.P. 1968. Some additional quantitative observations
of immediate memory in a patient with bilateral hippocampal lesions. Neuropsycholo-
gia, 6, 245–254.
Singer, J.M., & Fagen, J.W. 1992. Negative affect, emotional expression, and forgetting in
young infants. Developmental Psychology, 28, 48–57.
Skinner, B.F. 1953. Science and human behavior. New York: Macmillan.
Skinner, B.F. 1957. Verbal behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Sloman, S.A., Hayman, C.A.G., Ohta, N., Law, J., & Tulving, E. 1988. Forgetting in
primed fragment completion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory,
and Cognition, 14, 223–239.
Smith, M.L., & Milner, B. 1981. The role of the right hippocampus in the recall of spatial
location. Neuropsychologia, 19, 781–793.
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
REFERENCES 285
Smith, S.M., Heath, F.R., & Vela, E. 1990. Environmental context-dependent homophone
spelling. American Journal of Psychology, 103, 229–242.
Snodgrass, J.G. 1989. Sources of learning in the picture fragment completion task. In S.
Lewandowsky, J.C. Dunn, & K. Kirsner (Eds.), Implicit memory: Theoretical issues
(pp. 259–282). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Snodgrass, J.G., & Feenan, K. 1990. Priming effects in picture fragment completion:
Support for the perceptual closure hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
General, 119, 276–296.
Snodgrass, J.G., Smith, B., Feenan, K., & Corwin, J. 1987. Fragmenting pictures on the
Apple Macintosh computer for experimental and clinical applications. Behavior Re-
search Methods, Instruments & Computers, 19, 270–274.
Snodgrass, J.G., & Vanderwart, M. 1980. A standardized set of 260 pictures: Norms for
name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 6, 174–215.
Sokolov, E.N. 1963. Higher nervous functions: The orienting reflex. Annual Review of
Physiology, 25, 545–580.
Solomon, P.R., Groccia-Ellison, M., Levine, E., Blanchard, S., & Pendlebury, W.W. 1990.
Do temporal relationships in conditioning change across the life span? Perspectives
from eyeblink conditioning in humans and rabbits. In A. Diamond (Ed.), The develop-
ment and neural bases of higher cognitive functions (Vol. 608, pp. 212–232), Annals of
the New York Academy of Sciences. New York: New York Academy of Sciences.
Solomon, P.R., Pomerleau, D., Bennett, L., James, J., & Morse, D.L. 1989. Acquisition of
the classically conditioned eyeblink response in humans over the lifespan. Psychology
& Aging, 4, 34–41.
Spear, N.E. 1973. Retrieval of memories in animals. Psychological Review, 80, 163–194.
Spear, N.E. 1978. The processing of memories: Forgetting and retention. Hillsdale, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Spear, N.E., & Kucharski, D. 1984. Ontogenetic differences in stimulus selection during
conditioning. In R. Kail, & N.E. Spear (Eds.), Comparative perspectives on the develop-
ment of memory (pp. 227–252). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Spear, N.E., & Parsons, P.J. 1976. Analysis of a reactivation treatment: Ontogenetic
determinants of alleviated forgetting. In D.L. Medin, W.A. Roberts, & R.T. Davis
(Eds.), Processes of animal memory (pp. 135–165). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates.
Squire, L.R. 1986. Mechanisms of memory. Science, 232, 1612–1619.
Squire, L.R. 1987. Memory and brain. New York: Oxford University Press.
Squire, L.R. 1992a. Declarative and nondeclarative memory: Multiple brain systems sup-
porting learning and memory. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 4, 232–243.
Squire, L.R. 1992b. Memory and the hippocampus: A synthesis from findings with rats,
monkeys, and humans. Psychological Review, 99, 195–231.
Squire, L.R. 1994. Declarative and nondeclarative memory: Multiple brain systems sup-
porting learning and memory. In D.L. Schacter & E. Tulving (Eds.), Memory Systems
1994 (pp. 203–231). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Squire, L.R., Knowlton, B., & Musen, G. 1993. The structure and organization of memory.
Annual Review of Psychology, 44, 453–495.
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
286 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
Squire, L.R., Ojemann, J.G., Miezin, F.M., Petersen, S.E., Videen, T.O., & Raichle, M.E.
1992. Activation of the hippocampus in normal humans: A functional anatomical study
of memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (USA), 89, 1837–1841.
Squire, L.R., & Slater, P.C. 1975. Forgetting in very long-term memory as assessed by an
improved questionnaire technique. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1, 50–54.
Squire, L.R., & Zola-Morgan, S.M. 1991. The medial temporal lobe memory system.
Science, 253, 1380–1386.
Squire, L.R., Zola-Morgan, S.M., & Chen, K.S. 1988. Human amnesia and animal models
of amnesia: Performance of amnesic patients on tests designed for the monkey. Behav-
ioral Neuroscience, 102, 210–221.
Srinivas, K. 1996. Contrast and illumination effects on explicit and implicit measures of
memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22,
1123–1135.
Srinivas, K., & Roediger, H.L., III. 1990. Classifying implicit memory tests: Category
association and anagram solution. Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 389–412.
Stankiewicz, B.J., Cooper, E.E., & Hummel, J.E. 1994, November. The role of attention in
left-right reflectional invariance. Paper presented at the meeting of the Psychonomic
Society, St. Louis, MO.
Stehouwer, D.J., & Campbell, B.A. 1978. Habituation of the forelimb-withdrawal response
in neonatal rats. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 4,
104–119.
Stehouwer, D.J., & Campbell, B.A. 1980. Ontogeny of passive avoidance: Role of task
demands and development of species-typical behaviors. Developmental Psychobiology,
13, 791–806.
Steinert, P.A., Infurna, R.N., & Spear, N.E. 1980. Long-term retention of a conditioned
taste aversion in preweanling and adult rats. Animal Learning & Behavior, 8, 375–381.
Sullivan, M.W. 1982. Reactivation: Priming forgotten memories in human infants. Child
Development, 53, 516–523.
Sullivan, M.W., Rovee-Collier, C., & Tynes, D.M. 1979. A conditioning analysis of infant
long-term memory. Child Development, 50, 152–162.
Suzuki, W.A., Zola-Morgan, S.M., Squire, L.R., & Amaral, D.G. 1993. Lesions of the
perirhinal and parahippocampal cortices in the monkey produce long-lasting memory
impairment in the visual and tactual modalities. Journal of Neuroscience, 13, 2430–
2451
Terwilliger, R.F. 1968. Meaning and mind: A study of the psychology of language. New
York: Oxford University Press.
Teuber, H.L. 1955. Physiological psychology. Annual Review of Psychology, 6, 267–296.
Thomas, K.M. 1997. A developmental study of implicit and explicit motor sequence
learning. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis,
MN.
Thomson, D.M., & Tulving, E. 1970. Associative encoding and retrieval: Weak and strong
cues. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 86, 255–262.
Thompson, R.F. 1990a. Discussion of J.M. Mandler, “Recall of events by preverbal
children.” In A. Diamond (Ed.), The development and neural bases of higher cognitive
functions (Vol. 608, pp. 503–516), Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. New
York: New York Academy of Sciences.
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
REFERENCES 287
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
288 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
Ungerleider, L.G. 1995. Functional brain imaging studies of cortical mechanisms for
memory. Science, 270, 769–775.
Ungerleider, L.G., & Mishkin, M.M. 1982. Two cortical visual systems. In D.J. Ingle, M.A.
Goodale, & R.J.W. Mansfield (Eds.), Analysis of visual behavior (pp. 549–586).
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Uzgiris, I.C. 1981. Two functions of imitation during infancy. International Journal of
Behavioral Development, 4, 1–12.
Uzgiris, I.C., & Hunt, J.McV. 1970. Attentional preference and experience: II. An explor-
atory longitudinal study of the effect of visual familiarity and responsiveness. Journal of
Genetic Psychology, 117, 109–121.
Vander Linde, E., Morrongiello, B.A., & Rovee-Collier, C. 1985. Determinants of retention
in 8-week-old infants. Developmental Psychology, 21, 601–613.
Vargha-Khadem, F., Gadian, D.G., Watkins, K.E., Connelly, A., Van Paesschen, N., &
Mishkin, M.M. 1997. Differential effects of early hippocampal pathology on episodic
and semantic memory. Science, 277, 376–380.
Victor, M., & Agamanolis, D. 1990. Amnesia due to lesions confined to the hippocampus:
A clinical-pathologic study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 2, 246–257.
Victor, M., Angevine, J.B., Mancall, E.L., & Fisher, C.M. 1961. Memory loss with lesions
of hippocampal formation. Archives of Neurology, 5, 244–263.
von Hippel, W., & Hawkins, C. 1994. Stimulus exposure time and perceptual memory.
Perception & Psychophysics, 56, 525–535.
Vriezen, E.R., Moscovitch, M., & Bellos, S.A. 1995. Priming effects in semantic classifica-
tion tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21,
933–946.
Wallace, J.E., Krauter, E.E., & Campbell, B.A. 1980. Animal models of declining memory
in the aged: Short-term and spatial memory in the aged rat. Journal of Gerontology, 35,
355–363.
Warrington, E.K., & Weiskrantz, L. 1968. New method of testing long-term retention with
special reference to amnesic patients. Nature, 217, 972–974.
Warrington, E.K., & Weiskrantz, L. 1970. Amnesic syndrome: Consolidation or retrieval?
Nature, 228, 628–630.
Warrington, E.K., & Weiskrantz, L. 1982. Amnesia: A disconnection syndrome? Neuro-
psychologia, 20, 233–248.
Watkins, P.C., Matthews, A., Williamson, D.A., & Fuller, R.D. 1992. Mood-congruent
memory in depression: Emotional priming or elaboration? Journal of Abnormal Psy-
chology, 101, 581–586.
Watson, J.B. 1930. Behaviorism. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
Watson, J.B., & Rayner, R. 1920. Conditioned emotional reactions. Journal of Experimen-
tal Psychology, 3, 1–14.
Watson, J.S. 1984. Memory in learning: Analysis of three momentary reactions of infants.
In R. Kail & N.E. Spear (Eds.), Comparative perspectives on the development of
memory (pp. 159–179). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Weiskrantz, L. 1987. Neuroanatomy of memory and amnesia: A case for multiple memory
systems. Human Neurobiology, 6, 93–105.
Weiskrantz, L. 1990. Problems of learning and memory: One or multiple memory systems?
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
REFERENCES 289
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
290 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
Witherspoon, D., & Moscovitch, M. 1989. Stochastic independence between two implicit
memory tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cogni-
tion, 15, 22–30.
Woodruff-Pak, D.S. 1993. Eyeblink classical conditioning in H.M.: Delay and trace
paradigms. Behavioral Neuroscience, 107, 911–925.
Woodworth, R.S. 1938. Experimental psychology. New York: Henry Holt.
Wright, A.A., Santiago, H.C., Sands, S.F., Kendrick, D.F., & Cook, R.G. 1985. Memory
processing of serial lists by pigeons, monkeys, and people. Science, 229, 287–289.
Wyers, E.J., Peeke, H.V.S., & Herz, M.J. 1973. Behavioral habituation in invertebrates. In
H.V.S. Peeke & M.J. Herz (Eds.), Habituation I (pp. 1–57). New York: Academic
Press.
Zentall, T.R. 1973. Memory in the pigeon: Retroactive inhibition in a delayed matching
task. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 1, 126–128.
Zola-Morgan, S.M., & Squire, L.R. 1984. Preserved learning in monkeys with medial
temporal lesions: Sparing of motor and cognitive skills. Journal of Neuroscience, 4,
1072–1085.
Zola-Morgan, S.M., & Squire, L.R. 1985a. Medial temporal lesions in monkeys impair
memory on a variety of tasks sensitive to human amnesia. Behavioral Neuroscience, 99,
22–34.
Zola-Morgan, S.M., & Squire, L.R. 1985b. Complementary approaches to the study of
memory: Human amnesia and animal models. In N.W. Weinberger, J.L. McGaugh, &
G. Lynch (Eds.), Memory systems of the brain: Animal and human cognitive processes
(pp. 463–477). New York: Guilford Press.
Zola-Morgan, S.M., & Squire, L.R. 1990a. The primate hippocampal formation: Evidence
for a time-limited role in memory storage. Science, 250, 288–290.
Zola-Morgan, S.M., & Squire, L.R. 1990b. Neuropsychological investigations of memory
and amnesia: Findings from humans and nonhuman primates. In A. Diamond (Ed.), The
development and neural bases of higher cognitive functions (Vol. 608, pp. 434–456),
Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. New York: New York Academy of
Sciences.
Zola-Morgan, S.M., Squire, L.R., & Amaral, D.G. 1989a. Lesions of the amygdala that
spare adjacent cortical regions do not impair memory or exacerbate the impairment
following lesions of the hippocampal formation. Journal of Neuroscience, 9, 1922–
1936.
Zola-Morgan, S.M., Squire, L.R., Amaral, D.G., & Suzuki, W.A. 1989b. Lesions of
perirhinal and parahippocampal cortex that spare the amygdala and hippocampal forma-
tion produce severe memory impairment. Journal of Neuroscience, 9, 4355–4370.
Zola-Morgan, S.M., Squire, L.R., Clower, R.P., & Rempel, N.L. 1993. Damage to the
perirhinal cortex exacerbates memory impairment following lesions to the hippocampal
formation. Journal of Neuroscience, 13, 251–265.
Zola-Morgan, S.M., Squire, L.R., & Mishkin, M.M. 1982. The neuroanatomy of amnesia:
Amygdala-hippocampus vs. temporal stem. Science, 218, 1337–1339.
Zola-Morgan, S.M., Squire, L.R., Rempel, N.L., Clower, R.P., & Amaral, D.G. 1992.
Enduring memory impairment in monkeys after ischemic damage to the hippocampus.
Journal of Neuroscience, 12, 2582–2596.
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
Author Index
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
292 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
AUTHOR INDEX 293
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
294 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
AUTHOR INDEX 295
Gratch, G., 103, 264 137, 142, 145, 147, 148, 151, 152, 153,
Greco, C., 119, 150, 156, 157, 160, 175, 156, 157, 159, 160, 162, 163, 175, 184,
197, 202, 203, 264, 266 185, 186, 197, 198, 202, 203, 218, 229,
Greco-Vigorito, C., 119, 159, 160, 266, 234, 235, 237, 238, 239, 241, 244, 245,
281 255, 257, 259, 264, 266, 276, 281
Green, L., 69t, 283 Heath, F. R., 183, 285
Greenbaum, J. L., 75, 144, 265 Heimann, M., 114, 123, 266
Greenberg, R., 101, 123, 263 Heindel, W. C., 56, 266
Greene, R. L., 22, 23, 24, 158, 162, 265 Hellawell, D. J., 56, 283
Gregg, B. E., 69t, 265 Hembree, E. A., 173, 267
Griesler, P. C., 175, 184, 197, 203, 230, Herbert, J., 147, 148, 151, 198, 235, 266
266, 281 Hermann, D. J., 2, 266
Griffin, D. R., 22, 265 Hernandez-Reif, M., 100, 117, 255
Groccia-Ellison, M., 109, 285 Hertel, P. T., 172, 174, 266
Gross, C. G., 50, 259 Hertsgaard, L. A., 115, 118, 121, 151,
Gross, J., 142, 153, 157, 266 156, 186, 255
Grossman, R. G., 67, 289 Herz, M. J., 233, 290
Guanowsky, V., 69t, 265 Hildreth, K., 111, 140, 141, 142, 151,
Gulya, M., 80, 131, 142, 156, 166, 176, 153, 157, 220, 234, 235, 241, 267
178, 182, 186, 203, 257, 265 Hill, W. H., 145, 234, 267
Gunderson, V. M., 91, 92f, 94, 265 Hinderliter, C. F., 142, 275
Hintzman, D. L., 8, 128, 190, 267
H Hirsh, R., 54, 60, 190t, 267, 277
Habibi, A., 12t, 258 Hirst, W., 25, 28, 214t, 224, 226, 227,
Haden, P., 7, 8, 12t, 18, 128, 168, 264 228, 229, 231, 269
Hagger, C., 90, 94, 123, 254 Hitchcock, D. F.A., 131, 157, 186, 239,
Haklstead, C., 69t, 270 244, 267
Hamburg, M., 2, 148, 269 Holyoak, K. J., 244, 263
Handelmann, G. E., 41, 54, 276 Honig, W. K., 190t, 267
Hankins, E., 134, 182, 218, 281 Hood, B., 105, 267
Hankins, L. L., 69t, 71, 264 Hooker, D., 66, 267
Hanna, E., 185, 265 Hubbard, J. I., 21, 267
Hardin, T. S., 172, 174, 266 Hudson, J., 152, 267
Hartman, M., 26, 179, 274, 276 Hudson, J. A., 129, 131, 133, 149, 210,
Hartshorn, K., 81, 111, 112, 129, 145, 234, 253, 284
147, 148, 160, 184, 185, 197, 198, 229, Hummel, J. E., 179, 286
234, 238, 245, 265, 281 Humphrey, T., 66, 67, 267
Hasher, L., 11, 74, 191, 233, 265, 269, Humphreys, M., 183, 287
278 Humphries, M. S., 175, 272
Hashtroudi, S., 12t, 25, 26, 283 Hunt, J. McV., 100, 267, 288
Hawkins, C., 162, 288 Hunt, R. R., 172, 261
Hawkins, R. D., 107, 109, 269 Hunter, M. A., 99, 267
Hayman, C. A. G., 25, 150, 153, 175, Hunter, W. S., 104, 267
266, 284, 287 Hurt, C. A., 180, 269
Hayne, H., 98, 114, 115, 119, 123, 131,
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
296 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
AUTHOR INDEX 297
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
298 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
AUTHOR INDEX 299
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
300 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
AUTHOR INDEX 301
Swartz, K. B., 91, 94, 265 Van Paesschen, N., 54, 288
Sweeney, B., 156, 176, 182, 265 Van Zandt, B. J. S., 180, 271
Van Zandt, T., 20, 278
T Vargha-Khadem, F., 54, 288
Taylor, A. E., 56, 282 Vasquez, B. J., 69t, 71, 264, 268
Terwilliger, R. F., 22, 286 Vazoiu, F., 74, 267
Teuber, H. L., 3, 29, 30, 127, 273, 286 Vela, E., 183, 285
Tharan, M., 12, 162, 178, 202, 283 Verfaellie, M., 226, 258
Thomas, K. M., 79, 286 Victor, M., 30, 288
Thompson, R. F., 56, 125, 193, 286, 287 Videen, T. O., 59, 286
Thompson, W. C., 69t, 260 Vieira, A., 114, 123, 129, 139, 157, 187,
Thomson, D. M., 183, 286 205, 208, 210, 229, 255
Timmons, C. R., 139, 156, 204, 205, 208, Visser, G. H. A., 66, 261
210, 287 von Hippel, W., 162, 288
Tissot, R., 72, 253 Vriezen, E. R., 138, 288
Tomiyasu, U., 30, 260
Toth, J. P., 17, 19, 78, 268 W
Towle, C., 102, 262 Wallace, J. E., 69t, 71, 258, 288
Tranel, D., 56, 62, 253, 256 Ward, C. H., 172, 256
Treadwell, J., 178, 202, 283 Warrington, E. K., 3, 7, 8, 12t, 33f, 34,
Treisman, A., 12, 134, 137, 138, 139, 56, 109, 128, 190t, 255, 288, 289
149, 151, 155, 179, 217, 220, 233, 274 Watkins, K. E., 54, 288
Tuber, D. S., 109, 256, 287 Watkins, P. C., 172, 288
Tulving, E., 2, 7, 9, 12t, 15, 16, 21, 22, Watson, J. B., 1, 106, 196, 288
23, 24, 25, 73, 97, 125, 127, 129, 131, Watson, J. S., 110, 193, 288
138, 139, 143, 144, 150, 153, 168, 172, Webster, M., 94, 271
175, 179, 180, 183, 190t, 191, 192, Weiner, S., 182, 218, 219, 256
195, 196, 199, 200, 201, 211, 212, 213, Weiskopf, S., 103, 117, 118, 263
215, 221, 224, 225, 233, 239, 247, 260, Weiskrantz, L., 3, 7, 8, 12t, 33f, 34, 56,
266, 276, 283, 284, 286, 287 109, 128, 190t, 191, 192, 288, 289
Turner, M., 102, 277 Weizmann, F., 99, 289
Tynes, D. M., 145, 152, 234, 286 Weldon, M. S., 25, 77, 168, 214, 215,
216, 226, 280
U Werboff, J., 107, 257
Underwood, B., 153, 255, 278 Werker, J., 125, 289
Underwood, B. J., 183, 287 Werner, J. S., 2, 116, 289
Ungerleider, L. G., 58, 59, 88, 178, 254, Wewerka, S. S., 151, 156, 255
276, 288 White, B. C., 142, 258
Uzgiris, I. C., 100, 122, 267, 288 White, N. M., 60, 61, 272, 277
Whitmarsh, G. A., 175, 257
V Wickelgren, W. A., 150, 190t, 289
Vander Linde, E., 145, 156, 157, 159, Wilk, A., 142, 160, 166, 174, 176, 178,
160, 163, 276, 288 241, 242, 253, 265, 273, 289
Van der Linden, M., 14, 79, 273 Willatts, P., 105, 267
Vanderwart, M., 74, 285 Williams, J. M. G., 213, 262
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
302 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
Williamson, D. A., 172, 288 Wurtzel, N., 112, 145, 147, 148, 185,
Willingham, D., 26, 276 197, 198, 234, 265
Willingham, D. B., 9, 17, 24, 25, 26, 28, Wyers, E. J., 233, 290
216, 232
Willis, W. D., 67, 289 Y
Winocur, G., 43, 184, 289 Yengo, L. A., 202, 263
Wise, S. P., 56, 57, 289 Yonelinas, A. P., 17, 19, 78, 268
Witherspoon, D., 8, 12t, 25, 125, 128, Young, A. W., 56, 283
268, 290
Wolfe, J., 57, 257 Z
Wondoloski, T. L., 112, 145, 147, 148, Zacks, R. T., 74, 233, 265, 278
185, 197, 198, 234, 265 Zajonc, R. B., 12t, 174, 175, 180, 270
Wood, E. R., 53, 274 Zentall, T. R., 2, 290
Wood, F., 190t, 269 Zola-Morgan, S., 52, 53f, 90, 253
Woodruff-Pak, D. S., 12t, 15, 109, 290 Zola-Morgan, S. M., 16, 17, 24, 41, 42f,
Woodworth, R. S., 150, 290 46, 47f, 48, 52, 53f, 83, 88, 117, 148,
Wright, A. A., 76, 175, 178, 290 192, 253, 259, 275, 277, 286, 290
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
Subject Index
A priming in 232-233
absolute information, infant memory and affect, as dissociation variable 172-175,
182 173f
accessibility, of memory age and aging
context impact on 238-239, 245-246 as dissociation variable 144-150,
declarative/procedural 211-213 146f-148f
explicit 10-11, 142-143, 212, 239 in Jacksonian principle 66-68, 71-73
implicit 11, 212, 238 children’s memory and 75-82, 80f-
active memory 81f
mechanisms of 37, 193, 207, 249 multiple memory systems and 73-
priming with 233-234 82, 80f-81f
novel, retention effects of 242-245, memory performance per 17-18, 197,
243f 234-235, 235f
retention effects of 239-241, 240f multiple memory systems and 73-82,
adenosine monophosphate, see cyclic 228
AMP priming latency and 141-143, 142f
adult memory agendas, in MEM memory system 222f,
capacity of 1, 3, 247 223-224
experimental dissociations in 7-8, 127 alcoholic amnesia, see Korsakoff’s
independence in 23-24 syndrome
independent variables in 143-186, Alzheimer’s disease 26, 74
144t amnesia
parallels with amnesics 128 adult memory parallels with 128
parallels with infants 139-141 alcoholic, see Korsakoff’s syndrome
process-dissociation procedure for anterograde 30, 38, 40-41
19-20 conscious awareness with 8, 16, 21
infant memory versus 2-3 drug-induced temporary 14
Jacksonian principle of, multiple infant memory parallels with 115-126,
memory systems and 73-74 120f, 126t, 248
memory dissociations in 186, 188 infantile 73
task comparisons with infants 137- Jacksonian principle and 72-73
141, 138t memory dissociations with 3-4, 7,
tasks for 127-129 128, 193-194
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
304 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
SUBJECT INDEX 305
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
306 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
SUBJECT INDEX 307
specific versus general 196-198, 199f episodic memory system, see also
diencephalon, memory role of 25, 34, explicit memory
49-50, 57f context variable of 183
direct memory tasks 11-12 definition of 3, 9
discovering, in MEM memory system development of 196-200, 199f
222f, 223 infant modifications of
discrete-trials procedure, in operant between associations 204-211,
conditioning 110 206f-209f
discrimination tasks, see concurrent for associations 203-204
discrimination task, somesthetic for object functions 202-203
discrimination task, visual discrimina- structural descriptions system in
tion tasks 201-202
dissociation, see memory dissociations memory subsystems of 201-211
distractors, in transfer-appropriate semantic memory distinction from
processing 218-220, 219f 196-200, 225
DMS, see delayed-matching-to-sample test model for 24, 27
task evolution theory, of multiple memory
DNMS, see delayed-nonmatching-to- systems 194-195, 213
sample task examining, in MEM memory system
dualism 20-21 222f, 223
executive, in MEM memory system 222f
E experiences, previous
elaborative encoding 76, 169 age-related differences in 75, 118
emotional memory 56, 57f, 197 in explicit memory, see conscious
emotional response, auditory condi- awareness
tioned, of nonhumans 68-72, 69t, 70f in implicit memory 9, 232
enabling events, in imitation tasks 115 infants’ ability to recall 118-125, 120f
encoding, initial MEM memory system and 229
amnesics’ capability for 8, 128 response latency and 142-143, 143f
elaborative 76, 169 storage of, see episodic memory
memory development and system
context and 183 transfer-appropriate processing and
implicit component 24-25 215
in infants 76, 129, 148 experimental memory dissociations
level of processing in 168-171, 170f in adults versus infants 127-129, see
orthographic 76, 168-169 also adult memory, infant memory
phonemic 168 processing accounts for 213-214, 214t
transfer-appropriate processing and multiple-entry modular memory
215-217, 226 system 221-230, 222f
engram 98 transfer-appropriate 214-221, 219f-
entorhinal cortex 220f
damage to, memory impact of 52, 53f rule of 7-8, 127-128
spatial arrangement of 51, 51f variables in, see variables
environment, as memory variable 182- explicit memory
184 access to 10-11, 142-143, 212, 239
characteristics of 2-3, 8-11, 54
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
308 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
SUBJECT INDEX 309
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
310 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
SUBJECT INDEX 311
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
312 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
SUBJECT INDEX 313
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
314 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
SUBJECT INDEX 315
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
316 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
SUBJECT INDEX 317
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
318 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
SUBJECT INDEX 319
reliance, in MEM memory system 227 term, for preverbal infants 134-135
remembering, knowing versus 9-10 retrieval intentionality criterion 18-19
reperceiving, retrieval as 217 in children’s memory studies 76
repetition priming tasks, see priming in infants’ memory studies 129, 140
tasks retrieving, in MEM memory system
representation, capacity for 1, 213 222f, 223
representation systems retroactive interference 153-154, 154f
for memory research 191-192, 213 retrograde amnesia 41
perceptual 26-27, 201-202 in humans 30, 72
research, see memory research in nonhuman animals 41, 42f, 43
resolving, in MEM memory system 221, rhinal cortex
222f damage to, memory impact of 52, 53f
response-to-novelty task, for preverbal spatial arrangement of 51, 51f
infants 98-101, 99f, 123, 249 visual memory tasks and 44, 49f
independent variables of 150, 161, rotary pursuit task, MTL lesions and 31,
178 32f, 56
parallels with amnesics 123, 126t rule of experimental memory dissocia-
retention, see also forgetting, memory tions 7-8, 127-128
active memory and
novel priming impact on 242-245, S
243f sadness, as memory variable 172-174,
priming effects of 239-241, 240f 173f
in classical conditioning 15 scopolamine, for amnesia 14
infants’ capacity for 2, 103-104, 112, selective attention, age impact on 149-
137 150
amnesics’ capacity versus 122-123 self-knowing consciousness 16
duration of 234-235, 235f, 239-240 semantic memory, memory subsystems
independent variables of 143-186, of 201-211
144t semantic memory system, see also
magnitude of 234 implicit memory
renewed 142-143, 143f, 157, 218- context variable of 183
219, 219f definition of 3, 9
MTL lesions and 31-35, 32f-33f development of 196-200, 199f
postoperative 38, 41, 42f, 43 episodic memory distinction from
retention interval 196-200, 225
as dissociation variable 150-153, 152f, infant modifications of 201
180-182 between associations 204-211,
memory performance per 24, 40, 234 206f-209f
infants’ generalization and 198, for associations 203-204
199f for object functions 202-203
in preverbal infants 104-106, 105f, structural descriptions system in
116-117, 133, 133f 201-202
retention ratio, in preverbal infant tests level of processing and 168-169
135 subsystem of 27
retention tests, immediate versus long- test model for 24, 143
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
320 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
SUBJECT INDEX 321
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
322 THE DEVELOPMENT OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY
in preverbal infants 98-99, 123, word form system, implicit memory and
129-130 26-27
neural mediation of 48-51, 49f, 52, 63, word-fragment completion task
90 amnesics’ performance on 34-35
vulnerability, as dissociation variable as memory task 8, 13, 19-20, 250
153-155, 154f children’s studies on 76
functional imaging of 59
W independence in 22-23
what-is-it reflex 109 independent variables of 150-151,
win-shift strategy, for radial arm maze 162, 169, 172, 175-176, 183
60-62 visual stimuli in 26-27
win-stay strategy, for radial arm maze word-identification task, for memory
60-62 performance 25-27
wind chime tasks, for preverbal infants independent variables of 168
120f, 121-122
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
www.Ebook777.com
Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com
28. ZACHAR, Peter: Psychological Concepts and Biological Psychiatry. A philosophical analy-
sis. 2000.
29. VAN LOOCKE, Philip (ed.): The Physical Nature of Consciousness. 2001.
30. BROOK, Andrew and Richard C. DeVIDI (eds.): Self-awareness and Self-reference. n.y.p.
31. RAKOVER, Sam S. and Baruch CAHLON: Face Recognition. Cognitive and computa-
tional processes. n.y.p.
32. VITIELLO, Giuseppe: My Double Unveiled. The dissipative quantum model of the brain.
n.y.p.
33. YASUE, Kunio, Mari JIBU and Tarcisio DELLA SENTA (eds.): No Matter, Never Mind.
Proceedings of Toward a Science of Consciousness: fundamental approaches, Tokyo 1999.
n.y.p.
34. FETZER, James H.(ed.): Consciousness Evolving. n.y.p.
35. Mc KEVITT, Paul, Sean O’NUALLAIN and Conn Mulvihill (eds.): Language, Vision,
and Music. Selected papers from the 8th International Workshop on the Cognitive Science of
Natural Language Processing, Galway, 1999. n.y.p.
www.Ebook777.com