Advanced Syntax
Advanced Syntax
Wh-Movement is often analyzed within the framework of syntactic transformations, such as the
movement of the wh-word to a higher position called the specifier of a specialized interrogative
phrase. The precise mechanisms and constraints of Wh-Movement have been extensively studied by
linguists to explain question formation in various languages.
Subject-Verb Inversion: Inverted word order in sentences like "Rarely does he study" or "Not only did
she sing, but she also danced." The subject (NP) moves from its base position after the auxiliary verb
to a higher position for a particular syntactic or pragmatic effect.
Raising: The subject NP of a subordinate clause moves to a higher position within the main clause.
For example, in the sentence "He seems to be happy," the subject "he" moves from the embedded
clause to the subject position of the main clause.
Control Constructions: In sentences like "She wants to eat ice cream," the subject of the embedded
clause "she" moves to a higher position in the main clause, where it becomes the subject of the
matrix verb "wants."
1)
Generative grammar is a linguistic framework that originated in the mid-20th century, primarily
associated with Noam Chomsky and his influential work. It focuses on developing formal models to
describe the structure and generative capacity of natural languages. Here's an overview of generative
grammar, including its history, conception of language, goals, and major concepts:
History:
Conception of Language:
Generative grammar views language as a creative and rule-governed system. It posits that native
speakers have an innate language faculty, a set of principles and parameters that allow them to
generate an infinite number of grammatically correct sentences. Language is seen as a mental
capacity separate from other cognitive faculties.
Goals:
Explanatory Adequacy: To explain how speakers acquire and use language. Generative grammar aims
to uncover the cognitive mechanisms that enable language production and comprehension.
Predictive Power: To make accurate predictions about grammatical patterns and judgments, and to
account for linguistic variation and language change.
Phrase Structure: Generative grammar employs phrase structure rules to describe the hierarchical
organization of words into phrases and sentences. These rules outline the constituent structure of a
sentence, specifying the relationships between words and phrases.
Universal Grammar: Generative grammar assumes the existence of Universal Grammar, a set of
innate linguistic principles shared by all humans. Universal Grammar provides the framework for
acquiring and generating language and accounts for the common structural properties observed
across languages.
Principles and Parameters: Generative grammar proposes that languages exhibit a core set of
principles that are common to all languages, along with parameter settings that vary across
languages. Parameters determine specific grammatical properties, such as word order, case marking,
or agreement patterns.
Minimalist Program: The Minimalist Program is a framework within generative grammar that seeks
to minimize the complexity of linguistic representations and operations. It emphasizes economy and
simplicity in generating grammatical structures.
Generative grammar has undergone significant developments and refinements over the years,
resulting in various sub-frameworks and theoretical models. It has greatly influenced the field of
linguistics and continues to be a vibrant and evolving approach to studying the structure and nature
of human language.
The subsystems of UG can vary depending on the specific framework within generative grammar, but
here are some commonly recognized subsystems:
Phonological Component: This subsystem deals with the organization and representation of sounds
in language. It includes rules or constraints governing the sound patterns, phonemes, and
phonological processes specific to a particular language.
Morphological Component: The morphological component deals with the structure and formation of
words. It encompasses the rules and principles that govern the internal structure of words, including
inflectional and derivational processes. Morphological analysis focuses on morphemes, the smallest
meaningful units in language.
Syntactic Component: The syntactic component is concerned with the organization and structure of
sentences and phrases. It deals with the rules and principles that govern the formation and
arrangement of words and phrases to create grammatically well-formed sentences. It includes
phrase structure rules, transformations, and the hierarchical organization of constituents.
Semantic Component: The semantic component is responsible for the interpretation and meaning of
words, phrases, and sentences. It deals with the principles and processes involved in assigning
meaning to linguistic expressions and how they relate to the world.
Pragmatic Component: The pragmatic component focuses on the use of language in context,
including principles of conversation, speech acts, implicature, and the social and cultural aspects of
communication. It deals with how context influences the interpretation and use of language.
the DP hypothesis
Null constituents
X-bar theory is a key component of phrase structure within generative grammar. It provides a
framework for representing the hierarchical structure of phrases. According to X-bar theory, phrases
consist of a head and optional specifier and complement positions. The head is typically a lexical
element (e.g., a noun, verb, or adjective), and the specifier and complement positions provide
additional information or arguments.
The basic structure of X-bar theory involves three levels: X', XP, and X. The X' level represents the
phrase, XP represents the specifier and complement positions, and X represents the head. For
example, in a noun phrase (NP), the structure would be [NP [Specifier] [X' [X Noun] [Complement]]].
X-bar Theory and Clause Structure:
X-bar theory is also applicable to clause structure. In a basic sentence, the structure can be
represented as [CP [C'] [IP]], where CP represents the complementizer phrase, C' represents the head
of the CP (usually a complementizer), and IP represents the inflectional phrase, which contains the
subject and the verb.
Functional categories are syntactic elements that serve specific grammatical functions in clauses.
Examples of functional categories include complementizers (e.g., "that," "if"), tense markers (e.g.,
"will," "did"), and determiners (e.g., "the," "this"). These functional categories are often associated
with specific positions in the clause structure, such as the complementizer phrase (CP) or inflectional
phrase (IP).
The DP Hypothesis:
The DP hypothesis proposes that noun phrases (NPs) in many languages have a universal underlying
structure in which a determiner (D) phrase occupies the specifier position of the noun phrase. This
hypothesis suggests that noun phrases consist of a determiner (e.g., "the," "my") and a noun head,
with optional modifiers or complements.
Null Constituents:
Null constituents, also known as empty categories or proforms, are syntactic elements that are
present in the underlying structure but have no overt phonological realization. They can occur in
different positions within a sentence, such as null subjects (pro-drop) or null objects. Null
constituents are found in languages with certain syntactic properties, such as pro-drop languages like
Spanish or Italian, where the subject pronoun can be omitted.
Movement refers to the displacement of an element from its base position to another position within
a sentence. It is a fundamental aspect of generative grammar. There are different types of
movement:
Verb Movement: Verb movement, also known as verb raising, involves the movement of a verb from
its base position to a higher position within the verb phrase (VP) or to a higher functional projection.
XP Movement: XP Movement involves the movement of a phrase (XP) to a different position within a
sentence, which can include movements like topicalization or fronting.
_________________
Wh-movement in direct and indirect wh-questions + linguistic arguments for this operation and its
wh-trace
Wh-movement is a syntactic operation that occurs in both direct and indirect wh-questions. Let's
explore the characteristics of wh-movement in these question types and discuss some linguistic
arguments supporting this operation and its associated wh-trace.
Direct Wh-Questions:
Direct wh-questions involve the fronting of a wh-phrase to the beginning of a sentence. For example:
In direct wh-questions, the wh-phrase moves from its base position within the sentence to a higher
position, typically the specifier position of a dedicated interrogative phrase (CP). This movement is
motivated by the need to position the wh-phrase in a prominent position at the beginning of the
sentence to indicate that it is being questioned.
a. Islands and Constraints: Some languages have island constraints that restrict the movement of wh-
phrases across certain boundaries, such as relative clauses or embedded clauses. The existence of
these island constraints suggests that movement of wh-phrases is a grammatical operation, as it is
subject to syntactic restrictions.
Indirect Wh-Questions:
Indirect wh-questions involve the use of an introductory clause or phrase, often with a
complementizer, followed by an embedded question. For example:
"He asked me what you ate." (The embedded question "what you ate" is introduced by "what.")
In indirect wh-questions, the wh-phrase remains in its base position within the embedded clause and
does not undergo movement to a higher position. Instead, it is introduced by a complementizer, such
as "what," "whether," or "if."
a. Absence of Surface Movement: Unlike direct wh-questions, indirect wh-questions do not exhibit
surface movement of the wh-phrase. The wh-phrase remains in its base position within the
embedded clause, indicating that wh-movement does not occur.
b. Intact Binding Relations: Indirect wh-questions maintain intact binding relations between the wh-
phrase and other pronouns or reflexives within the embedded clause. This suggests that the wh-
phrase does not move and remains in its original position, allowing for proper interpretation and
binding.
Wh-Trace:
When wh-movement occurs, it leaves behind a trace, often referred to as a wh-trace. The wh-trace
represents the original position from which the wh-phrase moved. Linguistic arguments for the
existence of wh-traces include the detection of syntactic constraints on movement and the ability to
refer to the original position from which the wh-phrase was extracted.
_____________________________
Covert wh-movement
Wh-movement is not typically observed in relative clauses. Instead, relative clauses involve a
different type of syntactic operation known as "head movement" or "head internalization." In
relative clauses, the head of the relative phrase, such as a noun or a preposition, moves to a position
within the relative clause, forming a syntactic dependency with the relativizer (e.g., "that," "who,"
"which"). This head movement is distinct from wh-movement, which involves the movement of wh-
phrases (e.g., "who," "what," "where") to a higher position in the sentence.
However, wh-movement does occur in other wh-constructions, such as interrogative clauses and
certain types of embedded questions. These include direct wh-questions, indirect wh-questions, and
certain types of free relative clauses. In these constructions, the wh-phrase undergoes movement to
a higher position, typically to the specifier position of a dedicated interrogative phrase (CP), enabling
it to appear at the beginning of the sentence or clause.
Covert wh-movement refers to cases where wh-movement occurs in the syntax but does not result in
a surface overt wh-phrase. This can happen in languages that allow wh-phrases to move covertly,
leaving behind a trace or a phonetically empty position. The covertly moved wh-phrase still
influences the interpretation and structure of the sentence but does not have a visible presence in
the sentence's surface form.
Long distance or cyclic wh-movement refers to cases where a wh-phrase moves across multiple
intervening elements or clauses. It is observed when the wh-phrase undergoes successive cyclic
movement, moving to intermediate positions before reaching its final destination. This type of wh-
movement is motivated by the necessity to obey island constraints and other syntactic restrictions.
Wh-movement, island constraints, and the subjacency condition are interconnected concepts in
generative grammar. Island constraints are syntactic constraints that restrict the movement of wh-
phrases across certain boundaries, such as embedded clauses, relative clauses, or coordinate
structures. The subjacency condition is a principle that captures the generalization that movement
across multiple clauses or intervening elements is limited and subject to certain syntactic constraints.
It helps explain why long distance or cyclic wh-movement is restricted and why certain constructions
create islands that block the movement of wh-phrases.
__________________________________
A-movement and all the issues concerning it (all the linguistic constructions involving it, arguments
supporting this operation, long distance A-movement, A-movement and bounding theory, interaction
between A-movement and wh-movement..)
A-movement, also known as argument movement, refers to the movement of arguments (noun
phrases) within a sentence to a different position. Let's explore the various aspects and issues
concerning A-movement:
Passives: In passive constructions, the object of an active sentence moves to the subject position in
the passive sentence. For example, in the sentence "John was given a book," the object noun phrase
"a book" moves from its base position to the subject position.
Raising: Raising constructions involve the movement of a verb's subject from an embedded clause to
a higher position in the main clause.
In this sentence, the subject noun phrase "John" undergoes raising. The verb "seems" is a raising verb
that allows the subject to move from its base position within the embedded clause "John is a
talented musician" to the subject position of the main clause.
The subject "John" has been raised from the embedded clause to the subject position of the main
clause, indicated by the square brackets. The verb "seems" now agrees with the raised subject "John"
in terms of person and number. The raised subject "John" is interpreted as the entity that appears to
possess the quality or attribute described by the complement "to be a talented musician."
Control Constructions: Control constructions involve the movement of a pronominal subject from an
embedded clause to the subject position of the main clause. For example, in the sentence "John
wants [himself] to leave," the subject "John" moves from the embedded clause to the subject
position of the main clause.
Semantic Interpretation: A-movement often results in changes in the interpretation and scope of
noun phrases, indicating that their movement has semantic consequences. For example, in passives,
the object's movement to the subject position affects the interpretation of the sentence.
Binding Theory: The binding theory, which deals with the distribution and interpretation of pronouns
and anaphors, provides evidence for A-movement. It establishes that anaphors must be bound by a
suitable antecedent in a higher clause, supporting the notion of movement to establish the binding
relationship.
Long distance A-movement refers to cases where an argument moves across multiple clauses or
intervening elements. It is observed in constructions such as raising and control structures. Long
distance A-movement is subject to syntactic constraints and locality conditions, similar to long
distance wh-movement.
Bounding theory, proposed by Chomsky, explores the limitations on movement in terms of bounding
conditions. According to bounding theory, certain syntactic islands restrict the movement of
arguments. These islands include adjunct clauses, relative clauses, coordinate structures, and
embedded interrogatives. A-movement is subject to these island constraints, as it cannot freely occur
across such boundaries.
A-movement and wh-movement can interact in various ways within a sentence. They can occur
independently, but they can also interact when a wh-phrase and an argument move simultaneously.
The interaction between A-movement and wh-movement can result in complex sentence structures
where both types of movement play a role in forming grammatically well-formed sentences. The
details of this interaction are studied within the framework of generative grammar.