Itl Handouts 1 Final
Itl Handouts 1 Final
It is said that the first language (L1) is acquired, with the acquisition happening naturally,
as the child imbibes and infers the patterns and rules of the language as he or she is exposed to it
and experiments with its use. On the other hand, the second language (L2) and languages taught
in school or in any other structured environment, are learned instead of acquired.
LANGUAGE ACQUISITION
Children acquire a language that is normally spoken in in the surrounding community.
Acquisition takes place naturally in communicative situations with others who know the language.
Acquisition is relatively faster; it takes place with no language system in the background.
LANGUAGE LEARNING
Adults learn a language that is NOT normally spoken in the surrounding community.
Learning is a more conscious process of accumulating knowledge or language in an institutional
setting.
Learning is slower; it takes place against an already acquired language system.
The ability to produce speech sounds starts with the onset of babbling, when a baby is
around six months old. During this babbling stage, when the sounds they produce (usually
"bababa," "dada," and "mammam") children experiment and practice their vocal abilities, which is
important for later speech development. Before this stage, babies' sounds are mostly crying and
cooing (zero to two months), which are followed by other simple sounds like gurgling and laughing,
and even combinations of vowels and consonants ("ga" and "goo," for example). They will also
establish eye contact and make gestures before the babbling stage.
During the babbling stage, babies will also copy some of the sounds and gestures that their
caregivers and other adults make like coughing, laughing, clicking or "making raspberries" (making
the farting sound from the mouth), as well as waving, pointing or clapping. They also play with
different sounds like "aaieee," "booo," and "ahh" at different pitches and volumes. Furthermore,
they will put sounds together with rhythm and tone, in ways that sound like normal speech. This is
sometimes called the "jargon phase" that can continue even after the production of first intelligible
words.
A significant development at this stage is that babies now use intelligible sounds to convey
their own needs and wants and have very specific and appropriate reactions when communicating
with others. For instance, aside from producing actual words to convey their desire for milk or be
carried, or to indicate that they are uncomfortable, they will show excitement or become quiet (and
seem to listen intently) when talked to or when they hear their name. They also smile and play with
others, enjoy games like peekaboo or point-to, and so on. Caregivers, parents, and other family
members often respond by using 'Baby Talk' to adjust their speech to the children's still-developing
competence of their first language (L1). Babbling will continue until the age of about 12 months,
when children will be able to produce intelligible words. By their eighteenth month, children will
normally have acquired at least fifty words, and they will begin to adopt regular patterns of
pronunciation. According to Archibald O'Grady, Mark Aronoff, and Janie Rees- Miller in
Contemporary Linguistics; An Introduction (2017), 'children adopt three strategies in learning the
meanings of new words: the "whole object" assumption, where a new word refers to a whole
object; the "type" assumption, where a new word refers to a type of thing, not just a particular
thing; and the "basic level" assumption where a new word refers to types of objects that are alike
in basic ways.
Children acquiring L1 generally make errors in meaning in the form of overextensions and
overextensions. Overextension is when a child uses a more specific word to substitute for a word
with a less specific meaning (calling all eating utensils "spoon" for instance), which happens
because children are recognizing similarities among objects and concepts and are in the process
of refining their understanding of meanings. Under-extensions occur when a child uses a more
general word to refer to something more specific, such as calling a dog "doggie" but not accepting
that other dogs are also "doggie." Other general patterns in L1 development that children manifest
in their development are: the frequent occurrence in utterance in the final position (for example, it
is the last thing they hear that they will remember most, so when you give them a choice, they will
choose the last thing you mentioned); syllabicity, for example -ing and 's; and a few or no
exceptions in the way grammar rules are applied (for example, they will add the 's' to everything to
make it plural, even in the case of irregular nouns: mouses, child’s, and so on).
The acquisition of their L1 continues to proceed rapidly in the holophrastic stage, when
they utter single words. At around 18 to 24 months, they can combine words in two-word stages.
At around two years of age and shortly afterward, they move into the telegraphic stage, which is
when they can produce a clear phrase structure with head-complement and subject-VP patterns.
As the children's physical growth proceeds and their bodies mature, so too, does their linguistic
ability develop, such that they expand their vocabularies and understanding of the relationship
between language and their immediate social surroundings, thus enabling them to internalize
more complex linguistic structures.
In neurolinguistics, acquisition and learning are said to undergo different processes in the
brain. The two main areas in the brain (there are many) where these processes take place are the
following:
1. BROCA'S AREA is found in the left frontal cortex and functions as the word-production
center of the brain. This means that it is responsible for the production of patterns in vocal and
sign language.
2. WERNICKE's AREA, found in the left temporal cortex, functions as the word-recognition
center, which is primarily involved in language acquisition.
To learn more about how the brain works in language acquisition and learning, you may
want to look for the video of Linguists Online on "Language and Literacy Development: Global and
Local Issues" featuring Kenneth Pugh of Yale University and Arturo E. Hernandez of the University
of Houston, which was streamed live on June 19, 2020.
The focus is on spoken language, with the primary medium of language being oral, and so
the oral aspect should be prioritized in language teaching.
The focus of language learning is on the formation of habit based on conditioned reflexes.
This habit is based on conditioning from the stimulus-response (S-R) chain: the stimulus
elicits a response, and each response is provoked by the stimulus.
Role of social Primary role: parental Minor role: language Important role in
environment modeling and used by others merely interaction, especially
reinforcement are triggers LAD in early years when
major factors caregivers modify
promoting language input and carry much
acquisition of conversational load
INSTRUCTIONAL BEHAVIORIST INNATIST INTERACTIONIST
COMPONENTS
Source of linguistic Language dialogues Natural language from Natural language from
input and drills from teacher the teacher, friends, or the teacher, friends,
or audiotape books books
Nature of input Structured by Unstructured, but Unstructured, but
grammatical made comprehensible focused on
complexity by teacher communication
between learner and
others
Ideal All Target language Target language Native speakers
classroom learners of similar learners of similar together with target
composition second language second language language learners for
proficiency proficiency so I + 1 social interaction
can be achieved aimed at
communication
Student output Structured repetitions Output is not a Speaking occurs
and grammar pattern concern; it will occur naturally in
drill responses naturally. communication with
others.
Pressure to speak Students repeat "Silent period" No pressure to speak
immediately. expected except natural
impulse to
communicate
Treatment of errors Errors are corrected Errors are not Errors that impede
immediately. corrected; students communication will be
will correct corrected naturally as
themselves with time. meaning is
negotiated; some
errors may require
explicit corrective
instruction.
The first four macro skills are self-explanatory; even students in the elementary levels know
what these are. What about the fifth and sixth?
Viewing and Representing
We now live in a highly visual world. There is a saying, in social media, that "if there are no
pictures, then it did not happen." There is much greater reliance on the image rather than the
word. The digital revolution that gave us the internet and hand-held devices that combine functions
that used to need separate equipment (phone, calendar, email, directory, compass, calculator,
maps, etc.) have made visual content ubiquitous: via video-sharing on social media networks. All
these have made visual communication, especially the moving image, the primary mode of
communication around the world. Think about how many times you have received a photo instead
of words as a response to something you asked; it becomes up to you to interpret the message
based on the visual image. Most of the texts that circulate today are multimodal, which means that
the communicated messages are not confined to just one mode- whether written text, audio, still
pictures, moving pictures, a gesture, use of space, etc. Digital multimodal texts come in the form of
videos, slideshows and web pages, while live multimodal texts can include the more traditional
theatre, storytelling, and dance.
"Literacy" in the twenty-first century no longer means what it meant before the advent of
digital technology; it has moved away from print- dominated texts to the new digital text mode, and
from the oral-aural medium of teacher-led lectures and discussions in the face-to-face classroom
to the computer monitor and gadget screen. These visual texts, like their traditional counterparts
delivered via the print and oral-aural channels, have many functions and serve different purposes
relevant to the audiences that use them in many different contexts in contemporary life.
To learn more about viewing as a macro skill, you may want to check out online videos and
articles on what is being called "the fifth skill."
Representing" as a macro skill includes the abilities to create their own presentations and
messages using digital and/or multi-modal texts to convey their ideas, in ways not encompassed
in traditional print and exclusively oral channels, combining and linking information in spatial and
temporal forms via different modes.
Do you understand the logic in dividing the six macro skills into two categories?
Listening, reading, and viewing are comprehending or receptive skills, whereas
Speaking, writing, and representing are productive or expressive skills.
The first four are macro skills of language, and the added fifth and sixth are communication
macro-skills that involve not just language but other communication strategies (such as visuals)
as well.
Communicative Competence
When we speak of "Communicative Competence" (CC), we are talking about the kind
of knowledge that speakers and hearers need to have to successfully communicate in different
contexts. It was the innatism Noam Chomsky who articulated a clear distinction between
'competence' and 'performance,' but it was the sociolinguist Dell Hymes who developed the
communicative competence model. His model describes and account for the knowledge that
speakers and listeners have that enables them to communicate appropriately (i.e., convey and
interpret messages correctly') in varied social contexts via negotiation of meaning. Hymes'
communicative competence model is a central notion in sociolinguistics and other socially oriented
approaches to the study of language. How did this develop?
In Chomsky's construct, competence pertains to mastery or knowledge of syntax
rules and other language principles (usage), with an "ideal speaker and listener," while
performance refers to the language user's actual application (use) of language in specific contexts.
Chomsky posited that "an ideal speaker-listener" has completely mastered the
language that is spoken in his or her speech community. According to him, "every speaker of a
language has mastered and internalized a generative grammar" that shows their knowledge of
that language (Chomsky 1965).
Is the process of communication limited to knowing how to create well- structured
sentences, then? Is grammatic-linguistic competence the goal of language learning? To scholars
like Dell Hymes, who developed the notion of competence further to include aspects other than
linguistic competence, the answer to both questions is "No."
Hymes took the notion of competence further by saying that there is such a thing as
"appropriateness" in language use depending on context. To Hymes, Chomsky's concept is
incomplete because of "absence of a place for sociocultural factors and the linking of performance
to imperfection' (1972). Instead of just the 'ideal speaker' and listener equipped with competence,
Hymes' construct includes non-native speakers and L2 learners. To him, knowledge of usage rules
would be useless without a grasp of and a feel for language use (1972).
In the same vein, Canale and Swain (1980) stated that there are language rules that
would be useless without grammar rules, and so thus defined communicative competence as "the
relationship and interaction between grammatical competence and knowledge of the rules of
grammar and sociolinguistic competence" (or knowledge of the rules of language use). They also
say that communicative these competencies interact in the comprehension and production of
utterances (1980). There are four elements in their definition of communicative competence.