memory-and-age
memory-and-age
{B} Equipped with imaging techniques that capture the brain in action, Stanley Rapoport,
Ph.D., at the National Institutes of Health, measured the flow of blood in the brains of old
and young people as they went through the task of matching photos of faces. Since blood
flow reflects neuronal activity, Rapoport could compare which networks of neurons were
being used by different subjects. “Even when the reaction times of older and younger
subjects were the same, the neural networks they used were significantly different. The
older subjects were using different internal strategies to accomplish the same result in the
same time,” Rapoport says. Either the task required greater effort on the part of the older
subjects or the work of neurons originally involved in tasks of that type had been taken over
by other neurons, creating different networks.
{D} “When a rat is kept in isolation without playmates or objects to interact with, the
animal’s brain shrinks, but if we put that rat with 11 other rats in a large cage and give them
an assortment of wheels, ladders, and other toys, we can show–after four days– significant
differences in its brain,” says Diamond, professor of integrative biology. Proliferating
dendrites first appear in the visual association areas. After a month in the enriched
environment, the whole cerebral cortex has expanded, as has its blood supply. Even in the
enriched environment, rats get bored unless the toys are varied. “Animals are just like we
are. They need stimulation,” says Diamond.
{E} One of the most profoundly important mental functions is memory-notorious for its
failure with age. So important is memory that the Charles A. Dana Foundation recently
spent $8.4 million to set up a consortium of leading medical centers to measure memory
{F} When you forget a less vivid item, like buying a roll of paper towels at the supermarket,
you may blame it on your aging memory. It’s true that episodic memory begins to decline
when most people are in their 50s, but it’s never perfect at any age. “Every memory begins
as an event,” says Bahrick. “Through repetition, certain events leave behind a residue of
knowledge, or semantic memory. On a specific day in the past, somebody taught you that
two and two are four, but you’ve been over that information so often you don’t remember
where you learned it. What started as an episodic memory has become a permanent part
of your knowledge base.” You remember the content, not the context. Our language
knowledge, our knowledge of the world and of people, is largely that permanent or
semipermanent residue.
{G} Probing the longevity of knowledge, Bahrick tested 1,000 high school graduates to see
how well they recalled their algebra. Some had completed the course as recently as a
month before, others as long as 50 years earlier. He also determined how long each
person had studied algebra, the grade received, and how much the skill was used over the
course of adulthood. Surprisingly, a person’s grasp of algebra at the time of testing did not
depend on how long ago he’d taken the course–the determining factor was the duration of
instruction. Those who had spent only a few months learning algebra forgot most of it
within two or three years.
{H} In another study, Bahrick discovered that people who had taken several courses in
Spanish, spread out over a couple of years, could recall, decades later, 60 percent or more
of the vocabulary they learned. Those who took just one course retained only a trace after
three years. “This long-term residue of knowledge remains stable over the decades,
independent of the age of the person and the age of the memory. No serious deficit
appears until people get to their 50s and 60s, probably due to the degenerative processes
of aging rather than a cognitive loss.”
{I} “You could say metamemory is a byproduct of going to school,” says psychologist
Robert Kail, Ph.D., of Purdue University, who studies children from birth to 20 years, the
time of life when mental development is most rapid. “The question-and-answer process,
especially exam-taking, helps children learn–and also teaches them how their memory
works This may be one reason why, according to a broad range of studies in people over
60, the better educated a person is, the more likely they are to perform better in life and on
psychological tests. A group of adult novice chess players were compared with a group of
child experts at the game. In tests of their ability to remember a random series of numbers,
the adults, as expected, outscored the children. But when asked to remember the patterns
of chess pieces arranged on a board, the children won. “Because they’d played a lot of
chess, their knowledge of chess was better organized than that of the adults, and their
existing knowledge of chess served as a framework for new memory,” explains Kail.
Question 4. What is the author’s purpose of using “vocabulary study” at the end of the
passage?
Questions 5-10
Complete the following summary of the paragraphs of Reading Passage, using no more
Questions 11-14
Use the information in the passage to match the people (listed A-F) with opinions or deeds
below. Write the appropriate letters A-F in boxes 11-14 on your answer sheet.
11..................... Examined both young and old’s blood circulation of brain while testing,