Detailed Notes Clinical
Detailed Notes Clinical
Topic 5: Clinical
Psychology
Notes
www.pmt.education
Part 1 — Schizophrenia:
• Schizophrenia is defined as 1“a psychotic disorder marked by severely impaired thinking,
emotions, and behaviours. Schizophrenic patients are typically unable to filter sensory stimuli
and may have enhanced perceptions of sounds, colours, and other features of their
environment”.
• Sufferers may experience either or both positive and negative symptoms.
• Positive symptoms enhance the typical experience of sufferers, and occur in addition to their
normal experiences e.g. hallucinations and delusions.
• Negative symptoms take away from the typical experience of sufferers, and so represents a
‘loss’ of experience e.g. speech poverty and avolition.
• Hallucinations = A positive symptom of schizophrenia, which is characterised by a distorted
view/perception of real stimuli or perceptions of stimuli which have no basis in reality. Auditory
hallucinations may involve hallucinating the voices of loved ones or the deceased and are, for
example, thought to be caused by an excess of dopamine receptors in Broca’s area (a neural
correlate).
• Delusions = A positive symptom of schizophrenia and are a set of beliefs with no basis in reality
at all e.g. the sufferer may be paranoid that they are being stalked by the Royal Family. Different
types of delusions include persecutory, delusions of grandeur, delusional jealousy, erotonomania
and somatic delusional disorders.
• Speech poverty = A negative symptom of schizophrenia which occurs when there is an
abnormally low level of the frequency and quality of speech. A common type of speech poverty
is ‘derailment’, which is thought to be caused by dysfunctions in central control (Frith et al,
1992) and so the sufferer cannot suppress the automatic associations that come with each new
word or idea.
• Avolition = 2 “A subjective reduction in interests, desires and goals and a behavioural reduction
of self-initiated and purposeful acts, including motivational deficits”. Therefore, avolition means
the inability to cope with the normal pressures and motivations associated with everyday living
and day-to-day tasks.
• There are two types of classification systems for mental disorders: The Diagnostic and
Statistical Manual (currently the DSM-V) and the International Classification of Disease (currently
the ICD-10). These two systems have different requirements for the diagnosis of schizophrenia.
Despite both requiring persistence of symptoms for at least 1 month, the DSM-V has more
specific diagnostic criteria and so requires at least 2 or more of delusions, hallucinations,
disorganized speech and catononic behaviour, whereas the ICD-10 takes a broader approach to
diagnosis, simply stating that “the clinical picture is dominated by relatively stable, often
paranoid delusions, usually accompanied by hallucinations”.
• Therefore, the main differences between the DSM and the ICD is in terms of what organisations
produces them (the WHO or the American Psychiatric Association), the number of symptoms
and specificity of symptoms required for diagnosis, as well as the recognition of different
subtypes of schizophrenia.
• There are different subtypes of schizophrenia. For example, 3 positive schizophrenia is seen as
having the symptoms of prominent delusions, hallucinations and positive formal thought
disorders. On the other hand, in mixed schizophrenia, the prominent symptoms are either both
negative and positive, or neither is prominent. Subtypes are currently recognised in the ICD-10
only, whereas previous editions of the DSM also made these distinctions.
1
Schizophrenia. (n.d.) Gale Encyclopedia of Medicine. (2008). Retrieved August 8 2017
from http://medical-dictionary.thefreedictionary.com/schizophrenia
2Messinger JW, Trémeau F, Antonius D, et al. Avolition and expressive deficits capture negative
symptom phenomenology: Implications for DSM-5 and schizophrenia research. Clinical
psychology review. 2011;31(1):161-168. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2010.09.002.
3
Andreasen NC, Olsen S. Negative v Positive SchizophreniaDefinition and Validation. Arch Gen
Psychiatry.1982;39(7):789–794. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.1982.04290070025006
www.pmt.education
Particularly in the case of depression, this suggests that if schizophrenia is so frequently
diagnosed with other psychiatric disorders, then these two disorders may actually be the same,
and so a more accurate and valid method of diagnosis would be to combine these two. Therefore,
there are issues of validity in the diagnosis of SZ and attempting to differentiate its symptoms
from that of other disorders.
— A second type of bias which may reduce the validity of the diagnosis of SZ is the problem of
gender bias, as suggested by Escobar et al (2012). For example, African Americans are far more
likely to be diagnosed with SZ compared to patients belonging to Western cultures, due to their
increased openness about admitting to certain SZ symptoms which may appear normal in their
respective cultures. For example, the phenomenon of hearing voices may be considered a
desirable sign of increased spirituality and connectedness with ancestors, and so may even be
encouraged. However, both classification systems would view this as a hallmark characteristic of
SZ and, combined with the potential distrust in African Americans that white psychiatrists may
have, could increase the likelihood of false diagnoses.
4
Ripke et al (2013), Genome-wide association analysis identifies 13 new risk loci for
schizophrenia, Nature Genetics 45, pp.1150-1159
www.pmt.education
• The original dopamine hypothesis suggests that hyperdopaminergia (abnormally high dopamine
levels) in the subcortex is responsible for SZ, whereas the revised dopamine hypothesis
suggests that hypodopaminergeria (abnormally low dopamine levels) in the cortex is more likely
to be responsible for SZ. The modern understanding is that both hyper- and hypodopaminergia
in different areas of the brain contribute to the development of SZ.
• For example, hyperdopaminergia in the frontal lobe, and specifically Broca’s area which may
have an excess of D2 receptors, may be responsible for the positive SZ symptom of auditory
hallucinations, due to the overactivity of neurotransmission in the auditory areas of the brain.
• In addition, Goldman Rakic et al (2004) suggested that hypodopaminergia in the prefrontal
cortex may be responsible for negative symptoms of SZ, such as speech poverty and avolition.
This is because the prefrontal cortex is associated with logical thinking, so abnormally low
dopamine levels in this area may impair an individual’s ability to construct grammatical
sentences that are focused upon one topic (speech poverty) or the ability to make decisions
about how to function in day to day living (avolition).
• The dopamine hypothesis has particularly important implications for the development of drug
treatments for SZ, such as antipsychotics/dopamine antagonists.
• Neural correlates = Specific patterns of cortical activity or neural structures which coincide with
specific psychological symptoms, and so are assumed to contribute towards those symptoms.
• Juckel et al (2006) suggested that abnormally low levels of activation in the ventral striatum,
when compared to healthy neurotypical controls, may be associated with the negative symptom
of avolition. This is because the ventral striatum is associated with 5 evaluating reward values,
predictability and risks. Therefore, low levels of activation and neurotransmission may mean that
individuals cannot accurately assess the reward of having enough motivation to carry out
normal day-to-day tasks, and so are therefore unable to cope with ‘normal’ life.
• Allen et al (2007) concluded that 6“the mis-identification of self-generated speech in patients
with auditory verbal hallucinations is associated with functional abnormalities in the anterior
cingulate and left temporal cortex”, as SZ patients’ brain activity was recorded using fMRI
during auditory hallucinations, and compared to a control group who identified pre-recorded
words as their own or not. Therefore, this suggests that speech poverty (a positive symptom)
may be associated with this neural correlate, as shown by the SZ group also making more
mistakes compared to the control group.
+ There is evidence supporting the biological and genetic basis of schizophrenia. For example,
Brown et al (2002) found that the risk of having offspring with SZ increased by over 1.3% if the
father was over 50 years old, compared to if the father was under the age of 25. Therefore, this
suggests that mutations in the sections of DNA containing the candidate genes, such as those
coding for serotonin and dopamine production specifically, means that SZ is likely to have a
strong heritability coefficient and biological basis. This supports the use of family studies and
neural correlates as ways of studying and explaining incidence rates of SZ.
— The evidence for the dopamine hypothesis can be best described as ‘mixed’. On the one hand,
support comes from Tauscher et al (2014) who found that antipsychotics, which act as dopamine
antagonists and so reduce dopamine activity by binding to complementary receptors on the post-
synaptic membrane, alleviated the symptoms of SZ, suggesting that dopamine has a key role in
its development, in line with the predictions of the dopamine hypothesis. On the other hand, some
researchers such as Moghaddam and Javitt (2012) have criticised the dopamine hypothesis and
biological explanations of SZ as emphasising the role of dopamine too far. For example, the
neurotransmitters glutamate and serotonin may also play a key role, as evidenced by the
5Haber SN. Neuroanatomy of Reward: A View from the Ventral Striatum. In: Gottfried JA, editor.
Neurobiology of Sensation and Reward. Boca Raton (FL): CRC Press/Taylor & Francis; 2011.
Chapter 11. Available from: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK92777/
6
Neural correlates of the misattribution of speech in schizophrenia
PAUL ALLEN, EDSON AMARO, CYNTHIA H. Y. FU, STEVEN C. R. WILLIAMS, MICHAEL
J.BRAMMER, LOUISE C. JOHNS, PHILIP K. McGUIRE The British Journal of Psychiatry Feb
2007, 190 (2) 162-169; DOI: 10.1192/bjp.bp.106.025700
www.pmt.education
antipsychotic Clozapine acting upon both of these substances and being more effective than
other atypical antipsychotics in reducing SZ symptoms, as suggested by Meltzer (2012).
— The main issue associated with the use of neural correlates as a means of explaining
schizophrenia is that such evidence is correlational and so does not take into account the ‘third
variable problem’, whereby a third unstudied factor could be affecting both outcomes. Taking the
example of the link between lower levels of activation in the superior temporal gyrus and anterior
cingulate gyrus, and the experience of auditory hallucinations, one explanation would be the
lowered activation levels causing the hallucinations, or the hallucinations themselves causing the
lowered activation levels. A third possible explanation would be the third variable problem.
Therefore, this demonstrates that correlational research cannot be used to reliably demonstrate a
‘cause and effect’ relationship between two variables.
www.pmt.education
abilities, and so often suffer from derailment because they cannot suppress the automatic
associations that each new word in a sentence brings, and so begin to talk off-topic.
+ There is evidence supporting the idea that dysfunctional thought processes are implicated in the
development of schizophrenia, and that faulty central control skills may be responsible for some
SZ symptoms, as demonstrated by Stirling et al (2006). The researchers found that SZ sufferers
made significantly more mistakes and twice as long to complete the task, compared to a
healthy neurotypical control group. However, it should be emphasised that dysfunctional
thought processing can only offer explanations for the indirect, proximal causes of SZ, and not
the distal causes, meaning that such theories can explain the symptoms but not the origin of
SZ. This limits the utility of psychological explanations for schizophrenia.
— A comparison can be made between biological and psychological explanations for SZ. A
significant weakness of psychological explanations for SZ is that they do not accommodate for
biological factors. Since such biological factors can explain the distal origins of schizophrenia (i.e.
in terms of dopamine levels in the brain, candidate genes and patterns of activity coinciding with
symptoms/neural correlates), this suggests that psychological explanations would best be
reserved for the proximal causes of SZ, as these causes are more likely to be most affected by
psychological factors.
— A second major weakness would be the lack of support for family-based explanations. The
idea of the schizophrenogenic mother was based upon historical observations of families with SZ
members, where observers would be searching for ‘crazy-making characteristics’ (Harrington,
2012) which is hardly an objective and reliable indicator of the likelihood of developing SZ.
Psychological explanations also place an increasing amount of blame on the families and
caregivers of patients with SZ, as opposed to accommodating for the possibility of a genetic/
biological predisposition. For example, caregivers/ parents are further hurt when they are forced
to accept responsibility for their patient’s schizophrenia, which is likely to have already upset
family life and relationships through the development of severe and intrusive negative and positive
symptoms. This may explain the sudden popularity of community care in the 1980s, which could
have marked parents refusing to take responsibility for their child’s condition, seeing as they are
so dedicated to their care.
www.pmt.education
— The development of antipsychotics was mainly based upon the dopamine hypothesis, and so
their use depends on this theory too. For example, if antipsychotics appear to alleviate symptoms
by reducing the action of dopamine, this makes sense considering the original dopamine
hypothesis i.e. hyperdopaminergia in the subcortex. However, this action is not in line with the
revised version of the dopamine hypothesis, which suggests that abnormally low levels of
dopamine in the cortex are responsible for symptoms. Therefore, a further reduction in dopamine
levels should make symptoms worse, and not better. This paradox has caused some to question
the validity of the use of antipsychotics, as well as the accuracy of the dopamine hypothesis as an
explanation for schizophrenia.
— A serious consideration which must be made when using antipsychotics is thinking about the
side effects. The short-term side effects of typical antipsychotics are relatively mild (e.g. agitation
and weight gain), whilst the long-term risk include tardive dyskinesia (characterised by involuntary
contraction and relaxation of the facial muscles) and neuroleptic malignant syndrome (NMS).
7NMS is characterised by fever, altered mental states, muscle rigidity and autonomic dysfunction
and is thought to be caused by dopamine receptor blockage or central nervous system infections.
These side effects are not offset by atypical antipsychotics, where agranulocyotis remains a
serious concern for those taking Clozapine, whose state must be continually monitored using
blood tests. Therefore, a cost-benefit analysis should be carried out to consider whether the
benefit of symptom reduction outweighs the cost of side effects for each specific patient.
— Despite there being a range of evidence supporting the use of typical (Thornley et al, 2003) and
atypical antipsychotics (Meltzer, 2012), these studies still suffer from problems of validity, as
suggested by Healy (2012). For example, since Chlorpromazine has particularly powerful sedative
effects, then this suggests that studies reviewing the effectiveness of antipsychotics in terms of
symptom reduction may actually be measuring how calm and functional the patient appears to be
- simply suppressing the symptoms is not a way of controlling them, and so such studies may
lack validity due to not accurately assessing the actual effectiveness of antipsychotics in treating
the proximal cause of SZ. Secondly, drug companies are selective about what type of information
they publish: many focus on the short-term benefits as opposed to the long-term risks, and use
inappropriate control groups, such as patients suffering from withdrawal symptoms as they have
stopped taking their medication. This distorted focus brings into question, yet again, the validity
of research into the effectiveness of antipsychotics.
7
Berman BD. Neuroleptic Malignant Syndrome: A Review for Neurohospitalists. The
Neurohospitalist. 2011;1(1):41-47. doi:10.1177/1941875210386491.
www.pmt.education
• Token economy systems are based upon behaviourist principles and are frequently used in
psychiatric institutions. Target, desirable behaviours are identified by the staff. Every time a
patient displays one of these behaviours, they are rewarded with a token (which acts as a
secondary reinforcer) which can then be exchanged for a reward or privilege (which acts as a
primary reinforcer). Therefore, patients are motivated by the primary reinforcer to carry out the
desirable behaviours, and their frequency of doing so increases as they are positively reinforced.
Rewards may include extra TV time, exercise taken outside of the grounds of the hospitals and
favourite magazines.
— None of the three psychological therapies above actually treat the patient and ‘cure’ their
schizophrenia. Instead, these therapies simply improve their quality of life through making the
symptoms more manageable. For example, token economies increase the likelihood that the
patients act in accordance with hospital rules and breaks disruptive patterns of behaviour, whilst
family therapies reduce stress within a schizophrenic family and so increase the likelihood of the
patient complying with their medical advice, whereas CBT improves the patient’s understanding
of their symptoms. This suggests that an interactionist approach towards treatment is best
adopted: biological therapies can treat the distal causes of SZ, whilst psychological therapies can
treat the proximal symptoms!
— There are serious ethical issues associated with the use of psychological therapies, and
specifically concerning token economies. For example, some may argue that the ‘privileges’ that
patients receive upon displaying appropriate behaviours are actually rights. Preventing patients
from calling home or exercising outside may increase their stress and so aggravate their condition
further. In addition, patients with the most severe SZ may find it near impossible to comply with
these rules, and so will bear the most negative consequences. Similarly, CBT raises ethical issues
because the therapist essentially has control over the patient’s views. This means that by
challenging the idea of a Mafia as a controlling government instead, for example, the therapy is
infiltrating into the patient’s personal beliefs. These changes can be anything, and not always
beneficial.
+ There are alternative psychological therapies which may address the issues above, such as the
use of art therapy, as suggested by the National Institute for Health and Clinical Excellence. This
provides the patients with a creative outlet which reduces stress but does not require changing
the patient’s beliefs (as is the case with CBT) or discriminate against severely ill patients (as is
the case with token economies). Thus, this all suggests that psychological therapies are not
appropriate for all patients, but must be selected according to the type and severity of the
patient’s symptoms.
Neural explanations — Explanations which involves areas of the brain and nervous system and
the action of chemical messengers in the brain known as neurotransmitters as a means of
controlling and predicting behaviour.
www.pmt.education
Genetic explanations
- Family studies have shown that eating disorders such as AN run in families. First degree
relatives of individuals with AN have a 10 times greater chance of developing AN than relatives
of unaffected individuals (Strober et al, 2000). It therefore follows that people may inherit a
vulnerability towards developing eating disorders (Tozzi et al, 2005).
- Holland et al. found concordance rates of 56% for monozygotic twins, compared to only 5% of
dizygotic twins, out of a sample of 45 pairs of female twins. Since MZ twins share 100% of
their genes with each other, whilst DZ twins share only 50%, these concordance rates are
highly suggestive of a genetic basis to AN.
- Twin studies suggest a high heritability for AN, varying from 28% to 74%. For example, Wade
et al (2000) interviewed over 2163 female MZ and DZ twins, evaluating them on the DSM-IV
criteria for AN. The researchers found a heritability estimate of 58%, alongside reaching the
conclusion that “the comorbidity between anorexia nervosa and major depression is likely to be
due to genetic factors that influence the risk for both disorders”. Since these concordance
rates were over 50%, such twin studies suggests that there may be candidate genes which
lead to a genetic predisposition of some individuals towards AN.
- Conclusions drawn from twin studies are difficult to make reliably as MZ twins share the same
genes and environment, making it hard to separate the influences of nature and nurture.
Adoption studies avoid this because each pair of twins do not share the same environment.
For example, Klump et al (2009) studied 123 adopted sibling pairs and 56 biological sibling
pairs, finding heritability estimates ranging from 59% - 82% for the different aspects of
disordered eating.
— Problems with the genetic explanation = Fairburn et al (1999) found that although many studies
have been conducted, the actual heritability estimate is still unknown. The researchers point out
that results have been inconsistent and the studies not always replicated, making it difficult to
draw reliable conclusions. Many studies violate the equal environments assumption (8‘that
identical and fraternal twin pairs experience equivalent trait-relevant environmental exposures’).
Twin researchers also assume than MZ and DZ twins raised together have equal environments.
However, this may not be the case as it has been shown that MZ twins tend to be treated more
similarly than DZ twins (Loehlin and Nicols, 1976), and so their similar environmental factors may
have a greater influence on the development of AN compared to DZ twins.
— The genetic explanation ignores the role of media in AN = The media’s portrayal of ultra-thin
models is an important risk factor for eating disorders. However, Builk (2004) suggests that
genetically vulnerable individuals might seek out images themselves of thin role models. Vaughn
et Fouts conducted a longitudinal study which found that adolescent girls anorexia symptom
severity increased over a period of 16 months when they read more fashion magazines during this
time. This suggests that social learning theory, through the use of mediational processes and
vicarious reinforcement, may be an explanation for why adolescent girls identify with ultra-thin
models role models, and hence progress the development of AN.
—In the US, treatment for AN is restricted under many insurance plans as it is not considered to
have a biological basis = In 2014, the US tried unsuccessfully to get eating disorders included in
www.pmt.education
essential health benefits through legislation. However, research creates a case allowing AN to be
considered in the same way as schizophrenia as they are both considered to be biologically
based e.g. 9AN seems to affect between 1 and 4% of schizophrenia patients, and so there is a
high prevalence of comorbidity between schizophrenia and eating disorders.
Neural explanations
- Serotonin: Disturbances in the levels of the neurotransmitter serotonin may be responsible for
producing characteristic symptoms of AN, such as appetite suppression and obsessiveness.
For example, Bailer et al (2011) measured serotonin activity in women recovering from either
restrictive type anorexia or binge eating and found higher levels of serotonin activity. In
addition, the researchers found that the women with the highest levels of serotonin had the
highest level of anxiety, suggesting that persistent disruption of serotonin levels may lead to
increased anxiety which could trigger AN. Those suffering from AN were found to have low
levels of metabolites, such as 5-HIAA, which are produced by the breakdown of serotonin.
These low levels suggest that AN sufferers have abnormally functioning serotonin systems,
where the neurotransmitter is now always broken down.
- Limbic system dysfunction: Lipsman et al (2012) suggests that the neural roots of AN appear
to be related to a dysfunction in the limbic system. Researchers claim that dysfunction in these
9Youssef Kouidrat, Ali Amad, Jean-Daniel Lalau, and Gwenole Loas, “Eating Disorders in
Schizophrenia: Implications for Research and Management,” Schizophrenia Research and
Treatment, vol. 2014, Article ID 791573, 7 pages, 2014. doi:10.1155/2014/791573
www.pmt.education
areas whose normal functions is to regulate emotion, can lead to deficits in emotional
processing, which may lead to pathological thoughts and behaviours that are typical of AN.
— Problems with the serotonin explanation = SSRIs, which increase the levels of serotonin in the
brain by reducing the breakdown at the presynaptic cleft, are ineffective when used with AN
patients. Ferguson et al found no difference in symptom outcomes between patients taking SSRIs
and patients of a similar age who were not taking an SSRI. A second problem with serotonin
explanations is the effort to isolate serotonin as the single neurotransmitter responsible for AN. In
reality, serotonin interacts with dopamine, where both are precurosors to noradrenaline, which
may also interact with the neurotransmitter GABA. Therefore, the focus on serotonin is a very
simplified explanation for AN.
+ Research has supported the role of dopamine = Food aversions, weight loss, and distorted
body image cognition have been found to be related to an increased activity in dopamine
pathways (Kaye 2008). Barbeto et al (2006) found that increased blink activity is indicative of
higher levels of dopamine activity, with a correlation found between blink rate and the duration of
AN, suggesting that 10there is a significant positive correlation between blink rate and the duration
of illness. Kaye et al (1999) found that recovering AN patients displayed decreasing HVA/
homovanillic acid levels (a metabolite of dopamine). This study is particularly useful because it
removes the confounding variable of HVA levels decreasing during AN due to malnutrition or a
lack of exercise.
+ Biological Explanations of AN reduce stigma = Such explanations also prevent the belief that it’s
“their fault” , alongside offering the possibility of treatment. For example, deep-brain stimulation/
DBS , particularly of the nucleus accumbens in acute chronic cases, may therefore be a potential
treatment for AN (counteracting the abnormalities in the limbic system), where 11Lipsman et al.
found an average BMI of 3.5 points to 17.3 within the AN group, with 6 of the 16 patients
achieving a normal BMI.
Key words
Autonomy — The freedom to make decisions and determine actions without the constraints
imposed by others, and the extent to which we are dependent from others.
Control — Means to direct or to exercise authoritative influence over events of behaviours. In the
case of AN, sufferers exert control in an effort to reduce family dependence.
www.pmt.education
Minuchin et al (1978) developed the psychodynamic family theory, which suggests that a
dysfunctional family was the prerequisite for the development of AN. The psychodynamic family
was a necessary context for the development for AN, so treatment must aim to change the way in
which the family functions.
1. Enmeshment — Describes a family where the parents are excessively emotionally- involved
with their children but may be dismissive of their emotional needs. This can make it hard for the
child to develop an independent self concept. The self-identity of each family member overlaps
with other members, due to a lack of boundaries and respect for each other’s privacy. Enmeshed
families are over involved with each other and there is a lack of boundaries. Barber and Beuhler
contended that enmeshment stifles the development of children skills to deal with common social
stressors and make the development of AN more likely. The assumption is made that each
member understands and can speak for the other. This leads to the adolescent AN sufferer having
the desire to distinguish themselves from other family members, and particularly the mother.
3. Rigidity — Rigid families show an inflexibility in their adaptation to new situations. In the face of
stress, they increase the rigidity of their patterns of behaviour and resist any exploration of
alternatives. Any attempts at ‘self-differentiation’, such as the daughter trying to assert more
independence, is quickly extinguished by the family, who cannot and will not adapt to internal or
external threats to their circumstances. This may be reflected in the child starving themselves, in
an attempt to ‘control their own destiny’ and achieving autonomy by acting against the wishes of
her parents (Bruch, 1978).
4. Lack of conflict resolution/ Conflict Avoidance— Munichin believes that AN families were in
a state of constant but unresolved conflict, with conflicts being abandoned rather than solved. In
families with AN, patients typically present a strong façade of togetherness in order to avoid
conflict (Latzer et al, 2002). An excerpt from their (Latzer et al) report reads: 12 “The families of
eating disorder patients were found to be less cohesive, expressive, and encouraging of personal
growth than were controls … this may reflect family difficulties in supporting the child during the
process of separation individuation, and exploration of the outside world”.
Within the family where child is involved in conflict, Munichin identified 3 characteristics of
patterns of conflict-related behaviour:
A. Triangulation — Involves a pair of family members either incorporating or rejecting a third family
member.
12Yael Latzer, Zipora Hochdorf, Eitan Bachar, Attachment Style and Family Functioning as
Discriminating Factors in Eating Disorders, Contemporary Family Therapy, 2002, Volume 24,
Number 4, Page 581.
www.pmt.education
B. Parent-child coalition — The child now moves into a more stable coalition with one parent
against the other. The coalition parent will respond to the child with excessive concern
(enmeshment ) whilst the other parent withdraws.
C. Detouring — When parents are unable to solve conflicts by themselves they direct focus away
from themselves and onto the child. The child consequently becomes the scapegoat because
their wellbeing is sacrificed for avoidance of marital conflict.
+ Research support for enmeshment = Manzi et al (2009) found that cohesion between family
members was linked to positive outcomes and psychological wellbeing amongst adolescents,
whereas enmeshment had the opposite effect. Family cohesion was indicative of supportive
family interactions, whereas enmeshment was rooted in manipulation and control. This supports
the idea that enmeshment may be a trigger for AN because starvation may be an attempt of the
child to differentiate themselves from other family members and maintain a degree of control/
autonomy, which increases the thinner they become.
— Problems with the psychosomatic family model = Research has tried to establish
characteristics that are specific to families in which a member has AN. However, this has
produced inconsistent findings. For example, Kog et Vandereycken failed to find the
characteristics provided by the psychosomatic family model and instead proposed that this
model can be simplified to 133 components: the intensity of intrafamilial boundaries, the degree of
the family’s adaptability, and the family’s ways of handling conflicts. There is also growing
evidence that families in which someone has an eating disorder are a diverse group in terms of
the nature of family relations. Therefore, this suggests that the psychosomatic model is an over-
simplified explanation for AN, and can only be applied to some families, hence not being a
universal explanation.
— Inconclusive support from family-based therapy = The success of family focused therapy is a
key part of recovery. Carr et al (2000) concluded that there is compelling evidence for the
effectiveness of family intervention, citing Shadish and Baldwin (2003), who found that over 71%
of families experienced improved family dynamics, compared to a control group, after familial
interventions. However, Grange et Eisler (1987) pointed out that whilst there is some evidence that
family therapy is accompanied by changes these may not happen in all families, and therefore has
limited practical applications, further questioning the validity of the psychosomatic family model
explanations.
— Gender bias in family systems = Gremillion claims that there is a gender bias in family systems
because they tend to focus only on mother-daughter relationships, with enmeshment is nearly
always being seen as maternal. Focus on therapy tends to focus on reforming mothers and that
there is a lack of awareness that fathers too contribute to the enmeshment and that the father’s
overly controlling tendency is often overlooked. Therefore, this may lead to stereotyping of an
‘excessively overprotective and rigid’ mother and parent-blaming, whereas both parents in reality
have a role to play.
+ Research support for lack of conflict resolution = Latzer and Gaber (1998) carried out an
observational study of conflict resolution in 40 families with AN and 40 matched families without
AN. Parents and daughters were told to choose two areas of disagreement and were asked to
keep off the topic of food. The families with AN had greater difficulty choosing topics and
13 Kog, E., Vandereycken, W. and Vertommen, H. (1985), The psychosomatic family model. A
critical analysis of family interaction concepts. Journal of Family Therapy, 7: 31–44. doi:10.1046/j..
1985.00663.x
www.pmt.education
remaining focused on them whilst moving towards resolution. In addition, 14“the pattern of
conflict avoidance is also reflected in a low level of measured criticism and hostility in relatives
of AN patients”. This supports Munichin’s claim that there is an avoidance of conflict where one
family member has AN, and the role of conflict resolution in the development and maintenance
of AN.
Key words
Media — The various means of communication such as radio, television, newspapers and the
internet that reach or influence people widely.
Modelling — A form of social learning where individuals learn a particular behaviour by observing
and imitating a role model performing that specific behaviour. This effect is greater if the role
model is seen to be attractive, have high social status, being the same gender as the observer
and of a similar age. The role model does not need to be physically present in the environment to
exert influence.
Social learning — Learning through the observation and imitation of behaviours modeled by the
role model, who has been identified by the observer. The modeled behaviors that are seen to be
rewarded are more likely to be imitated by the observer, through the process of vicarious
reinforcement. Learning occurs both directly (operant and classical conditioning) and indirectly
(vicarious reinforcement).
Bandura believed that people learn by observing the behaviour of others as well as observing the
outcomes of these behaviours. Social learning theory suggests that children pay attention to the
role models in their lives and if they observe behaviour that is likely to produce a positive
response from others, as judged by the role model being vicariously reinforced for displaying this
behaviour, then they will imitate them in the belief that they will also receive the same positive
feedback. Such learning depends upon the 4 mediational processes (cognitive processes which
come between stimulus and response): Attention, retention, motivation and motor reproduction.
Peer influence — Peer reinforcement is particularly important during adolescence, so they may
be susceptible to peer influence over eating patterns. A US study found that dieting amongst
friends was linked to unhealthy weight control behaviours, such as taking diet pills or purging
(Eisenburg et al ). Jones and Crawford (2006) found that overweight girls and underweight boys
were more likely to be teased, suggesting that teasing serves to enforce gender based ideals.
14Yael Latzer, Lee B. Gaber, Pathological Conflict Avoidance in Anorexia Nervosa: Family
Perspectives, Contemporary Family Therapy, 1998, Volume 20, Number 4, Page 539
www.pmt.education
Their 15 results show that despite girls reporting more appearance conversations, boys perceived
more appearance pressure and teasing. In addition, Keel et al (2012) investigated the influence of
college peers, both male and female, on disorganised eating. The researchers found that 16 ‘for
women, college roommate dieting significantly predicted Drive for Thinness, Bulimia scores, and
purging at a 10-year follow-up’. This further demonstrates the strong impact of peer influences on
disorganized eating, through the mechanism of social learning, particularly due to some college
peers being identified as role models.
Media influence — The portrayal of thin models on TV and in fashion magazines is a contributory
factor for body image concerns. The media does not influence everyone in the same way as
individuals with low self esteem are more likely to compare themselves to idealised images. Low
self esteem plays a crucial role in the development of eating disorders. Button et al (2005) found
that girls with low self esteem aged 11-12 years old
were at a significantly greater risk of developing an
eating disorder aged 15-17. A report published by the
BMA expressed concern for the use of thin models
and reported that the degree of thinness exhibited
was not only unachievable but also biologically
inappropriate. These ideas were further supported by
Dittmar et al (2006), who found that a significant
proportion of the 162 British girls, aged 5-8 years old,
involved in the study reported low self-esteem and
increased body dissatisfaction when viewing the
Barbie dolls, compared to a control group who’d
viewed the Emme dolls (who have a more realistic
body shape). This deep-rooted body dissatisfaction at
a young age may manifest itself as AN in later life,
particularly due to Barbie being such an attractive role model and possessing many
characteristics (such as glamour, popularity and attractiveness) which girls in particular aspire to.
— Maternal influence is more complex than social learning = Research on the role of mothers has
not always produced consistent results. For example, Pike et Rodin (1991) found that there was
no evidence for daughters imitating the weight concerns of their parents. Ogden and Steward
(2000) found that although mothers and daughters were of a similar weight, there was no
association for restrained eating. This suggests that although most daughters would identify with
and view their mothers as role models, and so be expected to adopt similar eating habits through
social learning theory, this is not always the case. This therefore casts doubts on the idea that SLT
is a major explanation for AN.
+ Research support for peer influence = Costa-Fonta and Jofre Bonet (2009) investigated the
effects of peer weight and developing an eating disorder. The researchers found that peers with a
larger body mass had a smaller likelihood of developing AN, suggesting that having peers with an
average or higher BMI protects individuals from eating disorders, through the mechanisms of
15Jones, D.C. & Crawford, J.K. J Youth Adolescence (2006) 35: 243. https://doi.org/10.1007/
s10964-005-9006-5
16 Keel PK, Forney KJ, Brown TA, Heatherton TF. Influence of College Peers on Disordered Eating
in Women and Men at 10-Year Follow-up. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2013;122(1):10.1037/
a0030081. doi:10.1037/a0030081.
www.pmt.education
social learning theory. Secondly, 17several opinions from peers of an individual’s own body mass
were found to decrease the likelihood of women being thin or extremely thin. These pieces of
evidence further support the role of peer influence as an explanation for anorexia nervosa.
+ Research support for media influence = In a study conducted by Becker et al (2011), eating
attitudes amongst Fijian children were studied following the introduction of TV in 1995. After
exposure to TV, it was found that the children expressed a desire to lose weight, and this desire
increases with the more friends the individual has who had access to TV, videos or DVDs. This
increase in the will to lose weight may be due to the discussions made amongst friends about the
ultra-thin models they have seen on TV, creating a mutual goal. However, other research has
shown that intervention prior to media exposure can prevent the adverse effects. This suggests
that media can be a powerful influence but can be prevented through education.
+ Explains gender differences = Some studies have found that reading magazines is a more
consistent predictor of the development of eating disorders than watching TV. Harrison and
Cantor (1997) found no correlation between watching TV and eating disorders but found a
correlation between reading fitness magazine and attitudes towards food and dieting. As the
researchers found, 18 ‘For women, media use predicted disordered-eating symptomatology, drive
for thinness, body dissatisfaction, and ineffectiveness. For men, men, media use predicted
endorsement of personal thinness and dieting’. This shows that media influences, through the
mechanism of SLT, can explain gender differences in bodily attainment goals. Men suffer
predominantly from bigorexia (Jones and Morgan, 2010) due to increased exposure to athletic and
muscular role models, whereas females suffer increasingly from anorexia, due to increased
exposure to Size Zero models in socially-desirable contexts.
Key words
Cognitive theory — When applied to disorders, this is any explanation about the way in which a
person processes information that affects their feelings and their behaviour. The focus, according
to the cognitive approach, is on how our mental processes (e.g. attention and perception) can
affect our behaviour.
Distortions — Thinking that has a bias such that what is perceived by a person does not match
reality. These may be a result from comparison with others, (i.e. thin models) leading to a
misperception that the individual must be overweight, which in turn leads to feelings of self
disgust and an attempt to lose weight. For example, Murphy et al (2010) believes that cognitive
distortions are at the root of AN symptoms, particularly the tendency to overestimate personal
weight and body size. Using the Body Image Assessment, Williamson et al (1993) supported this
idea by showing how the (37) AN patients were significantly more likely to overestimate the size of
their silhouette compared to the (95) healthy controls.
Irrational beliefs — Beliefs that are unhelpful, illogical and inconsistent with our social reality. In
contrast, rational beliefs are helpful, logical and consistent with our social reality. Characteristic
irrational beliefs of AN include absolutist thinking (“I have eaten an apple and so I have no self-
control”) and catastrophising (“I have gained half a kilogram and so I will be obese”). These
irrational beliefs may stem from the anorexic’s tendency towards ‘never-ending’ perfectionism -
17Costa-Fonta and Jofre-Bonet., Body Image, Peer Effects and Food Disorders: Evidence from a
Sample of European Women, LSE Health, November 2009, Working Paper No: 15/2009.
18Harrison, K. and Cantor, J. (1997), The relationship between media consumption and eating
disorders. Journal of Communication, 47: 40–67. doi:10.1111/j.1460-2466.1997.tb02692.x
www.pmt.education
once they have reached their goals, the bar is set even higher, leading to a constant feeling of
failure. For example, Hewitt et al (2003) argues that this façade of perfectionism is made up of
19
‘perfectionistic self-promotion, non-display of imperfection and non-disclosure of imperfection’.
Individuals may also develop self-harming habits due to faulty beliefs about themselves and the
world around them. For example, a typical irrational belief of AN patients is that they must be thin
for others to like them.
The Cognitive Behaviour Model — Garner and Bemis (1982) found that anorexia patients have
many characteristics in common e.g. they are typically perfectionists and often full of self-doubt.
These characteristics, coupled with an individual’s exposure to cultural ideals of thinness, leads
them to form extreme ideas about the importance of body weight and the irrational belief that
losing weight will reduce their distress and make them more attractive to others.
Losing weight becomes self-reinforcing for the individual because of the positive comments from
others. This can be explained by positive reinforcement according to behaviourism. Once the
importance of thinness is established, anxiety about eating increases and this develops into a fear
of food and weight gain, meaning that food avoidance becomes the norm. As the individual
becomes more and more socially isolated, this reduces the chance of viewing their style of
thinking as irrational.
1) An enhanced sense of self control leads to increased self esteem. When an individual with AN
decides not to eat or to eat very little this provides positive reinforcement in the form of self
esteem (a behaviourist mechanism).
2) The physiological and psychological changes they receive, such as starvation, are perceived
as being the result of failure of self control, which in turn leads to a greater reliance on food
restriction.
3) Due to their focus on weight, the individual engages in increased self monitoring of their
weight, where any weight gain or loss perceived as being too slow leading to an increased
effort to restrict intake.
N.B. For information about the classic study of Rosenham (1973) On Being Sane in Insane Places,
please see the factsheets.
Carlsson et al (1999) investigated that idea of network interactions in schizophrenia, and the
therapeutic implications that such research has. Using PET scans (as detailed in Topic 3 -
Biological Psychology), the researchers found that the dopamine hypothesis was supported (i.e.
19Hewitt, P. L., Flett, G. L., Sherry, S. B., Habke, M., Parkin, M., Lam, R. W., . . . Stein, M. B.
(2003). The interpersonal expression of perfection: Perfectionistic self-presentation and
psychological distress. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(6), 1303-1325.
www.pmt.education
hyperdopaminergia in the subcortex and hypodopaminergia in the cortex), that dopamine is not
the only neurotransmitter involved in the development of SZ (e.g. an increased focus on the role of
glutamate by Moghaddam and Javitt, and that glutamate controls the release of dopamine and
interaction at the post-synaptic membrane), the idea of a thalamic filter (regulates the action of
neurotransmitters within the cerebral cortex), as well as the use and role of atypical and typical
antipsychotics in the treatment of SZ was reconsidered.
+ The brain scanning techniques used by Carlsson et al, such as PET scans, allows for reliable
data to be gathered and then interpreted objectively. This means that the conclusions drawn on
the basis of these findings have greater validity!
— Since the review was carried out in 2000, his findings can be said to have low temporal validity.
Particularly with current technological advances, the modern understanding of schizophrenia is
significantly different to that two decades ago, and so his findings may only be generalised to
contribute to the understanding of the field at that specific point in time.
Scott-Van Zeeland et al reviewed the evidence for the role of EPHX2 gene variants in anorexia
nervosa, using 261 individuals with early-onset and AN and then comparing these findings with 73
controls. The researchers concluded that 20“EPHX2 is known to influence cholesterol
metabolism… and found evidence for a modifying effect of a subset of variants on the relationship
between cholesterol and BMI. These findings suggest a novel association of gene variants within
EPHX2 to susceptibility to AN and provide a foundation for future study of this important yet
poorly understood condition”.
+ The use of a control group increases the validity and reliability of the findings because this group
acts as a baseline measurement, through which comparisons can be made. Statistical
comparisons between the control and experimental groups also dictate the confidence placed
in the conclusions.
+ The large sample size of over 800 participants in total, increases the reliability of the conclusions
drawn because it is less likely that the observed findings are due to chance. This means that the
findings are more likely to be generalised to the general population and represent their
experiences.
— However, it should be noted that the EPHX2 is only one specific candidate gene, hundreds of
which contribute to the development of Anorexia Nervosa, alongside environmental influences
such as enmeshment and the psychosomatic family. Therefore, it is important not to take the view
of biological reductionism and explain AN purely at the lower, biological level, as this is unlikely to
form a complete explanation.
20
Scott-Van Zeeland AA, Bloss CS, Tewhey R, et al. Evidence for the role of EPHX2 gene variants
in anorexia nervosa. Molecular Psychiatry. 2014;19(6):724-732. doi:10.1038/mp.2013.91.
www.pmt.education