Chapter 8
Chapter 8
Instructional Modes
Introduction 121
Comparison of performance: two examples 122
Heat transfer problem 122
Physical optics problem 127
Curricula that address problem-solving 129
Cooperative group problem-solving (UMinn) 130
Qualitative strategies for problem-solving (UMass) 132
Overview, case study physics 133
Bridging problems and problem-solving tutorials (UMd) 135
Lecture homework worksheets (UW) 136
Common elements 136
Summary 137
Chapter 8: Student Performance on Quantitative Exam Problems in
Two Instructional Modes
Introduction
In this chapter we compare performance on quantitative exam problems for
students in a tutorial class and students in a recitation class. The previous chapter
showed that even though almost all the students answered the qualitative question
correctly on exam 3, only 49% of the students could answer the corresponding
quantitative question correctly. Those results indicate that in certain contexts the
tutorial curriculum does not seem to help students make the connection between their
qualitative knowledge and their quantitative knowledge. In this chapter we look at
performance in two new contexts: heat transfer and physical optics. While the last
chapter evaluated the role of the tutorial section in problem-solving, this chapter will
evaluate the role of the tutorial and the role of the traditional problem-solving
recitation and compare the two.
Research results from The University of Maryland Physics Education Research
Group (PERG), as well as results from the University of Washington Physics
Education Group (PEG), have shown that tutorials1 can improve student qualitative
understanding of various topics.2 But there is little published work demonstrating how
students perform on more traditional type problems after going through tutorials.3 The
traditional recitation, where a teaching assistant shows students how to solve problems
at the board, is usually employed to teach students how to solve problems. If these
problem-solving sessions are replaced with conceptual activities such as tutorials, will
the students’ problem-solving ability degrade? Or will conceptually based exercises
foster the development of expert-like problem-solving skills?
As we have demonstrated, students often view quantitative problems
differently from qualitative problems. When given a qualitative problem in a class
using tutorials, students usually apply the qualitative knowledge that they have
developed. When the question is quantitative, even though the underlying physics
may be the same, students do not automatically apply a qualitative analysis to the
problem. We would therefore like to see whether the tutorial curriculum and the
recitation curriculum help students develop coherence between qualitative knowledge
and quantitative knowledge.
The two studies discussed in this chapter were conducted in the engineering
physics course (Physics 262 and Physics 263) at UMd. The questions were asked as
open-ended exam questions in a class with tutorials and in a class with traditional
recitations. The first study presented in this chapter is on the topic of heat transfer in
which the recitation students had instruction on the material and the tutorial students
had none. In our second study a physical optics question was asked in the tutorial
class and the recitation class. Students in the tutorial section had more instruction on
the topic of physical optics than the students in recitation. We find that students in the
two classes performed about the same on the heat transfer problem despite the
recitation class spending more time on the material. On the physical optics question
121
students in the tutorial class performed significantly better than the students in the
recitation class.
It should be noted that the tutorial curriculum is not designed to improve
student problem-solving although it is sometimes assumed that improved problem-
solving skills would be a side-effect of improved qualitative understanding. The
results from the last chapter and the results from this chapter show that this is only true
in certain cases. The last part of this chapter focuses on innovative curricula that use
physics education research (PER) to specifically address problem-solving skills. The
reform curricula that are presented in this dissertation all use PER as a guide to
curriculum development.
Tipler Problem
o o
100 C Cu Al 20 C
Figure 8 - 1
Tipler question on heat transfer asked as part of the
homework assignment.
122
exam in both classes that was similar to a problem both sets of students were given on
the homework assignment. The TA for the recitation section solved a similar
homework problem in each of his recitation sections. The TA for the course estimated
attendance in the recitation section to be between 70% and 80%.
The homework problem that was similar to our exam question came from
Tipler’s5 introductory physics text and is shown on the previous page in Figure 8 - 1.
If students were benefiting from the recitation we would expect that their performance
on the exam question would be better due to the extra exposure to the subject. After
the assignments were due in class, a solution to the homework problem was posted for
both classes. The exam question was written by the author and is shown in Figure 8 -
2. A solution to the exam problem involves applying the same physics concepts and
principles that were applied in the homework question. In addition, students can solve
the exam problem by applying steps almost identical to the steps used on the
homework problem. The exam problem can be solved in fewer steps if the student
applies more conceptual arguments.
The correct solutions to the homework problem and the exam problem are
shown in Figure 8 - 3. In the homework question the problem is broken down into
four parts. Students are first asked to calculate the resistance of each block, then the
equivalent resistance for the two blocks, then the thermal current that flows in the
blocks, and finally the temperature at the junction. Students can apply the same four
steps to calculate the temperature at the bottom of the pan in the exam problem. A
Ttop=71 oC
Junction Steel
Tj=80oC
Copper
Tbot= ? oC
Figure 8 - 2
Heat transfer exam question.
123
more direct method of solution requires more of a conceptual understanding of the
problem. Since the materials are in series, the currents in the two materials are the
same, therefore one can obtain the temperature at the bottom of the pan in two steps.
An important aspect of this research is examining the methods the students use in
solving the problems.
We conducted a careful analysis of student responses after the students (N =
111 in the tutorial class and N = 73 in the recitation class) took the exams. Fewer than
50% of the students answered the exam question correctly in both the recitation and
the tutorial class. Some student responses indicate that the students either used a
pattern matching approach or solved the problem without a deep conceptual
understanding. For example, 13% of the students in the tutorial class (and 11% of the
students in the recitation class) incorrectly used the change in temperature between the
top and the junction and the equivalent resistance to calculate the current through the
base of the pan. In addition 5% (1%) of the students obtained a non-physical answer
where the temperature at the bottom was less than the temperature of the junction. In
part (b), where the students were asked to calculate the current through a cross section
of the pan, 12% (14%) of the students made the mistake of calculating the thermal
current through the entire base of the pan. Another common error made by 17%
(11%) of the students was to find the thermal current in each material separately and
then add the currents.
In part (a) of the exam problem the students are asked to calculate the
temperature at the bottom of the pan, whereas in the homework problem students had
to calculate the temperature at the junction. The results shown in Figure 8 - 4 show
that students in the two sections performed equivalently within the uncertainty of the
measurements.
o o Ttop=71 oC
100 C Cu Al 20 C
Junction Steel
T j=80oC
Copper
Tbot= ? oC
Figure 8 - 3
Solutions to the heat transfer homework problem and the exam problem.
124
This result leads us to question the role of the traditional problem-solving
recitation. The recitation seems to have had little effect on the students even after
solving multiple problems from the chapter and solving an almost identical problem to
the one asked on the exam. Although the students in the tutorial class did not have
extra instruction on this material, one might hope that the emphasis on concepts in the
tutorial section would improve their performance on this question, but this does not
seem to be the case.
Part (b) of the exam is different from anything posed on the homework
assignment. On this section of the problem, the recitation class performed slightly
better, but not significantly better.
In order to get a better idea of student problem-solving ability we analyzed the
responses in greater detail, looking for the methods used in solving the problem. As
was mentioned earlier, a solution to the exam question could be obtained in two steps
instead of the four steps that were done in the problem assigned for homework.
50
45
40
35
% of Students
30
25
20
15
10
5
0
Part (a) Part (b)
Tutorial Class (N=111) Part of Question
Recitation Class (N=73)
Figure 8 - 4
Performance on the heat transfer exam problem in the tutorial and recitation classes.
125
supply of current (independent of the circuit). 6 Without a good conceptual
understanding of electric current it is reasonable that students will not posses a good
conceptual understanding of thermal current though there is no evidence that the same
or even similar misconceptions will appear.
The four-step solution indicates more of a pattern-matching approach to the
exam problem. By applying each of the four steps correctly students will obtain the
correct answer. Some instructors may feel that this is enough for students but one of
our goals in physics education research is to give students an understanding of the
materials that will allow them to think critically about a problem and allow them to
transfer knowledge from one situation to another situation.
In order to compare how the students in the two classes solved the problem we
classified each students’ response into one of the two categories. Figure 8 - 5 shows
two correct student responses.
The graph shown in Figure 8 - 6 shows the percentage of students in the
tutorial section and recitation section that attempted to solve the problem by using the
equivalent resistance. Note that the percentages shown are not necessarily students
who answered the problem correctly. The graph indicates that although the solution
using Req is more indirect, many students attempted to apply that method to solve the
problem. One possible explanation for this is that students are applying a pattern
matching approach to solve the problem. The graph also indicates that students in the
tutorial section were less likely to apply this approach. These results may indicate that
students in the recitation class are more likely to apply patterns of solution that they
recall using on similar problems instead of applying the underlying concepts to the
problem.
Figure 8 - 5
Two sample student responses on the exam problem. The first method of solution is
more indirect and follows the same steps as the homework problem.
126
% of Students using Req to Solve Problem
40
35
30
% of Students
25
20
15
10
5
0
Type of Question
Tutorial Class (N=111)
Recitation Class (N=73)
Figure 8 - 6
Graph showing how the students solved the problem in the two classes.
127
Light with λ = 500 nm is incident on two narrow slits separated by d = 30µm.
An interference pattern is observed on a screen a distance L away from the slits.
The first dark fringe is found to be 1.5 cm from the central maximum. Calculate
the distance, L, to the screen. Show all work.
Figure 8 - 7
Physical optics exam problem asked in a tutorial and a recitation class.
Students in the tutorial section performed significantly better than the students
in the recitation class on this problem. Figure 8 - 8 shows the comparison between the
two classes on this problem. The most common error made by the students in the
recitation class seemed to be in applying remembered formulas in a haphazard
manner. Many of the students in the recitation class used the condition for a
maximum instead of a minimum, therefore obtaining an answer of 0.9 m. The
students in the tutorial tended to attach more conceptual meaning to the formula for
the path difference. Almost none of the students answering the question correctly in
70
60
% of Students
50
40
30
20
10
0
Correct Incorrect: Used Incorrect: Other
equation for a
maximum
Tutorial Class (N=117)
Response
Recitation Class (N=165)
Figure 8 - 8
Performance of students on the physical optics question in the two instructional
modes. Students in the tutorial class performed significantly better on this
quantitative problem.
128
the tutorial section started with the derived formula, L = yd . Instead they began
nλ
( )
with the more fundamental equation 1 λ = d sin θ using the fact that ∆D = ½λ. This
2
shows that there was most likely some level of qualitative reasoning in the application
of the equation for the tutorial students who answered correctly. The difference in
performance for the two groups indicates that increased conceptual understanding can
improve quantitative problem-solving in certain contexts. For this to be the case,
qualitative knowledge and quantitative knowledge must be linked.
129
Analyze the Problem
Check
Figure 8 - 9
Problem-solving steps from Reif's Milikan lecture.
130
earlier by Polya.17 The five steps used by Minnesota are (1) visualize the problem, (2)
construct a physics description, (3) plan a solution, (4) execute the plan, and (5) check
and evaluate. Students are first asked to translate the problem statement into a visual
and verbal understanding. This often includes a sketch, identification of the relevant
information, and identification of the general approach they would apply to the
situation. The physics description takes the visualization and represents it in physics
terms. For instance a sketch of the situation may turn into a free body diagram. This
step is similar to the step experts take in going from the naïve representation to the
physical representation in the work of Larkin (described in chapter 2).18 Once this is
done students begin to plan a solution, where the physical representation is turned into
a mathematical representation. At this stage students make sure they have enough
information to solve the problem. Once all the information is available and the
mathematical description is completed, students obtain an answer and check to see
whether their answer is reasonable.
An important feature of this approach is the type of problems students will
solve in the sessions. If a problem is too easy or simply requires manipulating
formulas it will not foster group interaction and the general steps discussed earlier will
not be important to solve the problem. For this reason the Minnesota Group has
developed problems that are more challenging for the students. These context-rich
problems have a number of characteristics that separate them from the traditional
problems usually found in textbooks. (1) The problem statement will not always
identify the unknown variable; (2) the problem may include information that is not
important to the solution; (3) the problem may require the student to make estimations;
and (4) the problem may require the student to make reasonable assumptions. These
characteristics tend to make the context-rich problems too difficult for an individual
student to solve, but appropriate for the three or four member groups.19
Graded questions provided the students with motivation for participating and
working earnestly in the groups. Students are evaluated by group performance as well
as individual performance. During each class test, each group submits one solution
and each member of the group receives the same grade for that solution. A group is
given credit for using the problem-solving steps as well as for the correctness of the
solution. The individual component insures that students can not simply sit back and
allow the rest of their group to do all the work. It also aids the instructor in identifying
students that need additional help with the material. One way individuals were held
accountable is that students are selected at random to present their group’s solutions to
various problems.
Heller et al. performed three types of studies to evaluate the CGPS.20 The first
study looked at whether the cooperative group’s solutions were better than the
individual solutions of the top scoring students. In the second study they looked at the
dynamics of student problem-solving performance to see whether individual problem-
solving performance improved over time. In the third study they compared the
performance of students in the CGPS sections to students in a traditional section on
standard problems.
They based their evaluation of problem solutions on six criteria: evidence of
conceptual understanding, usefulness of description, match of equations with
131
description, reasonable plan, logical progression, and appropriate mathematics. This
scoring criteria was checked for reliability and validity.
To compare the solutions of the group to the solutions of the top individuals
Heller et al. used matched problems. Students first solved a context rich problem in a
group. Individual students were then given a similar problem that was less difficult
than the group problem. Group solutions were then compared to the solutions of the
individuals from each group who scored the highest on the individual exams and the
final exam problems. Scores on the group solutions and the individual solutions
showed that the performance of the group surpassed the performance of the
individuals, despite the fact that the individual problem was easier than the group
problem and the fact that the group problem was completed first. This result led
Heller et al. to conclude that the group work is not simply the work of the best
problem solvers in the class.21
Heller et al. also looked at the quality of solutions on the least difficult
individual problems throughout the course. The class was divided into the top third,
middle third, and bottom third, based on total individual grades on the tests and final
exams. They found that all students benefited from the CGPS on all six criteria except
conceptual understanding, where there was no appreciable gain.22 Saul has reported
that performance on the FCI in the CGPS class was comparable to performance in a
tutorial class but better than performance in a traditional recitation.23
The final investigation compared performance of students in the CGPS class
with performance of students in a traditional class on standard problems. (Instructors
in the standard class judged the context rich problems to be too difficult for their
students.)24 Students in the CGPS class scored significantly better on the criteria
outlined by Heller et al. The biggest different was in the qualitative analysis of the
problem. All CGPS students drew free-body diagrams compared to 57% in the
traditional class. Heller et al. therefore conclude that students in the CGPS curriculum
exhibit more expert-like characteristics in their problem solutions.25
132
given a textbook they could use to solve the problems and the third group used an
Equation Sorting Tool (EST) which contained a database of 178 equations that could
be searched. Students who used the HAT material tended to categorize problems by
principles more often than the other groups. Dufresne et al. also concluded that HAT
was more effective in improving student problem-solving than traditional methods.26
Based on earlier research, Leonard, Dufresne, and Mestre discussed a problem-
solving curriculum that could be implemented on a large scale.27 Their work describes
how qualitative strategies could be used to highlight the role of conceptual knowledge
in solving problems. They describe a strategy that can be employed in large
introductory physics classes without making large changes to the course. In solving
problems, they stress the use of identifying the concept or principle, justifying why the
principle is appropriate, and describing the procedure by which the principle or
concept could be used. Although the authors claim that a large restructuring of the
course is not required, they do make some significant revisions in the course. In
particular, one sample problem is worked out during each lecture. Students were also
encouraged to apply strategies in every problem they attempt in the course.
Leonard et al. performed two types of studies to evaluate the effectiveness of
the modified instruction. They looked at categorization tasks and students’ recall of
important ideas.
In the categorization tasks they administered five multiple-choice questions on
a final exam. Students were given a problem and asked to select the major principles
(from five choices) that should be used to solve the problem. Their results show that
the students in the modified curriculum were less inclined to choose principles based
on the surface features of the problem.28 These results were true for all students in the
modified curriculum.
The recall task occurred six months after students in the traditional class had
completed the course and 11 months after students in the modified class had
completed the course. The students were “asked to name the most important physics
ideas and principles used to solve problems in mechanics.”29 Both populations of
students were found to identify Newton’s three laws with about the same frequency
but the students in the modified curriculum cited the remaining four principles
(conservation of energy, work-energy theorem, linear momentum conservation, and
angular momentum conservation) at a consistently higher frequency than the other
students. The authors caution that this data does not mean that the students who went
through modified curriculum knew more than the students in the traditional class. But
the data does suggest that focusing attention on the principles does help students retain
the major ideas.
The researchers found that the modified curriculum they presented did help
students improve their explanations. In addition, the grading of strategies helped the
instructors become aware of how well students understood the ideas in the course.30,31
133
They then learn to build mathematical representations and then they apply their
knowledge to complex case study problems. OCS tries to first lay the foundation and
frame and then incorporate the details.33 In addition, students are active participants in
constructing this knowledge.
The frame and foundation are the overview, where the students are
qualitatively constructing the concepts in a particular block. The details start getting
filled in during the exposition, where the student translates the concepts into
mathematical form. At this stage students are also investigating different
representations for the problems, such as pictorial or physical representations. The
next stage is the case study, where students integrate a number of concepts that were
introduced earlier, in order to solve problems.34 A case study can last between several
days and several weeks. At the end of the semester a week-long review is conducted
in which students are shown a hierarchical chart of the different blocks of knowledge.
Throughout the semester students utilize a set of Active Learning Problem Sheets
(ALPS). ALPS are designed to make the lecture parts of the course more interactive.
The students complete an activity and then talk to their neighbors about their
reasoning.
We will discuss two examples of the type of evaluation done on the OCS
curriculum at New Mexico State University.35 In the first study they compared the
performance of students who went through the OCS curriculum to students who went
through the conventionally taught curriculum. Students in the OCS curriculum used
only one overview lasting about two weeks that covered material on NII and work-
energy concepts. Students were evaluated on responses to quantitative problems. In
the second study they compared the same types of populations (different students) on
qualitative questions. Students in the OCS curriculum used the first edition of the
OCS study guide and a standard calculus text. They compared student responses on
questions involving NII (24 questions) given as a pretest and a posttest.
Analysis of the quantitative problems asked on the final exam showed that
OCS students performed better than the students in the conventionally taught class.
51% of the OCS students answered correctly while only 13% of the conventionally
taught students answered correctly on the dynamics problem. In addition 94% of the
students in the OCS class included a free-body diagram in their solutions compared to
only 9% including the diagram for the conventionally taught population. Van
Heuvelen states that the conventionally taught students tried to solve the problems by
applying special memorized equations rather than applying the fundamental concept to
the problem.36
On the qualitative questions about NII both populations scored about the same
on the pretest. The conventionally taught students took the posttest at the end of the
semester, while the OCS students took the posttest after NII had been covered.
Conventionally taught students had a posttest score of 53% while OCS students had a
posttest score of 73%. On an additional posttest given at the end of the semester to the
OCS students, which included slightly more difficult questions, the OCS students had
a score of 86%.37
Research results show that the OCS curriculum does help students develop
better qualitative skills and quantitative skills.38 The integration of qualitative
representations into quantitative problem-solving was shown to improve performance
134
on qualitative questions. As we saw in the work done by the group at Minnesota,
curricula that have a strong problem-solving component can be used to improve
qualitative understanding as well as quantitative problem-solving.
135
These curricula are small perturbations to the course, therefore their evaluation
is difficult. Only one or two of the thirteen tutorials in a given semester are problem-
solving tutorials. In addition, only one of the problems out of about nine problems the
students solve on homework assignments each week are bridging problems. In order
to have an effect on creating links between qualitative knowledge and quantitative
problem-solving we expect that bridging problems and problem-solving tutorials must
be implemented on a much larger scale. Unfortunately, due to time constraints and
logistic constraints, a larger scale implementation was impossible at the University of
Maryland.
Common Elements
There are a number of elements that these curricula have in common that
separate them from the traditional physics curriculum. Students in these instructional
settings are actively participating in constructing their own knowledge. In the
traditional course students often sit passively watching instructors solve problems for
them. Even when instructors attempt to make the class interactive by asking
questions, only a small number of students participate in the discussion. In addition,
the students that participate are usually the students that already know the material.
These research-based curricula described above also rely on the students solving
problems that require them to use concepts and principles and not simply facts and
formulas.
As stated by Dufesne et al. traditional textbook problems often foster the use of
136
memory taxing strategies . . . to solve problems. Such strategies use much of
the novice’s available working memory to attend to the details of executing a
problem’s solution . . . there is little working memory left to extract from a
solution generalizable principles and procedures.43
Ward and Sweller, 44 as cited by Maloney,45 suggest that standard end of the
chapter textbook problems can actually be counterproductive. Our results, presented
in this chapter, suggest that a traditional problem-solving recitation where students
watch an instructor solve problems at the board may actually encourage students to
apply a pattern matching approach. Students sometimes think less about the
underlying physics and more about how similar a problem is to problems they have
already solved. My own experience in graduate school suggests that many physics
graduate students engage in similar activities. For instance, many students perform
well on the physics qualifier by solving many older problems, and being able to
recognize similarities between different questions. Although this may be a good
method to perform well on a test it is not necessarily the best way to learn physics.
Summary
In this chapter we compared responses given by students in two instructional
modes. The analyses of the responses to the two exam problems presented in this
chapter suggest some tentative conclusions. We have seen that the traditional problem-
solving recitation that is common at most large universities did not help students on
the problems presented in this chapter. An identical heat transfer question was given,
on an exam, to students in a physics 262 class with tutorials and a class with
traditional recitations. Even though students had additional instruction on heat
transfer in the recitation class, and solved a similar problem in the recitation section,
they performed no better than the tutorial class, which received no additional
instruction on heat transfer.
The recitation section seems to help the students apply pattern-matching
approaches to problems. This encourages students to simply look at the surface
features of a problem and not the underlying principles and concepts. The data from
the heat transfer problem also shows that simply using tutorials to develop conceptual
understanding does not mean students will develop more expert-like characteristics;
i.e. think qualitatively about quantitative problems.
When students have explicit instruction on a particular topic in the
tutorial class it is possible that they will perform better on quantitative problems than
students who go through a traditional recitation class. This was true in the physical
optics question. Students in the tutorial class performed much better than students in
the recitation class. It seemed as though students in the tutorial class were applying
the concepts more often than the students in the recitation class. Unfortunately, this
improvement for tutorial students, in applying the concepts, does not happen in topics
throughout the introductory sequence (as we saw in chapter 7.) It therefore depends
on the type of question.
The tutorial curriculum is not designed to address student problem-solving.
The PERG at the University of Maryland has supplemented the tutorial curriculum
with bridging problems and problem-solving tutorials to help students develop
137
coherence in their qualitative and quantitative content knowledge. Because these
supplements were implemented on such a small scale it was difficult to evaluate their
effectiveness.
Other curricula have been developed, using PER as a guideline, which place a
much greater emphasis on student problem-solving. Three well-established curricula
which teach problem-solving and use problem-solving to teach concepts are discussed
in some detail in this chapter. Each curriculum shares a number of common elements
including a strong research base, students actively participating in constructing their
knowledge, and the use of problems which require and encourage the use of
conceptual understanding. Research on the effectiveness of these curricula show that
they can be effective in helping students improve their problem-solving ability.
138
1
The tutorial curriculum is discussed in chapter 3.
2
See L.C. McDermott, and the Physics Education Group, A perspective on physics
education research as a guide to the improvement of instruction, unpublished
collection, (1998) and E.F. Redish, J.M. Saul, and R.N. Steinberg, “On the
effectiveness of active-engagement microcomputer-based laboratories,” Am. J. Phys.
66 (3) 212-224 (1998).
3
S. Kanim, "An investigation of Student difficulties in qualitative and quantitative
problem solving: Examples from electric circuits and electrostatics," Ph.D.
dissertation, Department of Physics, University of Washington, (1999); B. Thacker, E.
Kim, K. Trefz, S. Lea, “Comparing problem-solving performance of physics students
in inquiry-based and traditional introductory physics courses,” American Journal of
Physics 62 (7), 627-633 (1991); and B. A. Ambrose, P. S. Shaffer, R. N. Steinberg, L.
C. McDermott, "An investigation of Student Understanding of Single Slit Diffraction
and Double Slit Interference," Am. J. Phys., 67 (2) 146-155 (1999).
4
There was a single TA for the recitation class.
5
P. Tipler, Physics for Scientists and Engineers, 4th edition, (W. H. Freeman and Co.,
NY, 1999).
6
For more detail see L.C. McDermott and P.S. Shaffer, "Research as a guide for
curriculum development: An example from introductory electricity, Part I:
Investigation of student understanding." Am. J. Phys. 60 (11), 994-1002 (1992);
Erratum to Part I, Am. J. Phys. 61 (1), 81 (1993).
7
B. A. Ambrose, P. S. Shaffer, R. N. Steinberg, L. C. McDermott, "An investigation
of Student Understanding of Single Slit Diffraction and Double Slit Interference," Am.
J. Phys., 67 (2) 146-155 (1999).
8
G. Polya, How to solve it, (Doubleday, NY, 1945).
9
F. Reif, J.H. Larkin, and G.C. Brackett, "Teaching general learning and problem-
solving skills," Am. J. Phys. 44 (3) 212-217 (1976).
10
F. Reif, "Millikan Lecture 1994: Understanding and teaching important scientific
thought processes," Am. J. Phys. 63, 17-32 (1995).
11
P. Heller, R. Keith, and S. Anderson, "Teaching problem solving through
cooperative grouping. Part 1: Group versus individual problem solving," Am. J. Phys.
60 (7), 627-636 (1992).
12
P. Heller and M. Hollabaugh, "Teaching problem solving through cooperative
grouping. Part 2: Designing problems and structuring groups," Am. J. Phys. 60 (7),
637-644 (1992).
13
from their web site; see
http://www.physics.umn.edu/groups/physed/Research/MNModel/MMt.html
14
See ref. 11. (Heller, Keith, and Anderson)
15
See ref. 12. (Heller and Hollabaugh)
16
See F. Reif and J. I. Heller, "Knowledge structures and problem solving in physics,"
Educational Psychologist, 17 (2), 102-127 (1982)
17
G. Polya, How to solve it, (Doubleday, NY, 1945).
18
J.H. Larkin, "The role of problem representation in physics." In D. Gentner and A.
L. Stevens (Eds.), Mental models, (Lawrence Erlbaum, NJ, 1983), pp. 75-98
139
19
See Ref. 12.
20
See Ref. 11.
21
See Ref. 11.
22
See Ref. 11.
23
J. M. Saul, "Beyond Problem Solving: Evaluating introductory physics courses
through the hidden curriculum," Ph.D. dissertation, Department of Physics, University
of Maryland, College Park, (1998).
24
See Ref. 11.
25
See Ref. 11.
26
R. J. Dufresne, W. J. Gerace, P. T., Hardiman, and J. P. Mestre, "Constraining
novices to perform expert-like problem analyses: Effects on schema acquisition,"
Journal of the leaning Sciences, 2 (3) 307-331 (1992).
27
W. J. Leonard, R. J. Dufresne, and J. P. Mestre, "Using Qualitative Problem-Solving
Strategies to Highlight the role of conceptual knowledge in solving problems," Am. J.
Phys., 64 (12), 1495-1503 (1996).
28
See Ref. 27.
29
See Ref. 27.
30
See Ref. 27.
31
For more information see the group’s web site at http://www-perg.phast.umass.edu/.
They have developed a course at the high school level which ties together many of
these ideas called Minds on Physics. Kendall/Hunt publishes the curriculum.
32
A. Van Heuvelen, "Overview, Case Study Physics," Am. J. Phys. 59 (10), 898-907
(1991)
33
See ref. 32 in which Van Heuvelen makes a reference to a statement made by Steve
Kanim. The statement compare physics instruction to building a house room by room
versus foundation and frame first - then the details.
34
See Ref. 32.
35
See Ref. 32.
36
See Ref. 32.
37
See Ref. 32.
38
See Ref. 32.
39
Implementation of tutorials is discussed in detail in chapter 3 and in P. S. Shaffer,
"Research as a guide for improving instruction in introductory physics," Ph.D.
dissertation, Department of Physics, University of Washington, (1993).
40
A. B. Arons, Teaching introductory physics (Wiley, New York, 1997).
41
S. Kanim, "An investigation of student difficulties in qualitative and quantitative
problem solving: Examples from electric circuits and electrostatics," Ph.D.
dissertation, Department of Physics, University of Washington, (1999).
42
Bridging Problems are discussed in chapter 3
43
See Ref. 26.
44
D. S. Ward, and J. Sweller, “Structuring effective worked examples,” Cognition and
Instruction. 7, 1-39 (1990).
45
D. P. Maloney, "Rule-governed approaches to physics: Newton's Third Law," Phys.
Educ. 19, 37-42 (1984).
140