A Functional Contextualist Process Model of ADHD 18 03
A Functional Contextualist Process Model of ADHD 18 03
0000-0001-8539-9935
Abstract
ADHD is one of the most prevalent neurodevelopmental disorders and represents one extreme of
self-regulation within any population. However, no theory of ADHD yet explains the
behavioural processes behind its common symptoms, holistically accounts for how contextual
changes impact beahviour, or explains cases where people with ADHD perform better than their
and arguably encourage pharmaceutical treatment of them. In this paper, we extend current
dependent on an individual’s context. We use this model to explain basic and complex ADHD
symptoms, differentiate ADHD from a different process related to sluggish cognitive tempo, and
of children have clinical levels of ADHD symptoms (Ayano et al., 2023; Polancyzk et al., 2007;
Polanczyk et al., 2014), with another 5% experiencing issues with attention and impulsivity at
subclinical levels (Sayal et al., 2018), and approximately 2.5% of adults have clinical levels of
ADHD symptoms (Simon et al., 2009; Song et al., 2021). ADHD is associated with issues in
interpersonal relationships (Wymbs et al., 2021), education (Berchiatti et al., 2022), and
employment (Fuermaier et al., 2021), as well as frequently being comorbid with other
neurodevelopmental issues such as autism spectrum disorder (Hours et al., 2022), and dyslexia
ADHD symptoms vary as a trait within clinical and non-clinical populations. ADHD is highly
genetic, with a high heritability of approximately .75-80 across children and adults (Brikell et.,
2015; Faraone et al., 2005; Larsson et al., 2014). Genetic risk factors for ADHD exist within the
general population and people with ADHD have more of these risk factors than neurotypicals
(Martin et al., 2014; Stergiakouli et al., 2015). Additionally, regardless of symptom severity in
the individual, ADHD symptoms have similar relationships with other psychological variables
such as anxiety and personality traits (Li et al., 2019). Together these findings suggest that
clinical ADHD diagnoses are only one extreme of the normal variation in ADHD symptoms
within a population.
Mainstream ADHD theories and research are based within neuro- and cognitive psychology and
follow the biomedical, syndromes-focused paradigm. This paradigm categorises and defines
maladaptive behaviour as discrete illnesses with standardised symptoms and treatments (Hayes
et al., 2019). Some notable theories are the executive dysfunction theory (e.g., Barkley, 1997,
2011, 2022), the cognitive-energetic model (Sergeant, 2000), catecholamine dysfunction theory
(Prince, 2008), Sagvolden et al. (2005)’s dynamic developmental theory, synaptic gating theory
(Levy, 2004), and theories related to glial cell activity (Russell et al., 2006; Zhang et al., 2022).
However, these theories are arguably incomplete; without an underlying theory of behaviour
(e.g., Horne & Swettenham, 2024), they fail to account how their ADHD-related dysfunctions
integrate with other contextual factors, such as a person’s social environment and learned
experiences, to effect symptom expression. While in some cases other contextual factors are
somewhat considered (e.g., Sagvolden et al., 2005), there remains no clear underlying theory or
mechanism for how they influence behaviour. Overreliance on these neurocognitive theories
leads to issues in diagnosing, predicting, and treating ADHD behaviour (Champ et al., 2021), as
issues: ADHD becomes a catchall diagnosis for a variety of neurological deficits with similar
symptoms, and people with similar neurological deficits may receive different diagnoses if their
behaviour is more functional. However, ADHD research suggests that there is not only a variety
of causal factors associated with the disorder (Nigg et al., 2005; Sagvolden et al., 2005), but also
that contextual factors may affect how ADHD neurology leads to behaviour. For example, not all
people with ADHD have executive function deficits (Willcutt et al., 2005), and researchers have
argued that those who only experience inattentive symptoms may instead have a different
underlying deficit (Barkley, 2001; Milich et al., 2001). Additionally, higher IQ (Kosaka et al.,
2019; Soares et al., 2022), higher fitness levels (Mehren et al., 2019), better nutrition (Hawkey &
(Cairncross & Miller, 2020) have all been linked to lower ADHD symptoms. Therefore, people
that have or do these things may experience differing diagnoses to people that do not, even if
performances between people with and without ADHD. Take the following examples: Jylkkä et
al. (2023) found that people with ADHD do not perform worse on computer simulated everyday
tasks (d = 0.02, p = .862) but do worse in task efficacy (d = − 0.28, p = .015) and measures of
efficiency (e.g., empty clicks, d = .44, p <.001); and Boot et al. (2020) found that people with
ADHD reported more creative achievements (d = 0.71, p = .001) and that ADHD symptoms
were positively correlated with more original ideas on the alternative uses task (r = .28, p < .01).
Within the context of these experiments, overall performances of people with ADHD were equal
to, or better, than people with less severe/ non-clinical ADHD symptoms. While current theories
describe neurocognitive deficits associated with ADHD, they are unable to explain their
Finally, the neglect of context within neurocognitive theories may inappropriately prioritise
pharmaceutical interventions for ADHD. Currently, the neurological and cognitive deficits
associated with the order (Barkley, 1997; Prince, 2008; Sagvolden et al., 2005), at least in some
cases (Willcutt et al., 2005), provide a practical therapeutic target for pharmaceutical treatment:
reduced dopamine signalling (Prince, 2008; Sagvolden et al., 2005) can be treated with stimulant
medication which increases dopamine signalling. However, current ADHD medications often
have side effects (Barnard-Brak & Kudesy, 2022; Pan et al., 2022; Wynchank et al., 2017), and
are not always effective (Michielsen et al., 2021; Ophir, 2022). Recent studies have even
suggested that, despite potential withdrawals and a lack of other treatment options, most people
try to stop taking ADHD medication (Ophir, 2022), with 42.7% of people being successful in a
different sample (Michielsen et., 2021). Without a holistic, behavioural understanding of ADHD,
adopting a more contextual approach to improve functional behaviour with far fewer side effects.
underemphasises contextual factors that also influence ADHD symptom expression. At best,
these theories overlook the potential advantages of ADHD (e.g., Boot et al., 2020). At worst, the
neurological focus of these theories may promote misdiagnosis and clinical malpractice (BBC
Panorama, 2023), and cause shortages of ADHD medication (ADHD UK, 2024), as potentially
ineffective and harmful medications (Michielsen et al., 2021; Ophir, 2022), remain the first main
treatment option for adults and some children (Hertfordshire Partnership NHS, 2017), with other
treatment options often only being recommended after starting medication (e.g., Psychiatry UK,
2024).
In this paper, we look to refocus ADHD conceptualisation and treatment towards a holistic
model of behaviour, where medication is one, but the not the only option to manage ADHD
learned experiencess (relational frames; Hayes et al., 2001; Hayes et al., 1999; Horne &
Swettenham, 2024). As ADHD is one extreme of self-regulation behaviour (Martin et al., 2014;
Stergiakouli et al., 2015), we generalise behavioural self-regulation theory to the clinical self-
regulation issues linked with ADHD, and argue that ADHD symptoms are a result of faster
lead to more frantic, inefficient behaviour (Jylkkä et al., 2023) as people switch faster between
new goals and ideas (Boot et al., 2020). We also differentiate this behavioural process from
another behavioural process associated with sluggish cognitive tempo (SCT; Becker, 2021;
Becker et al., 2016), and explain how theoretically both processes could contribute to inattentive
symptoms within an individual. Based on this contextual understanding of ADHD, we then make
contextual recommendations to help people with ADHD live lives they value. In doing so, we
aim to provide practical alternatives for first-line ADHD treatment, especially for people who
Before introducing our process model of ADHD behaviour, we must first introduce our
underlying model of human behaviour and the philosophy behind it. It is best practice for both
clinical and sport and exercise psychology practitioners to use an underlying philosophy and
theory of behaviour to inform their work (Hughes, 2018; Poczwardowski et al., 2004; Tod &
Eubank, 2020). Doing so allows practitioners to integrate different interventions within a holistic
treatment program (e.g., Horne & Swettenham, 2024). To our knowledge, this is something that
no theory of ADHD currently does: neurocognitive deficits remain the priority while their
seem like an afterthought. This over-emphasises the role of the neurology shared among people
with ADHD and neglects the individual-level differences that may either worsen or reduce their
symptoms.
Our process model of ADHD behaviour instead is underpinned by functional contextualism and
relational frame theory (Hayes et al., 1999; Hayes et al., 2001; Horne & Swettenham, 2024; see
Figure 1). Within the model, behaviours (both internal thoughts, feelings and emotions, and
their environment (physical, social, physiological and neurological), and their relational frames.
Relational frames are webs of language-driven formed connections between different stimuli and
are how we learn as humans. Relational frames form verbal knowledge and rule-governed
behaviour. As we develop, behaviour becomes evermore mediated by our relational frames as we
behave according to our past experiences. Generally, Figure 1 argues that our behaviour is
dependent on our current environment and how we have learnt to perceive it, and our behaviour
can change our environment and provide new experiences to be learnt from.
Figure 1.
Our theory of behaviour based on Functional Contextualism and Relational Frame Theory
Within this theory of behaviour, existing ADHD theories document parts of an individual’s
context. Together, existing neurocognitive theories suggest that reduced dopamine signalling
associated with ADHD leads to a reduced functioning reward system (Prince, 2008; Sagvolden et
al., 2005); this relates to an individual’s neurocognitive environment in the model. Over time, the
reduced reward and satisfaction in life encourages people with ADHD to prioritise immediately
rewarding tasks over effortful work towards their long-term goals (Barry & Kelly, 2006;
Sagvolden et al., 2005); this relates to an individual’s the relational frames in our model.
However, while these theories model an individual’s context, they neglect the behaviour itself.
For example, while these theories argue that someone with ADHD would be more impulsive,
they do not describe how this impulsivity occurs in terms of motivational, attentional, and
emotional processes; they do not describe how these processes may be different from people
without ADHD; and they do not describe under what conditions and contexts this impulsive
behaviour will occur. Our contextually-bound process model below looks to solve these issues.
Elsewhere, there has been a growing call for a contextual, process-based understanding of
ADHD behaviour. As many of the neurocognitive ADHD theories were being published, there
was initial doubt around whether neurocognitive-level theories were the best suited for
understanding the disorder (Nigg et al., 2005). More recently too, there have been calls for a
processes (Hayes et al., 2019); instead of trying to reduce psychological symptoms that are
deemed problematic (e.g., anxiety), a greater emphasis should be given to how these conditions
prevent people living the lives they value (Hayes et al., 2019). Recent voices in scientific
needed (Timms, 2022): having a particular diagnosis, such as ADHD, is neither definitive nor
explanation is needed.
Where behavioural implications are touched upon, existing ADHD theories describe it as a
disorder of goal-directed behaviour and self-regulation. Barkley (1997, 2011, 2022) relates his
in task persistence and fluency. Additionally, Sagvolden et al. (2005) describe how neurological
signalling and learning impairments lead to impulsiveness, aversion towards waiting and delays,
and preferential hyperactivity towards immediately gratifying and novel situations. While a
mechanism for this action is not identified in either theory, together these results suggest that
people with ADHD will struggle to persist on long-term goals where they are uncomfortable,
models of goal-directed behaviour over time in general populations. Recent models of self-
regulation and self-control model it as a process (Inzlicht et al., 2014; Inzlicht et al., 2021), rather
than a resource (Inzlicht & Schmeichel, 2012): instead of a self-regulation being something that
attention, and emotion over time between long-term and short-term goals (Inzlicht et al., 2014),
motivation, attention, and emotions become more directed at immediately rewarding short-term
goals instead. While Inzlicht et al. (2014) only accounted for one task, we model self-regulation
across multiple tasks, with (long-term) goal-directed behaviour (attention, motivation, and
Goal-directed behaviour towards a particular goal in people with and without ADHD
We assert that individuals with ADHD experience faster changes in goal-directed behaviour
(attention, motivation, and emotion) towards any given goal, either short- or long-term.
Consequently, not only does goal-directed behaviour decline more swiftly for individuals with
ADHD than their neurotypical peers, but it also returns faster too, provided an individual's
context allows these changes. Figure 2 shows this process towards one long-term goal over time.
Faster changes in goal-directed behaviour leads to more erratic and chaotic behaviour, where
individuals with ADHD switch between different thoughts, ideas, and tasks more often. In
experimental environments such as Boot et al. (2020) and Jylkkä et al. (2023), this franticness
can lead to a faster generation of new ideas and creativity as attention to different things changes
more quickly (Boot et al., 2020), but these faster changes can also lead to inefficient
performances on singular tasks (Jylkkä et al., 2023). However, in different contexts where task-
irrelevant novelty is available (in conversations with others, or on social media, as examples)
Instead of being simply issues of attention control and self-regulation, now these symptoms mark
the shifts in goal-directed behaviour within different contexts. As attention, motivation, and
emotion declines at a faster rate for people with ADHD, current tasks become more unengaging,
boring, and burdensome more quickly, while a new task they could be doing instead becomes
more captivating, exciting, and fun. Hyperactive-impulsive behaviour occurs when individuals
follow these faster changes in attention, whereas inattentive behaviour occurs when people
cannot, or choose not to, change their actions due to contextual restraints. In such cases,
attention, motivation, and emotion remain directed towards different tasks, as they think about
other things, but their observable actions only reflect a disengagement with the current activity.
This process also provides insights into more complex behaviour associated with ADHD, such as
the ADHD paralysis. In our model of behaviour (see Figure 1), behaviour is both observable
actions, as well as thoughts, feelings and emotions, and all this behaviour can be goal-directed.
Paralysis associated with ADHD, referred to colloquially as ADHD paralysis (ADDA Editorial
about, but not beginning tasks, when they are faced with lots of information and jobs to do. This
paralysis behaviour can be seen as an extreme of the ADHD model shown in Figure 2. In
situations with much information and many tasks to do and little motivation, persistence may be
so low because goal-directed behaviour is limited to only thoughts on a particular task, and does
not extend to observable actions. Therefore, an individual’s thoughts may switch to a new task
before they have had the chance to start action on the one before.
attention and subsequent behaviour are inherently bound by the context and their tasks within it
(Horne & Swettenham, 2014). As a result, contextual changes can be used to functionally
improve behaviour, whether that be environmental changes (Duckworth et al., 2014, 2016;
Duckworth et al., 2018), or through new learned experiences forming new relational frames
(Hayes et al., 1997). In our model’s biopsychosocial conceptualisation of environment (Horne &
Swettenham, 2024), an ADHD brain is one environmental factor which can affect behaviour, as
For example, developmental changes in ADHD symptom expression are tied with contextual
changes over time. While initial hyperactive and impulsive behaviour are common in children
with ADHD, these external behaviours are often replaced with internal symptoms of inattention
in adulthood (Kumperscak, 2013). This delay in inhibiting observable ADHD symptoms can be
linked to delays in brain maturation (e.g., Shaw et al., 2007), but also the impaired learning of
social norms and rules over time (Barry & Kelly, 2006; Sagvolden et al., 2005); these represent
an individual’s environment and relational frames in Figure 1, respectively. Learning these social
norms prevents the faster changes in thoughts, motivations, and emotions being externalised into
impulsive, hyperactive actions. Children with ADHD and higher IQ, another contextual factor,
may learn these social norms faster and mask their symptoms better due to less delays in brain
development (De Zeeuw et al., 2012) and less impaired cognitive skills (Milioni et al., 2017).
Additionally, contextual differences can affect task persistence through different initial levels of
goal-directed behaviour (Hayes et al., 1997; Hayes, 2001; Horne & Swettenham, 2024). On the
one hand, positive past experiences of an environment or task may improve future task
persistence through increasing future motivation: Taylor et al. (2020) found that high motivation
towards an exercise endurance task led to increased persistence, while goal-directed behaviour
declined similarly across high and low motivation groups, as initial differences were maintained
the higher motivation group persisted longer. On the other hand, effortful, cognitively
demanding tasks have been linked with increased hyperactivity behaviour for people with
ADHD (d= 1.39), far more than in low cognitive load environments (d = .036; Kofler et al.,
2021). Intuitively, but contrary to neurocognitive ADHD theory, these results suggest that
unmotivating situations, especially for people with ADHD. In our process model (see Figure 2),
engaging or demanding contexts would then lead to differing goal-directed behaviour peaks.
Furthermore, contextual, physiological changes during and after exercise can slow the changes in
goal-directed behaviour associated with the disorder. Exercise programs aim to improve the
management of goal-directed behaviour (Ng et al., 2017; Silva et al., 2015). At a neurocognitive
level, these treatments help to minimise the deficits which have been found in some, but not all
people with the disorder (e.g., Mehren et al., 2019; Willcutt et al., 2005). At a behavioural level,
exercise has led to improved sustained attention for people with ADHD (Rassovsky & Alfassi,
2019), and neutralised possible effects of ADHD on goal valuation (Horne et al., 2024). Other
symptoms, such as omega-3 supplementation (Chang et al., 2018; Hawkey & Nigg, 2014), or
even transcranial direct current stimulation (Breitling et al., 2016; Salehinejad et al., 2020) could
similarly help slow changes in goal-directed behaviour for people with ADHD.
Our process model is distinct but interlinked with other processes within neurodiversity and
mental illness. Our model argues that ADHD behaviour such as inattention and hyperactivity-
impulsivity are due to faster changes in goal-directed behaviour (attention, motivation and
emotion). However, this is not the only process that exists within neurodiversity, nor is it even
the only process that contributes to inattention. People with ADHD may experience multiple
processes at once, and these processes combine to reach clinical threshold levels of inattentive
behaviour.
One example distinction is between ADHD and Sluggish Cognitive Tempo (SCT) processes.
SCT (Barkley, 2012; Becker, 2021; Becker et al., 2016), predominantly inattentive ADHD
(Barkley, 1997; Milich, 2001), and depression (Kroenke et al., 2001) lead to symptoms of
apathy, lethargy, brain fog, and inattention, but crucially not hyperactivity-impulsivity. There is
huge overlap between ADHD and SCT both diagnostically and behaviourally in terms of
inattention, but they are distinct disorders with different functional problems (Barkley, 2012;
Becker et al., 2020; Lee et al., 2016; Lee et al., 2014). Approximately half of adults with clinical
levels of ADHD also have clinical levels of SCT, and vice versa (Barkley, 2012).
We propose that SCT is related to lower initial goal-directed behaviour (attention, motivation,
and emotion) towards any goal (see Figure 3). Unlike our ADHD process, goal-directed
behaviour does not change faster, but is generally lower. Due to lower levels of goal-directed
attention and motivation, overall but task persistence and attention towards it will still be
impaired (Taylor et al., 2020). Therefore, people who only experience this second process would
still display inattentive behaviour quicker than healthy, neurotypical populations, but the loss of
attention and motivation towards one activity would not be replaced by attention and motivation
towards another.
Figure 3.
Goal-directed behaviour towards one task in people with and without SCT
A combination of ADHD and SCT processes helps to explain links between ADHD and fatigue.
Fatigue is a common comorbidity with ADHD; people with the disorder are more likely to feel
clinical levels of fatigue than healthy populations (62% to 26%) and only marginally less than
people diagnosed with chronic fatigue syndrome (86%; Rogers et al., 2017). Additionally,
ADHD and depressive symptoms, have both been shown to be unique predictors of fatigue
intensity (Sáez-Francàs et al., 2012). Our process models of ADHD (Figure 2) and SCT (Figure
3) could uniquely contribute to this fatigue both through doing more inefficient and frantic goal-
directed behaviour to complete the same task, and general feelings of lethargy, apathy and
amotivation, respectively.
Model limitations and future directions
This model assumes that poor self-regulation linked with ADHD is an extreme of poor self-
regulation in neurotypical people. This assumption is based on genetic research findings that
people with ADHD just have more ADHD risk factors than people without ADHD (Martin et al.,
2014; Stergiakouli et al., 2015), and allows us to generalise self-regulation theory and goal-
directed behaviour changes from population samples to clinical ADHD samples. To our
behaviour changes and task persistence other than Horne et al. (2024). Further research is needed
to test differences in goal-directed behaviour between people with and without ADHD across
Recommendations
We now make some recommendations on how an individual’s context, in both their environment
and relational frames (see Figure 1), can be manipulated to help people with ADHD manage
their symptoms and live meaningful lives. While theories of ADHD’s neurocognitive deficits
may promote pharmaceutical treatments which directly target them, these are not always
effective, and can lead to serious side effects (e.g., Michielsen et al., 2021). In contrast, our
model encourages people with ADHD to integrate many different, more accessible and
Our aim here is to promote more holistic treatment plans for ADHD, rather than defaulting on
Above, we have shared results that suggested that people with ADHD have more creative
achievements and novel ideas (Boot et al., 2020) and are able to switch in goal-directed
behaviour while maintaining a level of performance on one task (Jylkkä et al., 2023). Due to
their faster changes in goal-directed behaviour, especially during cognitively demand tasks
(Kofler et al., 2016), people with ADHD may benefit from having multiple tasks at once which
they can switch between at will when they have a new, novel idea, or get bored. While Jylkkä et
al. (2023) showed that people with ADHD perform similarly to neurotypicals at one task within
an experimental context, people with ADHD may thrive in jobs and contexts where goal-directed
behaviour towards a secondary task is still productive; what was inefficient, irrelevant behaviour
in Jylkkä et al. (2023) could be a groundbreaking new idea for a side project.
Environmental changes should also be used to manage the downsides of the disorder. While
faster changes in goal-directed behaviour can be useful for creativity, they can often lead to
forgetfulness and make it harder for people with ADHD to stay on track. Using a notepad or
other note-taking device to write down both goals and novel thoughts before attention is diverted
can both maintain organisation and goal-directed behaviour, while also making sure creativity is
not lost (Boyer et al., 2015; Duckworth et al., 2018; Fleming & McMahon, 2012; Murphy,
2005). People could also change their working environments so that potential distractors, such as
online devices, are harder to access (Duckworth et al., 2018). These extra barriers may reduce
initial motivation for the distraction, so goal-directed behaviour may revert back into the original
task quicker.
Other factors related to individuals’ physiological health, diet, and recovery should also be
prioritised to manage fatigue and work sustainably. Many alternative treatments to ADHD have
already been intensively researched and many have been shown effective to manage ADHD.
Exercise programs are arguably the most effective behavioural ADHD treatment and many
reviews have shown them to reduce ADHD symptoms (e.g., Sun et al., 2022 SMD = −0.60).
Other behavioural interventions promoting sleep have also been shown to reduce ADHD
symptoms (SMDs = −0.02 to −0.40; Larsson et al., 2023). And even small changes in diet, such
as omega-3 supplementation, moderately reduce ADHD clinical scores (g = 0.38; Chang et al.,
2018), with probiotics treatments showing promising, but preliminary findings too (Nahidi et al.,
2024). These treatments, which can be combined, are all comparative to medication efficacy (d =
.65; Mészáros et al., 2009), but without the potential side effects.
Finally, psychological, cognitive behavioural interventions can establish new relational frames to
encourage long-term functional behaviour. For example, Acceptance and Commitment Therapy
and behavioural interventions (Hayes et al., 1999). These interventions look to change how
people notice and relate to their thoughts, feelings and emotions through developing more
adaptive relational frames and verbal rules. For ADHD specifically, these new relational frames
could help counteract existing ones associated with the disorder which may prioritise short-term,
immediately gratifying avoidance behaviours (Hayes et al., 1999; Sagvolden et al., 2005).
been shown to reduce inattention (d = -.66) and hyperactivity/ impulsivity symptoms (d = -.53;
of behaviour. We then differentiate this process from SCT, a different behavioural process that
while interlinked with ADHD and inattentive symptoms, is itself distinct. Not only do we hope
behaviour, not symptoms, but we also hope our context-based recommendations encourage a
holistic management of ADHD symptoms, where medication is one option, rather than the only
option, for healthcare providers. Through our contextual recommendations for people with
ADHD, we hope to help people with the disorder play to their strengths, and around their
References
1. American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental
2. Ayano, G., Demelash, S., Gizachew, Y., Tsegay, L., & Alat, R. (2023). The global
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2023.07.071
3. Polanczyk, G., De Lima, M. S., Horta, B. L., Biederman, J., & Rohde, L. A. (2007). The
https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.164.6.942
4. Polanczyk, G. V., Willcutt, E. G., Salum, G. A., Kieling, C., & Rohde, L. A. (2014).
ADHD prevalence estimates across three decades: an updated systematic review and
https://doi.org/10.1093/ije/dyt261
5. Sayal, K., Prasad, V., Daley, D., Ford, T., & Coghill, D. (2018). ADHD in children and
young people: prevalence, care pathways, and service provision. The Lancet Psychiatry,
6. Simon, V., Czobor, P., Bálint, S., Mészáros, A., & Bitter, I. (2009). Prevalence and
7. Song, P., Zha, M., Yang, Q., Zhang, Y., Li, X., & Rudan, I. (2021). The prevalence of
romantic relationships: What we know and what we can do to help. Journal of Marital
9. Berchiatti, M., Ferrer, A., Badenes-Ribera, L., & Longobardi, C. (2022). School
https://doi.org/10.1080/15377903.2021.1941471
10. Fuermaier, A., Tucha, L., Butzbach, M., Weisbrod, M., Aschenbrenner, S., & Tucha, O.
(2021). ADHD at the workplace: ADHD symptoms, diagnostic status, and work-related
s00702-021-02309-z
11. Hours, C., Recasens, C., & Baleyte, J. M. (2022). ASD and ADHD comorbidity: what are
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2022.837424
12. Boada, R., Willcutt, E. G., & Pennington, B. F. (2012). Understanding the comorbidity
13. Brikell, I., Kuja‐Halkola, R., & Larsson, H. (2015). Heritability of attention‐deficit
15. Larsson, H., Chang, Z., D'Onofrio, B. M., & Lichtenstein, P. (2014). The heritability of
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291713002493
16. Martin, J., Hamshere, M. L., Stergiakouli, E., O’Donovan, M. C., & Thapar, A. (2014).
671. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.02.013
17. Stergiakouli, E., Martin, J., Hamshere, M. L., Langley, K., Evans, D. M., St Pourcain, B.,
Timpson, N. J., Owen, M. J., O’Donovan, M., Thapar, A., & Smith, G. D. (2015). Shared
children and clinical ADHD. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent
18. Li, T., Mota, N. R., Galesloot, T. E., Bralten, J., Buitelaar, J. K., IntHout, J.,
AriasVasquez, A., & Franke, B. (2019). ADHD symptoms in the adult general population
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.euroneuro.2019.07.136
19. Hayes, S. C., Hofmann, S. G., Stanton, C. E., Carpenter, J. K., Sanford, B. T., Curtiss, J.
E., & Ciarrochi, J. (2019). The role of the individual in the coming era of process-based
therapy. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 117, 40-53.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2018.10.005
20. Barkley, R. A. (1997). Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions:
https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.121.1.65
https://doi.org/10.1521/adhd.2011.19.4.1
22. Barkley, R. A. (2022). Improving clinical diagnosis using the executive functioning—
https://doi.org/10.1521/adhd.2022.30.1.1
https://doi.org/10.1016/s0149-7634(99)00060-3
https://doi.org/10.1097/JCP.0b013e318174f92a
25. Sagvolden, T., Johansen, E. B., Aase, H., & Russell, V. A. (2005). A dynamic
397-418. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X05000075
26. Levy, F. (2004). Synaptic gating and ADHD: a biological theory of comorbidity of
https://doi.org/10.1038/sj.npp.1300469
27. Russell, V. A., Oades, R. D., Tannock, R., Killeen, P. R., Auerbach, J. G., Johansen, E.
disorder: a neuronal and glial energetics hypothesis. Behavioral and Brain Functions,
28. Zhang, P., Fang, H., Lou, C., Ye, S., Shen, G., Chen, S., Amin, N., Botchway, B. O. A.,
& Fang, M. (2022). Enhanced Glial Reaction and Altered Neuronal Nitric Oxide
30. Champ, R. E., Adamou, M., & Tolchard, B. (2021). The impact of psychological theory
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0261247
31. Nigg, J. T., Willcutt, E. G., Doyle, A. E., & Sonuga-Barke, E. J. (2005). Causal
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.08.025
32. Willcutt, E. G., Doyle, A. E., Nigg, J. T., Faraone, S. V., & Pennington, B. F. (2005).
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.02.006
33. Barkley, R. A. (2001). The inattentive type of ADHD as a distinct disorder: What
https://doi.org/10.1093/clipsy.8.4.489
34. Milich, R., Balentine, A. C., & Lynam, D. R. (2001). ADHD combined type and ADHD
predominantly inattentive type are distinct and unrelated disorders. Clinical psychology:
35. Kosaka, H., Fujioka, T., & Jung, M. (2019). Symptoms in individuals with adult-onset
ADHD are masked during childhood. European archives of psychiatry and clinical
36. Soares, P. S. M., de Oliveira, P. D., Wehrmeister, F. C., Menezes, A. M. B., Rohde, L.
A., & Gonçalves, H. (2022). Does IQ Influence Association Between Working Memory
and ADHD Symptoms in Young Adults? Journal of Attention Disorders, 26(8), 1097-
1105. https://doi.org/10.1177/10870547211058813
37. Mehren, A., Özyurt, J., Lam, A. P., Brandes, M., Müller, H. H., Thiel, C. M., &
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00132
38. Hawkey, E., & Nigg, J. T. (2014). Omega− 3 fatty acid and ADHD: Blood level analysis
496-505. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2014.05.005
39. Ligezka, A. N., Sonmez, A. I., Corral-Frias, M. P., Golebiowski, R., Lynch, B., Croarkin,
110187. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pnpbp.2020.110187
40. Cairncross, M., & Miller, C. J. (2020). The effectiveness of mindfulness-based therapies
https://doi.org/10.1177/1087054715625301
41. Jylkkä, J., Ritakallio, L., Merzon, L., Kangas, S., Kliegel, M., Zuber, S., Hering, A.,
Laine, M., & Salmi, J. (2023). Assessment of goal-directed behavior and prospective
42. Boot, N., Nevicka, B., & Baas, M. (2020). Creativity in ADHD: goal-directed motivation
10.1177/1087054717727352
43. Barnard-Brak, L., & Kudesey, C. (2022). Reported Side or Adverse Effects Associated
with Medication Nonadherence Among Adolescents and Young Adults with Attention-
44. Pan, P. Y., Jonsson, U., Çakmak, S. S. Ş., Häge, A., Hohmann, S., Norrman, H. N.,
Buitelaar, J. K., Banaschewski, T., Cortese, S., Coghill, D., & Bölte, S. (2022). Headache
in ADHD as comorbidity and a side effect of medications: a systematic review and meta-
analysis. Psychological Medicine, 52(1), 14-25.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291721004141
45. Wynchank, D., Bijlenga, D., Beekman, A. T., Kooij, J. S., & Penninx, B. W. (2017).
46. Michielsen, M., Kleef, D., Bijlenga, D., Zwennes, C., Dijkhuizen, K., Smulders, J.,
Hazewinkel, A., Beekman, A. T. F., & Kooij, J. S. (2021). Response and side effects
using stimulant medication in older adults with ADHD: an observational archive study.
https://doi.org/10.1177/1087054720925884
47. BBC Panorama (2023, May 15). ADHD: Private clinics exposed by BBC undercover
investigation. https://www.bbc.co.uk/news/health-65534448
48. ADHD UK (2024, January). Situation Update: The ADHD Medication Crisis.
https://adhduk.co.uk/adhd-medication-crisis-report/
49. Ophir, Y. (2022). Reconsidering the safety profile of stimulant medications for ADHD.
2021-0007
50. Hertfordshire Partnership NHS (2017, July). Guidelines for the Pharmacological
pharmacological-management-of-adhd-july-2017.pdf
51. Psychiatry UK (2024). ADHD. Retrieved March 15, from
https://psychiatry-uk.com/adhd/#anchor4
52. Hayes, S. C., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Roche, B. (2001). Relational frame theory: A post-
53. Hayes, S. C., Strosahl, K. D., & Wilson, K. G. (1999). Acceptance and Commitment
54. Becker, S. P. (2021). Systematic review: Assessment of sluggish cognitive tempo over
the past decade. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry,
55. Becker, S. P., Leopold, D. R., Burns, G. L., Jarrett, M. A., Langberg, J. M., Marshall, S.
A., McBurnett, K., Waschbusch, D. A., & Willcutt, E. G. (2016). The internal, external,
and diagnostic validity of sluggish cognitive tempo: A meta-analysis and critical review.
Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 55(3), 163-178.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2015.12.006
56. Poczwardowski, A., Sherman, C. P., & Ravizza, K. (2004). Professional philosophy in
the sport psychology service delivery: Building on theory and practice. The Sport
57. Tod, D., & Eubank, M. (2020). Applied Sport, Exercise, and Performance Psychology:
Hayes, & S. G. Hofmann (Eds.), Process-based CBT: The science and core clinical
60. Timms, C. (2022. August 15). Neurodiversity: a process, not a ‘steady state’.
https://www.bps.org.uk/psychologist/neurodiversity-process-not-steady-state
61. Inzlicht, M., Schmeichel, B. J., & Macrae, C. N. (2014). Why self-control seems (but
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2013.12.009
62. Inzlicht, M., Werner, K. M., Briskin, J. L., & Roberts, B. W. (2021). Integrating models
https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-061020-105721
63. Inzlicht, M., & Schmeichel, B. J. (2012). What is ego depletion? Toward a mechanistic
64. ADDA Editorial Team (2022, December 7). ADHD Paralysis Is Real: Here Are 8 Ways
https://original.newsbreak.com/@jillian-enright-1590470/2420107533437-adhd-
paralysis-explained
66. Carminati, G. G., Carminati, F., & Zecca, G. (2023). Strangled by the Loop:
https://doi.org/10.4236/psych.2023.145044
67. Duckworth, A. L., Gendler, T. S., & Gross, J. J. (2014). Self-control in school-age
https://doi.org/10.1080/00461520.2014.926225
68. Duckworth, A. L., Gendler, T. S., & Gross, J. J. (2016). Situational strategies for self-
https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691615623247
69. Duckworth, A. L., Milkman, K. L., & Laibson, D. (2018). Beyond willpower: Strategies
for reducing failures of self-control. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 19(3),
102-129. https://doi.org/10.1177/1529100618821893
https://doi.org/10.5772/53963
71. Shaw, P., Eckstrand, K., Sharp, W., Blumenthal, J., Lerch, J. P., Greenstein, D. E. E. A.,
Clasen, L., Evans, A., Giedd, J., & Rapoport, J. L. (2007). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity
72. De Zeeuw, P., Schnack, H. G., Van Belle, J., Weusten, J., Van Dijk, S., Langen, M.,
Brouwer, R. M., van Engeland, H., & Durston, S. (2012). Differential brain development
with low and high IQ in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. PloS one, 7(4), e35770.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0035770
73. Milioni, A. L. V., Chaim, T. M., Cavallet, M., de Oliveira, N. M., Annes, M., Dos Santos,
B., Louzã, M., da Silva, M. A., Miguel, C. S., Serpa, M. H., Zanetti, M. V., Busatto, G.,
& Cunha, P. J. (2017). High IQ may “mask” the diagnosis of ADHD by compensating for
deficits in executive functions in treatment-naïve adults with ADHD. Journal of attention
74. Taylor, I. M., Smith, K., & Hunte, R. (2020). Motivational processes during physical
endurance tasks. Scandinavian journal of medicine & science in sports, 30(9), 1769-
1776. https://doi.org/10.1111/sms.13739
75. Kofler, M. J., Raiker, J. S., Sarver, D. E., Wells, E. L., & Soto, E. F. (2016). Is
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2016.04.004
76. Ng, Q. X., Ho, C. Y. X., Chan, H. W., Yong, B. Z. J., & Yeo, W. S. (2017). Managing
10.1016/j.ctim.2017.08.018
77. Silva, A. P., Prado, S. O., Scardovelli, T. A., Boschi, S. R., Campos, L. C., & Frere, A. F.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0122119
78. Rassovsky, Y., & Alfassi, T. (2019). Attention improves during physical exercise in
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02747
79. Horne, G., Fort, G., Drewes, L., & Quercia-Smale, S. (2024). ADHD and desire-goal
& Krauel, K. (2016). Improving interference control in ADHD patients with transcranial
https://doi.org/10.3389/fncel.2016.00072
81. Salehinejad, M. A., Nejati, V., Mosayebi-Samani, M., Mohammadi, A., Wischnewski,
M., Kuo, M. F., Avenanti, A., Vicario, C. M., & Nitsche, M. A. (2020). Transcranial
direct current stimulation in ADHD: a systematic review of efficacy, safety, and protocol-
https://doi.org/10.1007/s12264-020-00501-x
978–999. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023961
83. Kroenke, K., Spitzer, R. L., & Williams, J. B. (2001). The PHQ‐9: validity of a brief
https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1525-1497.2001.016009606.x
84. Becker, S. P., Burns, G. L., Smith, Z. R., & Langberg, J. M. (2020). Sluggish cognitive
85. Lee, S., Burns, G. L., Beauchaine, T. P., & Becker, S. P. (2016). Bifactor latent structure
cognitive tempo symptom dimension in children: Sluggish cognitive tempo and ADHD-
19. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-013-9714-3
87. Rogers, D. C., Dittner, A. J., Rimes, K. A., & Chalder, T. (2017). Fatigue in an adult
88. Sáez-Francàs, N., Alegre, J., Calvo, N., Ramos-Quiroga, J. A., Ruiz, E., Hernández-Vara,
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2012.04.041
89. Nigg, J. T., Sibley, M. H., Thapar, A., & Karalunas, S. L. (2020). Development of
ADHD: Etiology, heterogeneity, and early life course. Annual review of developmental
90. Murphy, K. (2005). Psychosocial treatments for ADHD in teens and adults: A practice‐
https://doi.org/10.1002/jclp.20123
91. Fleming, A. P., & McMahon, R. J. (2012). Developmental context and treatment
principles for ADHD among college students. Clinical child and family psychology
92. Boyer, B. E., Geurts, H. M., Prins, P. J., & Van der Oord, S. (2015). Two novel CBTs for
adolescents with ADHD: the value of planning skills. European child & adolescent
94. Larsson, I., Aili, K., Lönn, M., Svedberg, P., Nygren, J. M., Ivarsson, A., & Johansson, P.
(2023). Sleep interventions for children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sleep.2022.12.021
95. Chang, J. P. C., Su, K. P., Mondelli, V., & Pariante, C. M. (2018). Omega-3
96. Nahidi, M., Soleimanpour, S., & Emadzadeh, M. (2024). Probiotics as a Promising
https://doi.org/10.1177/10870547241228828
97. Mészáros, A., Czobor, P., Bálint, S., Komlósi, S., Simon, V., & Bitter, I. (2009).
https://doi.org/10.1017/S1461145709990198