Implementing Pretraining To Optimise Learning in I
Implementing Pretraining To Optimise Learning in I
Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of California, Santa Barbara, California, USA
ABSTRACT
Background: In recent years, immersive virtual reality in education has garnered attention, however, there have been mixed
findings on the efficacy of IVR in education. Thus, exploring which strategies are effective in transferring learning from IVR to
real-world applications is imperative.
Objective: This study aims to investigate the efficacy of the pretraining principle for acquiring procedural knowledge and skills
in an IVR setting that will transfer to real-world environments.
Methods: Ninety-three participants were randomly assigned to either a pretraining or no-pretraining group. The pretraining
group watched a video before the IVR lesson, providing the names and characteristics of the physical objects and actions of
a micropipette, while the no-pretraining group did not receive this video. During the IVR lesson, participants completed a
training phase, followed by a four-step serial dilution test. Afterwards, all participants completed a modified serial dilution test
in a real-life setting, along with a knowledge test and assessment on cognitive load, presence, self-efficacy and demographic
information.
Results and Conclusions: Analyses demonstrated the pretraining group scored significantly higher on the knowledge test and
committed fewer errors in the real-life serial dilution task compared to the no-pretraining group. The pretraining group also
reported lower cognitive load, with no observable differences in presence, self-efficacy ratings or errors during the virtual serial
dilution task between groups. Theoretical and practical implications are discussed.
1 | Objective and Rationale Learning in virtual reality can be both distracting and moti-
vating (Mayer, Makransky, and Parong 2022). Therefore, it is
Suppose we want to help someone learn a technical procedural crucial to explore effective strategies for acquiring new skills in
skill, such as how to use pipettes to transfer liquid from one con- IVR technologies that can be transferred to real-world applica-
tainer to another, through training in immersive virtual reality tions. The development of information technology continues to
(IVR). How can we increase the chances that the learner will be challenge all facets of education and training as technology pro-
able to transfer what they learned in a virtual environment to gresses to evolve from traditional teaching pedagogies to more
perform in a real-world environment? In the present study, we ex- technology-based ways of teaching. The importance of investi-
amine the instructional effectiveness of providing pretraining in gating what is the best practice or guidelines in developing IVR
the names and characteristics of the physical objects and actions curriculum is crucial for enhancing learning outcomes. This
represented in the virtual environment through a short video. study aims to investigate the efficacy of the pretraining principle
This is an open access article under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial License, which permits use, distribution and reproduction in any medium, provided the
original work is properly cited and is not used for commercial purposes.
© 2024 The Author(s). Journal of Computer Assisted Learning published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd.
In another study, Meyer, Omdahl, and Makransky (2019) used In conclusion, there is encouraging evidence that value-added
the pretraining principle in an IVR lesson in biology. They asked research can help identify a collection of features that aid learn-
some students to study a pretraining picture consisting of an ing in IVR, such as using spoken words rather than printed
educationally relevant biological cross-sectioned cell presented words, using conversational language rather than formal lan-
with key terms of the parts of the cell for 1 min and 30 s whereas guage and asking learners to engage in generative learning activ-
others had no pretraining prior to engaging in either an IVR ities such as summarising during pauses in the lesson. Although
environment or before watching a video on a biology lesson on some forms of pretraining have been successful in IVR (Meyer,
cells. Their results indicated that the IVR group with pretraining Omdahl, and Makransky 2019; Petersen et al. 2020), other
scored significantly higher on the retention and transfer post- forms have not (Lawson and Mayer 2024a) and none have been
tests than the IVR group with no pretraining. However, they did used for learning procedural skills, so there is a need to better
not find a difference on post-test scores between the video con- clarify which forms of pretraining are effective for learning a
ditions with and without pretraining, suggesting that pretrain- procedural skill. The present study fills this gap by examining
ing may be particularly helpful in learning academic content in a clearcut form of pretraining that has not yet been applied to
IVR. These findings are in line with studies (Mayer, Makransky, learning in IVR—exposing learners to video-based images of
and Parong 2022; Parong and Mayer 2020) that demonstrate the what the key elements in the IVR lesson look like in the real
limitations of learning in IVR settings, such as the distractibility world. The reason why we are interested in whether pretraining
caused by excessive sensory stimuli that can hinder learning. could be helpful in the present study is that it has been shown
3 of 16
to be effective in helping students learn in multimedia learning Hypothesis 2. The pretraining group will perform better than
environments involving computer screens. the no-pretraining group on the in-person pipetting performance
test (based on the number of correctly completed tasks and num-
ber of errors committed).
2 | Theoretical Framework and Hypotheses
Hypothesis 3. The pretraining group will perform better than
2.1 | Learning Outcomes the no-pretraining group on the pipetting knowledge test (based on
proportion correct).
According to the cognitive theory of multimedia learning
(CTML), learners have limited cognitive resources avail-
able for processing information in our working memory 2.2 | Extraneous Cognitive Load
(Mayer 2021, 2022). This theory posits that for meaningful
learning to occur learners must engage in appropriate cog- When the learning experience is overwhelming, as can happen
nitive processing during learning: selecting the relevant pre- in IVR, learners may become distracted by extraneous aspects of
sented information for further processing in working memory, the experience (Parong and Mayer 2020; Petersen, Klingenberg,
mentally organising it into a coherent representation and inte- and Makransky 2022). For instance, participants who viewed a
grating it with relevant prior knowledge. However, processing biology lesson in IVR reported more extraneous cognitive load
capacity in working memory is limited so when the demands and showed less engagement based on EEG measures than
are too great, learners are hindered in their knowledge con- those who viewed a slideshow lesson (Parong and Mayer 2020).
struction. When a multimedia lesson is not designed to aid the Similarly, Petersen, Klingenberg, and Makransky (2022) found
learner in selecting, organising and integrating complex infor- that students learning with IVR reported higher levels of extra-
mation in a manner that facilitates effective learning, it can neous cognitive load when compared to students learning in a
lead to cognitive overload. Cognitive overload occurs when real-life setting. When learners are distracted, this creates extra-
the task or information a learner attempts to process exceeds neous cognitive load—cognitive processing that does not serve
their working memory capacity. Several scenarios can lead to the instructional goal—that can waste the learner's limited
this situation, such as attempting to process an instructional processing capacity during learning. However, with pretrain-
message that contains an excessive amount of irrelevant in- ing, learners can be more focused on core material in the lesson
formation, trying to integrate novel and complex information and thereby reduce extraneous cognitive load. This leads to our
at an unmanageable pace, or encountering difficulty under- fourth prediction:
standing a topic due to a lack of prior knowledge. These types
of instances can lead to cognitive overload, which can lead to a Hypothesis 4. The pretraining group will produce a lower
lack of understanding of the material (Mayer 2021, 2022; Paas mean rating of extraneous cognitive load than the no-pretraining
and Sweller 2022; Sweller, Ayres, and Kalyuga 2011). group.
5 of 16
TABLE 1 | Six hypotheses and two research questions. or Pacific Islander, 5 classified themselves as Middle Eastern or
North African, and 1 classified themselves as other and opted
Pretraining group
to identify as Indian. The ages of the participants ranged from
will perform Pretraining group
18 to 22 years (Mage = 19.00, SDage = 1.13). Twenty-six students
No. better on will have
reported to be first-generation college students. We originally
1 Carrying out More correctly completed recruited 104 participants but had to eliminate 11 participants
the pipetting steps and fewer due to technical difficulties (n = 7), simulation sickness in the
procedure in IVR errors committed virtual reality simulation (n = 1), missing data (n = 2) and not
being 18 years of age or older (n = 1), yielding a final sample of
2 Carrying out the More correctly completed
93 participants. The experiment employed a between-subjects
pipetting procedure steps and fewer
design with 47 participants in the pretraining group and 46 in
in person errors committed
the no-pretraining group. Participants were randomly assigned
3 Answering questions Higher proportion correct to either an experimental group (i.e., pretraining group) or a con-
on the pipetting on the knowledge test trol group (i.e., no-pretraining group). The experimental group
knowledge test watched a pretraining video focused on the names and charac-
teristics of a pipette and a brief description of a completed serial
4 Experiencing lower Lower self-rating of
dilution, while the control group received no pretraining. Our
extraneous cognitive cognitive load
dependent variables included knowledge retention, assessed by a
load during learning
knowledge test and pipetting procedural performance measured
5 Experiencing a higher Higher self-rating through pipetting errors and accuracy in completing a four-step
sense of presence of presence serial dilution task. We also assessed extraneous cognitive load
during learning and sense of presence as additional dependent variables, using a
post-survey for both. The independent variable was the presence
6 Experiencing higher Higher self-rating
or absence of the pretraining video.
self-efficacy for the of self-efficacy
pipetting procedure
We used the software programme G*Power to conduct our power The prequestionnaire consisted of 11 items administered on
analysis. Our goal was to obtain a 0.80 power to detect a medium a laptop computer via the web-based platform, Qualtrics. The
effect size of 0.6 at the standard 0.05 alpha error probability. Our first five questions assessed general interest and experience in
recommended sample size was 90 participants. The participants science, with response options of ‘Yes’ or ‘No’ (e.g., ‘Science
were 93 college students recruited from the Psychology Subject is my favourite subject in school. (a) Yes. (b) No.’; Parong and
Pool at a large university in California. They received credit to- Mayer 2018). Responses were coded as: 1 for ‘No’ and 2 for
wards fulfilling a course requirement. Concerning gender, 36 ‘Yes’. The scores from these five questions were averaged to
students identified as male, 55 identified as female and 2 iden- obtain a total mean rating for general interest and experience
tified as non-binary. Concerning ethnic/racial category, 40 stu- in science. The next two questions asked about knowledge of
dents classified themselves as White, 18 classified themselves as micropipette and conducting a serial dilution. For the micro-
Latinx/Chicano/a, 6 classified themselves as Black, 36 classified pipette question, participants were asked to rate their knowl-
themselves as Asian, 4 classified themselves as Native Hawaiian edge of micropipetting on a scale from 1 ‘I don't know what a
FIGURE 1 | Screenshots of the pretraining video: (a) introducing the micropipette. (b) explaining the parts and characteristics of a micropipette.
(c) demonstrating how to insert a tip to a micropipette. (d) demonstrating how to aspirate liquid. (e) demonstrate how to dispense liquid. (f) briefly
introduce what a complete serial dilution looks like.
7 of 16
the Psychology Department at the University of Copenhagen. participants could transfer their simulation-based procedure
The virtual simulation contained two sections. The first section to the real world. To keep the materials consistent across tasks,
served as a tutorial for using a pipette to extract and transfer liq- we used physical materials that were closely similar to the ma-
uid from one test tube to another, in which the participant was terials in the virtual simulation. After the completion of the
prompted to carry out 15 steps. At every step, the virtual agent virtual simulation task, participants were asked to remove
first demonstrated the task, provided step-by-step instructions on the virtual headset and shown the real pipetting setup. The
how to complete it, and gave feedback at the end of every step. researchers showed the participants the physical materials
Each step needed to be completed accurately to move on to the to complete the task along with the printed instructions. The
following step. If participants were stuck on a particular step, printed four-step serial dilution instructions were a modified
they were allowed to ask the researcher for help. The purpose version of the virtual four-step serial dilution instructions, as
of the first section was to teach individuals on the basic safety seen in Figure 5.
guidelines when working in a laboratory and how to use a mi-
cropipette, which were necessary for conducting a serial dilution. A researcher was responsible for observing and evaluating the
participants performance during the completion of the serial
The second section served as the virtual pipetting test, where dilution on a printed assessment form. The scoring of the as-
participants were tasked to complete a four-step serial dilu- sessment followed the same structure as the virtual assess-
tion by applying the skills they learned in the first section of ment, consisting of two sections. The first section was scored
the training tutorial. In the virtual pipetting test, participants as either True or False, with True scored as 1 and False scored
were shown an image of the instructions on a monitor and as 0. The minimum score a participant could receive was 0,
were told the virtual agent would no longer be available to whereas the maximum score a participant could receive was
help them. The participants were given the same materials 5. The researcher indicated whether the participant put on a
presented from the first section of the lesson but were tasked lab coat and gloves as True or False and whether the partici-
to independently complete the four-step serial dilution. After pant correctly completed each of the four test tubes as True
the participant felt they completed the four-step serial dilu- or False.
tion, they were instructed to push on a button in the virtual
lab to see their feedback from the test. The feedback was dis- The second section was scored as a frequency and was summed
played on the monitor and outlined whether they put on gloves together to produce a total score of pipetting errors committed
and a lab coat, the number of times they tilted the pipette, the in the in-person serial dilution test. The minimum score one
number of times they did not use the first stop on the plunger, could receive was 0, whereas the maximum score one could
the number of times they did not use the second stop of the receive was unlimited. The researcher recorded the number of
plunger and the number of times they cross contaminated the instances in which a participant committed the following errors:
liquid from the beakers and the tubes. tilting the pipette tip, pressing the plunger after inserting the pi-
pette in liquid, dispensing liquid without reaching the second
The feedback shown to the participant was generated into stop and cross contaminating the pipette tip across all test tubes
a file in the virtual reality headset, wherein the researcher and beakers.
was able to transfer the participant data into a computer. The
scoring was calculated based on whether the participant ac-
curately transferred the required solution in each four test 3.3.6 | Knowledge Test
tubes. If a test tube contained the correct amount of liquid
the virtual simulation marked it either as ‘True’ for correct The knowledge test, which was administered in Qualtrics, was
and ‘False’ for incorrect. The virtual simulation also indi- intended to assess participants' knowledge of the scientific pro-
cated whether the participant put on a lab coat and gloves as cedure of pipetting and consisted of 15 questions (i.e., four mul-
‘True’ for correct and ‘False’ for incorrect. During the com- tiple choice questions), 10 true-false questions and one sorting
pletion of the serial dilution, the virtual simulation recorded question (α = −0.161). The low internal reliability reflects the
the number of instances in which a participant committed the construction of the test, which involves three different ques-
following errors: tilting the pipette tip, pressing the plunger tion formats and individual items that tap completely different
after inserting the pipette in liquid, dispensing liquid without pieces of information. The multiple-choice items consisted of
reaching the second stop, using a contaminated pipette tip and four multiple-choice options (e.g., ‘What happens during a se-
improperly mixing the beaker liquid. Figure 2 shows a series rial dilution? (a) The concentration of a substance is reduced at
of screenshots of the virtual pipetting simulation for the train- each step. (b) Water is distilled at each step to increase the con-
ing phase. Figure 3 shows a screenshot of the virtual pipetting centration of a substance. (c) Each step involves adding a new
instructions presented in the second section of the simulation substance. (d) Each step reduces the acidity of a substance’).
involving carrying out a serial dilution in IVR without guid- The true-false questions consisted of 10 questions (e.g., ‘Push
ance. Figure 4 shows images of the real-world lab setup during the plunger to the second stop during aspiration. (a) True (b)
the in-person serial dilution task. False’). The sorting question consisted of eight statements dis-
played in a random order, which required the participant to
drag and drop each statement in the correct order of the pro-
3.3.5 | In-Person Pipetting Test cedure (e.g., ‘Please indicate which step each statement cor-
responds to from first to last step. (a) Put lab coat (b) Put on
The in-person pipetting task was set up similarly to the virtual gloves (c) Pick up pipette (d) Adjust volume setting (e) Attach
pipetting task using concrete objects to determine whether pipette tip (f) Aspirate liquid (g) Dispense liquid (h) Discard
3.4 | Postquestionnaire
9 of 16
FIGURE 4 | Pictures of the real-world lab setup of the in-person serial dilution testing phase. (a) Lab setup for the in-person serial dilution task.
(b) Completed 4-step serial dilution.
4 | Results
and standard deviation for both the number of errors and cor- significant interaction between the pipetting tasks and condition,
rect solutions in the virtual simulation across the two groups. F(1, 91) = 5.130, p = 0.026, η2p = 0.053. Specifically, individuals in
Contrary to predictions, no significant difference was found be- the pretraining group committed approximately half the number
tween groups in the number of errors produced during the serial of errors (M = 6.30, SD = 6.26) during the in-person pipetting task
dilution task in the virtual simulation, t(91) = 1.046, p = 0.298, when compared to the virtual task (M = 11.68, SD = 9.07), whereas
d = 0.217, 95% CI [−0.191, 0.624], Similarly, the groups did not individuals in the no-pretraining group demonstrated a relatively
differ significantly on the proportion of correct solutions on consistent number of errors on the virtual pipetting task (M = 13.63,
the serial dilution task in the virtual simulation, t(91) = −1.263, SD = 8.90) and the in-person pipetting task (M = 12.43, SD = 9.18).
p = 0.210, d = −0.261, 95% CI [−0.668, 0.148]. We conclude that
Hypothesis 1 was not supported. Hypothesis 3. Does the pretraining group perform better
than the no-pretraining group on the knowledge test?
Hypothesis 2. Does the pretraining group perform better
than the no-pretraining group on pipetting performance in the in- The third hypothesis predicted that the pretraining group will per-
person pipetting test? form better than the no-pretraining group on the pipetting knowl-
edge test. Table 2 shows the mean number of correct responses on
The second hypothesis predicted that the pretraining group will the knowledge test (and SD) for the two groups. The groups signifi-
perform better than the no-pretraining group on the in-person cantly differed on the number of correct responses on the pipetting
pipetting test. Table 2 shows the mean and standard deviation knowledge test, t(91) = −3.773, p = 0.001, d = −0.729, 95% CI (−1.15,
for the number of errors and correct solutions in the in-person −0.31), indicating that those in the pretraining group scored higher
pipetting test for both groups. The groups significantly differed on the knowledge test than those in the no-pretraining group. We
in the number of errors produced during the serial dilution task, conclude that Hypothesis 3 was supported.
t(91) = 3.773, p < 0.001, d = 0.784, 95% CI [0.35, 1.20], indicating
that those in the pretraining group produced fewer pipetting er- Hypothesis 4. Does the pretraining group produce a lower
rors than those in the no-pretraining group. In contrast to our mean rating of extraneous cognitive load than the no-pretraining
predictions, the groups did not significantly differ on the pro- group?
portion of correct solutions on the in-person serial dilution task,
t(91) = −1.029, p = 0.306, d = −0.213, 95% CI [−0.621, 0.195]. The fourth hypothesis predicted that the pretraining group will
Thus, we conclude that Hypothesis 2 was partially supported. produce a lower mean rating of extraneous cognitive load than
the control group. Table 2 shows the mean number of ratings for
In addition to our above analysis, we conducted a 2 × 2 ANOVA extraneous cognitive load (and SD) for the groups. A Levene's test
to investigate whether the number of pipetting errors changed for equality of variances was found to be violated for the pres-
across pipetting tasks and conditions. Our results indicated a ent analysis, F(1, 91) = 9.804, p = 0.002. Given the violation of the
11 of 16
assumption of equal variances, our degrees of freedom were ad- In contrast, as summarised in Table 4, we did find that Condition
justed from 91 to 71. As seen in Table 2, the groups significantly was a significant predictor of extraneous cognitive load, indicat-
differed on the number of ratings for extraneous cognitive load, ing those in the pretraining condition had a 0.263 reduction of
t(71) = 2.012, p = 0.048, d = 0.42, 95% CI [0.007, 0.830]. Specifically, extraneous cognitive load, when compared to the no-pretraining
those in the pretraining group reported experiencing less extra- condition (b = −0.263, SE = 0.130, p = 0.046). Additionally, as
neous cognitive load than did those in the no-pretraining group. summarised in Table 5, we found that extraneous cognitive
These results demonstrate support for Hypothesis 4. load was also a significant predictor of total pipetting errors,
indicating a one-unit increase in extraneous cognitive load re-
Hypothesis 5. Does the pretraining group produce a higher sulted in an increase of 3.450 in committing an error (b = 3.450,
mean rating of presence than the no-pretraining group? SE = 1.563, p = 0.029) (see Table 5).
The fifth hypothesis predicted that the pretraining group Finally, we found that Condition was still a significant predic-
will produce a higher mean rating of presence than the no- tor of total pipetting errors when including presence and ex-
pretraining group. Table 2 shows that mean number of ratings traneous cognitive load in the model (b = −5.202, SE = 1.656,
for presence (and standard deviation) for the groups. The groups p = 0.002); this suggests that those in the pretraining condition
did not significantly differ on mean rating for sense of presence, had a 5.202 reduction in committing a pipetting error. However,
t(91) = 0.141, p = 0.888, d = 0.029, 95% CI (−0.377, 0.436). The as summarised in Table 6, after analysing the indirect effects, re-
findings do not provide support for Hypothesis 5. sults reveal Presence (ab = −0.025, 95% CI [−0.756, 0.567]) and
Extraneous Cognitive Load (ab = −0.908, 95% CI [−2.650, 0.044])
Hypothesis 6. Does the pretraining group produce a higher do not significantly mediate the relationship between Condition
mean rating of self-efficacy post-survey than the no-pretraining and total pipetting errors (see Table 6).
group?
The sixth hypothesis predicted that the pretraining group will 4.2.2 | Knowledge Test Score
produce a higher mean rating on the self-efficacy post-survey
than the no-pretraining group. Table 2 shows the mean ratings A mediation analysis was conducted on SPSS PROCESS macro
on the self-efficacy post-survey (and standard deviation) for the extension version 4. Our independent variable Condition was
two groups. We conducted an ANCOVA, with rating on the self- coded as 0 for the no-pretraining condition and 1 for the pre-
efficacy pre-survey as a covariate. Overall, the groups did not training condition. As shown in Table 7, our results indicated
differ significantly, F(1, 90) = 1.696, p = 0.196, η2p = 0.018. We that Condition was not a significant predictor of presence
conclude that Hypothesis 6 was not supported. (b = −0.206, SE = 1.46, p = 0.888); and as shown in Table 9,
presence was not a significant predictor of the total score in the
knowledge test.
4.2 | Research Questions: Does Extraneous
Cognitive Load and Presence Mediate TABLE 3 | Condition predicting presence.
the Relationship Between the Pretraining
Condition and Learning Outcomes? β b SE t p
Condition −0.029 −0.206 1.460 −0.140 0.888
Learning outcomes were operationalised using two separate as-
sessments: the number of pipetting errors committed during the
in-person pipetting task (i.e., a behavioural measure) and the
TABLE 4 | Condition predicting extraneous cognitive load.
score received on the knowledge test (i.e., a knowledge-based
measure). We chose to keep these two assessments distinct, as β b SE t p
they represent different dimensions of learning, one focused on
practical skill execution and the other on cognitive understand- Condition −0.412 −0.263 0.130 −2.023 0.046
ing. One set of research questions investigates whether presence Note: Bold indicates statistically significant values.
or extraneous cognitive load mediates the relationship between
pretraining conditions and learning outcomes, based on pipet-
ting errors and score on knowledge test scores. TABLE 5 | Condition, presence and extraneous cognitive load
predicting total pipetting errors.
13 of 16
those who experienced the IVR lesson without answering any constructed pretraining in the names and characteristics of the
questions. key objects and actions. This can be accomplished by showing a
video of the key parts and their possible actions.
Overall, the main contribution of this study to the knowledge
base is that pretraining in the names and characteristics of the
key objects in the lesson through short video was effective in 5.4 | Limitations and Future Directions
promoting transfer of learning of a technical procedural skill
from IVR to tasks outside of IVR including performing the tech- As an early study of how to improve learning of technical pro-
nical procedure with real objects and answering questions about cedural skills in IVR, this work is subject to several limitations.
the procedure as well as reducing extraneous cognitive load This was a short-term lab study with immediate tests, so it
during instruction. would be useful to examine whether pretraining is effective in
more realistic classroom environments over longer time periods.
This study involved a narrow learner population of young adults
5.2 | Theoretical Contributions at a highly selective university and focused on a single learning
topic, so it would be useful to examine whether pretraining is
The results from this study indicate the value-added of watch- effective with other kinds of learners and with other kinds of
ing a pretraining video before learning in IVR, specifically for technical procedural tasks. Although this work encourages the
transferring learning of a technical procedure skill performed use of pretraining for training of technical procedures in IVR,
in a virtual reality simulation to performing and using the skill work is needed to determine the robustness of the effects.
in the real world. The results of this study are in line with the
pretraining principle developed from the CTML, which states While the gross motor actions were designed to replicate real
that people learn more deeply from a multimedia lesson when micropipette handling, the execution of fine motor actions was
they know the names and characteristics of the main concepts subject to some limitations. Students received visual guidance
before experiencing the multimedia lesson (Mayer 2021; Mayer on micropipette use within the IVR lesson; however, discrepan-
and Fiorella 2022). Most of the previous research investigating cies occurred between the visual demonstrations and the pre-
the pretraining principle have been conducted on conceptual cise fine motor skills required for task completion. Moreover,
learning (Mayer 2021; Mayer and Fiorella 2022). However, this the task demanded more than replicating movements. Students
is the first study to incorporate the pretraining principle in the had to remember specific buttons and their corresponding ac-
acquisition of technical procedural skills. The proposed mech- tions, potentially imposing additional cognitive demands. For
anism is that pretraining reduces extraneous cognitive load instance, while the tutorial demonstrated using the thumb to
during training by helping learners know the names and charac- push the plunger to the first stop, students were required to use
teristics of the key components discussed in the lesson, so learn- their index finger and press halfway down the button to achieve
ers can allocate more of their cognitive processing resources to the same result. Subsequently, after completing the tutorial
learning the procedure, which is a form of essential processing phase, students had to recall which actions aligned with specific
(Mayer 2021, 2022). In addition, pretraining activates relevant buttons during the virtual serial dilution task. Research com-
prior knowledge about the physical objects and actions that are paring the usage of classic VR controllers and ergonomic con-
represented in the simulation, so that learners can connect what trollers, such as Valve's Knuckles presents encouraging results
they are doing in the simulation to concrete knowledge in their (Noblecourt et al. 2021). After familiarity with Knuckles in an
long-term memory, which is a form of generative processing initial study, participants demonstrated quicker task completion
during learning (Mayer 2021, 2022). and increased manipulation abilities at a subsequent task, show-
ing potential advantages of using more natural-like controllers,
such as Knuckles. Despite evidence supporting the pretraining
5.3 | Practical Contributions video's effectiveness in reducing errors during the in-person
serial dilution task, future studies might benefit from aligning
A practical question is whether and how IVR should be used fine motor actions more closely with natural actions. This ad-
for training of technical procedural skills. Based on the results justment could potentially yield stronger effects.
of this study, it is possible to recommend using IVR to enhance
learning outcomes, particularly for lessons that involve learning Another limitation pertained to the awareness of researchers
a technical scientific procedure. Our results are consistent with regarding participants' assigned conditions. Researchers were
the findings of Petersen, Klingenberg, and Makransky (2022) tasked with capturing real-time data as participants completed
that found learning a procedural lesson in IVR to predict real- the task. While instructed to observe and record participants'
life performance on a transfer test. Participants in the pretrain- actions during the in-person serial dilution task, the possibility
ing condition made fewer pipetting errors on an in-person task exists that some data might have been missed, or certain antici-
and scored higher on the knowledge test compared to those as- pated behaviours might have influenced observations based on
signed to the no pretaining condition, suggesting the potential participants' assigned conditions. All researchers underwent
benefits of including a pretraining video before immersing stu- extensive training with the lead researcher to standardise data
dents in a training simulation. This study extends the pretrain- recording and action and error identification. However, human
ing principle beyond learning conceptual knowledge to training error remains a possibility. Particularly, our study relied on a
of procedural knowledge. When the goal is to help students single researcher for data collection during the in-person serial
transfer what they have learned in a training simulation to per- dilution task. Future studies could mitigate potential errors by
formance in the real world, we recommend including carefully employing two researchers for data collection and conducting
Author Contributions Jensen, L. X., and F. Konradsen. 2017. “A Review of the Use of Virtual
Reality Head-Mounted Displays in Education and Training.” Education
Cynthia Y. Delgado: conceptualization, data curation, formal anal- and Information Technologies 23, no. 4: 1515–1529. https://doi.org/10.
ysis, writing – original draft, methodology, investigation, supervision, 1007/s10639 -017-9676-0.
writing – review and editing, software, project administration. Richard
E. Mayer: conceptualization, data curation, writing – original draft, Johnson, C. I., S. K. Bailey, B. L. Schroeder, and M. D. Marraffino.
methodology, investigation, supervision, writing – review and editing, 2022. “Procedural Learning in Virtual Reality: The Role of Immersion,
funding acquisition, resources, project administration. Interactivity, and Spatial Ability.” Technology, Mind, and Behavior 3, no.
4: 146. https://doi.org/10.1037/tmb000 0087.
Lawson, A. P., A. M. Martella, K. LaBonte, et al. 2024. “Confounded
Acknowledgements
or Controlled? A Systematic Review of Media Comparison Studies in
We appreciate that Guido Makransky and Gustav Peterson graciously Immersive Virtual Reality Research in STEM Education.” Educational
provided the pipetting simulation. This project was supported by Grant Psychology Review 36: 69.
N000142112047 from the Office of Naval Research.
Lawson, A., and R. E. Mayer. 2024a. “Individual Differences in
Executive Function Affect Learning With Immersive Virtual Reality.”
Disclosure Journal of Computer Assisted Learning 40, no. 3: 1068–1082.
The authors have nothing to report. Lawson, A., and R. E. Mayer. 2024b. “Effect of Pre-Training and Role of
Working Memory Characteristics in Learning With Immersive Virtual
Reality.” International Journal of Human Computer Interaction: 1–18.
Conflicts of Interest
Makransky, G., N. K. Andreasen, S. Baceviciute, and R. E. Mayer. 2021.
The authors declare no conflicts of interest. “Immersive Virtual Reality Increases Liking but Not Learning With
a Science Simulation and Generative Learning Strategies Promote
Data Availability Statement Learning in Immersive Virtual Reality.” Journal of Educational
Psychology 113, no. 4: 719–735. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu000 0473.
The data that support the findings of this study are available from the
corresponding author upon reasonable request. Makransky, G., T. Terkildsen, and R. E. Mayer. 2019. “Adding Immersive
Virtual Reality to a Science Lab Simulation Causes More Presence but
Less Learning.” Learning and Instruction 60: 225–236. https://doi.org/
References 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2 017.12.0 07.
Mayer, R. E. 2021. Multimedia Learning. 3rd ed. New York: Cambridge
Akgün, M., and B. Atici. 2022. “The Effects of Immersive Virtual Reality
University Press.
Environments on students' Academic Achievement: A Meta-A nalytical
and Meta-T hematic Study.” Participatory Educational Research 9, no. 3: Mayer, R. E. 2022. “Cognitive Theory of Multimedia Learning.” In The
111–131. https://doi.org/10.17275/per.22.57.9.3. Cambridge Handbook of Multimedia Learning, edited by R. E. Mayer
and L. Fiorella, 3rd ed., 57–72. Washington, DC: Cambridge University
Andersen, M. S., and G. Makransky. 2020. “The Validation And Further
Press.
Development of a Multidimensional Cognitive Load Scale for Virtual
Environments.” Journal of Computer Assisted Learning 37, no. 1: 183– Mayer, R. E., A. Mathias, and K. Wetzell. 2002. “Fostering
196. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcal.12478. Understanding of Multimedia Messages Through Pre-
Training:
15 of 16
Evidence for a Two- Stage Theory of Mental Model Construction.” Rogers, C., H. El-Mounaryi, T. M. Wasfy, and J. Satterwhite. 2017.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied 8: 147–154. “Assessment of STEM e-L earning in an Immersive Virtual Reality (VR)
Environment.” Computers in Education Journal 8: 4.
Mayer, R. E., and L. Fiorella. 2022. “Principles for Managing Essential
Processing in Multimedia Learning: Segmenting, Pre-Training, and Schunk, D. H., and M. K. DiBenedetto. 2016. “Self-Efficacy Theory
Modality Principles.” In The Cambridge Handbook of Multimedia in Education.” In Handbook of Motivation at School, edited by K. R.
Learning, edited by R. E. Mayer and L. Fiorella, 3rd ed., 243–260. New Wentzel and D. B. Miele, 2nd ed., 34–54. New York: Routledge.
York: Cambridge University Press.
Selzer, M. N., N. F. Gazcón, and M. L. Larrea. 2019. “Effects of Virtual
Mayer, R. E., G. Makransky, and J. Parong. 2022. “The Promise and Presence and Learning Outcome Using Low- End Virtual Reality
Pitfalls of Learning in Immersive Virtual Reality.” International Journal Systems.” Displays 59: 9–15. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.displa.2 019.
of Human Computer Interaction 39, no. 11: 2229–2238. https://doi.org/ 04.0 02.
10.1080/10447318.2 022.2108563.
Smutny, P. 2023. “Learning With Virtual Reality: A Market Analysis
Meyer, O. A., M. K. Omdahl, and G. Makransky. 2019. “Investigating of Educational and Training Applications.” Interactive Learning
the Effect of Pre-Training When Learning Through Immersive Virtual Environments 31, no. 10: 6133–6146.
Reality and Video: A Media and Methods Experiment.” Computers &
Sweller, J., P. Ayres, and S. Kalyuga. 2011. Cognitive Load Theory. New
Education 140: 103603. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.compedu.2 019.103603.
York: Springer.
Moreno, R., and R. E. Mayer. 2002. “Learning Science in Virtual Reality
Tai, K., J. Hong, C. Tsai, C. Lin, and Y. H. Hung. 2022. “Virtual Reality
Multimedia Environments: Role of Methods and Media.” Journal of
for Car-Detailing Skill Development: Learning Outcomes of Procedural
Educational Psychology 94, no. 3: 598–610. https://doi.org/10.1037/
Accuracy and Performance Quality Predicted by VR Self-Efficacy, VR
0022- 0 663.94.3.598.
Using Anxiety, VR Learning Interest and Flow Experience.” Computers
Moreno, R., and R. E. Mayer. 2004. “Personalized Messages That & Education 182: 104458. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.compedu.2 022.
Promote Science Learning in Virtual Environments.” Journal of 104458.
Educational Psychology 96, no. 1: 165–173. https://doi.org/10.1037/
Villena-Taranilla, R., S. Tirado-Olivares, R. Cózar-Gutiérrez, and J.
0022-0 663.96.1.165.
A. González- Calero. 2022. “Effects of Virtual Reality on Learning
Noblecourt, S., G. Bourgoin, V. Havard, and D. Baudry. 2021. “Evaluating Outcomes in K-6 Education: A Meta-A nalysis.” Educational Research
the Influence of Interaction Technology on Procedural Learning Review 35: 100434.
Using Virtual Reality.” In Proceedings of the 27th ACM Symposium on
Witmer, B. G., C. J. Jerome, and M. J. Singer. 2005. “The Factor
Virtual Reality Software and Technology. New York, NY: Association for
Structure of the Presence Questionnaire.” Presence Teleoperators and
Computing Machinery. https://doi.org/10.1145/3 489849.3 489918.
Virtual Environments 14, no. 3: 298–312. https://doi.org/10.1162/10547
Paas, F., and J. Sweller. 2022. “Implications of Cognitive Load Theory 4605323384654.
for Multimedia Learning.” In The Cambridge Handbook of Multimedia
Wu, B., X. Yu, and X. Gu. 2020. “Effectiveness of Immersive Virtual
Learning, edited by R. E. Mayer and L. Fiorella, 3rd ed., 73–81. New
Reality Using Head-Mounted Displays on Learning Performance: A
York: Cambridge University Press.
Meta-A nalysis.” British Journal of Educational Technology 51, no. 6:
Parong, J. A. N. 2019. “Cognitive and Affective Mechanisms of 1991–2005. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjet.13023.
Immersive Virtual Reality Learning Environments (Dissertation).”
Zhang, K., J. Suo, J. Chen, X. Liu, and L. Gao. 2017. “Design and
In Cognitive and Affective Mechanisms of Immersive Virtual Reality
Implementation of Fire Safety Education System on Campus Based on
Learning Environments. Santa Barbara, CA: University of California.
Virtual Reality Technology.” In 2017 Federated Conference on Computer
Parong, J., and R. E. Mayer. 2018. “Learning Science in Immersive Science and Information Systems (FedCSIS). Prague, Czech Republic:
Virtual Reality.” Journal of Educational Psychology 110, no. 6: 785–797. IEEE. https://doi.org/10.15439/2 017f376.
https://doi.org/10.1037/edu000 0241.
Parong, J., and R. E. Mayer. 2020. “Cognitive and Affective Processes for
Learning Science in Immersive Virtual Reality.” Journal of Computer
Assisted Learning 37, no. 1: 226–241. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcal.12482.
Parong, J., and R. E. Mayer. 2021. “Learning About History in Immersive
Virtual Reality: Does Immersion Facilitate Learning?” Educational
Technology Research and Development 69, no. 3: 1433–1451. https://doi.
org/10.1007/s11423 -021-0 9999-y.
Petersen, G. B., S. L. Klingenberg, and G. Makransky. 2022. “Pipetting
in Virtual Reality Can Predict Real- Life Pipetting Performance.”
Technology, Mind, and Behavior 3, no. 3: 1–18. https://doi.org/10.1037/
tmb000 0076.
Petersen, G. B., S. L. Klingenberg, R. E. Mayer, and G. Makransky. 2020.
“The Virtual Field Trip: Investigating How to Optimize Immersive
Virtual Learning in Climate Change Education.” British Journal of
Educational Technology 51, no. 6: 2099–2115. https://doi.org/10.1111/
bjet.12991.
Radianti, J., T. A. Majchrzak, J. Fromm, and I. Wohlgenannt. 2020.
“A Systematic Review of Immersive Virtual Reality Applications for
Higher Education: Design Elements, Lessons Learned, and Research
Agenda.” Computers & Education 147: 103778. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
compedu.2 019.103778.