PIIS0896627311007926
PIIS0896627311007926
Article
Neuron 72, 665–678, November 17, 2011 ª2011 Elsevier Inc. 665
Neuron
Functional Brain Networks
666 Neuron 72, 665–678, November 17, 2011 ª2011 Elsevier Inc.
Neuron
Functional Brain Networks
Neuron 72, 665–678, November 17, 2011 ª2011 Elsevier Inc. 667
Neuron
Functional Brain Networks
Figure 2. Many Modified Voxelwise Subgraphs Replicate across Cohorts and Even within Single Subjects
Select subgraphs from the modified voxelwise analysis are presented from a dorsal view for both cohorts and for an additional single subject. Cohort subgraphs
are taken from the 2% tie density analysis and subgraphs in the individual are taken from a 0.5% tie density analysis. The overall NMI between cohort assignments
at this threshold was 0.71, and NMI values between subgraphs from different cohorts are shown in the matrix to the right. Additional views of this data and
replications of subgraphs from other thresholds are found in Figure S3.
compositions for visual (blue), default (red), or fronto-parietal tend to merge, and subgraphs come to resemble a patchwork
(yellow) regions over thresholds, and as refinement of the large of local subgraphs across the cortex (see circled regions in
cyan subgraph into hand somatosensory-motor (cyan), face Figure S4).
somatosensory-motor (orange), auditory (pink), and cingulo- To more quantitatively assess subgraph correspondence to
opercular (purple) subgraphs. This bottom panel of Figure 1 plots functional systems, we used NMI to compare groups of coordi-
areal assignments (spheres) in the main cohort over the modified nates from functional systems with the subgraph identities of
voxelwise assignments (surfaces) in the replication cohort, the nodes nearest to the coordinates under each network defini-
demonstrating the similarity of subgraphs over thresholds tion. A one-factor ANOVA of NMI demonstrates an effect of
across different cohorts and even across graph definitions. As graph (p < 10 7; see Figure S5). The AAL-based graph displays
Figure 2 shows, the modified voxelwise graphs also replicate the lowest correspondence (NMI = 0.37 ± 0.04, significantly
well across cohorts and even in single subjects. Fuller visualiza- lower than all other graphs) across thresholds, and the variable
tions of these data and replications of subgraphs from other structure of the voxelwise graph is reflected in NMI that ranges
thresholds are found in Figure S3. widely over thresholds (0.58–0.86), in contrast to the stable
We predicted that well-formed graphs would possess well- and high NMI found in the areal (0.72 ± 0.06) and modified
formed subgraphs corresponding to major functional systems voxelwise graphs (0.87 ± 0.15). Importantly, as thresholds rise,
of the brain. Figure 3 gives an overview of how well each network NMI between functional systems and subgraphs increases for
met this prediction. At left, PET and fMRI data defining major the modified voxelwise analysis, but decreases for the standard
functional systems are shown. The next three columns display voxelwise analysis.
subgraphs from a single threshold of analysis for each graph
(a high threshold, tailored to each graph). In the second column, Choosing Network Definitions for Further Analysis
areal and modified voxelwise assignments are shown simulta- The areal and modified voxelwise graphs best meet our predic-
neously because they are in such good agreement. The areal tions about the correspondence between functional systems
and modified voxelwise graphs contain subgraphs that corre- and subgraphs within brain-wide networks. The poorer corre-
spond to each of the functional systems, and these subgraphs spondence in the AAL-based and standard voxelwise graphs
contain most or all of the brain regions implicated in the func- likely results from coarse, nonfunctionally based nodes in the
tional systems, and sometimes also some extra brain regions. AAL-based graph, and the effects of millions of artificially high
In contrast, the AAL-based graph is incapable of representing short-range correlations between nearby voxels in the standard
most functional systems at this threshold (or any threshold; voxelwise graph. We turn now from our focus upon confirmatory
see Figure S4). The standard voxel-based graph represents findings to novel observations about functional brain organiza-
some functional systems well (e.g., the default mode system), tion that can be drawn from the areal and modified voxelwise
but others are only incompletely represented. Examination of graphs. We shall continue to focus on the network at the level
other thresholds of the standard voxelwise graph (Figure S4) of subgraphs. We begin by discussing the identities of
indicates that at low to moderate thresholds, reasonable subgraphs, then examine the relationships and properties of
subgraph representations of some functional systems are found, particular subgraphs, and end with observations about relation-
but that as thresholds rise, portions of functional systems ships between all subgraphs.
668 Neuron 72, 665–678, November 17, 2011 ª2011 Elsevier Inc.
Neuron
Functional Brain Networks
Subgraph Identities ized to either side of the central sulcus (Penfield and Boldrey,
The identities of the red (default), yellow (fronto-parietal task 1937), a finding consistent with recent investigations of primary
control), green (dorsal attention), and teal (ventral attention) motor and somatosensory cortex in rodents (Matyas et al.,
subgraphs are already clear. The remaining major subgraphs 2010). The division into ventral and dorsal subgraphs roughly
are now considered. separates the face from the rest of the body, a distinction
Several subgraphs correspond to sensory and motor regions confirmed by button-pushing and verb generation meta-analysis
(Figure 4, left). A visual system (blue) was identified, spanning data (Figure S1). Similar dorsal/ventral distinctions have recently
most of occipital cortex, often including a small portion of been found (Yeo et al., 2011). Intriguingly, correlations between
superior parietal cortex and a portion of the postero-lateral meta-analytic face SSM (orange) and auditory (pink) ROIs are
thalamus (potentially lateral geniculate nucleus [LGN], see hori- higher than correlations between body SSM (cyan) and auditory
zontal sections). At moderate thresholds, somatosensory-motor ROIs (auditory-face r = 0.16, auditory-hand r = 0.05, p < 0.001,
(SSM) cortex (S1, M1, and some pre- and postcentral-gyrus significant in both cohorts). These differential correlations are
cortex) was divided into dorsal (cyan) and ventral (orange) unlikely to reflect only anatomical connectivity, but instead might
subgraphs. These subgraphs also included voxels in the parietal be related to the history of coactivation that these regions surely
operculum that likely correspond to the second somatosensory share as a function of oral/aural language. Thus, it appears that
area (S2) (Burton et al., 2008), as well as a portion of the thalamus somatosensory and motor cortex are functionally divided into
possibly corresponding to ventral posterior thalamus (VP). At a ventral facial representation and a dorsal representation of
high thresholds, an auditory subgraph (pink) emerged from the the rest of the body (called ‘‘hand’’ for brevity).
purple cingulo-opercular subgraph. Two cingulo-opercular subgraphs (black and purple, Fig-
Rather than a division between somatosensory and motor ure 4, middle) are identified, both encompassing regions in
regions, a division between dorsal and ventral SSM regions anterior cingulate/medial superior prefrontal cortex (aCC),
is found. Although motor and sensory function are typically anterior prefrontal cortex (aPFC), and the anterior insula (aI)
localized to the pre- and postcentral gyri, respectively, classic (with additional regions in inferior and middle frontal gyrus and
descriptions of stimulus-evoked responses and sensations in supramarginal gyrus at multiple thresholds). Two distributed
humans indicate that these processes are not exclusively local- functional systems have been ascribed to cingulo-opercular
Neuron 72, 665–678, November 17, 2011 ª2011 Elsevier Inc. 669
Neuron
Functional Brain Networks
cortex: a cingulo-opercular control system first described by subgraph more likely relates to a salience system, though the
Dosenbach et al. (2006) as the ‘‘core’’ of a task performance evidence for such assignments is provisional.
system, which is thought to instantiate and maintain set during At least three distributed subgraphs with previously unknown
task performance, and the salience system of Seeley et al. functional identities are also found (Figure 4, right). The first
(2007). Relative to the black subgraph, the purple subgraph subgraph (salmon in Figure 4, gray in Figure 1) includes parts
lies anterior and ventral in aCC, lateral in aPFC, and dorsal in of posterior cingulate, posterior medial parietal, and lateral
the aI. Three pieces of data hint at the identities of these parietal cortex. We are unaware of any earlier characterizations
subgraphs. First, the coordinates reported for the task control of this collection of brain regions as a coherent functional
network are dorsal to salience coordinates in the insula (Dosen- system, but we found that these regions display the strongest
bach et al., 2007; Seeley et al., 2007), although most other coor- activation in our memory retrieval meta-analysis. Another distrib-
dinates do not distinguish the competing functional systems. uted subgraph (light blue) is found in frontal, parietal, and
Second, on-cue activity localizes to the purple subgraph in the temporal cortex at higher thresholds of the modified voxelwise
aI, aCC, and aPFC (the task control system was defined over analysis. This set of regions is not a commonly described
a range of tasks by on-cue activity entering a task block, sus- functional system, but recent work (fMRI and rs-fcMRI) (Nelson
tained activity during a task block, and error-related activity). et al., 2010a) has indicated that a very similar set of regions
Finally, the fc-Mapping technique detects a strong border (tan spheres in Figure 4) interposed between fronto-parietal
between the black and purple subgraphs at many locations, indi- and default regions may be a functional system, also implicated
cating that rs-fcMRI signal differs strongly between these in memory retrieval. Another novel subgraph is shown in
subgraphs, consistent with prior reports (Nelson et al., 2010b). plum, with representation in fusiform cortex, the precuneus,
We suggest that the purple subgraph more closely represents lateral and medial posterior parietal cortex, and superior frontal
the cingulo-opercular task control system, whereas the black cortex.
670 Neuron 72, 665–678, November 17, 2011 ª2011 Elsevier Inc.
Neuron
Functional Brain Networks
Neuron 72, 665–678, November 17, 2011 ª2011 Elsevier Inc. 671
Neuron
Functional Brain Networks
Figure 6. Default, Visual, and Somatosensory-Motor Systems Are Well-Integrated on Local Scales but Are Relatively Isolated in Relation to
Other Functional Systems
At top, the subgraphs, local efficiencies, and participation coefficients for all nodes in the areal network over a range of thresholds are shown. The local efficiency
of each node indicates the extent to which a node is embedded in a richly connected local environment. High (hot color) values indicate a richly connected local
environment. The participation coefficient of each node indicates the extent to which a node has ties to other subgraphs. Here, low (cool color) values indicate that
nodes are connected almost exclusively to members of their own subgraph. One-factor ANOVAs indicate a significant effect of subgraph at all thresholds for both
indices (all with p < 10 6), and post hoc t tests indicate that the cyan, blue, and red subgraphs have significantly higher local efficiencies and lower participation
coefficients at most or all thresholds than the yellow subgraph. Node assignments for a single threshold (4% tie density) are shown on a brain and in a spring
embedded layout, and the local efficiencies and participation coefficients of relevant subgraphs at this threshold are shown. Note that local efficiency is inde-
pendent of subgraph assignment, whereas participation coefficients depend upon subgraph assignment.
outlined above, these similarities would suggest that the default indeed have many ties, the isolated nature of the default mode
mode system acts more as a ‘‘processing system’’ than subgraph recasts the meaning of these nodes as hubs in the
a ‘‘control system’’ (in contrast with the fronto-parietal system). context of brain-wide rs-fcMRI networks.
Viewed from a perspective of temporal dynamics, the high simi-
larity of node relationships within SSM and visual systems and Functional Systems Are Arranged in Topological Motifs
the default mode system might indicate that these systems in across the Cortex
particular are relatively stationary, whereas other subgraphs One of the more striking features of the modified voxelwise anal-
such as task control systems might have more dynamic sets ysis is that subgraphs appear to be arranged in spatial motifs
of relationships. It should also be noted that several studies throughout the cortex. Figure 7 demonstrates the presence of
(Buckner et al., 2009; Cole et al., 2010) have implicated the motifs at a single threshold of the modified voxelwise analysis.
default mode system as the seat of the most prominent ‘‘hubs’’ For each subgraph, the distribution of its spatial interfaces
in rs-fcMRI brain graphs. Although default mode nodes may (defined as en face voxels) with other subgraphs is plotted,
672 Neuron 72, 665–678, November 17, 2011 ª2011 Elsevier Inc.
Neuron
Functional Brain Networks
Figure 7. Functional Systems Are Arranged into Topological Motifs across the Cortex
In charts, particular subgraphs at a single threshold are selected, the spatial boundaries of that subgraph are found, and the distribution of spatial interfaces (en
face voxels) to other subgraphs are calculated. The most frequent interfaces are plotted as percents of the total subgraph interface volume. Motifs are inferred by
finding instances where subgraphs interfacing with a subgraph are themselves very unlikely to interface. For instance, in the top chart, the light blue subgraph
interfaces most frequently with the yellow and red subgraphs, but red is only 3.6% of yellow’s interface, and yellow is only 2.6% of red’s interface. Below each
chart, plots of relevant subgraphs on brain surfaces visually demonstrate the repeated spatial patterns of subgraphs. Data from the modified voxelwise analysis at
1% tie density in the replication cohort are presented.
and then these neighboring subgraphs are examined to see predominantly with red and yellow subgraphs, which are them-
whether they are themselves unlikely to interface (implying a selves miniscule portions of each others’ borders (red is 3.5%
3-step motif). For example, the light blue subgraph interfaces of yellow’s border, and yellow is 2.6% of red’s border), implying
Neuron 72, 665–678, November 17, 2011 ª2011 Elsevier Inc. 673
Neuron
Functional Brain Networks
a yellow-light blue-red motif. Plots of relevant subgraphs on inform the degree to which the default mode system is seen as
brain surfaces visually confirm the presence of motifs. Three a processing type of system versus a control type of system.
instances of this motif are demonstrated, for the light blue, black Such a finding need not contradict the description of posterior
(salience), and green (dorsal attention) subgraphs. Other 3-step members of the default mode system as cortical hubs (Buckner
motifs are present but not shown (e.g., red-teal-purple), and et al., 2009), but it may alter the understanding of what it means
these motifs can be found up and down subgraph hierarchies to be a hub.
(i.e., thresholds).
A principal concern about such spatial motifs is that they are Integrating the Present Findings with Other Approaches
artifactual—that they arise as intermediate mixtures of adjacent to Whole-Brain rs-fcMRI Analysis
signals, particularly when averaging over subjects. Although Recent investigations into the structure of functional brain orga-
these concerns cannot be entirely excluded, several interposed nization using a variety of methods (Erhardt et al., 2010; Yeo
subgraphs (e.g., the green dorsal attention system or the teal et al., 2011) have found some similar (but not identical) sets of
ventral attention system) have firm and extensive experimental resting state networks as the subgraphs reported here. We
bases. If these are not considered artifactual, then other consider convergence across methods to be a key indicator of
subgraphs deserve similar consideration. the validity of findings. We find the graph theoretic framework
to be especially useful, because it is capable of describing
DISCUSSION the overall graph (no such measures are presented in this article,
but small-world measures are an example), portions of the
Task-Free Approaches Delineate Functional Systems system (e.g., subgraphs), or individual nodes of the system
across the Cortex (e.g., local efficiency) within a common framework.
At the onset of functional neuroimaging some 25 years ago, Our findings have substantial implications for past and future
investigators made educated guesses about the types of opera- graph-based analyses. By examining multiple network defini-
tions that the human brain must perform, and designed experi- tions within a single data set, we were able to show how network
mental paradigms to elicit such operations (Lueck et al., 1989; definition profoundly affects the properties of a network, and
Pardo et al., 1991; Petersen et al., 1988; Posner et al., 1988). therefore the conclusions one would draw about the brain. Our
Over time, evidence accumulated implicating collections of brain results demonstrate drawbacks in some previous approaches,
regions that were assumed to share the burden of some set of while offering new approaches that appear to more plausibly
cognitive operations, defining functional systems (Corbetta and represent brain organization.
Shulman, 2002; Dosenbach et al., 2006; Raichle et al., 2001). It is important to recognize that these new approaches to
Until the study of spontaneous BOLD activity, however, the graph definition are not equivalent or interchangeable. Note
association of regions within a functional system was to some that in this article we examine several graph theoretic properties
extent dependent upon sets of task paradigms. Task-based of the areal graph, but restrict our discussions of modified voxel-
approaches left functional systems open to an interpretation wise data to spatial observations. The areal graph is formed
that rather than being a fundamentally related group of brain using our best estimates of the functional ‘‘units’’ in the brain,
regions within a brain-wide context, a functional system thus and many properties of this network should be fairly direct reflec-
defined might be just a transient and task-specific association tions of functional brain organization. On the other hand, the
of brain regions. modified voxelwise graph is defined using volumetric elements
The subgraphs presented herein were derived in task-free (voxels), and this graph reflects volumetric properties of func-
data using methods with no prior information about node iden- tional organization. In this graph, most functional areas are prob-
tity. There is substantial agreement between aspects of para- ably represented by many voxels, and large functional areas (and
digm-driven functional system definition in neuroimaging, and functional systems) will dominate the graph structure regardless
paradigm-free subgraphs derived in task-free activity. Even if of their roles in information processing relative to smaller areas or
one were to object that the areal network included functional systems. This volume-based definition thus warps representa-
assumptions via meta-analytic localizers, the modified voxelwise tions of information processing, limiting the conclusions that
analysis, which returned very similar results, made no such can be drawn from this graph.
assumptions. In a brain-wide context, several functional systems
are distinguished from each other by spontaneous activity. Directions for Future Work
This task-free definition of brain functional organization can The analyses presented here suggest several avenues for future
inform perspectives on cognitive function. For example, dorsal inquiry. Within graphs that possess many subgraphs with strong
and lateral frontal cortex appears to be apportioned among correspondence to functional systems, we have detected addi-
a variety of distributed subgraphs, many of which correspond tional subgraphs with no such identity but with hints of shared
to functional systems with known characteristics (Figure 2). activity in certain contexts (e.g., memory retrieval activity in the
This organization does not appear consistent with accounts of salmon and light blue subgraphs). Unifying functional attributes
cognition that posit rostro-caudal gradients or hierarchies across among these subgraphs should be sought and tested. Our
frontal cortex (Badre and D’Esposito, 2009; O’Reilly, 2010). results demonstrate strong within-subgraph connectivity in
In a related manner, the finding of similar graph properties sensory, motor and default mode systems, especially in contrast
(relatively dense internal relationships and relatively few external to task control systems, suggesting that these systems may
relationships) in visual, SSM, and default mode systems may differ in the dynamics of their relationships with other subgraphs
674 Neuron 72, 665–678, November 17, 2011 ª2011 Elsevier Inc.
Neuron
Functional Brain Networks
over time. Our analyses only examined static pictures of graphs analyses demonstrate, the human cortex possesses a complex
obtained by summarizing activity over entire epochs into a single and dense topography of functional systems, underscoring the
correlation coefficient, and future work should explore if and need for ‘‘tedious anatomy’’ in neuroimaging studies (Devlin
how these relationships change over time. Perhaps the most and Poldrack, 2007).
obvious avenue for future work will lie in the comparison of graphs
across the lifespan and in disease. A recognized limitation within EXPERIMENTAL PROCEDURES
graph theoretic investigations of structural and functional
Subjects
brain networks is the current lack of validated parcellation strat-
Healthy young adults were recruited from the Washington University campus
egies (see Fornito et al., 2010; Wig et al., 2011; Zalesky et al.,
and the surrounding community. All subjects were native English speakers and
2010) for comprehensive discussions). We have derived and pre- right-handed. All subjects gave informed consent and were compensated for
sented a graph of 264 putative functional areas that displays a their participation.
plausible functional structure that should be sensitive to the orga-
nization of many functional systems. If the locations of functional Data Sets and Data Collection
areas do not greatly differ across populations (Barnes et al., This study utilized multiple data sets. The first and second data sets were used
for meta-analytic and fc-mapping analyses, respectively. The third data set
2011), this graph should be applicable to a wide variety of
was used for rs-fcMRI network analysis. The first (n > 300, detailed in Table
populations, such as clinical or developmental cohorts. S1) and second data sets (n = 40) were acquired on a Siemens 1.5 Tesla
MAGNETOM Vision MRI scanner (Erlangen, Germany) as described in Dosen-
Limitations bach et al. (2010). The third data set (n = 106: a 53 subject cohort, 52 subject
The present study should be considered a preliminary draft cohort, and an additional single subject) was acquired on a Siemens
of functional brain networks and has many limitations. The MAGNETOM Tim Trio 3.0T Scanner with a Siemens 12 channel Head Matrix
Coil (Erlangen, Germany) as described in Dosenbach et al. (2010). See Supple-
methods of locating putative functional areas may certainly
mental Experimental Procedures for acquisition details.
have overlooked, misplaced, or fabricated some areas. Addi-
tionally, the spherical ROIs used to model functional areas Data Processing
do not reflect the true shapes of functional areas. However, Functional images underwent standard fMRI preprocessing to reduce arti-
because subgraph structures in areal and modified voxelwise facts, register subjects to a target atlas, and resample the data on a 3 mm
networks were remarkably alike, this does not seem to have isotropic grid (Shulman et al., 2010). See Supplemental Experimental Proce-
crippled the endeavor. This study used a single signal (BOLD) dures for further details.
with known susceptibility artifacts in temporal and orbitofrontal
rs-fcMRI Processing
cortex. Accordingly, much remains to be discovered about For rs-fcMRI analyses, several additional preprocessing steps were utilized to
the organization of the ventral surface of the brain, as well as reduce spurious variance unlikely to reflect neuronal activity (Fox et al., 2009).
subcortical and cerebellar organization (see Buckner et al., These steps included: (1), a temporal band-pass filter (0.009 Hz < f < 0.08 Hz)
2011). One additional limitation inherent to fMRI is resolution: and spatial smoothing (6 mm full width at half maximum); (2), regression of six
voxels are 3 mm on each side, and partial voluming as well as parameters obtained by rigid body head motion correction; (3), regression of
the smoothing inherent in data processing limit the resolution the whole brain signal averaged across the whole brain; (4), regression of
ventricular signal averaged from ventricular ROIs; and (5), regression of white
that these studies can achieve. To offset these undesired effects,
matter signal averaged from white matter ROIs. The first derivatives of these
short-distance relationships were eliminated from areal and regressors were also regressed.
modified voxelwise analyses, and single subjects were exam-
ined. Future efforts that refine rs-fcMRI techniques and integrate Meta-Analytic ROI Definition
findings from other modalities, such as structural imaging, EEG, The first method of identifying putative functional areas searched a large fMRI
or MEG, will provide valuable additions and refinements to our data set acquired in a single scanner (data set 1) for brain regions that reliably
displayed significant activity when certain tasks were performed (e.g., button-
observations, both in terms of identifying the functional ‘‘units’’
pressing) or certain signal types (e.g., error-related activity) were expected
of the human brain and in more completely modeling functional (see Table S1). Meta-analyses identified 322 ROIs (10 mm diameter spheres,
brain networks in space and time. see Figure S1), which were reduced to a final collection of 151 nonoverlapping
meta-analytic ROIs. Full details of meta-analyses are available in Supple-
CONCLUSIONS mental Experimental Procedures.
We close with two broad points. First, there is a growing trend fc-Mapping ROI Definition
fc-Mapping techniques were applied to eyes-open fixation rs-fcMRI data from
to examine healthy and pathological brain activity in terms of
40 healthy young adults (data set 2: 27 M/13 F, average age = 26.4 years old,
networks (Bullmore and Sporns, 2009; Church et al., 2009; average RMS movement = 0.42 mm, average number of volumes = 432). See
Seeley et al., 2009). The sensitivity and specificity of such anal- Cohen et al. (2008) and Nelson et al. (2010a) for full conceptual and technical
yses is directly linked to the comprehensiveness and accuracy descriptions of fc-Mapping on cortical patches. Here, patches extending over
of the framework used to examine brain networks. The frame- the entire cortical surface (one per hemisphere) were used to define putative
work used in this report appears to be reasonably accurate, functional areas. This technique generated 254 ROIs across the cortex, which
were reduced to a final set of 193 nonoverlapping ROIs. See Supplemental
and is capable of describing networks as a whole, as subgraphs,
Experimental Procedures for further details.
or as individual nodes, making it a powerful tool for examining
functional relationships in the human brain. Second, the accu- Areal ROI Set Formation
racy of connectivity analyses depends upon the isolation of Meta-analytic ROIs and fc-Mapping ROIs were merged to form a maximally-
relevant or unique signals. As the areal and modified voxelwise spanning collection of ROIs. Meta-analytic ROIs were given preference, and
Neuron 72, 665–678, November 17, 2011 ª2011 Elsevier Inc. 675
Neuron
Functional Brain Networks
nonoverlapping fc-Mapping ROIs were then added, resulting in 264 indepen- Subgraph Detection and Graph Analysis
dent ROIs. For a given network at a given threshold, the correlations below the threshold
were set to zero, and the resulting matrix was subjected to subgraph detection
Parcel-Based, Voxel-Based, and Modified Voxelwise algorithms. We utilized the Infomap algorithm, one of the best-performing
Network Formation algorithms on multiple benchmark networks (Fortunato, 2010; Lancichinetti
A 90-node parcel-based network was formed by using the 90-parcel auto- and Fortunato, 2009). Other algorithms were tried, with similar results.
mated anatomical labeling (AAL) atlas (Tzourio-Mazoyer et al., 2002) to assign Subgraph assignments were returned as numbers, which were then mapped
all voxels (n = 44,100) within the atlas into 90 parcels. An average timecourse onto nodes and ROIs as colors.
was formed for each parcel by averaging the timecourses of all nodes within Local efficiency was calculated after (Latora and Marchiori, 2001). Participa-
the parcel. A 44,100-node voxelwise network was defined from all voxels tion coefficients were calculated after (Guimerà et al., 2005). Binary networks
within the AAL atlas (Tzourio-Mazoyer et al., 2002). The modified voxelwise were used for calculations.
networks arose by masking out ties that terminated within 20 mm of the
source voxel. Distances of 15–25 mm were tested, with similar results across Computations and Visualizations
networks. Analyses were performed on all voxels in both hemispheres (n = MRI images were processed using in-house software. Network calculations
44,100), and also on all voxels within a single hemisphere (n = 22,050). Single were performed using MATLAB (The MathWorks, Natick, MA). The Infomap
hemisphere analyses were much less computationally demanding, permitting algorithm was provided by Rosvall and Bergstrom (2008). Network visualiza-
a wider range of analysis), and results between single- and dual-hemisphere tions were created using the Social Network Image Animator (SoNIA) software
analyses were similar. All figures except Figure 3 (both hemispheres were package (Bender-deMoll and McFarland, 2006). Brain surface visualizations
used for consistency with the voxelwise analysis and the rest of the literature were created using Caret software and the PALS surface (Van Essen, 2005;
in this figure) in the article portray single-hemisphere analyses. Van Essen et al., 2001).
Formation of Two Subject Cohorts for rs-fcMRI Network Analysis SUPPLEMENTAL INFORMATION
rs-fcMRI networks were studied in continuous eyes-open fixation data from
two cohorts (data set 3) of healthy young adults, matched for age, sex, move- Supplemental Information includes five figures, two tables, and Supplemental
ment and number of volumes in scans, as shown in Table S1. These subjects Experimental Procedures and can be found with this article online at
underwent a rigorous quality control process to correct for subject motion doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2011.09.006.
(Power et al., 2011). See Supplemental Experimental Procedures for details.
Reported numbers of volumes (time frames of rs-fcMRI data) and RMS are ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
for the final, usable, data (Table S1). Data cleaning for subject movement
during the scan removed 6% of the data from subjects (range 4%–8%), and We thank Nico Dosenbach, Thomas Pearce, Bradley Miller, and our reviewers
each cohort contained a mean of 350 frames of data per subject (range for their attentive reading of this manuscript. We thank Olaf Sporns and
215–501 frames). The single subject in Figure 2 had 1181 frames of data. Mika Rubinov for technical help with graph analysis, and Joe Dubis for help
with meta-analyses. This work was supported by NIH R21NS061144 (S.P.),
rs-fcMRI Graph Formation NIH R01NS32979 (S.P.), a McDonnell Foundation Collaborative Action
Given a collection of N ROIs (parcels, voxels, or putative areas), within each Award (S.P.), NIH R01HD057076 (B.L.S.), NIH F30NS062489 (A.L.C.), NIH
subject, timecourses are extracted for all ROIs and an N 3 N correlation matrix U54MH091657 (David Van Essen), and NSF IGERT DGE-0548890 (Kurt
is calculated. An average matrix is formed across all subjects in a cohort, and Thoroughman).
the diagonal is set to zero. This defines a weighted graph.
Typical graph analyses of weighted networks ignore negative ties and are Accepted: September 2, 2011
obliged to explore a range of thresholds to characterize the properties of Published: November 16, 2011
a network (Power et al., 2010; Rubinov and Sporns, 2010). Recent proposals
to incorporate negative weights into analyses of subgraph detection have REFERENCES
been made (Rubinov and Sporns, 2011; Traag and Bruggeman, 2009), but
here we follow the traditional approach. Many real-world networks have tie Badre, D., and D’Esposito, M. (2009). Is the rostro-caudal axis of the frontal
densities of a few percent or less (Newman, 2010), and the graph analytic lobe hierarchical? Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 659–669.
techniques utilized here were developed upon such networks (Fortunato, Barnes, K.A., Nelson, S.M., Cohen, A.L., Power, J.D., Coalson, R.S., Miezin,
2010; Newman, 2010; Rosvall and Bergstrom, 2008). Accordingly, the anal- F.M., Vogel, A.C., Dubis, J.W., Church, J.A., Petersen, S.E., and Schlaggar,
yses presented here typically span a threshold range on the order of 10% B.L. (2011). Parcellation in left lateral parietal cortex is similar in adults and chil-
down to 1% tie density though the precise range depends upon the network dren. Cereb. Cortex., in press. Published online August 1, 2011. 10.1093/cer-
(for example, the AAL-based parcel network becomes severely fragmented cor/bhr189.
below 4% tie density and we do not present results from such thresholds). Bender-deMoll, S., and McFarland, D.A. (2006). The art and science of
In general, results are presented over a range of thresholds to give the reader dynamic network visualization. J. Soc. Struct. 7.
a sense of the dependence of a property upon thresholds, and no formal defi- Biswal, B., Yetkin, F.Z., Haughton, V.M., and Hyde, J.S. (1995). Functional
nition of threshold ranges is proposed, because it is essentially arbitrary. connectivity in the motor cortex of resting human brain using echo-planar
As noted in the text, short-range correlations can arise from shared patterns MRI. Magn. Reson. Med. 34, 537–541.
of local neuronal activity, but they can also arise from aspects of data process-
Buckner, R.L., Sepulcre, J., Talukdar, T., Krienen, F.M., Liu, H., Hedden, T.,
ing (e.g., reslicing, blurring), as well as motion-induced artifacts (Power et al.,
Andrews-Hanna, J.R., Sperling, R.A., and Johnson, K.A. (2009). Cortical
2011). Local correlations are thus combinations of neurobiological and artifac-
hubs revealed by intrinsic functional connectivity: mapping, assessment of
tual signal. To minimize the effects of questionable correlations on network
stability, and relation to Alzheimer’s disease. J. Neurosci. 29, 1860–1873.
structure, ties terminating within 20 mm of the source ROI are set to zero in
all areal network analyses and in the modified voxelwise analysis. Although Buckner, R.L., Krienen, F.M., Castellanos, A., Diaz, J.C., and Yeo, B.T.T.
this process does not completely remove the effect of reslicing and blurring (2011). The organization of the human cerebellum estimated by intrinsic func-
on correlations in the data (consider a voxel’s correlations to distant but adja- tional connectivity. J. Neurophysiol., in press. Published online July 27, 2011.
cent voxels), it removes a considerable portion of correlations of questionable 10.1152/jn.00339.2011.
origin. This procedure eliminated 635 (4.1%) of the 15,375 positive ties in the Bullmore, E., and Sporns, O. (2009). Complex brain networks: graph theoret-
areal network, and 15.3 million (4.2%) of 470 million ties in the single hemi- ical analysis of structural and functional systems. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10,
sphere voxelwise network. 186–198.
676 Neuron 72, 665–678, November 17, 2011 ª2011 Elsevier Inc.
Neuron
Functional Brain Networks
Burton, H., Sinclair, R.J., Wingert, J.R., and Dierker, D.L. (2008). Multiple pari- Guimerà, R., Mossa, S., Turtschi, A., and Amaral, L.A.N. (2005). The worldwide
etal operculum subdivisions in humans: tactile activation maps. Somatosens. air transportation network: Anomalous centrality, community structure, and
Mot. Res. 25, 149–162. cities’ global roles. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 102, 7794–7799.
Butts, C.T. (2009). Revisiting the foundations of network analysis. Science 325, Hartman, D., Hlinka, J., Palus, M., Mantini, D., and Corbetta, M. (2011). The
414–416. role of nonlinearity in computing graph-theoretical properties of resting-state
Church, J.A., Fair, D.A., Dosenbach, N.U., Cohen, A.L., Miezin, F.M., Petersen, functional magnetic resonance imaging brain networks. Chaos 21, 013119.
S.E., and Schlaggar, B.L. (2009). Control networks in paediatric Tourette Hayasaka, S., and Laurienti, P.J. (2010). Comparison of characteristics
syndrome show immature and anomalous patterns of functional connectivity. between region-and voxel-based network analyses in resting-state fMRI
Brain 132, 225–238. data. Neuroimage 50, 499–508.
Cohen, A.L., Fair, D.A., Dosenbach, N.U.F., Miezin, F.M., Dierker, D., He, Y., Wang, J., Wang, L., Chen, Z.J., Yan, C., Yang, H., Tang, H., Zhu, C.,
Van Essen, D.C., Schlaggar, B.L., and Petersen, S.E. (2008). Defining func- Gong, Q., Zang, Y., and Evans, A.C. (2009). Uncovering intrinsic modular orga-
tional areas in individual human brains using resting functional connectivity nization of spontaneous brain activity in humans. PLoS ONE 4, e5226.
MRI. Neuroimage 41, 45–57.
Lancichinetti, A., and Fortunato, S. (2009). Community detection algorithms:
Cole, M.W., Pathak, S., and Schneider, W. (2010). Identifying the brain’s most a comparative analysis. Phys. Rev. E Stat. Nonlin. Soft Matter Phys. 80,
globally connected regions. Neuroimage 49, 3132–3148. 056117.
Corbetta, M., and Shulman, G.L. (2002). Control of goal-directed and stimulus- Latora, V., and Marchiori, M. (2001). Efficient behavior of small-world
driven attention in the brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3, 201–215. networks. Phys. Rev. Lett. 87, 198701.
Corbetta, M., Shulman, G.L., Miezin, F.M., and Petersen, S.E. (1995). Superior Liu, Y.-Y., Slotine, J.-J., and Barabási, A.L. (2011). Controllability of complex
parietal cortex activation during spatial attention shifts and visual feature networks. Nature 473, 167–173.
conjunction. Science 270, 802–805.
Lowe, M.J., Mock, B.J., and Sorenson, J.A. (1998). Functional connectivity in
Corbetta, M., Patel, G., and Shulman, G.L. (2008). The reorienting system of single and multislice echoplanar imaging using resting-state fluctuations.
the human brain: from environment to theory of mind. Neuron 58, 306–324. Neuroimage 7, 119–132.
Deco, G., Jirsa, V.K., and McIntosh, A.R. (2011). Emerging concepts for the Lueck, C.J., Zeki, S., Friston, K.J., Deiber, M.P., Cope, P., Cunningham, V.J.,
dynamical organization of resting-state activity in the brain. Nat. Rev. Lammertsma, A.A., Kennard, C., and Frackowiak, R.S.J. (1989). The colour
Neurosci. 12, 43–56. centre in the cerebral cortex of man. Nature 340, 386–389.
Devlin, J.T., and Poldrack, R.A. (2007). In praise of tedious anatomy. Matyas, F., Sreenivasan, V., Marbach, F., Wacongne, C., Barsy, B., Mateo, C.,
Neuroimage 37, 1033–1041, discussion 1050–1058. Aronoff, R., and Petersen, C.C.H. (2010). Motor control by sensory cortex.
Dosenbach, N.U.F., Visscher, K.M., Palmer, E.D., Miezin, F.M., Wenger, K.K., Science 330, 1240–1243.
Kang, H.C., Burgund, E.D., Grimes, A.L., Schlaggar, B.L., and Petersen, S.E. Meunier, D., Achard, S., Morcom, A., and Bullmore, E. (2009a). Age-related
(2006). A core system for the implementation of task sets. Neuron 50, 799–812. changes in modular organization of human brain functional networks.
Dosenbach, N.U.F., Fair, D.A., Miezin, F.M., Cohen, A.L., Wenger, K.K., Neuroimage 44, 715–723.
Dosenbach, R.A.T., Fox, M.D., Snyder, A.Z., Vincent, J.L., Raichle, M.E.,
Meunier, D., Lambiotte, R., Fornito, A., Ersche, K.D., and Bullmore, E.T.
et al. (2007). Distinct brain networks for adaptive and stable task control in hu-
(2009b). Hierarchical modularity in human brain functional networks. Front.
mans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 104, 11073–11078.
Neuroinform. 3, 37.
Dosenbach, N.U., Nardos, B., Cohen, A.L., Fair, D.A., Power, J.D., Church,
Nelson, S.M., Cohen, A.L., Power, J.D., Wig, G.S., Miezin, F.M., Wheeler, M.E.,
J.A., Nelson, S.M., Wig, G.S., Vogel, A.C., Lessov-Schlaggar, C.N., et al.
Velanova, K., Donaldson, D.I., Phillips, J.S., Schlaggar, B.L., and Petersen,
(2010). Prediction of individual brain maturity using fMRI. Science 329,
S.E. (2010a). A parcellation scheme for human left lateral parietal cortex.
1358–1361.
Neuron 67, 156–170.
Erhardt, E.B., Rachakonda, S., Bedrick, E.J., Allen, E.A., Adali, T.l., and
Nelson, S.M., Dosenbach, N.U., Cohen, A.L., Wheeler, M.E., Schlaggar, B.L.,
Calhoun, V.D. (2010). Comparison of multi-subject ICA methods for analysis
and Petersen, S.E. (2010b). Role of the anterior insula in task-level control and
of fMRI data. Hum. Brain Mapp., in press. Published online December 15,
focal attention. Brain Struct. Funct. 214, 669–680.
2010. 10.1002/hbm.21170.
Newman, M.E.J. (2010). Networks: An Introduction (Oxford: Oxford University
Fornito, A., Zalesky, A., and Bullmore, E.T. (2010). Network scaling effects in
Press).
graph analytic studies of human resting-state FMRI data. Front. Syst.
Neurosci. 4, 22. Norman, D.A., and Shallice, T. (1986). Attention to action: willed and automatic
control of behavior. In Consciousness and Self-Regulation, R.J. Davidson,
Fortunato, S. (2010). Community detection in graphs. Phys. Rep. 486, 75–174.
G.E. Schwartz, and D. Shapiro, eds. (New York: Plenum Press), pp. 1–18.
Fox, M.D., Snyder, A.Z., Vincent, J.L., Corbetta, M., Van Essen, D.C., and
O’Reilly, R.C. (2010). The what and how of prefrontal cortical organization.
Raichle, M.E. (2005). The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic,
Trends Neurosci. 33, 355–361.
anticorrelated functional networks. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 102, 9673–
9678. Pardo, J.V., Fox, P.T., and Raichle, M.E. (1991). Localization of a human
system for sustained attention by positron emission tomography. Nature
Fox, M.D., Corbetta, M., Snyder, A.Z., Vincent, J.L., and Raichle, M.E. (2006).
349, 61–64.
Spontaneous neuronal activity distinguishes human dorsal and ventral atten-
tion systems. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 103, 10046–10051. Penfield, W., and Boldrey, E. (1937). Somatic motor and sensory representa-
Fox, M.D., Zhang, D., Snyder, A.Z., and Raichle, M.E. (2009). The global signal tion in the cerebral cortex of man as studied by electrical stimulation. Brain
and observed anticorrelated resting state brain networks. J. Neurophysiol. 60, 389–443.
101, 3270–3283. Petersen, S.E., Fox, P.T., Posner, M.I., Mintun, M., and Raichle, M.E. (1988).
Fransson, P., Aden, U., Blennow, M., and Lagercrantz, H. (2011). The func- Positron emission tomographic studies of the cortical anatomy of single-
tional architecture of the infant brain as revealed by resting-state fMRI. word processing. Nature 331, 585–589.
Cereb. Cortex 21, 145–154. Posner, M.I., and Petersen, S.E. (1990). The attention system of the human
Greicius, M.D., Krasnow, B., Reiss, A.L., and Menon, V. (2003). Functional brain. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 13, 25–42.
connectivity in the resting brain: a network analysis of the default mode Posner, M.I., Petersen, S.E., Fox, P.T., and Raichle, M.E. (1988). Localization
hypothesis. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 100, 253–258. of cognitive operations in the human brain. Science 240, 1627–1631.
Neuron 72, 665–678, November 17, 2011 ª2011 Elsevier Inc. 677
Neuron
Functional Brain Networks
Power, J.D., Fair, D.A., Schlaggar, B.L., and Petersen, S.E. (2010). The devel- Spoormaker, V.I., Schröter, M.S., Gleiser, P.M., Andrade, K.C., Dresler, M.,
opment of human functional brain networks. Neuron 67, 735–748. Wehrle, R., Sämann, P.G., and Czisch, M. (2010). Development of a large-
Power, J.D., Barnes, K.A., Snyder, A.Z., Schlaggar, B.L., and Petersen, S.E. scale functional brain network during human non-rapid eye movement sleep.
(2011). Spurious but systematic correlations in functional connectivity MRI J. Neurosci. 30, 11379–11387.
networks arise from subject motion. Neuroimage, in press. Published online Tian, L., Wang, J., Yan, C., and He, Y. (2011). Hemisphere- and gender-related
October 14, 2011. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.10.018. differences in small-world brain networks: a resting-state functional MRI study.
Raichle, M.E., MacLeod, A.M., Snyder, A.Z., Powers, W.J., Gusnard, D.A., and Neuroimage 54, 191–202.
Shulman, G.L. (2001). A default mode of brain function. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. Tomasi, D., and Volkow, N.D. (2011). Functional connectivity hubs in the
USA 98, 676–682. human brain. Neuroimage 57, 908–917.
Rosvall, M., and Bergstrom, C.T. (2008). Maps of random walks on complex Traag, V.A., and Bruggeman, J. (2009). Community detection in networks with
networks reveal community structure. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 105, 1118– positive and negative links. Phys. Rev. E Stat. Nonlin. Soft Matter Phys. 80,
1123. 036115.
Rubinov, M., and Sporns, O. (2010). Complex network measures of brain Tzourio-Mazoyer, N., Landeau, B., Papathanassiou, D., Crivello, F., Etard, O.,
connectivity: uses and interpretations. Neuroimage 52, 1059–1069. Delcroix, N., Mazoyer, B., and Joliot, M. (2002). Automated anatomical
Rubinov, M., and Sporns, O. (2011). Weight-conserving characterization of labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation
complex functional brain networks. Neuroimage 56, 2068–2079. of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. Neuroimage 15, 273–289.
Seeley, W.W., Menon, V., Schatzberg, A.F., Keller, J., Glover, G.H., Kenna, H., van den Heuvel, M.P., Stam, C.J., Boersma, M., and Hulshoff Pol, H.E. (2008).
Reiss, A.L., and Greicius, M.D. (2007). Dissociable intrinsic connectivity Small-world and scale-free organization of voxel-based resting-state func-
networks for salience processing and executive control. J. Neurosci. 27, tional connectivity in the human brain. Neuroimage 43, 528–539.
2349–2356. Van Essen, D.C. (2005). A Population-Average, Landmark- and Surface-based
Seeley, W.W., Crawford, R.K., Zhou, J., Miller, B.L., and Greicius, M.D. (2009). (PALS) atlas of human cerebral cortex. Neuroimage 28, 635–662.
Neurodegenerative diseases target large-scale human brain networks. Neuron Van Essen, D.C., Drury, H.A., Dickson, J., Harwell, J., Hanlon, D., and
62, 42–52. Anderson, C.H. (2001). An integrated software suite for surface-based
Shulman, G.L., Fiez, J.A., Corbetta, M., Buckner, R.L., Miezin, F.M., Raichle, analyses of cerebral cortex. J. Am. Med. Inform. Assoc. 8, 443–459.
M.E., and Petersen, S.E. (1997). Common blood flow changes across visual Wig, G.S., Schlaggar, B.L., and Petersen, S.E. (2011). Concepts and principles
tasks: II. Decreases in cerebral cortex. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 9, 648–663. in the analysis of brain networks. Ann. N Y Acad. Sci. 1224, 126–146.
Shulman, G.L., Pope, D.L.W., Astafiev, S.V., McAvoy, M.P., Snyder, A.Z., and Yeo, B.T.T., Krienen, F.M., Sepulcre, J., Sabuncu, M.R., Lashkari, D.,
Corbetta, M. (2010). Right hemisphere dominance during spatial selective Hollinshead, M., Roffman, J.L., Smoller, J.W., Zollei, L., Polimeni, J.R., et al.
attention and target detection occurs outside the dorsal frontoparietal (2011). The organization of the human cerebral cortex estimated by functional
network. J. Neurosci. 30, 3640–3651. connectivity. J. Neurophysiol. 106, 1125–1165.
Smith, S.M., Miller, K.L., Salimi-Khorshidi, G., Webster, M., Beckmann, C.F., Zalesky, A., Fornito, A., Harding, I.H., Cocchi, L., Yücel, M., Pantelis, C., and
Nichols, T.E., Ramsey, J.D., and Woolrich, M.W. (2011). Network modelling Bullmore, E.T. (2010). Whole-brain anatomical networks: does the choice of
methods for FMRI. Neuroimage 54, 875–891. nodes matter? Neuroimage 50, 970–983.
678 Neuron 72, 665–678, November 17, 2011 ª2011 Elsevier Inc.