Mixed_Methods_Study_Protocol_
Mixed_Methods_Study_Protocol_
Environmental Research
and Public Health
Study Protocol
Mixed Methods Study Protocol: Language Identity, Discrimination,
and Mental Health among Multilingual 1.5 Generation
Asian/Asian American Immigrant Young Adults
Chulwoo Park 1, * , Mark Edberg 2 , Janet Yougi Bang 3 and Avizia Yim Long 4
1 Department of Public Health and Recreation, San José State University, San Jose, CA 95192, USA
2 Department of Prevention and Community Health, Milken Institute School of Public Health, George
Washington University, Washington, DC 20052, USA; [email protected]
3 Department of Child and Adolescent Development, San José State University, San Jose, CA 95192, USA;
[email protected]
4 Department of World Languages and Literatures, San José State University, San Jose, CA 95192, USA;
[email protected]
* Correspondence: [email protected]
Abstract: Language identity, an understudied factor, can influence isolation and discrimination,
leading to disparities in well-being and mental health among immigrants. This study aims to
investigate the role of language identity on structural racism and discrimination among 1.5 generation
Asian/Asian American immigrants in a diverse U.S. state. We developed a three-step sequential
approach: Stage 1—qualitative analysis (1A, focus group discussion; 1B, in-depth interviews); Stage
2—quantitative analysis (2A, language identity measurement scale; 2B, cross-sectional online survey;
2C, multivariate multiple linear regression); Stage 3—another round of qualitative analysis (3A,
follow-up in-depth chronological interviews). Therefore, this study will contribute to the field by
introducing a novel three-step mixed methods approach, marking a notable improvement over
conventional explanatory or exploratory sequential designs.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2024, 21, 1311. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph21101311 https://www.mdpi.com/journal/ijerph
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2024, 21, 1311 2 of 19
culture, and familiarity with this new environment is essential for them to feel accepted
into U.S. society.
Although language identity is not well understood, acculturation has been a common
related area of study. Acculturation involves changes in identification, social skills, attitudes,
and values that immigrants experience while adapting to the host culture and is also linked
to mental health issues—psychological distress, depression, and anxiety [10–14]. While
some immigrants willingly adjust to the dominant culture, others might still be attached
to their culture of origin, the minority culture, and feel that adjusting to the other culture
is challenging [15]. Norton (2010) highlights that language learners’ identities are shaped
by their access to social and educational resources and interactions with dominant and
marginalized linguistic groups [16]. This work also introduces the concept of investment,
which contrasts with traditional motivation by focusing on how learners invest in language
learning to enhance their social identity [16]. This theory is related to poststructuralist views,
suggesting that identity is fluid and negotiated through power dynamics, with language
acquisition success depending not just on ability but also aligning with sociocultural or
ethnic factors [17].
In recent decades, societal advancements and theories illustrate the numerous complex-
ities to consider for how identities are shaped for 1.5 generation Asian/Asian Americans.
Social media use among recent migrants in the era of globalization, such as exchange
students or foreign students, to interact with host country nationals was predicted to lead
to poorer adaptation to the host country [18]. Their attempts to connect with host nationals
through social media could lead to feelings of disconnect, reduced perceived support, and
misunderstandings due to language barriers, ultimately hindering their well-being and
psychological adaptation [19–22]. In contemporary societies, migration, transnationalism,
and intersectionality have been pivotal to human settlement [23]. For example, Kimberlé
Crenshaw’s reconceptualization of race through intersectionality illustrated how multiple
dimensions intersect for women of color who experience violence, racism, and sexism. This
work exemplified how multiple social identities intersect and influence individuals’ expe-
riences of oppression or privilege for marginalized communities [24]. For 1.5 generation
Asian/Asian Americans, the intersection of multiple dimensions can be seen through how
language interacts with other facets of identity, such as race, gender, class, ethnicity, and
immigration status. Additionally, transnationalism, defined by immigrants’ new social en-
tanglement leaving behind their former life [25], could help explore how language identity
is formed through cross-border interactions and multiculturalism.
In addition, during the COVID-19 pandemic, there was a significant increase in
instances of anti-Asian violence, xenophobia, and bias against Asian/Asian American
immigrants in the U.S. [26–29]. Asian Americans are often treated as foreigners by others in
the dominant society, regardless of their citizenship status or how long they have lived in the
U.S., a phenomenon known as the perpetual foreigner stereotype [30]. This often appears
in subtle forms, such as microaggressions, including compliments on an individual’s
proficiency in English or inquiries about their nationality or place of origin [31,32]. A 2023
survey by the Pew Research Center stated that one-third of Asian Americans have been told
to ‘go back to their home country’, a sentiment that surged alongside the rise in anti-Asian
hate crimes during the COVID-19 pandemic [33]. According to the Center for the Study
of Hate & Extremism, anti-Asian hate crimes in select U.S. cities reached a record high of
369 incidents in 2021; 121 of those cases (33%) were reported in California [34]. Among
selected U.S. cities, San Francisco and San Jose in California showed a notably high increase
of hate crimes from 2020 to 2022, or 567% and 89%, respectively [34]. Still, hate crime
incidents have been significantly under-reported due to challenges with understanding
systems of reporting and cultural norms that uphold feelings to ‘keep quiet’ [35–37].
The mental health of Asian/Asian American immigrant young adults is impacted
by pressure to meet high parental academic expectations, the challenges of balancing two
cultures, family obligations, and experiences of discrimination or isolation [38]. Asian
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2024, 21, 1311 3 of 19
cultural norms stigmatize the importance of seeking mental health care, which leads to
mental health programs being considered unimportant [39].
Figure1.1.Study
Figure Studytimeline
timeline(Stages
(Stages1–3).
1–3).
includes individuals who were born in their home country but migrated to new countries in
elementary, intermediate, or high school [62,63]. We specified 1.5 generation Asian/Asian
American immigrant young adults, our target participants in this study, as those who
migrated to the U.S. with their parents (1st generation) from Asian countries when they
were children or adolescents aged between 5 and 17 years, have been living in the U.S. for
at least 12 months, and whose current ages are between 18 and 29 years. Our study will be
conducted in California, home to 11 million immigrants (25% of the foreign-born population
nationwide) with varied levels of educational attainment [64], most of whom speak at least
two languages [65]. Nearly half (48.3%) of California children are living with foreign-born
parents [66]. Further, more than half of recent arrivals in California (53%) between 2010 and
2019 were from Asian countries [65]. Foreign-born residents represent more than one-third
of the population in 5 California counties, and 4 of them—Alameda, San Mateo, Santa
Clara, and San Francisco—are located in the San Francisco Bay Area [65], where 23.3% of
the population is Asian (1,664,384 out of 7,150,739) [67]. The Asian community in the Bay
Area is one of the fastest-growing groups representing diverse ancestry backgrounds and
cultures [68]. Thus, we will focus our study and recruitment efforts in the Bay Area, which
is the main site for this research study.
3.1.2.
3.1.2. Recruitment
Recruitment
We
We will
will use
useaapurposive
purposivesampling
samplingstrategy
strategyto toidentify
identifyandandrecruit
recruittarget
targetparticipants.
participants.
The
The inclusion
inclusion criteria
criteria of
of the
the participants
participants for
for this
this study
study are: (1) (1) Asian
Asian or or Asian
Asian American
American
young
young adults,
adults,(2) (2)they
theywere
werechildren
children oror
adolescents
adolescents agedaged5–17 years
5–17 when
years whencoming to the
coming to
U.S., (3) they
the U.S., (3) came
they with
cametheir
withparents (1st generation),
their parents (4) they have
(1st generation), livedhave
(4) they in the U.S. in
lived forthe
at
least
U.S. 12formonths,
at least 12(5) months,
their current agescurrent
(5) their are 18–29 years,
ages and (6)years,
are 18–29 they currently
and (6) they livecurrently
in Santa
Clara
live inCounty. The term
Santa Clara 1.5 generation
County. The term implies that the implies
1.5 generation individual thatgrew up in a country
the individual grew
outside
up in a the U.S., and
country thusthe
outside wasU.S.,
exposed to one
and thus or more
was exposed languages
to one or that differ
more from Ameri-
languages that
differ
can from American
English. English. that
This also suggests Thisthey
also are
suggests
able tothat they are able
understand and/orto understand
communicate and/or
(e.g.,
communicate
gestures) (e.g., gestures) inthat
in a language/culture a language/culture
differs from American that differs
English from American
norms. We willEnglish
send
norms.invitations
email We will send email invitations
to university to university
or college-wide offices or college-wide
and buildings offices
(e.g., and buildings
international
(e.g., international
student student offices,
offices, undocumented undocumented
student studentfinancial
service centers, service centers,
aid andfinancial
scholarshipaid and
of-
scholarship offices, and dormitories) and various student clubs or organizations
fices, and dormitories) and various student clubs or organizations for young adults (e.g., for young
adults (e.g.,
academic andacademic
honoraryand honorary organizations,
organizations, cultural and
cultural and religious religious organizations,
organizations, special inter-
special
est interest organizations,
organizations, and non-profit andorganizations)
non-profit organizations)
located in Santalocated in Santa
Clara County Clara County
(Figure 2),
(Figure
and post2),theand post theon
invitations invitations
their socialonmedia
their and
social media and
university university
mobile app. A mobile
purposive app.and A
purposivesampling
snowball and snowball sampling
strategy will bestrategy
employed,will recruiting
be employed, recruitingwho
participants participants who
express their
express their
willingness towillingness
participate in to participate
a focus group in adiscussion.
focus group discussion.will
Participants Participants
be contacted willviabe
contacted via email and invited to advertise and disseminate qualitative
email and invited to advertise and disseminate qualitative study information to potential study information
to potential
other other participants
participants through social through
mediasocial media (e.g.,X,
(e.g., Instagram, Instagram, X, and LinkedIn).
and LinkedIn).
Figure
Figure 2.
2. Distribution
Distributionof
oftarget
target educational
educational institutions
institutions and
and organizations.
organizations.
does this make you feel in relation to your sense of identity?’, and ‘Do the languages you
use affect your sense of belonging as an immigrant? If so, why?’. Each of the focus group
discussions will be audio-recorded and transcribed verbatim. To gain initial transcriptions,
Otter.ai Business (Otter, Los Altos, CA, USA), a speech-to-text transcription software, will
be used. Undergraduate research assistants will review those initial transcriptions to final-
ize verbatim transcriptions. Transcript preparation will also include redacting identifying
information. The recordings will be viewed only by the research team and not shared.
conversations. Some of the most common Asian languages in the Bay Area are Mandarin,
Cantonese, Tagalog, Vietnamese, Korean, Hindi, Tamil, and Japanese [70,71]. Through
a multilingual research team, we will allocate time and resources for translators and
student interviewers with specific language skills to provide translation services as needed.
The expected duration of each interview will be 60–90 min. An online interview will be
conducted via Zoom (Video Communications, San Jose, CA, USA) for those who are not
able to meet in person. To protect participants’ confidentiality, we will inform them before
beginning the interview that they can choose to use their nickname or a pseudonym in
Zoom as well as choose to turn off their video.
3.2.2. Recruitment
We will recruit a subset of focus group (Stage 1A) participants for the in-depth in-
terviews in this Stage. It is important that focus group participants also join an in-depth
interview to achieve consistent qualitative analysis with the same subjects. We will thor-
oughly explain to participants about the importance of their continued engagement and
commitment to the study. Additionally, after focus group discussions are completed, we
will email preliminary findings (e.g., sharing an executive summary) as well as regular
updates (e.g., news articles about Asian/Asian American immigrant young adults) to
participants for pursuing a high retention rate for in-depth interviews. We aim for approx-
imately 50% of participants from focus group discussions to sequentially join in-depth
interviews. If there are more than 50% of subjects who withdraw from the study after
focus group discussions, we will contact other potential in-depth interview participants.
Approximately a total of 20 young adults will be recruited; thus, there will be 20 in-depth
interviews. As an incentive, a $40 gift card will be provided to all participants.
This information may either reinforce, support, or present discordance with the findings
from focus group discussions.
4.2.3. Recruitment
The same recruitment strategy will be used as in Stage 1, but the catchment area will be
expanded to the San Francisco Bay Area. Because the criteria are very specific to variables
associated with lower response rates (e.g., race, exposure to English, age, family type, the
period of residence in the U.S., and geography), there could be challenges in reaching the
desired sample size; thus, our online survey will be distributed to all 57 higher education
institutions—across the University of California, California State University, the California
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2024, 21, 1311 10 of 19
4.2.5. Measurement
The survey will consist of 9 main categories: (1) demographic information (con-
founders), (2) self-perceptions of language proficiency in the languages that participants are
exposed to (independent variable I), (3) language identity (newly developed from Stage 1;
independent variable II), (4) ethnic identity (mediator I), (5) acculturative stress (mediator
II), (6) racism and discrimination (mediator III), (7) anti-Asian hate crime victimization
(moderator), (8) psychological well-being for satisfaction/optimism levels of adjustment in
the U.S. (dependent variable I), and (9) mental health (dependent variable II). For demo-
graphic information, the country of origin, current age, age of arrival, length of time in the
U.S., gender, socioeconomic status, family type, place of residence, religion, educational
attainment, family background, and learning environment will be collected. Participants’
perceived language skills in their languages will be measured through a published self-
report classification tool [74] and the Language Experience and Proficiency Questionnaire
(LEAP-Q) [75]. To assess language identity, we will use our version of a language iden-
tity measurement scale developed in Stage 1. Ethnocultural identity confusion will be
measured using the Ethnic Identity Scale (EIS) [76] and the Multigroup Ethnic Identity
Measure-Revised (MEIM-R) [77]. To measure acculturative stress due to L2 learning for
adjustment to U.S. society, the Demands of Immigration Scale (DIS) [78] will be used, which
measures demands related to immigration, occupational adjustment, language accommo-
dation, discrimination, and resettlement issues. For measuring racism and discrimination,
the +20-item Perceived Discrimination Scales [79] will be used. Regarding anti-Asian hate
crime victimization, we will use the National Crime Victimization Survey (NCVS) [80],
modifying it specifically for Asians/Asian Americans. The Ryffs Scales of Psychological
Wellbeing-54 [81] will be used for well-being measurement. Mental health disparities will
be assessed using the Center for Epidemiologic Studies-Depression Scale (CES-D) [82],
the Patient Health Questionnaire-9 (PHQ-9) [83], and the Generalized Anxiety Disorder-7
(GAD-7) [84]. To measure participants’ experiences related to the pandemic, we will add a
quantitative measurement scale, II. Coronavirus (COVID-19) Pandemic section from the
Baylor Religion Survey (Wave 6, 2021) [85], to our survey. Table 3 displays variable types,
constructs, and measurement names.
Table 3. Cont.
Figure 3. Possible
Figure Possiblepath
pathmodel
modelofof
thethe
association between
association perceived
between language
perceived proficiency
language and and
proficiency lan-
guage identity and health disparities and mental health conditions with mediators.
language identity and health disparities and mental health conditions with mediators.
4.3.2.
4.3.2. Data Analysis
The
The same
same approach
approachas
asthe
thecross-sectional
cross-sectionalonline
onlinestudy forfor
study thethe
quantitative analysis
quantitative of
analysis
OLS
of OLSwillwill
be be
used.
used.
4.3.3.
4.3.3. Primary
Primary Outcome
Outcome
We aim to disentangle the previously confounded constructs from the existing per-
We aim to disentangle the previously confounded constructs from the existing per-
ceived language proficiency and the newly developed language identity scales from Stage 1
ceived language proficiency and the newly developed language identity scales from Stage
by employing those two distinct measures. We will assess whether the two constructs,
1 by employing those two distinct measures. We will assess whether the two constructs,
perceived language proficiency and language identity, make distinct contributions to psy-
perceived language proficiency and language identity, make distinct contributions to psy-
chological well-being/mental health. This approach will allow us to examine the potential
chological well-being/mental health. This approach will allow us to examine the potential
differential effects of these two language-related constructs on psychological well-being
differential effects of these two language-related constructs on psychological well-being
and mental health disparities in 1.5 generation Asian/Asian American immigrant young
and mental health disparities in 1.5 generation Asian/Asian American immigrant young
adults in the Bay Area, California. Analyses of the effects of potential mediator variables,
adults in the Bay Area, California. Analyses of the effects of potential mediator variables,
including ethnocultural identity confusion, acculturative stress, racism, and discrimination,
including ethnocultural identity confusion, acculturative stress, racism, and discrimina-
will be presented. Additionally, we will measure the moderating effect of the variable
tion, will be presented. Additionally, we will measure the moderating effect of the variable
anti-Asian hate crime victimization, which may influence the magnitude of the relationship
anti-Asian
between thehate crime victimization,
independent and moderatorwhich may influence the magnitude of the relation-
variables.
ship between the independent and moderator variables.
5. Stage 3: Qualitative Analysis
5. Stage 3: Qualitative Analysis
Subsequent in-depth interviews will be conducted to flesh out contextual information
Subsequent
and patterns in-depth about
of experience interviews will be conducted
the cross-sectional to flesh
information out contextual
examined infor-
in the quantita-
tive analysis, using a subsample of quantitative survey respondents in Stage 2. Based the
mation and patterns of experience about the cross-sectional information examined in on
quantitative
their analysis, using
survey responses, a subsample
we will seek to gainof anquantitative survey respondents
in-depth understanding in Stage
of experiences 2.
with
Based on theirand
acculturation survey responses,and
discrimination we how
will seek
thesetoexperiences
gain an in-depth understanding
relate to of expe-
their own perceptions
riences
of withskills
language acculturation and discrimination
and psychological and howhealth
well-being/mental these experiences relate
(Stage 3A). An to their
analysis of
own perceptions
qualitative of provide
data will languagefurther
skills context
and psychological well-being/mental
around the statistical results ofhealth (Stage
the quantita-
tive analysis [86]. Translation support for those who note that they prefer a bicultural or
bilingual interviewers will be provided, just as in Stage 1.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2024, 21, 1311 13 of 19
5.1.2. Recruitment
The recruitment area will be expanded from Santa Clara County, used in Stage 1, to the
San Francisco Bay Area, Northern California, as it is for the survey. There will be two dif-
ferent groups: (1) a group that reported mental health consequences, and (2) a group did
not report mental health issues. Approximately 20 young adults who completed the online
survey will be recruited, and there will be 20 in-depth interviews, 10 participants from
one group and the other 10 participants from the other group. We will choose participants
selectively to obtain well-balanced samples that reflect our various demographic variables,
such as age range and the distribution of countries of origin [68]. An incentive of $40 gift
cards will be provided to all participants, and additionally, to encourage participants to
join an interview, a transportation voucher of up to $30 will be provided for those who live
farther than 57 miles from the study location—this is the average distance from all 80 target
institutions/organizations to the main study location.
Table 4. Stage 3A: Deductive approach for in-depth life history interviews.
Table 4. Cont.
6. Discussion
6.1. How the Proposed Research Would Shift Current Research Paradigms
This proposed research and its methodology is innovative because it will be the first
to investigate how the constructs of perceived language proficiency and language identity
can link to a sense of belonging, psychological well-being, and mental health among
multilingual 1.5 generation Asian/Asian American immigrant young adults, who are
vulnerable to becoming victims of anti-Asian hate crimes. Moreover, immigrant-tailored
social adjustment strategies for feelings of acceptance in American society associated
with learning an L2 have not been substantially discussed. This specific circumstance of
multilingualism presents unique sociocultural benefits (i.e., a multilingual and multicultural
worldview), but also unique challenges to learning more than one language under strong
societal norms and pressures when there is a new dominant language and new culture.
Ultimately, we expect this study will be the cornerstone of establishing culturally tailored
multilingual education for Asian/Asian American immigrant young adults’ strong sense
of acceptance, inclusion, identity, and psychological well-being/mental health in the U.S.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2024, 21, 1311 15 of 19
6.3. Limitations
This research encounters a few limitations. Firstly, our inclusion criteria for recruiting
potential participants who fit the definition of the 1.5 generation are stringent, requiring
additional effort to identify suitable candidates. Specifically, participants must be Asian
or Asian American young adults residing in the San Francisco Bay Area, who arrived in
the U.S. with their first-generation parents between the ages of 5 and 17, have resided in
the U.S. for at least 12 months, and currently fall within the age range of 18 to 29 years.
While the anticipated number of 40 participants in the focus group discussions in Stage 1A
and the minimum sample size of 146 participants in Stage 2B align with the consideration
of potential challenges in identifying eligible participants, they may still present certain
difficulties in fully validating this long-term protocol. Additionally, achieving thematic
saturation and managing potential subjective variability might be areas to carefully monitor
with the anticipated sample size for the Stage 1A focus group discussions and the Stage 2C
survey analyses. Secondly, the geographic scope of our participant pool is confined to the
San Francisco Bay Area in California, thereby limiting the representativeness of the broader
1.5 generation Asian/Asian American young adult population in California and across
the U.S. A more geographically diverse sample can be considered throughout the study
protocol to increase the validity of the study.
Supplementary Materials: The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https:
//www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/ijerph21101311/s1, Table S1. List of higher education institutions
in the San Francisco Bay Area.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2024, 21, 1311 16 of 19
Funding: Research reported in this publication was supported by the National Institute of General
Medical Sciences of the National Institutes of Health under Award Number R16GM150715. The
content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views
of the National Institutes of Health.
Institutional Review Board Statement: This study was approved by San José State University’s
Institutional Review Board. Type of review was an exempt registration: Category of approval
§46.104(d)(2iii) (IRB Number: 23105).
Informed Consent Statement: Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.
Data Availability Statement: Data sharing is not applicable to this article as no datasets were
generated or analyzed during the current study.
Conflicts of Interest: The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
References
1. Howe, P.J. The United States as a Plural Society: Towards a Consociational Solution? Swiss Political Sci. Rev. 2019, 25, 476–497.
[CrossRef]
2. Gans, H.J. Toward a Reconciliation of “Assimilation” and “Pluralism”: The Interplay of Acculturation and Ethnic Retention. Int.
Migr. Rev. 1997, 31, 875–892.
3. Healey, J.F.; Stepnick, A. Diversity and Society: Race, Ethnicity, and Gender; Sage Publications: Thousand Oaks, CA, USA, 2019.
4. LaFromboise, T.; Coleman, H.L.K.; Gerton, J. Psychological impact of biculturalism: Evidence and theory. Psychol. Bull. 1993, 114,
395–412. [CrossRef]
5. Phinney, J.S. Ethic identity and acculturation. In Acculturation: Advances in Theory, Measurement, and Applied Research; American
Psychological Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2003; pp. 63–81.
6. Goodz, N.S. Interactions between parents and children in bilingual families. In Educating Second Language Children: The Whole
Child, the Whole Curriculum, the Whole Community; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1994; pp. 61–81.
7. Kohnert, K. Second language acquisition: Success factors in sequential bilingualism. ASHA Lead. 2008, 13, 10–13. [CrossRef]
8. Berken, J.A.; Gracco, V.L.; Klein, D. Early bilingualism, language attainment, and brain development. Neuropsychologia 2017, 98,
220–227. [CrossRef]
9. Nwokah, E. Simultaneous and sequential language acquisition in Nigerian children. First Lang. 1984, 5, 57–73. [CrossRef]
10. Smart, J.F.; Smart, D.W. Acculturative stress of Hispanics: Loss and challenge. J. Couns. Dev. 1995, 73, 390–396. [CrossRef]
11. Berry, J.W. Immigration, acculturation, and adaptation. Appl. Psychol. 1997, 46, 5–34. [CrossRef]
12. Rosenthal, D.A.; Feldman, S.S. The acculturation of Chinese immigrants: Perceived effects on family functioning of length of
residence in two cultural contexts. J. Genet. Psychol. 1990, 151, 495–514. [CrossRef]
13. Yoon, E.; Chang, C.T.; Kim, S.; Clawson, A.; Cleary, S.E.; Hansen, M.; Bruner, J.P.; Chan, T.K.; Gomes, A.M. A meta-analysis of
acculturation/enculturation and mental health. J. Couns. Psychol. 2013, 60, 15–30. [CrossRef]
14. Romero, A.J.; Roberts, R.E. Stress within a bicultural context for adolescents of Mexican descent. Cult. Divers. Ethn. Minor. Psychol.
2003, 9, 171–184. [CrossRef]
15. Cheung, M. Elderly Chinese Living in the United States: Assimilation or Adjustment? Soc. Work. 1989, 34, 457–461.
16. Norton, B. Language and Identity. In Sociolinguistics and Language Education; Nancy, H.H., Sandra Lee, M., Eds.; Multilingual
Matters: Bristol, UK, 2010; pp. 349–369.
17. Morgan, B. Poststructuralism and applied linguistics: Complementary approaches to identity and culture in ELT. In International
Handbook of English Language Teaching; Springer: Boston, MA, USA, 2007; pp. 1033–1052.
18. Gaitán-Aguilar, L.; Hofhuis, J.; Bierwiaczonek, K.; Carmona, C. Social media use, social identification and cross-cultural adaptation
of international students: A longitudinal examination. Front. Psychol. 2022, 13, 1013375. [CrossRef]
19. Billedo, C.J.; Kerkhof, P.; Finkenauer, C.; Ganzeboom, H. Facebook and Face-to-Face: Examining the Short- and Long-Term
Reciprocal Effects of Interactions, Perceived Social Support, and Depression among International Students. J. Comput.-Mediat.
Commun. 2019, 24, 73–89. [CrossRef]
20. Szabó, Á.; Papp, Z.Z.; Nguyen Luu, L.A. Social contact configurations of international students at school and outside of school:
Implications for acculturation orientations and psychological adjustment. Int. J. Intercult. Relat. 2020, 77, 69–82. [CrossRef]
21. Bethel, A.; Szabo, A.; Ward, C. Parallel lives?: Predicting and enhancing connectedness between international and domestic
students. In Multi-Dimensional Transitions of International Students to Higher Education; Routledge: London, UK, 2016; pp. 21–36.
22. Taušová, J.; Bender, M.; Dimitrova, R.; van de Vijver, F. The role of perceived cultural distance, personal growth initiative,
language proficiencies, and tridimensional acculturation orientations for psychological adjustment among international students.
Int. J. Intercult. Relat. 2019, 69, 11–23. [CrossRef]
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2024, 21, 1311 17 of 19
23. Shizha, E.; Makwarimba, E. Introduction to Transnationalism, Intersectionality, and Migration Ecological Trajectories. In Immigrant
Lives: Intersectionality, Transnationality, and Global Perspectives; Shizha, E., Makwarimba, E., Eds.; Oxford University Press: Oxford,
UK, 2023; pp. 1–22.
24. Crenshaw, K. Mapping the Margins: Intersectionality, Identity Politics, and Violence against Women of Color. Stanf. Law Rev.
1991, 43, 1241–1299. [CrossRef]
25. Pries, L. Transnationalism. In Introduction to Migration Studies: An Interactive Guide to the Literatures on Migration and Diversity;
Scholten, P., Ed.; Springer International Publishing: Cham, Switzerland, 2022; pp. 233–247.
26. AAPI Equity Alliance; Chinese for Affirmative Action; San Francisco State University. Stop AAPI Hate. Available online:
https://stopaapihate.org/ (accessed on 19 December 2022).
27. Asian Americans Advancing Justice. Asian Americans Advancing Justice. Available online: https://www.advancingjustice-aajc.
org/ (accessed on 19 December 2022).
28. NYC Commission on Human Rights. Stop Asian Hate. Available online: https://www1.nyc.gov/site/cchr/community/stop-
asian-hate.page (accessed on 19 December 2022).
29. California State University San Bernardino. Center for the Study of Hate and Extremism. Available online: https://www.csusb.
edu/hate-and-extremism-center (accessed on 19 December 2022).
30. Huynh, Q.-L.; Devos, T.; Smalarz, L. Perpetual Foreigner in One’s Own Land: Potential Implications for Identity and Psychological
Adjustment. J. Soc. Clin. Psychol. 2011, 30, 133–162. [CrossRef]
31. Sue, D.W.; Bucceri, J.; Lin, A.I.; Nadal, K.L.; Torino, G.C. Racial microaggressions and the Asian American experience. Cult.
Divers. Ethn. Minor. Psychol. 2007, 13, 72–81. [CrossRef]
32. Lau, S.B.; Bersamira, C.S.; Lee, Y.J.; Yamauchi, J. Asian American social workers’ experiences and perspectives on anti-Asian hate.
J. Soc. Work. 2023, 24, 21–39. [CrossRef]
33. Ruiz, N.G.; Im, C.; Tian, Z. Asian Americans and the ‘Forever Foreigner’ Stereotype. Available online: https://www.pewresearch.
org/2023/11/30/asian-americans-and-the-forever-foreigner-stereotype/ (accessed on 25 September 2024).
34. California State University San Bernardino. Report to the Nation: 2020s—Dawn of a Decade of Rising Hate (Summer 2022); Center for
the Study of Hate and Extremism: San Bernardino, CA, USA, 2022.
35. Miu, A.S.; Moore, J.R. Behind the Masks: Experiences of Mental Health Practitioners of Color during the COVID-19 Pandemic.
Acad. Psychiatry 2021, 45, 539–544. [CrossRef]
36. The White House. FACT SHEET: President Biden Announces Additional Actions to Respond to Anti-Asian Violence, Xenophobia
and Bias—The White House. Available online: https://www.whitehouse.gov/briefing-room/statements-releases/2021/03/30
/fact-sheet-president-biden-announces-additional-actions-to-respond-to-anti-asian-violence-xenophobia-and-bias/ (accessed
on 23 September 2022).
37. NPR. Activists Say Anti-Asian Attacks Go Unreported Due to Stereotypes, Language Barriers. Available online: https://www.
npr.org/transcripts/980075515 (accessed on 23 September 2022).
38. Murphy-Shigematsu, S.; Sein, K.; Wakimoto, P.; Wang, M. Asian American student stress: The other side of achievement. In Asian
Pacific American Experiences: Past, Present, and Future; Kendall Hunt Publishing Company: Dubuque, IA, USA, 2012; pp. 204–219.
39. Lee, S.; Juon, H.S.; Martinez, G.; Hsu, C.E.; Robinson, E.S.; Bawa, J.; Ma, G.X. Model minority at risk: Expressed needs of mental
health by Asian American young adults. J. Community Health 2009, 34, 144–152. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
40. Montemitro, C.; D’Andrea, G.; Cesa, F.; Martinotti, G.; Pettorruso, M.; Di Giannantonio, M.; Muratori, R.; Tarricone, I. Language
proficiency and mental disorders among migrants: A systematic review. Eur. Psychiatry 2021, 64, e49. [CrossRef]
41. Upton, T.A.; Lee-Thompson, L.-C. The role of the first language in second language reading. Stud. Second. Lang. Acquis. 2001, 23,
469–495. [CrossRef]
42. Poulisse, N.; Bongaerts, T. First Language Use in Second Language Production. Appl. Linguist. 1994, 15, 36–57. [CrossRef]
43. Golkova, D.; Hubackova, S. Productive Skills in Second Language Learning. Procedia-Soc. Behav. Sci. 2014, 143, 477–481.
[CrossRef]
44. Asher, J.J. Children’s First Language as a Model for Second Language Learning. Mod. Lang. J. 1972, 56, 133–139. [CrossRef]
45. Littlewood, W.; Yu, B. First language and target language in the foreign language classroom. Lang. Teach. 2011, 44, 64–77.
[CrossRef]
46. Armon-Lotem, S.; de Jong, J.H.; Meir, N. Assessing Multilingual Children: Disentangling Bilingualism from Language Impairment;
Multilingual Matters: Bristol, UK, 2015.
47. Paradis, J. The interface between bilingual development and specific language impairment. Appl. Psycholinguist. 2010, 31, 227–252.
[CrossRef]
48. Ebert, K.D.; Kohnert, K. Language learning impairment in sequential bilingual children. Lang. Teach. 2016, 49, 301–338. [CrossRef]
49. Kramsch, C. Language and Culture. AILA Rev. 2014, 27, 30–55. [CrossRef]
50. Battiste, M. Language and culture in modern society. In Reclaiming Indigenous Voice and Vision; University of British Columbia
Press: Vancouver, BC, Canada, 2000; pp. 192–208.
51. Mazari, A.; Derraz, N. Language and culture. Int. J. Humanit. Cult. Stud. (IJHCS) 2016, 2, 350–359.
52. Sherzer, J. Discourse-centered approach to language and culture. Am. Anthropol. 1987, 89, 295–309. [CrossRef]
53. Crawford-Lange, L.M.; Lange, D.L. Integrating language and culture: How to do it. Theory Pract. 1987, 26, 258–266. [CrossRef]
54. Norton, B. Language, Identity, and the Ownership of English. TESOL Q. 1997, 31, 409–429. [CrossRef]
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2024, 21, 1311 18 of 19
85. Baylor University. Baylor Religion Survey. Available online: https://baylorreligionsurvey.research.baylor.edu/ (accessed on 20
July 2024).
86. Creswell, J.W. Designing and Conducting Mixed Methods Research, 3rd ed.; SAGE: Thousand Oaks, CA, USA, 2018.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual
author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to
people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.
Reproduced with permission of copyright owner. Further reproduction
prohibited without permission.