Instant download Psychiatric Expert Testimony Emerging Applications 1st Edition Kenneth J. Weiss pdf all chapter
Instant download Psychiatric Expert Testimony Emerging Applications 1st Edition Kenneth J. Weiss pdf all chapter
Instant download Psychiatric Expert Testimony Emerging Applications 1st Edition Kenneth J. Weiss pdf all chapter
com
https://ebookname.com/product/psychiatric-expert-testimony-
emerging-applications-1st-edition-kenneth-j-weiss/
OR CLICK BUTTON
DOWNLOAD EBOOK
https://ebookname.com/product/financial-expert-witness-communication-
a-practical-guide-to-reporting-and-testimony-1st-edition-bradley-j-
preber/
ebookname.com
https://ebookname.com/product/psychiatric-genetics-review-of-
psychiatry-1st-edition-kenneth-s/
ebookname.com
https://ebookname.com/product/genetics-and-the-logic-of-evolution-1st-
edition-kenneth-m-weiss/
ebookname.com
https://ebookname.com/product/fluid-dynamics-and-transport-of-
droplets-and-sprays-2ed-edition-william-a-sirignano/
ebookname.com
TMR Research in Insulating Granular Magnetic Materials 1st
Edition Shigeo Honda
https://ebookname.com/product/tmr-research-in-insulating-granular-
magnetic-materials-1st-edition-shigeo-honda/
ebookname.com
https://ebookname.com/product/the-faraway-war-personal-diaries-of-the-
second-world-war-in-asia-and-the-pacific-1st-edition-richard-j-
aldrich/
ebookname.com
https://ebookname.com/product/joseph-stalin-an-annotated-bibliography-
of-english-language-periodical-literature-to-2005-david-r-egan/
ebookname.com
https://ebookname.com/product/the-world-bank-legal-review-law-and-
justice-for-development-1st-edition-world-bank/
ebookname.com
https://ebookname.com/product/el-nino-in-history-storming-through-the-
ages-1st-edition-cesar-n-caviedes/
ebookname.com
Understanding Quality Peace Peacebuilding after Civil War
Madhav Joshi
https://ebookname.com/product/understanding-quality-peace-
peacebuilding-after-civil-war-madhav-joshi/
ebookname.com
Psychiatric Expert Testimony: Emerging
Applications
Psychiatric Expert
Testimony: Emerging
Applications
1
1
Oxford University Press is a department of the University of
Oxford. It furthers the University’s objective of excellence in research,
scholarship, and education by publishing worldwide.
Oxford New York
Auckland Cape Town Dar es Salaam Hong Kong Karachi
Kuala Lumpur Madrid Melbourne Mexico City Nairobi
New Delhi Shanghai Taipei Toronto
With offices in
Argentina Austria Brazil Chile Czech Republic France Greece
Guatemala Hungary Italy Japan Poland Portugal Singapore
South Korea Switzerland Thailand Turkey Ukraine Vietnam
Oxford is a registered trademark of Oxford University Press
in the UK and certain other countries.
Published in the United States of America by
Oxford University Press
198 Madison Avenue, New York, NY 10016
© Oxford University Press 2015
All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in
a retrieval system, or transmitted, in any form or by any means, without the prior
permission in writing of Oxford University Press, or as expressly permitted by law,
by license, or under terms agreed with the appropriate reproduction rights organization.
Inquiries concerning reproduction outside the scope of the above should be sent to the
Rights Department, Oxford University Press, at the address above.
You must not circulate this work in any other form
and you must impose this same condition on any acquirer.
Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data
Psychiatric expert testimony : emerging applications / edited by Kenneth J. Weiss,
Clarence Watson.
p.; cm.
Includes bibliographical references and index.
ISBN 978–0–19–934659–2 (alk. paper)
ISBN 978–0–19–934660–8 (updf)
ISBN 978–0–19–934661–5 (epub)
I. Weiss, Kenneth J. editor. II. Watson, Clarence, editor. [DNLM: 1. Forensic Psychiatry—
United States. 2. Brain—pathology—United States. 3. Criminal Law—United
States. 4. Criminals—psychology—United States. 5. Expert Testimony—United States.
W 740] RA1148 614.15—dc23
2014022899
This material is not intended to be, and should not be considered, a substitute for medical or other
professional advice. Treatment for the conditions described in this material is highly dependent on
the individual circumstances. And, while this material is designed to offer accurate information
with respect to the subject matter covered and to be current as of the time it was written, research
and knowledge about medical and health issues is constantly evolving and dose schedules for
medications are being revised continually, with new side effects recognized and accounted for
regularly. Readers must therefore always check the product information and clinical procedures
with the most up-to-date published product information and data sheets provided by the
manufacturers and the most recent codes of conduct and safety regulation. The publisher and the
authors make no representations or warranties to readers, express or implied, as to the accuracy or
completeness of this material. Without limiting the foregoing, the publisher and the authors make
no representations or warranties as to the accuracy or efficacy of the drug dosages mentioned in the
material. The authors and the publisher do not accept, and expressly disclaim, any responsibility
for any liability, loss or risk that may be claimed or incurred as a consequence of the use and/or
application of any of the contents of this material.
9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1
Printed in the United States of America
on acid-free paper
CON TEN T S
Foreword vii
by Robert L. Sadoff
Contributors ix
Introduction xi
by Kenneth J. Weiss and Clarence Watson
Index 173
FORE WORD
BY ROBERT L . SA DOFF
The field of forensic psychiatry has grown significantly during the past
half-century. Rising from the few physicians who were referred to as alienists
in the 19th century, forensic psychiatry has become one of the major subspe-
cialties of psychiatry. Spurred by the inauguration of the American Academy
of Psychiatry and the Law in 1969, the field has burgeoned to include a
number of major professional organizations, scientific journals, and scores
of textbooks. Under the leadership of Dr. Richard Rosner, fellowship train-
ing in forensic psychiatry began in the 1980s and has led to a current 40
ACGME-accredited training programs in forensic psychiatry throughout the
country. Board certification in forensic psychiatry began with the American
Board of Forensic Psychiatry, and currently certification is authorized under
the aegis of the American Board of Psychiatry and Neurology.
Forensic psychiatry is but one of the many forensic sciences that are
essential to modern-day court cases in which scientific evidence is presented
in both civil and criminal cases. Standards for presenting expert psychiat-
ric testimony have developed through legislation, case law, and professional
organizations. The judge is the gatekeeper to prevent “junk science” from
being presented at trial, and rules have been developed to limit the testi-
mony of various professionals. In addition, professional organizations have
developed ethical guidelines under which their members must testify.
As the use of forensic specialists has grown, so has the need for training
and education in the emerging issues that confront the expert testifying in
court. Drs. Weiss and Watson have assembled a group of authors to illus-
trate the emerging issues for the forensic psychiatric expert. Dr. Weiss is a
senior forensic psychiatrist who has developed an expertise in the history
of medicine, psychiatry, and forensic psychiatry. Dr. Watson, who is also an
attorney, has used his integrative education to improve his skills in the field.
As the editors point out, most cases do not go to trial but are settled. The
forensic psychiatrist has multiple roles to play in civil and criminal cases
aside from testifying in court. The 4 major efforts by the forensic psychiatrist
are examining the plaintiff or defendant, writing a report, consulting with
attorneys, and testifying either at deposition or at trial. Increasingly, the role
of the forensic psychiatrist has become that of a consultant to the lawyer
or at times to the court. The forensic psychiatrist may use his or her educa-
tional skills in teaching lawyers, judges, or even juries emerging issues that
need clarification for a lay audience. Thus, it becomes increasingly important
for the forensic psychiatrist to have expertise in many areas of psychiatry.
However, the ethics of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law pro-
hibit the expert witness from testifying in areas beyond his or her expertise.
Training programs in forensic psychiatry have increased not only in number
but also in the scope of the educational experiences of the fellows in training.
This book highlights the emerging issues in forensic psychiatry that every
psychiatrist who enters the courtroom should be aware of before agreeing to
become an expert witness. Even though the expert witness is often a consul-
tant and does not testify, he or she still needs to know the issues surrounding
the case.
The growth of forensic psychiatry has accelerated and the field will con-
tinue to expand and develop in the future. The expert psychiatrist needs to
keep up with advances in the field as well as changes in the legal system.
There are pitfalls and areas of concern that need to be avoided or clearly
understood before entering the legal system. This book is essential for any-
one willing to provide expert testimony and advice to lawyers and judges.
[ viii ] Foreword
CONTRIB U TOR S
[ x ] Contributors
IN TROD U C T ION
The word forensic, the adjective of the Latin forum, refers to matters in the
public sphere, such as the market or court. Often conflated with the more
famous forensic pathology, forensic psychiatry and psychology are not stud-
ies of death. They are applications of psychiatry and psychology, each having
its own professional body of literature, training, and accreditation. Mental
health professionals and other expert witnesses are visitors to an alien planet
when they enter the legal arena. Those of us who specialize in forensic work
can become comfortable with the language, procedures, and overall feeling
of legal proceedings. Still, experts who are accustomed to being “captain of
the ship” in their own domains may be jolted by differences between mental
health treatment settings and litigation.
Implied in the word forensic, mental health applications to litigation
(criminal or civil) are conducted in a public setting. In contrast to the sanc-
tity of the therapeutic encounter, once a person’s mental state is brought into
legal proceedings, what was private is no longer so. Of course, there are lim-
its, restrictions, and ethical constraints on certain disclosures. Nevertheless,
raising mental health issues, especially in adversarial proceedings, can put
intimate—even shameful—details of someone’s life under a microscope. For
this reason, it is not advisable for mental health professionals to blur roles
by giving expert testimony for their own clients, a situation referred to as
“wearing two hats.”1 The two-hats problem diminishes objectivity when tes-
tifying about one’s patient and can have a disruptive effect on the therapeu-
tic relationship.
[ xii ] Introduction
not have been sane at the time of a homicide. No, an expert witness must
provide a diagnosis and “connect the dots” between this condition and the
legal standard for criminal responsibility in that jurisdiction (there are dif-
ferences from state to state). It would make sense that a court could appoint
an expert to provide this indispensible function. History tells us, however,
that there is great resistance from lawyers to dispense with partisan experts.
There was a huge spike in using forensic expertise in the 19th century,
coupled with extensive treatises on forensic medicine, toxicology, and the
jurisprudence of insanity.2–4 The culture of American jurisprudence shifted
toward misuse of experts, many of whom lacked credentials and personal
integrity.5 To a degree, the justice system has never fully recovered. Whenever
there is a battle of the experts, citizens tend to look on it as more of a circus
than a truth-seeking venture. One of this book’s objectives is to push the tide
the other way.
Tension is also found in the interplay between the ends of justice, the
march of science, the manner in which experts interpret science, and trends
in societal values. This is best illustrated in a succession of cases of the insan-
ity defense in England6 and in America. Before 1800, traditional jurispru-
dence was that mental excuses for behavior were to be reserved for those
whose capacities were no keener than those of a wild beast. But in 1800,
a deluded James Hadfield shot at King George of England and was acquit-
ted. This thrust the “delusion test” for insanity into the forefront of juris-
prudence, to the dismay of Parliament. The legislature quickly passed a law
permitting the confinement of insanity acquittees, and Hadfield spent his
remaining 40 years in institutions.
Then in 1840, it was Queen Victoria’s turn to face a madman, when
Edward Oxford attempted to assassinate her with pistols. Oxford was found
insane and confined for decades. Three years later, a deluded Scotsman,
Daniel M’Naghten, believing he was shooting Prime Minister Robert Peal,
shot his secretary, Mr. Drummond. By then, defense attorney Cockburn
took advantage of an American textbook, Isaac Ray’s A Treatise on the Medical
Jurisprudence of Insanity.4 Indeed, M’Naghten was acquitted by reason of
insanity, which created tension in Buckingham Palace and Parliament. The
House of Lords quickly responded to restrict the use of the insanity defense,
creating M’Naghten’s Rules. The surviving rule, adopted by many states in
America, is a purely “cognitive” test: Acquittal depends on a thought disorder
causing a “defect of reason” such that, at the time of the act in question, the
defendant did not know that the behavior was wrong. This narrow defini-
tion of insanity continues to be a problem for expert witnesses, who might
want to introduce evidence of emotional or impulse-control disorders into a
defense—and may even demonstrate the basis for them via neuroimaging.
In America, the insanity standard came into focus when John Hinckley
shot President Reagan in 1981. At that time, the federal standard for insanity
Introduction [ xiii ]
included both cognitive (appreciating wrongfulness) and volitional (reduced
ability to conform one’s behavior) prongs. Hinckley was acquitted in his 1982
trial, where an early use of psychiatric testimony based on brain computed
tomography findings was permitted after a difficult process. More impor-
tant, the public outrage at the acquittal, despite Hinckley’s civil commitment,
gave rise to the Insanity Defense Reform Act of 1984. This Act restricted
the role of the expert witness and has been viewed as an aid to prosecutors.
Meanwhile, there is no constitutionally mandated right to an insanity plea
for criminal defendants and, as of 2012, the U.S. Supreme Court has declined
to consider the matter.7 Kansas, Montana, Idaho, and Utah have abolished
insanity pleas, further restricting the possibility of expert witnesses shed-
ding light on criminal behavior.
[ xiv ] Introduction
objection, photographs using the X-ray process could be produced as sec-
ondary evidence (only in conjunction with expert testimony), on a par with
maps and charts. The matter was settled, but the issues raised about seeking
deeper—or definitive—truth remain with us.9,10 Several of our authors will
discuss the problems and prospects for the use of indirect data in answering
legal questions. The main thing to remember is that these emerging technol-
ogies do not stand on their own as evidence; they support the expert’s opin-
ions, which must be defended sometimes in pretrial evidentiary hearings.
In the 19th century, psychiatrists provided expert testimony on their
observations of individuals. Rather than giving direct opinions on legal mat-
ters, they would respond to hypothetical (“what if”) questions. There was
little other science to shed light on questions of mental states. Phrenology,
dominant through much of the 19th century, did not make its way into the
courtroom by way of a psychiatric defense.11 Yet, the search for indirect
markers of psychological truth remained an elusive goal. The most famous
example of this is the use of the polygraph as a means of lie detection. Other
20th-century techniques, such as hypnosis and narcosis (truth serum), have
had their day but remain controversial because the results tend to border on
the forbidden area of prejudicing the jury. Inferential data from the auto-
nomic nervous system, such as from the polygraph, have a clinical truth but
lack forensic power. The universal phenomenon of blushing may indicate
many things; this is the subject of a recent book.12 As legendary Harvard psy-
chiatry professor Dr. Elvin Semrad used to say, “The autonomic nervous sys-
tem doesn’t lie.” That may be true in the treatment context, where blushing
or dry mouth betrays the patient’s words. But we think that Dr. Semrad, who
likely was never cross-examined, would agree that courts require a different
type of truth, one that is usually beyond the reach of the testifying expert.
Besides, as criminologist Adrian Raine points out, psychopaths lie with their
autonomic nervous systems as well as through their mouths.13 So much for
clinical impressionism!
In the early 20th century, Harvard psychologist Hugo Münsterberg, intent
on applying science to the courtroom, touted a scientific method for verify-
ing eyewitness testimony.14 After a series of psychology experiments using
word-association analysis, he believed he had broken through the barrier
between the laboratory and the real world, thus initiating forensic psychol-
ogy. Professor Münsterberg, otherwise known for originating applied and
forensic psychologies in America, was severely criticized, setting back the
use of expert testimony in cases of questionable memory. The most severe
criticism came from John H. Wigmore, dean of Northwestern Law School,
whose name is virtually synonymous with the law of evidence. Around the
same time, Swiss psychiatrist C. G. Jung researched a word-association test
as a clinical shortcut to uncover neurotic complexes. Sigmund Freud, skepti-
cal of Jung’s methods, belittled his colleague while lecturing to a group of
Introduction [ xv ]
law students in Vienna in 1906.15,16 Freud took a dim view of forensic applica-
tions of psychoanalytic theory and generally refused to participate in trials.
He knew intuitively the difference between psychological theory and forensi-
cally robust testimony.
The idea of technologically aided truth finding will be echoed in this vol-
ume. First, Clarence Watson will review the standards for scientific certainty,
including the Frye17 test for general acceptance of scientific findings and the
Daubert18 test for admissibility. In the 1923 D.C. Circuit Court opinion in
U.S. v. Frye, the proposed scientific lie detection (systolic blood pressure eleva-
tion) was rejected as evidence because it lacked acceptance within the scientific
community. In Chapter 6, Octavio Choi will review the iterations of lie detec-
tion and the circumstances under which expert testimony can be admitted.
The search for truth goes on, but will it be found in brain function or structure?
Expert witnesses rarely enter the courtroom, because more than 9 out
of 10 cases, civil and criminal, do not result in trials. The reason is sim-
ple: Settlements are often a safer course than placing one’s fate in the hands
of strangers. On the civil side, a negotiated or arbitrated settlement means
that the injured party, the plaintiff, will receive monetary damages in a
smaller amount than hoped. The defendant, by settling, will have eliminated
the possibility of a huge jury award. On the criminal side, a settled case means
that the defendant has accepted responsibility for an unlawful act, but often
one that carries a smaller sentence (e.g., for assault with a weapon rather
than attempted murder). The settlement, when approved by the parties and
the judge, ends the litigation and precludes a trial. This outcome, however,
does not eliminate the role of expert witnesses. To the contrary: Experts may
be crucial in bringing closure to these difficult situations.
If most cases are never tried, why would we bother to write about expert
testimony? By testimony we are referring to the totality of the expert’s work
product. Some professionals serve as “consulting experts,” who assist attor-
neys on the sidelines and never issue reports or testimony. Most profes-
sionals engaged by attorneys are “testifying experts,” who form scientific
opinions on matters before the court, put their findings into written reports,
and provide live testimony under oath in depositions or trials.
By far the most prevalent type of work product is the written report.
Indeed, more often than not, the expert report is the only work product in a
case; its importance cannot be overestimated. The psychiatric expert report
has been the standard since the 19th century and is the subject of a book of
its own.19 As Clarence Watson will explain in Chapter 1, most expert medical
opinions, psychiatric or otherwise, must be expressed to a level of confidence
[ xvi ] Introduction
called “reasonable medical certainty” (or a variant, such as reasonable scien-
tific certainty or probability). Over the years, legal authorities have arrived
at this convention, which, simply stated, says that experts must state that
it is more likely than not that there is a causal relationship in the matter at
hand—for example, that the motor vehicle accident caused traumatic stress,
or that schizophrenia caused a cognitive defect that prevented someone from
knowing that an otherwise criminal act was wrong. Sometimes this conven-
tion is called the 51% rule, although generally we would not use a quantitative
description; it is just understood by the lawyers and judge. A higher thresh-
old of certainty is required in civil commitment, where there is a potential for
deprivation of liberty by virtue of what we say. By contrast, the expert witness’
burden is never as high as that of a criminal juror, who, to vote for conviction
of a defendant, must find that the prosecution proved its case beyond a reason-
able doubt. Legal authorities recognize that scientific testimony cannot be held
to that standard. Even fingerprint analysis20 and polygraphic lie detection21 are
not infallible and can be challenged, but do not stand alone as proof.
It is clear that expert witnesses play a supporting role in trials, although
sometimes that role is the center of attention. This is true in medical malprac-
tice cases, where the parties and trier of fact look to experts to define whether
the defendant clinician’s actions were above or below an acceptable level of
competence (standard of care). We will not be focusing on malpractice mat-
ters in this book. How, then, does the expert witness apply his or her educa-
tion, specialized knowledge, skill, and experience to legal matters? Unlike
the medical setting that asks us a health-related question, the legal setting
always engages us with a legal question. We are rarely expected to answer the
legal question itself—only to shed light on it so that a jury or judge can decide.
Examples of typical legal questions are: Does an individual have the mental
capacity to enter into a contract? Was an employee injured by the actions of
the employer? Is a criminal defendant’s mental illness a bar to participating in
a trial? Although one can see how expert testimony would play a pivotal role
in aiding the decision maker, the witness is always subservient to the court
(judge) and its officers (attorneys). The fact that there are usually expert opin-
ions on both sides of cases should keep us humble. However, it does not always
prevent some experts from acting arrogant or grandiose, which can be harmful
to the case and to the profession. And while many expert witnesses in adver-
sarial settings identify with the cause of the party that hired them, assuming
the role of combatant may reduce objectivity and scientific credibility.
The persuasive power of the expert witness must be used with caution and
mindfulness. As Robert Sadoff has urged, the expert must try to be efficacious
Introduction [ xvii ]
while minimizing harm.22 Aware of the need for professional standards and
the importance of maintaining credibility in non-healthcare settings, the
professions of forensic psychiatry and forensic psychology have developed
ethical guidelines.23,24 Because the aim of this book is to familiarize forensic
practitioners with emerging applications, we would be remiss if we did not
review essential ethics. It is especially relevant to review ethics when we con-
template using scientific information that might be considered speculative.
The expert witness must remember to stay a step back from any emotional
attachment to a diagnostic tool or interviewing technique to ensure that the
focus of the testimony is on the legal question and not on self-promotion.
The following are key tenets of forensic psychiatric ethics, excerpted from
the guidelines published by the American Academy of Psychiatry and the
Law (AAPL).23
The AAPL ethics guidelines begin by acknowledging the complex-
ity and role ambiguity created by physicians working within legal set-
tings: “Psychiatrists in a forensic role are called upon to practice in a manner
that balances competing duties to the individual and to society. In doing so,
they should be bound by underlying ethical principles of respect for persons,
honesty, justice, and social responsibility”.”23
The fact of a shift in the forensic psychiatrist’s role from healthcare pro-
vider to objective evaluator does not lessen or eliminate the need for confi-
dentiality. While it may be true that placing one’s mental state into litigation
renders the information public to a degree, forensic professionals are bound
to adhere to enhanced requirements for privacy and declaration of intent.
Thus, the AAPL guidelines call for the following:25
[ xviii ] Introduction
The AAPL guidelines call for honesty and striving for objectivity: “The
adversarial nature of most legal processes presents special hazards for the
practice of forensic psychiatry . . . Psychiatrists should not distort their opin-
ion in the service of the retaining party.”25 Adherence to these principles
requires self-awareness and vigilance. The main pitfalls are overidentifi-
cation with the retaining attorney’s legal position and the shading of one’s
position to earn more.25 Basing financial compensation to the expert on the
outcome of the case (contingency fee), for example, is forbidden. Another
way to ensure objectivity is for the expert to conduct comprehensive evalu-
ations: “Psychiatrists practicing in a forensic role enhance the honesty and
objectivity of their work by basing their forensic opinions, forensic reports
and forensic testimony on all available data.”25 Thus, the cherry-picking of
data or the intentional disregard of information that could conflict with the
referring attorney’s goal lacks integrity. While it may be ethical for lawyers to
do so (with the exception of criminal prosecutors), our code tells us otherwise.
As we discuss emerging applications of psychiatry to the law, the ques-
tion may arise in the reader’s mind as to whether to get involved in a case
requiring knowledge outside of general psychiatry (e.g., polysomnogra-
phy, brain development, or neuroimaging): “Expertise in the practice of
forensic psychiatry should be claimed only in areas of actual knowledge,
skills, training, and experience. When providing expert opinion, reports,
and testimony, psychiatrists should present their qualifications accurately
and precisely.” 25 A treating psychiatrist may send a patient to a diagnostic
radiologist for brain imaging and use the reading to guide therapy. A more
confident clinician will also read the images. However, when it comes to
using meaningful interpretations of images to answer legal questions, the
clinician as expert witness must either be prepared to explain thoroughly
the science and technology behind the imaging technique or to delegate
it to a neuroradiologist. In terms of representing one’s credentials, the
radiologist will not pretend to give a behavioral analysis of the findings
and the psychiatrist will not overclaim expertise on technical matters.
The AAPL guidelines add that “there are areas of special expertise, such
as the evaluation of children, persons of foreign cultures, or prisoners,
that may require special training or expertise.” 25 Thus, as you explore the
potential applications in this book, bear in mind that testimony must be
backed up by technical knowledge or specialized experience that will hold
up to cross-examination.
Our theme is integration of mental health expertise into civil and criminal
proceedings. Each chapter can stand alone as a review of a type of litigation
Introduction [ xix ]
requiring expert testimony. We encourage all readers to read Chapter 1 first,
to be grounded in the rules of the game. Beyond that, readers can explore
the applications as need or interest arises. We have incorporated DSM-5
nomenclature into these discussions, since finding a common clinical lan-
guage is often a key to formulating and communicating forensic opinions.
Throughout the book we have avoided a prescriptive approach, understand-
ing that each practitioner will incorporate current practice and case law into
reasonably evidence-based reports and testimony. We urge all practitioners
to consult local statutes and court opinions in preparation of their work
product, as the examples given here may not be applicable in all jurisdictions.
In Chapter 1, Clarence Watson will discuss the rules governing expert tes-
timony: when experts are used, what their qualifications must be, and, to a
degree, what they can say. It is important to remember that these rules (e.g.,
the Federal Rules of Evidence) were invented by lawyers in the interest of jus-
tice. That is, we experts do not make the rules—but we must conform to them.
You may indeed be the world’s expert in a scientific matter, but your opinions
on a legal matter may be rejected. How is this possible? As Dr. Watson explains,
the rules governing expert witnesses tell us that testimony must be helpful to
the “trier of fact” (jury or judge) without prejudicing the outcome—in addition
to being scientifically sound. The importance of this principle, for the purposes
of this book, cannot be overemphasized. Our contributors will be discussing
applications of psychiatric testimony that may be challenged as, for example,
too prejudicial or ahead of their time in terms of scientific weight.
We continue the specific content areas with chapters that include two
broad categories: human developmental perspectives in illuminating legal
issues (Chapters 2 through 5) and technological advances that support
expert testimony in emerging fields of interest (Chapters 6 through 10). In
Chapter 2, Nicole Foubister and Frank Tedeschi discuss how knowledge of
human psychological development informs testimony about children and
adolescents. This is increasingly important, as the distinctions between the
juvenile and adult brain structure and functioning are played out in court-
rooms and legislatures. In Chapter 3, Julia Curcio-Alexander, Jacqueline
Block Goldstein, and Kenneth Weiss explore child sexual abuse by way of
interpreting victims’ behavior. They note the phenomenon of “accommoda-
tion” of the victim, which can lead to difficulty prosecuting the perpetrator.
The question they discuss is how clinical evidence of accommodation (such
as denial or recantation) can be expressed in court without the testimony
being considered incriminating against the defendant. In Chapter 4, John
Northrop and Steven Berkowitz add the special circumstances of adverse
childhood experiences and early trauma to the discussion of brain develop-
ment as it pertains to morality, impulse control, and accountability. While it
may sound like a cliché to associate a deprived childhood with later antisocial
behavior, we now have science to back it up. The authors take the position that
[ xx ] Introduction
adverse childhood experiences are, in fact, brain injuries. They also empha-
size that labels, such as posttraumatic stress disorder, are often insufficient
to describe the downstream effects of maltreatment, as manifestations may
take multiple forms. Translating that for juries may make big differences in
outcomes, when culpability is in question. In Chapter 5, Kenneth Weiss and
Alexander Westphal assess our knowledge of autism spectrum disorder as
it applies to analysis of criminal responsibility. The apparent lack of empa-
thy in autism spectrum disorder is a defect in Theory of Mind, distinguished
from the callousness in psychopathy. The authors emphasize the importance
of making this distinction for judges and juries.
Turning to more technology-based applications, we begin with Octavio
Choi’s discussion of neuroimaging and lie detection in Chapter 6. The question
has now become: To what degree can expert witnesses speak to juries with-
out invading their territory of truth finding? This is a deep and leading-edge
subject that forces us to focus on the borders of science and behavior. The
science behind lie detection has evolved since the era of word-association
and systolic blood pressure tests. Here, Dr. Choi focuses on functional MRI
imaging and takes us to the current limits of knowledge and legal admis-
sibility. Clarence Watson, Mark Pressman, and Kenneth Weiss in Chapter 7
discuss scientific knowledge about parasomnias as they apply to analyses of
criminal responsibility. How we measure consciousness and the capacity for
criminal intent may be affected by disorders of sleep. While the law tells us
that sleeping persons are incapable of committing crimes, the proof of sleep
has become an intriguing challenge. In Chapter 8, Susan Rushing and Daniel
Langleben explain how brain imaging can increase insight into human
behavior in criminal and civil cases. The resulting testimony must conform
to the rules of evidence and must not be substituted for ordinary clinical
analysis. Increasingly, courts have been interested in measurable param-
eters of brain development that inform sentencing and how culpability is
decided. In Chapter 9, Manish Fozdar and Helen Farrell raise forensic issues
in an especially active area of clinical concern: chronic traumatic encepha-
lopathy. This condition may give rise to criminal and civil litigation and has
become a fertile area for expert witnesses. Of special interest is the relation-
ship between sports-induced chronic traumatic encephalopathy and claims
that team owners, football helmet makers, and sports leagues share liability
for possible dementia-related outcomes. Samson Gurmu and Kenneth Weiss
take on the emerging interface of designer drugs and criminal responsibility
in Chapter 10. Although there is little guidance from case law, the authors
outline the potential avenues criminal defendants might take to diminish
criminal responsibility. There is no civil right to present a defense of intoxi-
cation, but when someone uses new substances with unknown effects, can a
claim be made for involuntary intoxication? Here, as in other chapters, the
authors will discuss types of testimony from both sides of the case.
Introduction [ xxi ]
Whereas the rules of evidence are not the domain of science, expert
witnesses have responsibilities to maintain scientific integrity and to help
triers of fact with their difficult jobs. There are many ways to assist legal
processes, and each witness will develop a style that is ethical and informa-
tive. Accordingly, we have embraced an approach that explores each subject
without being prescriptive or formulaic. We urge readers to use the informa-
tion within these chapters as a launching point for fashioning reports and
testimony, not necessarily as a blueprint for constructing the work product.
It is our wish and that of the contributors to assist the justice system by dem-
onstrating scientific progress while maintaining respect for legal traditions.
REFERENCES
1. Strasberger LH, Gutheil TG, Brodsky A. On wearing two hats: role conflict in
serving as both psychotherapist and expert witness. Am J Psychiatry. 1997;
154(4): 448–456.
2. Beck TR. Elements of Medical Jurisprudence. Albany, NY: Webster & Skinners; 1823.
3. Wharton F, Stillé M. A Treatise on Medical Jurisprudence. Philadelphia, PA:
Kay & Brother; 1855.
4. Ray I. A Treatise on the Medical Jurisprudence of Insanity. Boston, MA: Chas.
Little & Jas. Brown; 1838.
5. Mohr JC. Doctors and the Law: Medical Jurisprudence in Nineteenth-Century
America. Baltimore, MD: The Johns Hopkins University Press; 1993.
6. Eigen JP. Witnessing Insanity: Madness and Mad-Doctors in the English Court.
New Haven, CT: Yale University Press; 1995.
7. Morse SJ, Bonnie RJ. Abolition of the insanity defense violates due process.
J Am Acad Psychiatry Law. 2013; 41(4): 488–495.
8. Goldberg DS. The transformative power of X-rays in U.S. scientific & medical
litigation: mechanical objectivity in Smith v. Grant (1896). Perspect Science.
2013; 21(1): 23–57.
9. Golan T. Laws of Men and Laws of Nature: The History of Scientific Expert Testimony
in England and America. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 2004.
10. Weiss KJ. Head, examined: Clarence Darrow’s X-ray vision of criminal respon-
sibility. J Psychiatry Law. 2011; 39: 627–661.
11. Weiss KJ. Isaac Ray at 200: phrenology and expert testimony. J Am Acad
Psychiatry Law. 2007; 35(3): 339–345.
12. Crozier WR, de Jong PJ, eds. The Psychological Significance of the Blush.
New York: Cambridge University Press; 2013.
13. Raine A. The Anatomy of Violence. New York: Random House; 2013.
14. Münsterberg H. On the Witness Stand: Essays on Psychology and Crime.
New York: Doubleday, Page & Co.; 1909.
15. Freud S: Psycho-analysis and the establishment of the facts in legal proceed-
ings (1906). The Standard Edition of the Complete Psychological Works of Sigmund
Freud, vol. 9. Edited and translated by J. Strachey. London: The Hogarth Press;
1959: 103–114 (editor’s notes, pp. 99–102).
[ xxii ] Introduction
16. Weiss KJ. Classics in psychiatry and the law: Francis Wharton on involuntary
confessions. J Am Acad Psychiatry Law 2012; 40: 67–80.
17. Frye v. U.S. 293 F. 1013 (D.C. Cir. 1923).
18. Daubert v. Merrell Dow Pharmaceuticals, Inc. 509 U.S. 579, 113 (1992).
19. Buchanan A, Norko MA. The Psychiatric Report: Principles and Practice of Forensic
Writing. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press; 2011.
20. Saks MJ. The legal and scientific evaluation of forensic science (espe-
cially fingerprint expert testimony). Seton Hall Law Review. 2003;
33(4): 1167–1187.
21. Grubin D. The polygraph and forensic psychiatry. J Am Acad Psychiatry Law.
2010; 38(4): 446–451.
22. Sadoff RL. Ethical Issues in Forensic Psychiatry: Minimizing Harm. Chichester,
UK: John Wiley & Sons, Ltd; 2011.
23. American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law. Ethics guidelines for the prac-
tice of forensic psychiatry, adopted May 2005. Available online at www.aapl.
org/ethics.htm. These guidelines are supplemental to the American Psychiatric
Association’s annotations to the American Medical Association’s general
ethics.
24. American Psychological Association. Specialty guidelines for forensic psychol-
ogy. Am Psychol. 2013; 68(1): 7–19.
25. Mnookin JL. Expert evidence, partisanship, and epistemic competence.
Brooklyn Law Review. 2008; 73(3): 1009–1033.
Introduction [ xxiii ]
Random documents with unrelated
content Scribd suggests to you:
and incoherence of dreams through a partial forgetting of what we
have known during the day.
One might even think of reducing the phenomenon of dreaming to
that of memory, and of regarding the dream as the manifestation of
an activity of reproduction which does not rest even at night, and
which is an end in itself. Views like those expressed by Pilcz[51] would
corroborate this, according to which intimate relations are
demonstrable between the time of dreaming and the contents of the
dream from the fact that the impressions reproduced by the dream in
sound sleep belong to the remotest past while those reproduced
towards morning are of recent origin. But such a conception is
rendered improbable from the outset by the manner of the dream’s
behaviour towards the material to be remembered. Strümpell[66]
justly calls our attention to the fact that repetitions of experiences do
not occur in the dream. To be sure the dream makes an effort in that
direction, but the next link is wanting, or appears in changed form,
or it is replaced by something entirely novel. The dream shows only
fragments of reproduction; this is so often the rule that it admits of
theoretical application. Still there are exceptions in which the dream
repeats an episode as thoroughly as our memory would in its waking
state. Delbœuf tells of one of his university colleagues who in his
dream repeated, with all its details, a dangerous wagon ride in which
he escaped accident as if by miracle. Miss Calkins[12] mentions two
dreams, the contents of which exactly reproduced incidents from the
day before, and I shall later take occasion to report an example which
came to my notice, showing a childish experience which returned
unchanged in a dream.[F]
(c) Dream Stimuli and Dream Sources.—What is meant by dream
stimuli and dream sources may be explained by referring to the
popular saying, “Dreams come from the stomach.” This notion
conceals a theory which conceives the dream as a result of a
disturbance of sleep. We should not have dreamed if some disturbing
element had not arisen in sleep, and the dream is the reaction from
this disturbance.
The discussion of the exciting causes of dreams takes up the most
space in the descriptions of the authors. That this problem could
appear only after the dream had become an object of biological
investigation is self-evident. The ancients who conceived the dream
as a divine inspiration had no need of looking for its exciting source;
to them the dream resulted from the will of the divine or demoniacal
powers, and its content was the product of their knowledge or
intention. Science, however, soon raised the question whether the
stimulus to the dream is always the same, or whether it might be
manifold, and thus led to the question whether the causal
explanation of the dream belongs to psychology or rather to
physiology. Most authors seem to assume that the causes of the
disturbance of sleep, and hence the sources of the dream, might be of
various natures, and that physical as well as mental irritations might
assume the rôle of dream inciters. Opinions differ greatly in
preferring this or that one of the dream sources, in ranking them,
and indeed as to their importance for the origin of dreams.
Wherever the enumeration of dream sources is complete we
ultimately find four forms, which are also utilised for the division of
dreams:—