2012DelVecchioErlangerSmithTheoriesofchildabusechapter
2012DelVecchioErlangerSmithTheoriesofchildabusechapter
2012DelVecchioErlangerSmithTheoriesofchildabusechapter
net/publication/256462078
CITATIONS READS
6 57,727
3 authors:
SEE PROFILE
All content following this page was uploaded by Tamara Del Vecchio on 04 May 2020.
Del Vecchio, T., Erlanger, A. C.E., & Slep, A. M. S. (2012). Theories of child abuse. In
M. A. Fine & F. D. Fincham (Eds.), Handbook of Family Theories: A Content-
based Approach (pp.) New York: Taylor and Francis/Routledge.
2
Lore in the field of child maltreatment suggests that the start of child protection from abuse
began in the 1870s with the case of Mary Ellen. A child who was abused by her caregivers, Mary
Ellen was removed from the home through work with the Society for the Prevention of Cruelty
to Animals. The New York Society for the Prevention of Cruelty to Children was created as a
consequence of the public outrage and negative media coverage (Pfohl, 1977). Almost 100 years
later, prompted by the seminal book The Battered Child (Helfer & Kempe, 1968), the field began
progressing at a rapid rate with the establishment of mandated reporting (1960s; Besharov, 1983)
and the child welfare system (1970s; Child Abuse Prevention and Treatment Act, CAPTA,
Our knowledge of the impact of child maltreatment has expanded tremendously, as has
our understanding of the factors that place parents at risk for perpetration. Although rates of child
maltreatment in the United States are high, at approximately 1 million per year, prevalence rates
interventions for the prevention (e.g., Olds, Henderson, Chamberlin, & Tatelbaum, 1986) and
treatment (e.g., Kolko, 1996; Lutzker, 1984) of child maltreatment perpetration has grown
substantially over the last several years. However, several critical gaps remain.
maltreatment. Without such definitions, scholars, researchers, and clinicians are all placed at a
disadvantage and left unsure as to how to best consider data from various sources and
outcome of the natural progression and maturing of a field. However, the slowing also reflects
shifts in research focus, driven, in part, by a change in priorities among funding agencies to
emphasize intervention and impact on victims. Despite the current state of research, the
3
foundations to move forward, address gaps, and significantly advance theory in the next decade
are strong.
In this chapter, we will present the key findings and results in child abuse and neglect and
review theoretical frameworks for conceptualizing child maltreatment. This review will include
limitations of these theories and possible future theoretical directions for understanding child
maltreatment.
Key Findings
Child maltreatment is a broad term and typically includes both abuse (i.e., acts of
commission) and neglect (i.e., acts of omission). Furthermore, abuse is often categorized as
failures to ensure adequate education and medical care. Throughout this chapter, we will use
child maltreatment as the umbrella term that includes all forms of maltreatment, and will specify
neglect or specific types of abuse as relevant for describing specific theories or research findings.
In less than 50 years, the issue of child maltreatment in the United States has undergone
remarkable shifts. The last 10 years has seen a tremendous maturing of our societal perspective
on, as well as our empirical understanding of the treatment of maltreatment. In this section, we
briefly review several recent key findings that are shaping the future of child maltreatment
research.
Defining child maltreatment in a manner that allows consistent decisions in fieldwork and
research settings has been largely elusive. Although the rate of research on child maltreatment
has slowed, the last 10 years has seen considerable progress on this front. Heyman and Slep
(2006, 2009; Slep & Heyman, 2006) developed and tested operationalized definitions of child
4
maltreatment for the United States Air Force Family Advocacy Program. Criteria were
developed for types of abuse (physical, emotional, sexual) and neglect of children. Each set of
criteria includes acts committed by the offender and the impact (actual or potential) on the
victim. In a series of field trials and a dissemination trial, when implemented by decision-making
boards in the military, percent agreement with master reviewers (experienced clinicians trained
in the criteria serving as the “gold standard”) improved from 50% to over 90%. In addition to the
criteria, clinical assessment interviews and self-report questionnaires were developed to support
the community (e.g., committee members, clinicians, leaders) as unbiased and fair to the alleged
perpetrators and victims (Slep & Heyman, 2006), was associated with a significant reduction in
recidivism among alleged and substantiated perpetrators as compared with recidivism under the
prior definitions (Snarr, Heyman, Slep, Malik, & USAF Family Advocacy Program, in press)
This work is now being extended in an effort to establish systems for reliably classifying the
severity of maltreatment and the application of these definitions in civilian child protective
systems. If consistent definitions of maltreatment were reliably applied in both community and
research based efforts, this would result in more reliable decision making processes for families,
more accurate data, and better generalizability of research findings to community and clinical
systems.
One of the most provocative recent findings has been the apparent declining rate of child
maltreatment, the first decline since it began being rigorously tracked. Even though rates of child
maltreatment are decreasing, large numbers of children are still maltreated. An estimated
5
1,256,600 children were maltreated during 2005-2006 (17.1 per 1,000 children), according to the
Fourth National Incidence Study of Child Abuse and Neglect (NIS–4; Sedlak et al., 2010). In a
representative sample of youth in the United States, as many as 60.6% reported being victimized
or witnessing victimization; 10% of these children had been maltreated (Finkelhor, Turner,
Still, there is converging evidence child maltreatment is on the decline. NIS–4 results
indicated a statistically significant decline in the frequency of child sexual abuse, with a marginal
decrease in child physical abuse and child emotional abuse (Sedlak et al., 2010). During the
1990s, substantiated cases of child sexual abuse by child protective agencies decreased by 39%
(Jones, Finkelhor, & Kopiec, 2001). Between 1993 and 2004, child maltreatment and child
survey of children and adolescents) declined by 40-70%, after a peak in 1993 (Finkelhor &
Jones, 2006). The evidence suggests that the frequency of physical and sexual maltreatment has
declined, with a possible decline in child neglect masked due to changes in reporting (Jones,
There are several possible influences on this decline in child maltreatment. During the
1980s and 1990s, substantial funding and attention were devoted to child sexual abuse
prevention and intervention (both community and criminal justice based). Perhaps these
activities were effective public health measures (Jones & Finkelhor, 2003). Additional economic
changes and increased use of psychiatric medication by would-be perpetrators might also
contribute to the decline in physical and sexual maltreatment. Moreover, the decline could be
reflective of a change in awareness of and decreasing tolerance of physical and sexual abuse of
children.
6
Outcomes
Over the last several years, two important and disparate conclusions have emerged from
research on the impact of maltreatment. First, the effects of maltreatment can be substantially
broader than the emotional, behavioral, and social effects that were initially the focus of
research. The Adverse Childhood Experiences (ACEs) study from 1995 through 1997 by the
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) utilized a sample of 17,000 participants to
examine the physical outcomes of a negative childhood (Felitti, Vincent, Anda, Robert, &
Nordenberg, 1998). ACEs, which include child maltreatment, along with family dysfunction,
have been found to influence physical health. For example, ACEs increase the risk of liver
disease; this relation is strongly mediated by substance abuse and dangerous sexual activities
Physical differences in the brain are associated with child maltreatment victimization.
Parental verbal abuse (a behavior that can be considered child emotional maltreatment) is related
adults, it was found that those exposed to parental verbal abuse had altered neural pathways
related to language and psychopathology (e.g., anxiety, depression); (Choi, Jeong, Rohan,
Polcari, & Teicher, 2009). The corpus callosum is also affected by child maltreatment.
Psychiatric patients who were maltreated (abused or neglected) had a smaller corpus callosum
when compared to healthy controls (by 17%) and other psychiatric patients who were not
maltreatment, played the biggest role in reduced corpus callosum area (15-18% difference;
Teicher et al., 2004). In an interesting series of studies examining the impact of emotional
neglect, operationalized as children who spent time in orphanages and then were adopted into
7
nurturing homes, the children with a history of institutionalization had deficits in inhibition,
visual memory, attention, and visually mediated learning. Yet, these children scored at
processes (Pollak et al, 2010). In other work, severe neglect history in institutionalized children
appeared to increase levels of stress hormones (cortisol) with extended elevations after mother-
child interactions (Fries, Shirtcliff, & Pollak, 2008) even after years in responsive environments.
These results indicate that even within the context of a close relationship, these children can
(DeBellis, Chrousos, et al., 1994; DeBellis, Lefter, Trickett, & Putnam, 1994; Ito, et al., 1993).
Although, the majority of child maltreatment research focuses on negative results and
excludes resilient outcomes, there is also evidence to show that children can experience
maltreatment with no apparent negative effects, and perhaps fair better than might be expected.
In categorizing abused and neglected individuals, McGloin and Widom (2001) defined resiliency
through the following areas: employment, education, socialization, psychiatric health, and
absence of homelessness, substance abuse, and criminality. Using these categories, 22% of
victims of child maltreatment were classified as resilient. Looking at gender difference, females
were labeled as more resilient than males (i.e., females met more resiliency criteria as outlined in
Even in maltreatment traditionally thought of as more severe (child sexual abuse), there
college students that were sexually abused as children shows that they, on average, are slightly
less well adjusted in terms of psychological functioning and social adjustment when compared to
college students without a sexual abuse history. However child sexual abuse explained less than
8
1% of the variance; poor family environment was a much better predictor of adjustment than
sexual abuse itself (Rind, Tromovitch, & Bauserman, 1998). Parental support and level of stress
resulting from abuse predict better outcomes among sexually abused females (Spaccarelli &
Kim, 1995).
So, what role does the onset of maltreatment play in later challenges? Although child
maltreatment has been linked with an array of problematic outcomes, much of this literature
suffers from significant methodological limitations. Often, comparison groups of children differ
from maltreated groups in a number of ways other than merely maltreatment. Types of child
maltreatment covary (Edwards, Holden, Felitti, & Anda, 2003) and child maltreatment covaries
with a wide variety of other problematic parenting behaviors (Brown, Cohen, Johnson, &
Salzinger, 1998), as well as a myriad of other family (e.g., intimate partner violence, Appel &
Holden, 1998; Slep & O’Leary, 2005) and more macro-level factors (e.g., poverty, Coulton,
Korbin, Su, & Chow, 1995) that relate to problematic child outcomes. As research on the impact
of maltreatment moves forward, it will be important to continue to try to tease apart the role of
maltreatment as part of this stew of adverse contexts and experiences for children.
Behavioral Theories
maltreatment, serving as the basis for several more specific theoretical arguments. Behavioral
reinforcement increases the likelihood that aggression will occur in the future by a removal of an
Coercion theory (Patterson 1982; Reid, Patterson, & Snyder, 2002) suggests positive and
negative reinforcement that occur in dyadic conflict result in a learned pattern of aggressive
escalation. A parent and child in a conflict escalate with increasingly noxious behaviors, until
one person capitulates. The person who “wins” is negatively reinforced for escalating through
the removal of the aversive conflict behavior and often positively reinforced through the
attainment of a reward such as earning privileges or gaining compliance. The person who “loses”
is negatively reinforced via the other’s cessation of aversive behavior. Physically abusive
behavior patterns can occur as a result of the escalation process that crests with aggressive
discipline and is reinforced due to its variable effectiveness at “winning”. Moreover, consistent
with coercion theory, abusive mothers are also more inconsistent and likely to provide positive
consequences for their child’s aversive behavior than non-abusive mothers (Oldershaw, Walters,
& Hall, 1986). Patterson (1976) established that the most likely antecedent for aversive behavior
is the aversive behavior displayed by another family member. As such, the “victim” in these
reinforcement to the aggressor. Through this process, abusive parents and their children may
develop dysfunctional patterns for parent-child interactions that encourage conflict and
aggression and the escalation of that aggression to injurious forms of discipline. Moreover,
children learn maladaptive behavior patterns that contribute to externalizing behavior problems
(Snyder, 2002) and may develop a proclivity to aggression resulting in an intergenerational cycle
Social learning theory (e.g., Bandura, 1977; 1986), an extension of behavioral theory,
posits that behavioral change occurs through vicarious, or observed, reinforcement. In their oft-
cited study on imitative learning of aggressive behavior, Bandura, Ross, and Ross (1961)
demonstrated that aggressive behavior could be learned through the observation of aggressive
actors without the need for direct reinforcement. Thus, observational learning of aggressive
behaviors bypasses the need for learning of aggression through directly experienced
what they experienced and saw as children with their own children. Social learning is most often
Social learning theory, as applied to the intergenerational transmission hypothesis, postulates that
abusive behavior is learned through the witnessing of abusive parenting and subsequent
modeling of an abusive parent. Parents who used severe physically aggressive techniques were
more likely to report witnessing partner aggression in childhood (Straus, 1994; Straus & Smith,
1990). Moreover, numerous studies have found relations between parent abuse history and the
occurrence and severity of child abuse perpetration (Ethier, Couture, & Lacharite, 2004; Gelles
& Straus, 1987; Gil, 1971; Heyman & Slep, 2002; Kaufman & Zigler, 1993; Stith, et al, 2009).
Heyman and Ezzell (2005) estimate that intergenerational transmission of child physical abuse
occurs in one-third of the cases. Although fewer than 1% of parents without a child physical
abuse history maltreat their own children within the first few years of life, only about 7% of
parents with abuse history maltreat (Browne 1995; Dixon, Brown, & Hamilton-Giachritsis,
11
2009). Thus, even though abuse history remains a significant risk factor, it does not follow that
abused children will necessarily be abusive parents (Heyman & Ezzell, 2005)
Child sexual abuse perpetrators are also more likely to be abused as children, physically
(d =.49) and sexually (d =.70; Whitaker et al, 2008). On average, 28% of sex offenders have a
child sexual abuse history, with higher rates among male victims of male offenders (Starzyk &
Marshall, 2003). Koyabashi, Sales, Becker, Figueredo, and Kaplan (1995) suggest that male
offending has a greater impact on victims and may result in sexual confusion for male victims.
Although not directly evaluated, modeling of inappropriate sexual behavior may be the
mechanism through which child sexual abuse is transmitted to new perpetrators. As with
physical abuse, however, although a history of victimization is more often found among sexual
abuse perpetrators than people who do not perpetrate sexual abuse, the vast majority of sexual
Cognitive models
cognitive processes, such as schemas, executive functioning deficits, and attributions and
appraisals as predictors of abusive parenting (Azar, Reitz, & Goslin, 2008). Parenting schemas,
or mental scripts, develop from past experiences, influenced by family experiences and cultural
norms. Schemas represent people’s views of themselves as parents, the parenting role in general,
and expectations for children; and are proposed to mediate the relation between child behavior
and abusive parental responses. New information, such as a specific instance of child behavior, is
then filtered through these schemas. This results in selective attention to schema-consistent cues
Adaptive schemas are flexible and can be modified through trial-and-error learning
(Azar, Nix, & Makin-Byrd, 2005). Contingent, responsive parenting requires the ability to
modify responding based on the needs of the particular situation (Azar &Weinzierl, 2005). In
contrast, abusive parents rely on schemas that are rigid and negative affect laden (Azar, Nix, &
Makin-Byrd, 2005; Milner, 2000; Nayak & Milner, 1998). These rigid cognitive schemas may
reflect deficits in executive functioning; that is, cognitive flexibility. Consistent with this model,
several studies have indicated that abusive parents are more likely to underestimate task demands
(Azar & Rohrbeck, 1986; Spinetta, 1978; Twentyman & Plotkin, 1982). When the child naturally
disconfirms expectations, parents are angered and respond in an overly harsh manner.
Within the social information processing framework, parents’ negative perceptions and
evaluations of child behavior and positive outcome expectancies for abusive behavior are
thought to contribute to abusive responding (Milner 1993; Milner, 2000). Abusive parents are
more likely to endorse negative attributions for child behavior, appraise child behavior more
negatively, and have unrealistic expectations for child behavior. For example, Bauer and
Twentyman (1985) asked mothers to respond to hypothetical vignettes and found that abusing
mothers were the most likely to view their child as acting intentionally to annoy them. Moreover,
Larrance and Twentyman (1983) report that physically abusive parents make more internal and
stable attributions for negative child behavior and external, unstable attributions for positive
child behavior. Chilamkurti and Milner (1993) found that mothers at high-risk for child abuse
evaluated minor child transgressions as more wrong and endorsed more power assertive
disciplinary techniques than low-risk mothers. In one of the few studies to incorporate
observational measures of child behavior, Reid et al (2002) found that even though abusive
13
parents reported significantly more aggression and hyperactivity than non-abusive parents,
independent observers did not identify differences in the mean rate of aversive behavior or
development and are more likely to misinterpret children’s behaviors as sexual advances (Ward
Parents at-risk for abuse may evidence positive outcome expectancies for abusive
behavior; high-risk mothers perceive the use of power assertive discipline by others as more
appropriate than their counterparts (Chilamkurti & Milner, 1993). Results regarding attitudes
about physical discipline are mixed. Slep and O’Leary (2007) found it to uniquely contribute to
aggressive parenting in the context of a multivariate prediction model. In contrast, Stith and
colleagues (2009) found non-significant effect sizes for the association between approval of
corporal punishment and child physical abuse. Perhaps the inconsistent findings are due to
interactive effects. Crouch and Behl (2001) found that the association between stress and child
physical abuse risk was moderated by the parents’ level of belief in the value of corporal
punishment; the relation was stronger for those who value corporal punishment. In addition,
sexual abuse perpetrators are more likely to rationalize their behaviors and report that children
benefit from the sexual behavior or actively sought out sexual contact (Stermac & Segal, 1989).
The roles of stress and anger in etiological models of child maltreatment have garnered
considerable attention in the literature (Dopke & Milner, 2000; Gaudin, Polansky, Kilpatrick, &
Shilton, 1993; Whipple & Webster-Stratton, 1991; Williamson, Bourduin, & Howe, 1991).
Abusive and high-risk parents report more stressful life events and more parenting stress than
non-abusive parents (Coohey & Braun, 1997; Crouch & Behl, 2001; Dopke & Milner, 2000;
14
Mash, Johnston, & Kovitz, 1983; Rodriguez & Green, 1997; Rosenberg & Reppucci, 1983; Stith
et al, 2009). The number of stressful life events is also associated with child sexual abuse, as
mothers of sexually abused children report a greater number of stressful life events compared to
mothers of non-abused children (Pianta, Egeland, & Erikson, 1989). Clearly, not all parents who
encounter stress abuse their children. In a prospective study of low-income mothers, Egeland,
Breitenbucher, and Rosenberg (1980) examined differences between stressed mothers who
abused their children and stressed mothers who did not abuse their children. Although the
mothers did not differ in type of life stress event experienced, abusive mothers reported more
aggression and defensiveness, and less seeking of support in reaction to the stress. Moreover,
abusive mothers, compared to non-abusive mothers, are more likely to report loneliness (Milner
& Robertson, 1990; Spinetta, 1978). Although the availability of supportive resources does not
distinguish abusive from non-abusive parents (Chaffin, Kelleher, & Hollenberg, 1996), abusive
parents report receiving less social support (Corse, Schmid &Trickett, 1990; Pianta, Egeland, &
Erikson, 1989).
Thus, current theory suggests that stress is not a sufficient condition for child
maltreatment perpetration. Pianta (1984) argues that abusive parents have poor coping skills and
are more likely to cope in negative ways, such as drinking and hostility, resulting in an increased
risk for child-directed violence. When stress cannot be coped with effectively, parents are more
likely to develop negative perceptions of child behavior and engage in abusive or neglectful
behaviors (Belsky, 1993). Hillson and Kupier (1994) suggest that specific coping strategies are
associated with specific abuse types. Neglectful parents are more likely to engage in behavioral
mental distractors to avoid the stressor. In contrast, physically abusive parents are more likely to
15
engage in rumination and venting of negative emotions. The coping pattern may change over
time as automatic coping responses are developed. Moreover, physically abusive and neglecting
parents show deficits in problem-solving skills related to child-rearing and child-care (Azar,
Robinson, Hekimian, & Twentyman, 1984; Dawson, de Armas, McGrath, & Kelly, 1986).
Hansen, Pallotta, Tishelman, Conaway, and MacMillan (1986) compared physically abusive and
neglectful parents to clinic and community parents and found that although abusive parents were
associated with child behavior problems. In addition, even though clinic parents and abusive
parents reported similar levels of child behavior problems, clinic parents generated more
solutions and more effective solutions than did the abusive parents. Thus, abusive parenting may
reflect deficits in coping and the ability to determine and select effective solutions to child
rearing problems.
Anger is associated with child abuse risk and abusive status (Ammerman, 1990; Dopke &
Milner, 2000; Rodriguez & Green, 1997). Aversive child rearing events occur with great
frequency during early childhood (e.g., temper tantrums, defiance) and elicit anger, annoyance,
and frustration. Angry emotional responses to children may result in an overwhelming flood of
techniques (Gottman, Katz, & Hooven, 1996). Moreover, abusive mothers evidence increased
sensitivity to child negative affect. Milner, Halsey, and Fultz (1995) found that high-risk,
compared to low-risk, mothers reported more negative affectivity (e.g., distress, hostility) to
infant cries. In addition, when presented with videotaped infant cries, abusive mothers evidence
increased physiological reactivity (e.g. heart rate, skin conductance, and diastolic blood pressure)
(Frodi & Lamb, 1980). Abusive parents also report feeling more angry or annoyed in response to
16
children’s behavior in general, and specifically with regard to social or moral transgressions,
than non-abusive parents (Bauer & Twentyman 1985; Dopke & Milner, 2000; Spinetta, 1978;
Trickett & Kuczynski, 1986). A recent meta-analytic review by Stith et al. (2009) examining 155
studies found that parent anger consistently predicted both child physical abuse and neglect.
Psychopathology
health as an etiological factor. Although several disorders have been implicated as predictors of
child physical abuse and neglect perpetration, two disorders have consistently evidenced
associations with perpetration: depression and substance abuse. A large body of research
documents the relation between depression and child physical abuse and neglect (Cummings &
Cicchetti, 1990; Downey & Coyne, 1990; Gil, 1971; Kinard, 1982; Scott, 1992; Stith et al,
2009). Using wave II data from National Institute for Mental Health's Epidemiologic Catchment
Area survey, Chaffin, Kelleher, and Hollenberg (1996) found that major depression was a
significant predictor of the onset of physical abuse, controlling for substance use and
demographic variables. Although depression was also found to significantly predict neglect
univariately, the strength of the relation decreased to nonsignificant levels when substance use
Data from the Epidemiologic Catchment Area survey also indicated that substance use
predicted the onset of child physical abuse and neglect (Chaffin, Kelleher, & Hollenberg, 1996).
In addition, Murphy, Jellinek, Quinn, Smith, Poitrast, and Goshko (1991) examined the
prevalence of substance abuse in a sample of cases of serious child abuse or neglect brought
before a juvenile court. The presence of parental substance abuse in at least one of the parents
was indicated in 43% of the cases. Among addicted parents, Black and Mayer (1980) found that
17
41% of their children met criteria for serious neglect, abuse, or both, although all experienced
some degree of neglect. In a community sample of over 11,000 individuals, abusive parents were
more likely than their matched controls to report substance abuse; the association was
independent of levels of depression and social support (Kelleher, Chaffin, Hollenberg, & Fischer,
1994). However, depression and substance use are neither necessary nor specific to child abuse
perpetrators.
child sexual abuse, Whitaker and colleagues (2008) identified several psychopathological
correlates of maltreatment. Consistent with the findings for child physical abuse and neglect,
substance abuse was a significant predictor of perpetration. However, the associations with
depression and maltreatment were less consistent; studies evidenced positive and negative
effects. In addition, Whitaker et al. identified a relation between anxiety symptoms and antisocial
Family Environment
low level of education, influence the capacity to parent and have been implicated in the etiology
of child physical abuse and neglect. Parenting age is associated with a number of risk factors
including low level of education, higher levels of stress, less social support, and coercive
discipline practices (Brooks-Gunn & Chase-Lansdale, 1995; Ketterlinus, Lamb, & Nitz, 1991;
Stevens-Simon, Nelligan, & Kelly, 2001). Young mothers are at a slightly increased risk for
physical abuse behaviors and neglect (Dixon, Brown, & Hamilton-Giachritsis, 2009; Stith, et al,
2009; Wolfner & Gelles, 1993). Although several studies have found that age was negatively
related to the severity of physical abuse (Connelly & Straus, 1992; Gil, 1971), using
18
Epidemiological Catchment Area data Chaffin, Kelleher, and Hollenberg (1996) failed to find a
significant association between parents' age and whether or not severe physical abuse was
perpetrated. The relation between parent age and child neglect is unclear with several studies
indicating no effect (Schumacher, Slep & Heyman, 2001; Stith et al., 2009). Moreover, Allen
and Pothast (1994) found no significant age difference between sexual abusers and non-abusers.
Income level has been consistently found to be negatively associated with child physical
abuse (Chaffin, Kelleher, & Hollenberg, 1996; Garbarino, 1976; Straus, Gelles, & Steinmetz,
1980; Gil, 1971; Sedlak, 1997; Wolfner & Gelles, 1993) and neglect (Brown, Cohen, Johanson,
& Salzinger, 1998; Pelton, 1994). In addition to occurrence, severity of abuse may be associated
with poverty; severe injuries have been found to be more likely among low-income families
(Gelles, 1992; Gil, 1971; Kruttschnitt, McLeod, & Dornfeld, 1994; Sedlak & Broadhurst, 1996).
However, both the Epidemiological Catchment Area and the National Family Violence Survey
failed to find an association between severity of perpetration and income level (Chaffin,
Kelleher, & Hollenberg, 1996; Ross, 1996). Perhaps the inconsistent findings may be due to type
of child maltreatment evaluated: neglect, physical abuse, or sexual abuse. For example, using
1990 Census data and Child Protective Services data, Drake and Pandey (1996) found that
neighborhood poverty was most strongly associated with neglect as compared with other forms
of abuse. In addition, in a meta-analytic review of 155 studies, Stith et al (2009) found that
unemployment was moderately related to child neglect, but only minimally related to child
physical abuse.
Demographic factors, such as age and socioeconomic status, are unlikely to be direct
determinants of abuse status. Instead, the association between poverty and abuse status is likely
to be indirect, mediated by stress associated with poverty (Pianta, 1984). Furthermore, poverty is
19
correlated with educational level, single parent status, and psychopathology, and each is
associated with child maltreatment status (Allen & Pothast, 1994; Gil, 1971; Milner &
Chilamkurti, 1990). Poverty is associated with a low level of education, and although educational
level predicts child maltreatment risk when controlling for income level, the converse relation is
not supported (Rodriguez, 2008). Lower income families also tend to have more children and
having multiple children under 18 increases the risk for child physical abuse and neglect
perpetration (Gil, 1971). Single parent status is also associated with both the incidence and
chronicity of child maltreatment (Ethier, Couture, & Lacharite, 2004; Sedlak & Broadhurst,
1996), although severity of perpetration and single parent status are not significantly related
(Chaffin, Kelleher, & Hollenberg, 1996; Connelly & Straus, 1992). Mothers who work full-time
are less likely to report physical abuse perpetration (Gelles & Hargreaves, 1990). Black,
Heyman, and Slep (2001) suggest that perhaps time spent with children is a better predictor of
perpetration than employment status in itself. Although variables indexing social disadvantage,
such as poverty and a low level of education, are associated with abuse perpetration, child
maltreatment is not exclusively a lower SES condition. Lower income parents are more likely to
come in contact with Child Protective Services and, as a result, the association between poverty
and child maltreatment perpetration may also be an artifact of sampling biases (Gil, 1971). In
contrast to physical abuse and neglect, income and educational level are not related to child
Ecological model
multidimensional integration of diverse factors operating at more proximal and more distal levels
of influence. Parent characteristic models are greatly limited by the failure to identify variables
20
that account for a large portion of the variance. Based on Bronfenbrenner’s perspective (1979),
the ecological model of child maltreatment suggests that the environment influences parents at
multiple levels of the ecological system (Belsky, 1980). The systems represent levels of
influence from the most proximal (i.e., ontogenic – psychopathology, family of origin
influences) to the most distal (i.e., cultural norms). Relations are not necessarily unidirectional
(e.g.,Cicchetti & Lynch, 1993; Wolfe, 1987). For example, abusive parenting contributes to child
behavior problems and likely is a source of parental stress, and these negative sequelae affect
other systems such as work and school performance (Swick & Williams, 2006).
the society, community and family ecology supporting or undermining the quality of parent-child
interactions. The empirical literature has identified risk and protective factors that exist at all
levels of the ecology (see Black, Heyman, & Slep, 2001; Stith et al., 2009, for reviews).
Ecological theories suggest that more proximal factors, such as parental psychopathology, have
the most direct effect on parent-child interaction and abuse, and that the effects of more distal
factors are often mediated through their impact on more proximal factors. In other words, the
presence of risk factors (e.g., neighborhood stressors) directly results in negative outcomes or
increases the likelihood of exposure to additional risk factors. Negative interactions that can
occur between person and environment may result in multiple challenges (e.g., Felitti, Vincent,
Anda, Robert, & Nordenberg, 1998) and poor outcomes (e.g., Choi, Jeong, Rohan, Polcari, &
Teicher, 2009). In contrast, protective factors buffer against the effects of the risk factors, and
can compensate for other, even proximal, risk factors, resulting in more successful outcomes
(Cicchetti, 2004). Thus, it is not only the number of risk factors at each level of the ecology that
determines the likelihood of child maltreatment, but also the balance among the factors; if risk
21
factors outweigh protective factors, the risk for child maltreatment is increased. However, there
is little data to suggest which risk factors are most important or how risk factors interact to
Limitations
In this section, we briefly highlight some of the main limitations to how theory has been
Perhaps one of the biggest limitations in the field of child maltreatment is the lack of
unreliable data collected by community-based systems poorly inform policy makers and can
thresholds for maltreatment can result in unfair decision-making at the family level. For
example, two hypothetical child neglect cases with virtually the same set of circumstance might
(i.e., officially dismissed as below threshold or lacking evidence or “unfounded”). This sends
profoundly inconsistent messages to both the perpetrator and the victim about society’s view of
their behaviors and experiences. Further, assessment approaches that are not designed to collect
researchers to compare outcomes and progress the field in a targeted way. Application of
uniform definitions will greatly facilitate the integration of research across different samples and
with different methodologies, thereby speeding the rate at which theories can be refined.
Moreover, varying assessment methods could be considered strengths if better incorporated into
22
the study outcomes. For example, Stockhammer’s (2001) examination of Child Protective
Services records and parent report suggests that agency records provide more reliable
information about proximal factors, like specific incidents of maltreatment, and parent reports
provide more accurate information about distal factors, such as family context.
There are fundamental gaps in our understanding of the relations among different types
of abuse. Most child welfare contexts do not carefully assess for all forms of maltreatment, with
most systems preferring to pursue the most salient type in an incident or family. Researchers
typically categorize families either through often-limited child protective service records, or
based on the assessment of a single type of maltreatment intended to be the focus of the research.
In addition, measures of specific forms of abuse and neglect are lacking. Therefore, we have
little information about the distinctiveness of abuse types (i.e., how overlapping versus distinct
physical and emotional abuse are or how often neglect and abuse co-occur). In addition,
aggregating maltreatment types muddles our models of abuse perpetration. Differences in child
maltreatment are found at a fundamental level; namely, most physical and emotional abuse, as
well as neglect, occurs at the hands of a parent or caregiver, while much sexual abuse occurs at
the hands of other adults in the child’s circle (e.g., Finkelhor, 1984). Conflating abuse types also
makes it difficult to assess differential predictors. For example, theories of sexual abuse
specific variables. Low prevalence rates of child sexual abuse and the tendency for studies on
risk factors for child abuse to combine all forms of maltreatment make it difficult to identify
specific predictors of sexual abuse. These gaps in our knowledge contribute to limitations in our
Theories of child maltreatment will be able to better specify testable mechanisms when
they identify whether theories of maltreatment are specific to maltreatment type or generalize
across types, that is, physical abuse only, physical abuse and neglect, or all types of maltreatment
including sexual abuse. Theories specific to abuse type, or that allow for generalization across
types, would result in stronger etiological theories that can be refined through scientific
significant risk or protection would inform theoretical advancements and interventions for
Theory Testing
Several limitations exist in child maltreatment theory as it currently exists. First, despite
the fact that transactional, ecological conceptualizations of child maltreatment have been in the
literature for over 20 years, the field has not moved much beyond fairly broad, non-specific
difficult; the necessary studies would be large, and likely be the product of interdisciplinary
research teams using a wide variety of methods to adequately capture different types of
variables. Although studies would be expensive and time consuming, they are necessary if we
are going to move beyond theoretical heuristic frameworks to testable ecological models that
identify specific predictors and interrelations among predictors. In contrast, much of the recent
risk factor research was not developed to test etiological theories directly. Thus, although such
efforts contribute to the knowledge base, results can only offer support for model components
important gap. Risk of abuse varies by age for overall harm, physical abuse, sexual abuse,
emotional abuse, emotional neglect, and educational neglect (Sedlak et al., 2010). It is likely that
at least some of the processes that contribute to abuse play out differently with infants as
compared with adolescents. Specifying mechanisms that change over the course of children’s
development, as has been done with respect to the role of parenting in the etiology of conduct
disorder, for example (e.g., Forgatch & Patterson, 2010), would be an important advance.
power if the role of development in determining the impact of exposure to family of origin
Third, existing theories are more developed with respect to risk mechanisms than
protective effects. Although broad conceptualizations of child maltreatment all acknowledge the
importance of protective factors, theories are silent with respect to where in the causal chain
specific protective factors are hypothesized to operate. Mistakenly, protective factors are often
conceptualized as a lack of risk factors instead of separate variables that influence child
maltreatment in the presence of vulnerabilities. Although specific protective factors are often
unidentified, resiliency studies and models (e.g., McGloin & Widom, 2001; Spaccarelli & Kim,
1995) suggest that internal and external protective factors can result in a synergistic effect,
bolstering a child’s functioning in the face of maltreatment. It is critical that theories of child
maltreatment explain not only the occurrence of maltreatment, but also non-occurrence in the
context of risk.
Fourth, there are few longitudinal studies of maltreating parenting, with only a few
notable exceptions (e.g., Chaffin, Kelleher, & Hollenberg, 1996; Erickson, Egeland, & Pianta,
25
1989), studying families early enough that they can truly test theories of onset of maltreatment as
compared with the maintenance of maltreatment. No longitudinal studies speak to the onset of
sexual abuse perpetration. This limits the field’s ability to disentangle mechanisms that are truly
etiological from those that might contribute to the recurrence or severity of maltreatment. Thus,
most theories do not distinguish these two causal processes, yet important differences might exist
between them.
Finally, theories of child maltreatment, especially those that are informed by work with
families involved with child protective services, need to account for the disproportionate
detection of child maltreatment by systems (e.g., child welfare) in addition to the rates of child
maltreatment itself. For example, poverty is a risk factor for all forms of child maltreatment.
However, it is likely also a risk factor for being detected for child maltreatment and becoming
involved in the child welfare system. People with few financial resources often live in close
quarters, and neighbors are more likely to overhear episodes of abuse and notice neglect than
people who live in affluent neighborhoods. Low income families are also involved with many
more service providers, most of whom are mandated reporters, and see them more frequently
than high income families. These sorts of factors could contribute to easier detection of
maltreatment in low income as compared with high-income families resulting in higher apparent
rates of child maltreatment among lower income families as compared with families who are at
lower risk for detection. This conflation of maltreatment risk and detection risk could result in
inappropriate to generalize the risk factors derived from and theories developed on families
involved in the child welfare system to rest of the population. In fact, more representative
26
samples find smaller effect sizes for the link between income and abuse (e.g., Slep & O’Leary,
2007). Disentangling these issues will enable greater precision and specificity in our theories.
Theoretical Advances
The child maltreatment literature has a substantial research base; however, progress in the
field has slowed considerably. As reviewed earlier, this change in progress can be attributed to
several factors. To move forward, we need to begin to do more integrative research that cuts
across focal theories and develops well-specified, but testable, integrative and comprehensive
theories.
Theories in other areas in negative emotion and aggression have informed theories of
child maltreatment to some degree, but there continues to be room for improvement. Theories of
anger, emotion regulation, coping, general aggression, as well as theories of other forms of
family violence all can be brought to bear on child maltreatment theory. The literature clearly
demonstrates that emotional and physical abuse of children is associated with emotional and
physical abuse of partners (O’Leary, Slep, & O’Leary, 2000). Theories that can account for
unique and shared predictors of the co-occurrence of aggression are overdue and will inform our
understanding of both child and partner abuse. In the first study to investigate whether different
risk profiles characterize different types of aggressors, Slep and O’Leary (2009) sampled males
and females, including those who were physically aggressive only with their children or their
partners, or aggressive toward both children and partners. Individuals who were aggressive
toward only their partners or only their children had distinct risk profiles, with highest levels of
risk on the role-specific variable sets. Role-specific variables were risk factors tied to the specific
relationship. They included factors such as jealousy and partner-blaming attributions in the
couple dyad and parenting style and child-blaming attributions in the parent-child dyad. With the
27
exception of men who were physically aggressive only with their children, single role aggressive
individuals’ risk levels were significantly lower on role-independent (e.g., stress, depressive
symptoms, alcohol problems) and unrelated role-specific risk factors than they were on role-
related risk factors. Being the first work of this type, it is premature to say that profiles based on
whether people perpetrate both child and partner abuse or only one form of abuse might translate
not only into different risk profiles, but might also suggest different etiological theories and
and partners would provide additional guidance for child care workers who address whether
there is risk for a child in a maritally violent household. Theories accounting for specific and
common risk factors for child maltreatment are in their infancy, but hold promise.
represent the complex nature of child maltreatment. Although ecological theory has been popular
for years, researchers have typically tested components of the theory separately rather than the
theory as a whole. This is a bit counter-intuitive, because the crux of ecological theory is the
entire environment, its systems, and the interactions among those systems. However, the study of
singular risk factors dominates the literature. Moreover, distal predictors of child maltreatment
have been evaluated with a broad stroke and as a result there is little understanding of the
mechanisms through which distant variables influence child maltreatment. Slep and O’Leary
(2007), using a representative sample, did test an ecological model of parental aggression,
finding support for an ecological approach. Risk factors from all ecological levels included in the
study were retained in the model, and more distal factors were mediated through more proximal,
parent-level variables. This study suggests that the field can begin to integrate disparate, focal
28
theories of abuse and work toward an empirically supported, integrated, ecological model of
child maltreatment.
Recent efforts have been made to address the lack of operational definitions. Most
settings. By incorporating the best of existing definitions and adding operationalizations when
necessary, Heyman and Slep (2006, 2009; Slep & Heyman, 2006) were able to develop
definitions that could be reliably applied in both field and research settings. These definitions
include both the act committed/omitted and the impact (actual or potential) on the victim. For
example, child physical abuse includes two criteria, as well as a few exclusions.
Criterion A is, “The non-accidental use of physical force on the part of a child’s caregiver.
Physical force includes, but is not limited to, spanking with hand; dropping; pushing;
throwing; biting; kicking; hitting with fist; hitting with a stick, strap, or other object;
scalding; burning; poisoning; stabbing; applying force to throat; cutting off air supply;
Criterion B is, “Significant impact on the child involving any of the following: (1) More than
inconsequential physical injury; (2) Reasonable potential for more than inconsequential
physical injury given the inherent dangerousness of the act, the degree of force used and the
physical environment in which the acts occurred; (3) More than inconsequential fear
reaction.” There are three exclusions, (1) “acts committed to protect self from imminent
physical harm,” which includes three specific criteria [not excerpted here], (2) (a)cts
committed to protect child, others, or pet from imminent harm, as evidenced by…” two
29
specific criteria [not excerpted here], and (3) (a)cts committed during developmentally
A full appendix of these definitions has been published (see Slep & Heyman, 2006).
(i.e., substantiation decisions) in the field as compared with those of master reviewers. After
between the master reviews and committee members increased from about 50% to over 90%
(Heyman & Slep, 2006). Agreement remained at over 90% after widespread dissemination
throughout the United States Air Force (Heyman & Slep, 2009). Social workers involved in the
maltreatment cases reported finding the new system fair. In an unexpected finding, re-offense
was cut in half. At one year, offenders whose incident met substantiation criteria went from 14%
to 7%. We believe that this effect occurred through a few key mechanisms. First, alleged
perpetrators are imbedded within a system that is aware of and can react to maltreatment
findings. When these findings were made in a clearer, more consistent, and fairer manner, the
entire process and the substantiation decisions likely take on more weight in the eyes of
leadership. Because the Air Force member’s supervisors are part of the proceedings, it is easy to
imagine that a clearer and fairer system could result in clearer messages about inappropriate
behavior being sent to involved families. These findings indicate that reliable criteria, in
combination with a system viewed as just could result in maltreatment prevention (Snarr,
Heyman, Slep, Malik, & USAF Family Advocacy Program, in press) and better inform research.
Conclusion
To move into the next phase of child maltreatment theory, there needs to be a two-fold
approach: use of the uniform definitions of child maltreatment and, as a result, the logical
30
integration of tested theories. A significant barrier to the progression of the field is the lack of
agreed upon definitions of child maltreatment upon which to base the research frameworks.
Without clear and specific definitions, meaningful advances will be challenging and potentially
unreliable. Operational definitions are needed to create a consistent system in practical and
research settings. The extensive work done with military child welfare systems as discussed
earlier could offer a uniform set of criteria (see Heyman & Slep, 2009; Heyman & Slep, 2006;
Slep & Heyman, 2006; Slep & Heyman, 2004). However, the investigation of the operational
definition of child maltreatment has been limited to military populations. The utility of the
Greater integration across theoretical models of child maltreatment would improve our
understanding of the etiology and maintenance of child maltreatment. Etiological models of child
maltreatment have emphasized individual risk factors, and therefore, they lack the interplay of
that person with the environment. This interplay is the central tenet of ecological theory and in
general, the move into ecological models has been beneficial. Although ecological models of
child maltreatment have been available for several decades, little progress has been made with
regard to identification of risk factors specific to child maltreatment types, protective factors, and
the interactions among them (e.g., Slep & O’Leary, 2007). Given the flexibility of the ecological
model, it lends itself to integration with other theories of violence and maltreatment. All of the
more specific theories reviewed in this chapter can be considered within the ecological
framework. For example, coercion theory clearly operates at the family level. However,
individual risk factors found in other specific theories (e.g., cycle of violence, anger and stress)
operate at the ontogenetic level and might influence a parent’s risk for engaging in coercive
conflict. By carefully considering how specific theories might intersect, we can pose testable
31
integration of the vast amount of literature on child maltreatment is essential to move the field
References
Allen, C. M., & Pothast, H. L. (1994). Distinguishing characteristics of male and female child
Appel, A. E., & Holden, G. W. (1998). The co-occurrence of spouse and physical child abuse: A
Azar, S. T., Nix, R. L., & Makin-Byrd, K. N. (2005) Parenting schemas and the process of
Azar, S. T., Reitz, E. B., & Goslin, M. C. (2008). Mothering: Thinking is part of the job
29, 295–304.
Azar, S. T., Robinson, D. R., Hekimian, E., & Twentyman, C. T. (1984). Unrealistic
Azar, S. T., & Rohrbeck, C. A. (1986). Child abuse and unrealistic expectations: Further
Azar, S. T., & Weinzierl, K. M. (2005). Child maltreatment and childhood injury research: A
Bandura, A., Ross, D., & Ross, S. A. (1961). Transmission of aggression through imitation of
Bauer, W. D., & Twentyman, C. T. (1985). Abusing, neglectful, and comparison mothers'
320–335.
Besharov, D. J. (1983). Child protection: Past progress, present problems and future directions.
Black, D. A., Heyman, R. E., & Slep, A. M. S. (2001). Risk factors for child physical abuse.
Black. R., & Mayer, J. (1980). Parents with special problems: Alcohol and opiate addiction.
(Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Vol.3. Status and social conditions of parenting (pp. 113–
Brown, J., Cohen, P., Johanson, J. G., & Salzinger, S. (1998). A longitudinal analysis of risk
recorded and self-reported child abuse and neglect. Child Abuse & Neglect, 22, 1065-
1078.
Chaffin, M., Kelleher, K., & Hollenberg, J. (1996). Onset of physical abuse and neglect:
Psychiatric, substance abuse, and social risk factors from prospective community data.
Chilamkurti, C., & Milner, J. S. (1993). Perceptions and evaluations of child transgressions and
disciplinary techniques in high- and low-risk mothers and their children. Child
Child Abuse Prevention and Treatment Act (CAPTA) of 1974, Pub. L. No. 93-247 (1974).
Choi, J., Jeong, B., Rohan, M., Polcari, A., & Teicher, M. (2009). Preliminary evidence for white
matter tract abnormalities in young adults exposed to parental verbal abuse. Biological
56, 96–118.
Connelly, D. C., & Straus, M. A. (1992). Mother's age and risk for physical abuse. Child Abuse
Coohey, C., & Braun, N. (1997). Toward an integrated framework for understanding child
Corse, S. J., Schmid, K., & Trickett, P. K. (1990). Social network characteristics of mothers in
abusing and nonabusing families and their relationships to parenting beliefs. Journal of
Coulton, C. J., Korbin, J. E., Su, M., & Chow, J. (1995). Community level factors and child
Crouch, J. L., & Behl, L. E. (2001). Relationships among parental beliefs in corporal
punishment, reported stress, and physical child abuse potential. Child Abuse & Neglect,
25, 413–419.
Cummings, E. M., & Cicchetti, D. (1990). Attachment, depression, and the transmission of
during the preschool years (pp. 339-372). Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
Dawson, B., de Armas, A., McGrath, M. L., & Kelly, J. A. (1986). Cognitive problem-solving
training to improve the child-care judgment of child neglectful parents. Journal of Family
Violence, 1, 209-226.
DeBellis, M. D., Chrousos, G. P., Dorn, L. D., Burke, L. Helmers, K., Kling, M. A.,…Putnam, F.
DeBellis, M. D., Lefter, L., Trickett, P. K., & Putnam, F. W. (1994). Urinary catecholamine
Dixon, L., Brown, K., & Hamilton-Giachritsis, C. (2009) Patterns of risk and protective factors.
Dong, M., Dube, S. R, Felitti, V. J., Giles, W. H., & Anda, R. F. (2003). Adverse childhood
experiences and self-reported liver disease: New insights into the causal pathway.
Dopke, C. A., & Milner, J. S. (2000). Impact of child noncompliance on stress appraisals,
attributions, and disciplinary choies in mothers at high and low risk for child physical
Downey, G., & Coyne, J. (1990). Children of depressed parents. An integrative review.
Drake, B., & Pandey, S. (1995). Understanding the relationship between neighborhood poverty
and specific types of child maltreatment. Child Abuse & Neglect, 20, 1003-1018.
Edwards, V. J., Holden, G. W., Felitti, V. J., & Anda, R. F. (2003). Relationship between
respondents: Results from the adverse childhood experiences study. American Journal of
Egeland, B., Breitenbucher, M. C., & Rosenberg, M. S. (1980). Prospective study of the
significance of life stress in the etiology in child abuse. Journal of Consulting and
Erickson, M. F., Egeland, B., & Pianta, R. (1989). The effects of maltreatment on the
maltreatment: Theory and research on the causes and consequences of child abuse and
Ethier, L. S., Couture, G., & Lacharite, C. (2004). Risk factors associated with the chronicity of
high potential for child abuse and neglect. Journal of Family Violence, 19, 13–24.
Felitti, M., Vincent, J., Anda, M., Robert, F., & Nordenberg, M. (1998). Relationship of
childhood abuse and household dysfunction to many of the leading causes of death in
adults: The Adverse childhood experiences (ACE) study. American Journal of Preventive
Finkelhor, D. (1984). Child Sexual Abuse: New Theory and Research. New York, NY: The Free
Press.
Finkelhor, D., & Baron, L. (1986). Risk factors for child sexual abuse. Journal of Interpersonal
Violence, 1, 43–71.
Finkelhor, D. & Jones, L. M. (2006). Why have child maltreatment and child victimization
Finkelhor, D., Turner, H., Ormond, R., & Hamby, S. L. (2009) Violence, abuse, and crime
Forgatch, M. S., & Patterson, G. R. (2010). Parent Management Training – Oregon Model: An
Kazdin (Eds.), Evidence-based psychotherapies for children and adolescent (2nd ed.).
Fries, A. B., Shirtcliff, E. A., & Pollak, S. D. (2008). Neuroendocrine dysregulation following
Frodi, A. M., & Lamb, M. E. (1980). Child abusers’ responses to infant smiles and cries. Child
Garbarino, J. (1976). A preliminary study of some ecological correlates of child abuse: The
Gaudin, J. M., Polansky, N. A., Kilpatrick, A. C., & Shilton, P. (1993). Loneliness, depression,
63, 597−605.
Gelles, R. J. (1992). Poverty and violence towards children. American Behavioral Scientist, 35,
258-274.
Gelles, R. J., & Hargreaves, E. F. (1990). Maternal employment and violence toward children. In
factors and adaptation in 8,154 families (pp. 263–277). New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction
Books.
Gelles, R. J., & Straus, M. A. (1987). Is violence toward children increasing?. A comparison of
1975 and 1985. National Survey Rates. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 2, 212-222.
Gil, D. G. (1971). Violence against children: Physical abuse in the United States. The American
Gottman, J. M., Katz, L., & Hooven, C. (1996). Parental meta-emotion philosophy and the
emotional life of families: Theoretical models and preliminary data. Journal of Family
Hansen, D., Pallotta, G., Tishelman, A., Conaway, L., & MacMillan, V. (1989). Parental
Helfer, R. E., & Kempe, C. H. (Eds.). (1968). The battered child. Chicago, IL: The University of
Chicago Press.
39
Heyman, R. E., & Ezzell, C. E. (2005). Interpersonal violence. In A. P. Giardino & R. Alexander
(Eds.) Child maltreatment: Two-volume set (pp. 639-658). St. Louis, MO: G. W. Medical
Publishing.
Heyman, R. E., & Slep, A. M. S. (2002). Do child abuse and interpersonal violence lead to
Heyman, R. E., & Slep, A. M. S. (2006). Creating and field-testing diagnostic criteria for partner
Heyman, R. E., & Slep, A. M. S. (2009). Reliability of family maltreatment diagnostic criteria:
Hillson, J. M. C., & Kuiper, N. A. (1994). Stress and coping model of child maltreatment.
processes. In A. Raine, P. A. Brennan, et al. (Eds.), Biosocial bases of violence (pp. 69-
Ito, Y., Teicher, M. H., Glod, C. A., Harper, D., Magnus, E., & Gelbard, H. A. (1993). Increased
Jones, L. M., & Finkelhor, D. (2003). Putting together evidence on declining trends in sexual
Jones, L. M., Finkelhor, D., & Halter, S. (2006). Child maltreatment trends in the 1990s: Why
does neglect differ from sexual and physical abuse?. Child Maltreatment, 11, 107-120.
Jones, L. M., Finkelhor, D., & Kopiec, K. (2001). Why is sexual abuse declining?. A survey of
state child protection administrators. Child Abuse & Neglect, 25, 1139-1158.
40
Kaufman, J., & Zigler, E. (1993). The intergenerational transmission of abuse is overstated. In R.
J. Gelles, & D. R. Loseke (Eds.), Current controversies on family violence (pp. 209–
Kelleher, K., Chaffin, M., Hollenberg, J., & Fischer, E. (1994). Alcohol and drug disorders
Ketterlinus, R. D., Lamb, M. E., & Nitz, K. (1991). Development and ecological sources of
Kinard, E. M. (1982). Child abuse and depression: Cause or consequences?. Child Welfare, 7,
403–413.
Psychometric characteristics and treatment comparisons. Child Abuse & Neglect, 20, 23–
43.
Koyabashi, J., Sales, B. D., Becker, J. V., Figueredo, A. J., & Kaplan, M. S. (1995). Perceived
parental deviance, parent–child bonding, child abuse, and child sexual aggression. Sexual
Kruttschnitt, C., McLeod, J. D., & Dornfeld, M. (1994). The economic environment of child
Larrance, D. T., & Twentyman, C. T. (1983). Maternal attributions and child abuse. Journal of
Lutzker, J. R. (1984). Project 12-Ways: Treating child abuse and neglect from an ecobehavioral
Mash, E. J., Johnston, C., & Kovitz, K. (1983). A comparison of the mother–child interactions of
physically abused and non-abused children during play and task situations. Journal of
McGloin, J. M. & Widom, C. S. (2001). Resilience among abused and neglected children grown
Milner, J. S. (1993). Social information processing and physical child abuse. Clinical Psychology
Milner, J. S. (2000). Social information processing and child physical abuse: Theory and
Milner, J. S., & Chilamkurti, C. (1991). Physical child abuse perpetrator characteristics: A
Milner, J. S., Halsey, L. B., & Fultz, J. (1995). Empathic responsiveness and affective reactivity
to infant stimuli in high and low-risk for physical child abuse mothers. Child Abuse &
Milner, J. S., & Robertson, K. R. (1990). Comparison of physical child abusers, intrafamilial
sexual child abusers, and child neglecters. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 5, 37−48.
Mineka, S., & Hamida, S. (1998). Observational and nonconscious learning. In W. O'Donohue
(Ed.), Learning and behavior therapy (pp. 421-439). Needham Heights, MA: Allyn and
Bacon.
42
Murphy, J. M., Jellinek, M., Quinn, D., Smith, G., Poitrast, F. G., & Goshko, M. (1991).
Substance abuse and serious child mistreatment: Prevalence, risk, and outcome in a court
at high and low risk for child physical abuse. Child Abuse & Neglect, 22, 687−703.
O'Leary, K. D., Slep, A. M. S., & O'Leary, S. G. (2000). Co-occurrence of partner and parent
Oldershaw, L., Walters, G. C., & Hall, D. K. (1986). Control strategies and noncompliance in
Olds, D. L., Henderson, C. R., Chamberlin, R., & Tatelbaum, R. (1986). Preventing child abuse
and neglect: A randomized trial of nurse home visitation. Pediatrics, 78, 65-79.
Patterson, G. R. (1976). The aggressive child: Victim and architect of a coercive system.
Pelton, L. (1994). The role of material factors in child abuse and neglect. In G. Melton & F.
Barry (Eds.). Protecting children from abuse and neglect: Foundations for a new
Pfohl, S. J. (1977). The "discovery" of child abuse. Social Problems, 24, 310-323.
Pianta, R., Egeland, B., & Erikson, M. F. (1989). The antecedents of maltreatment: Results of the
maltreatment: Theory and research on the causes and consequences of child abuse and
Pollak, S. D., Nelson, C. A., Schlaak, M. F., Roeber, B. J., Wewerka, S. S., Wiik, K. L., Frenn,
Putnam, F. W. (2003). Ten-year research update review: Child sexual abuse. Journal of the
Reid, J., Patterson, G. R., & Snyder, J. (2002). Antisocial behavior in children and adolescents.
Rind, B., Tromovitch, P., & Bauserman, R. (1998). A meta-analytic examination of assumed
properties of child sexual abuse using college samples. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 22-
53.
Rodriguez, C. M. (2008). Ecological predictors of disciplinary style and child abuse potential in
a Hispanic and Anglo-American Sample. Journal of Child & Family Studies, 17, 336–
352.
Rodriguez, C. M., & Green, A. J. (1997). Parenting stress and anger expression as predictors of
Rosenberg, M. S., & Reppucci, N. D. (1983). Abusive mothers: Perceptions of their own and
their children's behavior. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 51, 674-682.
Ross, S. M. (1996). Risk of physical abuse to children of spouse abusing parents. Child Abuse &
Schumacher, J. A., Slep, A. M. S., & Heyman, R. E. (2001). Risk factors for child neglect.
Sedlak, A. J. (1997). Risk factors for the occurrence of child abuse and neglect. Journal of
Sedlak, A. J., & Broadhurst, D. D. (1996). Third National Incidence Study of Child Abuse and
Neglect: Final report. Washington, DC: US Department of Health and Human Services,
Administration for Children and Families, National Center on Child Abuse and Neglect.
Sedlak, A. J., Mettenburg, J., Basena, M., Petta, I., McPherson, K., Greene, A., & Li, S. (2010).
Fourth national incidence study of child abuse and neglect (NIS–4): Report to Congress.
Starzyk, K. B., & Marshall, W. L. (2003). Childhood family and personological risk factors for
Slep, A. M. S. & Heyman, R. E. (2004). Severity of partner and child maltreatment: Reliability of
scales used in America’s largest child and family protection agency. Journal of Family
Slep, A. M. S., & Heyman, R. E. (2006). Creating and field-testing child maltreatment
Slep, A. M. S., & O’Leary, S. G. (2005). Parent and partner vviolence in families with yyoung
Slep, A. M. S., & O’Leary, S. G. (2007). Multivariate models of mothers’ and fathers’ aggression
45
toward their children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 75, 739-751.
Slep, A. M. S., & O’Leary, S. G. (2009). Distinguishing risk profiles among parent-only, partner-
only, and dually-perpetrating physical aggressors. Journal of Family Psychology, 23, 705-
716.
Snarr, J. D., Heyman, R. E., Slep, A. M. S., Malik, J., & USAF Family Advocacy Program. (in
Spaccarelli, S., & Kim, S. (1995). Resilience criteria and factors associated with resilience in
Spinetta, J. J. (1978). Parental personality factors in child abuse. Journal of Consulting and
Stermac, L. E., & Segal, Z. V. (1989). Adult sexual contact with children. An examination of
Stevens-Simon, C., Nelligan, D., & Kelly, L. (2001). Adolescents at risk for mistreating their
Stith, S. M., Liu, T., Davies, C., Boykin, E. L., Alder, M. C., Harris, J. M., … Dees, J. E. M. E.
Stockhammer, T. F., Salzinger, S., Feldman, R. S., & Mojica, E. (2001). Assessment of the effect
Straus, M. A. (1994). Beating the devil out of them: Corporal punishment in American families.
Straus, M. A., Gelles, R. J., & Steinmetz, S. K. (1980). Behind closed doors: Violence in the
Straus, M. A., & Smith, C. (1990). Family patterns and child abuse. In M. A. Straus & R. J.
Gelles (Eds.), Physical Violence in American families: Risk factors and adaptations to
perspective for early childhood educators: Implications for working with families
Teicher, M. H., Dumont, N. L., Ho, Y., Vaituzis, C., GIedd, J. N., & Andersen, S. L. (2004).
Trickett, P. K., & Kuczynski, L. (1986). Children's misbehaviors and parental discipline
Twentyman, C. T., & Plotkin, R. C. (1982). Unrealistic expectations of parents who maltreat
Whipple, E. E., & Webster-Stratton, C. (1991). The role of parental stress in physically abusive
Whitaker, D. J., Le, B., Hanson, R. K., Baker, C. K., McMahon, P. M., Ryan, G.,…Donovan, R.
(2008). Risk factors for the perpetration of child sexual abuse: A review and meta-
Williamson, J. M., Bourduin, C. M., & Howe, B. A. (1991). The ecology of adolescent
Wolfe, D. A. (1987). Child abuse: Implications for child development and psychopathology (Vol.
Wolfner, G. D., & Gelles, R. J. (1993). A profile of violence toward children: A national study.