Homework 5 - Economics
Homework 5 - Economics
1. Suppose you are looking at some survey data, and you find that people who report
reading newspapers more regularly are also more likely to vote.
4. What are some confounders or issues of reverse causality that might be driving this
relationship?
5. How would you control for these confounders? Think carefully about how you would
measure the confounding variables.
6. Are there some confounders or problems that you may not be able to fully control for?
Why?
7. Now, suppose you decided to run an experiment to see if there is a causal relationship
between reading the newspaper and voting. Let’s say you had a large amount of money
to run the experiment. Describe how you would do it. Make sure you answer the
following questions:
8. How or why does the experiment address the confounders or problems you were not
fully able to control for as discussed question 6?
9. What problems of non-compliance might you face? How would you address them?
10. What problems of attrition might you face? How would you address them?
11. Read the paper by Gerber et al (2009). It is attached to the end of this document. Discuss
the research design, answering the same questions you did in question 7.
12. In what ways does the author’s design and your design differ? How might the design
differences affect the results and interpretation?
13. What are the author’s main results? Are they what you expected? How did the results
change your beliefs about media consumption and voting?
JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide
range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and
facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected].
Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at
https://about.jstor.org/terms
is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to American Economic
Journal: Applied Economics
Citizensvision
learn and read
about in newspapers.
politics Recent from
and government work has
the examined
news theyhow theon
watch media
tele
shapes the public's political knowledge, attitudes, and behavior (Timothy J. Besley
and Robin Burgess 2002, Alexander Dyck and Luigi Zingales 2002, and James
T. Hamilton 2003). Media sources may influence the public not only through the
slant of a particular report (Stefano DellaVigna and Ethan Kaplan 2007) but also by
choosing what to cover (Lisa M. George and Joel Waldfogel 2006).
This paper reports the results of a natural field experiment1 to measure the effect
of political news content on political behavior and opinions. The Washington, DC
area is served by two major newspapers, the conservative Washington Times and the
more liberal Washington Post (Tim Groseclose and Jeffrey Milyo 2005). The presence
of a liberal and conservative paper serving the same region creates an outstanding
opportunity to study the effect of media slant in a naturalistic setting within a single
population.2 Approximately one month prior to the Virginia gubernatorial election in
* Gerber: Department of Political Science, Yale University, 77 Prospect St, New Haven, CT 06520, and
National Bureau of Economic Research (e-mail: [email protected]); Karlan: Yale University, 27 Hillhouse
Ave, New Haven, CT 06511, Innovations for Poverty Action and Massachusetts Institute of Technology Jameel
Poverty Action Lab (e-mail: [email protected]); Bergan: Department of Communications, Michigan State
University, 463 Communications Arts & Sciences Building, East Lansing, MI 48824 (e-mail: [email protected]).
We would like to thank Stefano DellaVigna, Don Green, Tim Groseclose, and Brian Knight for very helpful com
ments. We also thank Chris Mann and Brenner Tobe for providing the turnout data.
f To comment on this article in the online discussion forum visit the articles page at:
http://www.aeaweb.org/articles.php?doi=10.1257/app. 1.2.35.
1 As per the taxonomy put forth in Glenn W. Harrison and John A. List (2004).
2 The Washington Post is also a more nationally prominent newspaper than the Washington Times. Thus, read
ers may perceive a difference in quality and trustworthiness, not just slant, between these two newspapers.
35
Our research contributes to a large and growing literature on the effect of news
media on political attitudes and behavior. The earliest media studies discounted the
impact of mass communications, but most recent studies conclude that media expo
sure can have a sizable effect. The most common method of measuring the effect
of media content is to use data from surveys to measure the association between a
respondent's reported media exposure and his or her political views. Among the find
ings adduced to suggest media effects, researchers frequently document strong asso
ciations between exposure to media with a distinctive slant and the viewers' political
attitudes and information (Peter Clarke and Eric Fredin 1978; Arthur Miller, Edie N.
Goldenberg, and Lutz Ebring 1979; Carl R. Bybee et al. 1981; Gina M. Garramone
and Charles K. Atkin 1986; Joel Lieske 1989; Craig Leonard Brians and Martin P.
Wattenberg 1996; Russell J. Dalton, Paul A. Beck and Robert Huckfeldt 1998; and
John R. Hibbing and Elizabeth Theiss-Morse 1998). While suggestive, this research
design may produce upwardly biased estimates of media influence due to biased
reports of media exposure and to selection bias from the tendency for individuals
to seek out information that agrees with their pre-existing views (Timothy C. Brock
1965, Paul D. Sweeney and Kathy L. Gruber 1984, Matthew Gentzkow and James
M. Shaprio 2005, and Sendhil Mullainathan and Andrei Shleifer 2005).
Several recent papers employ natural experiments to measure media effects on
voter turnout. Gentzkow (2006) studies the introduction of television and shows that
the introduction of television was associated with a decline in voter turnout, a sharp
drop in the number of newspapers, and a drop in newspaper readership. Noting the
political science literature linking newspaper readership to political participation
(Michael Morgan and James Shanahan 1992), he argues that the rise in television is
responsible for between one-quarter and one-half of the total decline in voter turnout
from the 1950s to the 1970s. George and Waldfogel (2002) study the expansion of
the national edition of the New York Times. They find that, as the New York Times
displaces local newspaper readership, turnout in nonpresidential elections falls rela
tive to turnout in presidential elections.
A recent study of the persuasive effect of political news (DellaVigna and Kaplan
2007) uses variation in the availability of the Fox News Channel across cable sys
tems to measure the effects of the channel's news coverage on the Republican Party
vote share in presidential elections (as well as the Republican share in Senate races
and voter turnout). They estimate that Fox News caused about one-half of a per
centage point shift toward George W. Bush in the 2000 presidential election. Since
A. Experimental Procedures
> get news or political magazine 9.20 9.36 8.81 9.37 0.88
(0.50) (0.77) (0.91) (0.95)
[28.91] [29.13] [28.36] [29.15]
% prefers Democratic candidate for governor in VA 14.43 14.53 14.61 14.11 0.94
(0.61) (0.93) (1.14) (1.13)
[35.15] [35.25] [35.34] [34.83]
Notes: Standard errors reported in parentheses; standard deviations in brackets. Column 5 reports the p-values
for chi-squared tests of independence between treatments for each variable. The second through fourth rows (per
cent voted) apply only to the voter registration (i.e., nonconsumer) sample frame. All regressions in Tables 2-4
include controls for which sample frame provided the observation. A multinomial logit model predicting assign
ment to treatment using all of the above baseline variables yields a chi-squared test value of 9.21 (d.f. 18, p-value
of 0.95).
Post subscription list (although it may be the case that these households were receiv
ing only Sunday delivery). The Times has a lower subscription rate and reported only
five households already subscribing. As group assignment was random, this suggests
that some portion of the control group and Times treatment group, perhaps around 8
percent, was getting the Post at least on Sunday, and a much smaller portion of the
Post treatment group and the control group was getting the Times. Since the treatment
effect estimates are based on the difference in treatment rates between the treatment
and control group, this suggests the treatment effect should be interpreted as the effect
of boosting the household exposure rate to the Post by approximately 92 percentage
points rather than 100 percentage points. Thus, any observed difference between the
Post treatment group and the other groups will tend to underestimate, most likely by
a small amount, the impact of exposure to the Post.
We followed the news reporting of both papers throughout the study period and
recorded the choice of topics and headlines in each paper. Previous researchers
have found that, as is widely believed, the Post leans left and the Times leans right
(Groseclose and Milyo 2005). Our analysis of coverage confirms this assessment.
While both papers gave extensive attention to the Iraq War, the Times had three above
the fold headlines mentioning Iraqi efforts at forging a constitution and only one men
tion of the controversies involving Iraq detainees. The pattern for the Washington
Post was the opposite, featuring three stories on detainees and one on the constitu
tion. The Post gave much greater attention to Republican political difficulties. The
disclosure of the identity of Valerie Plame, a Central Intelligence Agency (CIA)
officer married to an administration critic, was given very extensive coverage in the
Post and much less prominence in the Times. In place of the Posfs emphasis on the
Plame issue and other administration controversies, the Times gave more coverage to
the filling of the impending Supreme Court vacancy. The Times had about twice as
many above the fold stories on the nominations of Harriet Miers and Samuel Alito
for the Supreme Court. Both papers covered Hurricane Wilma, but the Post also had
several stories about preparations for and the response to Hurricane Katrina.7
The manner in which subjects were covered also suggests the Times was more
favorable to the administration than was the Post. A comparison of same day head
lines illustrates this. On Tuesday, October 18, 2005, both papers had front page
stories on Iraq. The Times story had the headline "No tears for Saddam in Iraq,"
while the Post ran the headline "Iraqis Say Airstrikes Kill Many Civilians." On
November 4, 2005, the Times ran a story with the headline "Recruits Join Armed
Forces Seeking War?A Sort of Vendetta Spurs Youth to Enlist After 9/11," whereas
on November 7, 2005, the Post ran a story with the headline "Youths in Rural U.S.
Are Drawn to Military?Worries About Jobs Outweigh War Fears." When Miers
nomination to the Supreme Court was withdrawn, a Times headline read simply
"Miers Rules Herself Out" (October 28, 2005), while the Post used the headlines
"Miers Withdrawn as Nominee for the Court" and "Nomination was Plagued by
Missteps from the Start."
Both newspapers gave the governor's race extensive coverage. The Post had 15
stories on the front page or first page of the Metro section while the Times had ten
articles. National events may have affected the gubernatorial race in Virginia as
well. News reports cited national issues as persuasive to many voters in the Virginia
election.8
7 Two tables summarizing the front page, above-the-fold stories, by topic, for the Post and the Times, along
with a listing of every headline as well as every headline on the Metro page that related to the Virginia gubernato
rial election, are available at http://karlan.yale.edU/p/index.php.
8 "Moments foreshadowing a political collapse" The Virginian-Pilot, November 13, 2005; '"Twas a Famous
Victory, & Republicans Have Some Issues" Richmond Times Dispatch, November 13, 2005; "New GOP Agenda,
Many Things Combined to Cripple Kilgore's Gubernatorial Hopes" Richmond Times Dispatch, November 13,
2005.
Notes: Standard errors reported in parentheses; standard deviations in brackets. Column 5 reports the p-values for
chi-squared tests of independence between treatments for each baseline variable. A multinomial logit model pre
dicting assignment to treatment using all of the above baseline variables yields a chi-squared test value of 17.62
(d.f. 18,p-value of 0.48).
C. Outcome Data
During the week after the November election, we reinterviewed 1,081 of the 3,347
individuals in our sample for the follow-up survey. Response rates of 30 or 40 percent
are typical in the public opinion literature (Herbert Asher 2004).9 Survey respondents
were not told of any link between the free subscriptions and the phone surveys. The
follow-up survey asked questions about the 2005 Virginia gubernatorial election (e.g.,
did the subject vote, which candidate was voted for or preferred), national politics (e.g.,
9 Public opinion literature suggests that increasing the response rate from 30-40 percent to 60 percent pro
duces similar results for many topics including attention to media, engagement in politics, and social and political
attitudes (Scott Keeter et al. 2000). The complete set of dispositions was: Survey completed 31.8 percent, Refused
to answer/Not interested 29.7 percent, Person not available 10.3 percent, Answering machine 9.8 percent, Partial
survey/refused 6.0 percent, Disconnect 4.1 percent, Do not call/irate 3.1 percent, Ring no answer 1.7 percent,
Wrong number 1.5 percent, Language barrier 1.1 percent, Busy signal 0.8 percent, Rescheduled call 0.1 percent,
Deceased 0.03 percent.
Notes: Standard errors in parentheses; standard deviation in brackets. Number of observations varies due to refusal
to answer or "no opinion" responses.
a Sample frame for the administrative data outcomes in rows 2 and 3 include all individuals for whom we suc
cessfully matched state administrative data with the baseline, whereas all other rows include only the individuals
who completed the follow-up survey.
favorability ratings for Bush, the Republicans, the Democrats, support for Supreme
Court nominee Alito), and knowledge of news events (e.g., does subject know the num
ber of Iraq war dead, has subject heard of I. Lewis "Scooter" Libby).
For analyzing the effects on voter turnout, we also obtained administrative records
of individual voter turnout and successfully merged these data for 2,571 (76.8 per
cent) of the individuals in the baseline (not all individuals in the sample frame were
registered voters).
Table IB shows baseline sample statistics for the subsample of subjects that
completed the post-election survey, and Table 1C shows the summary statistics for
all outcome measures analyzed in further tables. Table IB shows that assignment
to treatment appears orthogonal to all covariates, even after attrition. Appendix
Table Al provides further evidence of this by examining whether the interaction of
baseline covariates and assignment to treatment predicts attrition. We do not find
that either treatment led to a sample selection bias in terms of the characteristics of
individuals who responded to the follow-up survey. Individuals who voted in 2002
and subscribed to a news magazine (hence are more engaged in politics), as well as
those who preferred the Democratic candidate for governor in the baseline, were
more likely to complete the follow-up phone survey. However, this sample selection
bias is not correlated with assignment to treatment (Appendix Table Al, column 2).
Regardless, all results we present later in the paper include these same baseline
covariates in order to address potential bias from the sample response. If there is
attrition based on unobserved variables that are correlated with the outcome mea
sures but not predicted by the observables, our results may be biased.
One limitation of this study is that while we know which households received
newspapers, we cannot be sure that the newspapers were read. Our follow-up sur
vey provides three measures of the effect of newspaper provision on newspaper
reading: whether subjects receive a newspaper, which newspaper they receive, and
the frequency with which they read a newspaper. Table 2 shows the relationship
between treatment assignment and five distribution and readership outcomes. All
Frequency reads a
Receives Receives Receives paper (0 = Never, Reads at least
a newspaper the Post the Times 3 = Every day) several times/week
(5)
_0)_(2) (3)_(4)
Panel A: Separate treatment effects estimated for Washington Post an d Washington Times
Washington Post treatment 0.287 0.344 -0.006 0.151 0.089
(0.035) (0.035) (0.021) (0.084) (0.038)
Washington Times treatment 0.100 0.031 0.133 0.086 0.057
(0.034) (0.035) (0.021) (0.083) (0.037)
Adjusted/?2 0.32 0.18 0.19 0.24 0.18
F-test: Post = Times 24.76 66.96 37.81 0.51 0.63
p-value 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.48 0.43
Panel B: Pooled treatment effect estimated for receiving either newspaper
Received either Post or Times 0.191 0.183 0.066 0.118 0.072
treatment (0.029) (0.031) (0.018) (0.070) (0.032)
Adjusted/?2 0.30 0.11 0.15 0.24 0.18
Observation counts for both panels
Observations 1,080 1,080 1,080 1,075 1,075
Refused/no opinion 1116 6
Total surveyed in follow-up 1,081 1,081 1,081 1,081 1,081
Notes: Standard errors in parentheses. Dependent variables are dummy variables based on responses to the
post-election survey. We include the following covariates: gender; reported age; three separate indicators for vot
ing in the 2001, 2002, and 2004 general elections; an indicator for whether the respondent was drawn from a con
sumer list; self reports of receiving any news or political magazines; baseline survey self reports of preferring the
Democratic candidate in the gubernatorial election and having no preference in the gubernatorial election; and an
indicator for wave of the study. If a covariate value was missing, an indicator variable was included and the cova
riate was coded as zero. We include strata indicators, variables for each strata formed prior to the randomization,
which included unique combinations of the following: intention to vote, receive a paper (non-Post/non-Times),
mentions ever reading a paper, gets a magazine, and asked whether they wish they read the paper more. We also
include surveyor/date indicators, a set of indicator variables for each unique combination of surveyor, and date
for the follow-up survey.
pooled treament effect estimates are statistically significant at the .01, .05, or .1 level
and the pattern of results matches the subject treatment group assignment (i.e., the
Post treatment group reported receiving the Post and not the Times, and the Times
treatment group reported receiving the Times and not the Post). The coefficients,
however, suggest less than full readership of those in the treatment groups. No doubt
some of this is due to individuals ignoring the free subscription altogether, but it may
partly be due to the wording of the question. Individuals may have answered "no" to
"receiving a newspaper" because they did not see their free trial as a true subscrip
tion. Additionally, questions about whether one reads the paper may have been inter
preted as inquiring about typical rather than very recent behavior. We also found in
the baseline survey that some respondents reported not receiving a newspaper, when
in fact the newspaper was delivered to them regularly. There is further evidence that
the newspapers were not disregarded. The Post informed us that, as of March 2006
(three months after the free subscription ended), approximately 17 percent of the
treatment group had decided to subscribe to the Post}0
Notes: Standard errors in parentheses. Dependent variables are indexes constructed by summing the standard
deviations from the mean for each of the specific questions for each index. See Appendix Table A2 for regressions
showing the treatment effects for each question used in the construction of the indices. The Fact Accuracy Index
is based on responses to three factual questions (identified number dead in Iraq in a closed-ended question, iden
tified "Scooter" Libby from a list of four individuals as Dick Cheney's chief of staff who recently resigned, iden
tified Harriett Miers from a list of four individuals as a recent female US Supreme Court nominee). The Specific
Issue Index is based on five questions on political issues (three questions about Iraq and the war, a question on the
Plame leak, a question about the Alito confirmation). The Broad Policy Index is based on four questions about atti
tudes toward the political parties, President Bush, and ideological self-placement on a 7 point scale.
We include the following covariates: gender; reported age; three separate indicators for voting in the 2001,
2002, and 2004 general elections; an indicator for whether the respondent was drawn from a consumer list; self
reports of receiving any news or political magazines; baseline survey self reports of preferring the Democratic
candidate in the gubernatorial election and having no preference in the gubernatorial election; and an indicator
for wave of the study. If a covariate value was missing, an indicator variable was included and the covariate was
coded as zero. We include strata indicators, variables for each strata formed prior to the randomization, which
included unique combinations of the following: intention to vote, receive a paper (non-Post/non-Times), mentions
ever reading a paper, gets a magazine, and asked whether they wish they read the paper more. We also include
surveyor/date indicators, a set of indicator variables for each unique combination of surveyor, and date for the
follow-up survey. All results remain qualitatively similar, and statistical significance remains as-is, using probit
or ordered probit specifications instead of OLS.
II. Results
Notes: Standard errors in parentheses. The following covariate variables are included in all specifications: gender;
reported age; three separate indicators for voting in the 2001, 2002, and 2004 general elections; an indicator for
whether the respondent was drawn from a consumer list; self report of receiving any news or political magazines;
baseline survey self reports of preferring the Democratic candidate in the gubernatorial election and having no
preference in the gubernatorial election; and an indicator for wave of the study. If a covariate value was missing,
an indicator variable was included and the covariate was coded as zero. We include strata indicators, which are
variables for each strata formed prior to the randomization, which included unique combinations of the following:
intention to vote, receive a paper (non-Post/non-Times), mentions ever reading a paper, gets a magazine, and asked
whether they wish they read the paper more. All results remain qualitatively similar, and statistical significance
remains as-is, using probit specifications instead of OLS.
Data source:
a Survey.
b Administrative voting records.
five specific issues (most important problem in America, most important problem in
Iraq, progress in Iraq, investigation regarding Plame, and the Alito confirmation).
Column 3 is a "Broad Policy" index of four broad political views (Bush's approval
rating, Republican favorable rating, Democrat favorable rating, and respondent's
reported political ideology). For each of these indices, more conservative is a higher
number.
The Post and Times treatment had no significant effect on shifting either the
Broad Policy index toward the Democrats (Post coefficient of -0.085 std, p < 0.12;
Times coefficient of -0.051 std, p < 0.34; Post and Times groups, -0.068 std, p <
0.14) or the Specific Policy index (Post coefficient of -0.045 std, p < 0.35; Times
coefficient of -0.027 std, p < 0.57). Considering the newspaper treatment groups
together, we also find negative (toward liberal) point estimates, but they are not sta
tistically significant.
Table 4 shows the effect of the newspapers on voter turnout (self-reported and
from administrative data) and which candidate individuals voted for or preferred.
The first set of regressions measure the turnout effects. There was no effect on either
self-reported or administratively measured turnout for the 2005 election (panel B,
column 1: coefficient for self-reported outcome is 0.2 percent, s.e. = 2.8 percent;
column 2: coefficient for administrative outcome is 0.3 percent, s.e. = 1.6 percent).
In November 2006 (column 3), however, the administrative data show a 2.8 percent
age point higher voter turnout, with a standard error of 1.6 percentage points (p <
0.11). It is surprising to see a result in 2006 but not in 2005. This could be a result of
the post-election receipt of the remainder of the ten-week newspaper subscriptions
or the treatment that resulted from the 17 percent of the Post treatment group who
renewed their subscription after the free period ended.
The dependent variables in columns 4 and 5 of Table 4 are dummy variables equal to
one if the subject reported voting for the Democratic candidate in the 2005 gubernato
rial election. In column 4, the sample is restricted to those who reported voting, while
column 5 includes all respondents (coding those who did not vote at all as zero, i.e., as not
voting for the Democratic candidate). The newspapers had an effect on which candidate
the subject supports. Getting the Post is estimated to increase the probability of selecting
the Democrat by 11.2 percentage points (p < 0.014) among those who reported voting
(column 4) and by 7.2 percentage points (p < 0.043) across all respondents. Contrary to
initial expectations, the right leaning Times was also associated with an increase in the
probability of a Democratic vote in the Virginia governor's race. The effect was about %
as large as that estimated for the Post treatment (7.4 percentage points and p < 0.10, and
6.0 percentage points and p < 0.084, respectively for columns 4 and 5). The difference
between the Post and Times point estimates is not statistically significant.
III. Conclusion
11 These numbers are based on an average of the polls compiled by the Web site www.pollingreport.com. The
average percentage of respondents approving of Bush's job performance using all polls in the field with starting
dates between October 12-October 18 (TV = 4 polls) was 40.0, starting October 19-October 25 (N = 3) was 41.3,
and starting November 9-November 16 (N = 3) was 37.3.
opinions put forward were not heavily against the Democratic candidate. A third
explanation is sampling error. Given the 4 percentage point standard errors for each
treatment group, it is possible that there are meaningful differential effects, consis
tent with the news slant of the papers, which we did not detect due to inadequate
power. Hence, while prior beliefs about media and bias should be updated in light of
the evidence we present, it would be useful for future work to obtain more precise
measurement of the differential treatment effects through use of larger samples.
Finally, there is some evidence that getting either newspaper produced an increase
in voter turnout especially in the 2006 national elections. Our finding that turnout
among the treatment groups was about 3 percentage points higher than the con
trol group is consistent with previous work showing positive turnout effects from
newspaper reading (Gentzkow 2006) and suggests newspaper exposure might have
an important long term effect on the level of political interest. However, this result
was nonexistent for the immediate election in 2005 and of only borderline statisti
cal significance for 2006. Therefore, both the existence of an effect and its potential
mechanism should be treated guardedly until further study.
There were also some important outcomes that were not affected by treatments.
There is only limited evidence that the newspapers increased the subject's factual
knowledge of politics. Since conveying facts about politics is a plausible channel
for how the papers might induce change in opinions about candidates and the deci
sion to vote, this null result stands in contrast to the measured effects on candidate
preference and turnout. However, while greater political information among subjects
in the treatment groups would have helped to explain the movement in candidate
preference among the subject groups, changes in opinion often occur without a sub
ject being able to recall the facts that caused opinions to shift. Results of this type
are commonplace in political science. Drawing on work from psychology (Norman
H. Anderson and Stephen Hubert 1963, and W. A. Watts and W. J. McGuire 1964),
political scientists have constructed models of online processing of political infor
mation, where citizens update their judgments in response to the flow of information
but do not retain memory of the particular facts that caused them to revise their
views (Milton Lodge, Kathleen M. McGraw, and Patrick Stroh 1989; Lodge and
Stroh 1993). These models receive substantial empirical support (Lodge, Marco R.
Steenbergen, and Shawn Brau 1995).
Our field experiment directly addresses the problem of selection bias in standard
observational studies. As in all empirical work, experimental or not, there is still the
important question of generalizing from our particular findings. Any broad inferences
from this study to the effects of media bias on political decisions, in general, should
recognize that the results may depend on several specific features of our experiment
such as the political context, choice of subjects, intensity of treatment, length of the
study, timing of the study, and choice of media outlets. Of particular interest would be
to investigate whether the findings we report, which suggest that the common effects
of greater news exposure are of greater consequence than the news slant, generalize to
periods with a more balanced news flow and are confirmed in larger studies that mea
sure differential effects across treatment groups with greater precision. To address these
issues, we suggest that this field experiment approach should be applied in different
political contexts, with different subjects, for different durations, using different media.
Appendix
_0)_(2)_
Post treatment group 0.003 -0.048
(0.020) (0.046)
Times treatment group 0.018 0.052
(0.020) (0.050)
Female -0.026 -0.040
(0.017) (0.026)
Voted in 2002 0.095 0.103
(0.024) (0.038)
From consumer database sample frame 0.044 0.046
(0.021) (0.032)
Subscribes to news magazine 0.069 0.026
(0.029) (0.043)
Reported preferring Democratic candidate for governor 0.126 0.126
(0.026) (0.040)
Wave 2 of experiment -0.037 -0.035
(0.017) (0.026)
Post x female 0.094
(0.045)
Post x voted in 2002 -0.037
(0.054)
Post x from consumer database sample frame 0.011
(0.050)
Post x subscribes to news magazine 0.053
(0.071)
Post x reported preferring Democratic candidate for governor 0.032
(0.059)
Post x wave 2 of experiment 0.043
(0.043)
Times x female -0.040
(0.040)
Times x voted in 2002 0.014
(0.057)
Times x from consumer database sample frame ?0.018
(0.049)
Times x subscribes to news magazine 0.092
(0.072)
Times x reported preferring Democratic candidate for governor ?0.031
(0.056)
Times x wave 2 of experiment -0.048
(0.039)
Notes: Standard errors in parentheses. Indicator variable included (but not reported) if gender
information is missing (applicable for 134 observations). All variables (except assignment to
treatment and gender) are from the baseline survey.
Separate treatment effects estimated for Washington Post and Washington Times
Notes: Standard errors in parentheses. Dependent variables include factual questions (ability to identify the num
ber dead in Iraq in a closed-ended question, identification of "Scooter" Libby from a list of four individuals as
Dick Cheney's chief of staff who recently resigned, identification of Harriett Miers from a list of four individu
als as a recent US Supreme Court nominee), political opinion quesitons (a closed-ended question about the most
important problem facing the country, a closed-ended question about the most important problems in the Iraq
War, attitudes about the leak case, the Alito confirmation), and attitudes about general national issues (Bush
approval, favorability towards Republicans and Democrats, and Conservatism). We include covariates: gender,
reported age, three separate indicators for voting in the 2001, 2002, and 2004 general elections, an indicator for
whether the respondent was drawn from a consumer list, self report of receiving any news or political magazines,
and baseline survey self reports of preferring the Democratic candidate in the gubernatorial election and having
no preference in the gubernatorial election, and an indicator for wave of the study. If a covariate value was miss
ing, an indicator variable was included and the covariate was coded as zero. We also include strata indicators,
variables for each of the strata formed prior to the randomization, which included unique combinations of the
following: intention to vote, receive a paper (non-Post/non-Times), mentions ever reading a paper, gets a maga
zine, and asked whether they wish they read the paper more. We also include surveyor/date indicators, which are
a set of indicator variables for each unique combination of surveyor, and date for the follow-up survey. All results
remain qualitatively similar, and statistical significance remains as-is, using probit or ordered probit specifica
tions instead of OLS.
REFERENCES
Anderson, Norman H., and Stephen Hubert. 1963. "Effects of Concomitant Recall on Order Effects in
Personality Impression Formation." Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 2: 379-91.
Ansolabehere, Stephen, and Shanto Iyengar. 1995. Going Negative. New York: The Free Press.
Asher, Herbert B. 2004. Polling and the Public: What Every Citizen Should Know. Washington, DC:
CQ Press.
Besley, Timothy J., and Robin Burgess. 2002. "The Political Economy of Government Responsive
ness: Theory and Evidence from India." Quarterly Journal of Economics, 117(4): 1415-51.
Brians, Craig Leonard, and Martin P. Wattenberg. 1996. "Campaign Issue Knowledge and Salience:
Comparing Reception from TV Commercials, TV News and Newspapers." American Journal of
Political Science, 40(1): 172-93.