Ada 117555
Ada 117555
Ada 117555
- j
and
Vernon E. Carter
Northrop Corporation
i-1
I DTIC
, I•ELECTE•
~JUL 29 1982
U.S. Army
August 1981
Approved for public release; distribution unlimited.
41, 82 07 29 036
-"Mm
U. S. ARMY RESEARCH INSTITUTE
FOR THE BEHAVIORAL AND SOCIAL SCIENCES
L. NEALE COSBY
JOSEPH ZEIDNER Colonel, IN
Technical Director Commander
NOTICES-
__________ _____ ~- A
UNCLASSIFIED
SECURITY CLASSIFICATION OF THIS PAGE 'W"en Date Entered) _
17. DISTRIBUTION STATEMENT (of th. aLbetrect entered in Block 20. If different from Report)
19. -KEY WOROS-(Continue on reveree eade If neceesary iad Identify by block nzmblr)
-Training Simulators Flight Simulation Army Aircraft
Pilot Training Training Devices Army Training
Transfer of Training Simulation Visual Perception
Flight Training Simulator Fidelity Motion Perception
20. A
A*TRACT f C W.-r*vWv
,atfnsu abb If nrcemsy and idoetify by block nu*beir)
• Fidelity requirements for Army flight trainink simulators, are explored
-using a manual control theory approach. The firsi step is to define
simulator fidelity" in operational terms which provide a basis for each
of the subsequent steps. This definition is accompan -d-Sy--a taxonomy-.ofL,
-measureable fidelity parameters. The next step, also of a preparatory
nature, is the analysis of Army flight training missions. It describes
how specific flight tasks and piloting techniques can ben terms
Item 7 (Cont'd)
Vernon E. Carter, Northrop Corporation
Item 20 (Cont'd)
Accession For°V
IIDTIC GRA&I
NTIS TAB
1nannouxacd
ti
J-ustiricttio
iDi-t r ib i oný__..___-
Avail',bilitY Codes
"-- -lAvai 8and/or -
,i 1specilnA
iiUNCLASSIFIED
SECURITY CLASSIFICATION OF THIS PAGE(When Data Emored)
Techmicol Report 546
A
and
S ~Vernon E. Carter
Northrop Corporation
t. Submitted by:
AR IEDCharles A. Gainer, Chief
ARI FIELD UNIT AT FORT RUCKER, ALABAMA
Approved by:
Edgar M. Johnson, Director
SYSTEMS RESEARCH LABORATORY
U.S. ARMY RESEARCH INSTITUTE FOR THE BEHAVIORAL AND SOCIAL SCIENCES
"5001 Eisenhower Avenue, Alexandria, Virginia 22333
Office, Deputy Chief of Staff for Personnel
Department of the- Army
August 1981
Ii
ARI Research Reports and TeChnical Reports are intended for sponsors of
R&D tasks and for other research and military agencies. Any findings ready
for implementation at the time of Iublication are presented in the last part
of the Brief. Upon completion o, a major phase of the task, formal recom-
mendations for official action nforonliy are conveyed to appropriate military
agencies by briefing or Disposition Form.
- '5,
FORAN ORD
r; - "'the
The Army of
mission Research Institute
providing timelyField Unit at
research andFort Rucker, Alabama,
development in
support has
aircrew training for the US Army Aviation Center. Research and development
are conducted in-house, augmented by contract research as required. This
research report documents contract work performed as a part of the Field
Unit's thrust in flight simulation developmenL.
Aviation Center, Fort Rucker, and to the US Army Project Manager for
Training Devices (PM-TRADE).
JOS H ZEI R
Tec ical Director
J.
77j
DETERMINATION OF MOTION AND VISUAL SYSTEM REQUIREMENTS FOR FLIGHT TRAINING
SIMULATORS
BRIEF
r Requirement:
Procedure:
Findings:
The model developed here and the indicated areas in which additional,
more quantitative research is needed will serve as a basis for an alter-
native approach to investigation of simulator visual and motion system
requirements.
- vii
ACKNhnLoDGEMENTS
The work reported herein was performed for the US Army Research
Institute Field Unit at Fort Rucker, Alabama, under Contract MDA 903-
80-C-0235. The Systems Technology, Inc., project engineer was Mr. Warren
F. Clement and the Army Contracting officer's technical representative was
Dr. William R. Bickley. Dr. Kenneth D. Cross of Anacapa Sciences, Inc.,
and Mr. Vernon E. Carter of Northrop were project consultants to Systems
Technology, Inc. The final report manuscript was prepared by Mrs. Sharon
A. Duerksen of Systems Technology, Inc. Work on the project was begun in
February 1980 and completed in July 1981.
%I
iIx
Sii
Il
£ • •
TABLE OF CONTENTS
Page
Backg round 1
Technical Approach 2
Definition of Fidelity 2
Research Needed 5
"INTRODUCION 7
Background 7
Technical Approach 10
An Operational Definition 14
xiF
Page
xii
Page
GLOSSARY 228
i• xiii
LIST OF TABLES
Number Page
3 Simulator-Fidelity-Related States 38
Rotary-Wing Aircraft 55
xiv
"Y
_ _ _ _ __ _ _ _ _-__ _ _ _ _
- - - - - - - - - -- - **- -
"umber Page
LIST OF FIGURES
Number Page
xv
Nuaber Page
xvi
I Number
32 inference of Aiming Point Location From the Apparent
Page
xI.
,,1xvi
LIST OF ABBREVIATIONS .
K, kt Knot(s)
rad Radians
xviii-
RPM, rpm Revolutions per minutes
sec Seconds
TV Television
II
S~xix
LIST OF SYMBOLS
e System error
e Error rate
g Gravity constant
h Altitude
hVertical velocity
i System input
J Imaginary operator
K or k Gain
L() Roll stability or control derivative with respect to
LLr Roll stability derivative with respect to yaw rate
xx
Mq Pitch stability derivative with respect to pitch rate
m Aircraft mass
p Roll rate
q Pitch rate
R Range
RRange rate
R Deceleration
r Yaw rate
s Laplace operator
U Airspeed
U0 Trim airspeed
V Speedp4
IJxx
VG Ground speed
X X-axis force
xxii
a Angle of ettack
A Incremental quantity
Control deflection
G Pitch attitude
0 Pitch rate
1 Backscattering coefficient
7t 3.14159..
Tc Computing delays
Te Effective pure time delay
* Bank angle
Heading
Wc Crossover frequency
xxiii
••sp Short period frequency
h Height
Si Lag
L Lead
m Measurement
* 0 Objective
ppkl Peak
R Range
r Rudder control
U Airspeed
T Thrust control
t Threshold,
v Visual
xxiv
Heading, yaw
xIi
LLi
.L
1j
• iI
* I
K Iv
EXECUTIVE SUMMARY
rules to tell us what to do if training was not successful. Was the. de-
ficiency in the motion or visual system? What must be done to correct
that deficiency? What must be provided for new aircraft -or new training
objectives? These questions lead us to the objectives of this study.
ip -
?'1!
TECHNICAL APPROACH
DEFINITION OF FIDELITY
2
"Simulator fidelity is the degree to which characteristics
of perceivable states induce correct psychomotor and
cognitive control strategy for a given task and envi-
ronment. This leads to s'ecial consideration of essential
feedback loops required to execute a task and the esse-
tial cues provided by the simulator or training device
which support those essential loops."
r 3 U
___ _! __.J
PILOT MODELING TECHNIQUES
ing have been well developed; and it is possible to predict the nature of -
li
4 Ii•
-- - - - - - -- -- - - ----- - - -- - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - -
SIDJLATOR FIDELITY BOOKKEEPING
RESEARCH NEEDED
V
(X)NCLUSIONS ANDl RECOMINDATIONS
A
The application of control theory to training simulator fidelity of-
fers a poteutially powerful tool. All of the task, physical, and
5
SIMJLN-TOR FIDELITY BOOKKEEPING
ticular devices and the motion and visual cue detail which must be
A specified when constructing a new device.
RESEARCH NEEDED
centered areas involving piloting technique for specific tasks and pilot
perception of motion and visual cues. In effect the data required are
those which would fill out the basic simulator fidelity bookkeeping ma-
Iz
pilot-centered constraints are brought together in a way which can produce
a meaningful basis of analysis. However, what confounds the execution of
this procedure is a lack of quantification in some of the pilot-centered
areas of control technique and perception. At the same time it is fea-
sible to go after these needed measurements. This can be done over the
long term using targets of opportunity or over a shorter term with delib-
erate, well-planned research programs involving both flight vehicles and
simulators.
I
|•.
'I ý
INTRODUCTION
•I NLA(ECROUND
effort has been expended in trying to answer these questions with few
1
definitive results, yet economic pressure to use training simulators in
and requirements for these perceptual aspects are not well known with 51I
3 Key, David L., (Ed.), Fidelity of Simulation for Pilot Training, AGARD
Advisory Report No. 159, October 1980.
7 .i
-. -
7--i - 'AZ
The Army Research Institute (ARI) Field Unit at Ft. Rucker, Alabama,
is engaged in conducting flight simulator research in three areas:
In recent rears, the Ft. Rucker Field Unit has been engaged in near-term
research to answer some questions in the three categories defined above.
In some cases, information is not sufficient to provide complete and ac-
curate answers, particularly in the areas of visual and motion system
fidelity.
V Roscoe, Stanley N., Halim Ozkaptan, and Aaron Hyman, Review of Flight
4 -.----
___ ___ ____
.,- ___ ~4 .-
training literature 5 does not concentrate on piloting strategy or tech-
nique, rather on systems performance (scoring, tracking dispersions,
There are,- however, established methods for dealing with the mechan-
isms of pilot behavior, especially'- on a psychomotor level. McRuer and -1
provided by AGARD Working Group 107, the objectives of this study are to
identify simulator requirements for training by using available data on
motion and visual senses in combination with maiiual control theory. There
will be no original data collection involved; rather, a review of the
9,~~:
literature and an expression of a -methodology for addressing simulator I
requirements will be presented. The specific tasks to be addressed in
this study include: I
The scope of this study includes a full range of .,my aircraft including
fixed-wing and helicopters, skill levels from undergraduate through con-
tinuation- training, both instrument and visual meteorological conditions,
and a variety of tasks including the critical scenarios involving NOE, and
weapon delivery.
TE CBNICAL APPROACH
10
feedback control theory. Pilot modeling techniques are then discussed,
first in terms of pilot control and then in terms of pilot perception.
Next, armed with compatible descriptions of fidelity, the training con-
text, and pilot behavior, we describe a procedure for studying visual and
motion stimuli. Finding that there are serious gaps in the basic experi-
mental data, however, prevents the systematic execution of this
procedure. Thus, because of the lack of experimental data, we outline a
formal bookkeeping scheme to guide our investigation of fidelity require-
ments. This leads us finally to a discussion of the kind of research
needed and the applicable. measurement methods for defining fidelity re-
quirements. Conclusions and recommendations are then drawn. As aids to
the reader, an executive summary and a glossary of terms are provided.
V1
Mr'
1- L
These two steps, taken together, provide the basis of the subsequent final
report topics.
I
With regard -to fidelity, we shall begin by reviewing some current
notions of fidelity which, in turn, will be incorporated into a special
definition of fidelity suited to our approach. This definition will then
lead us to consider parameters which are explicitly related to fidelity. 1]
It will be noted that these parameters are fundamentally separate from the
more implicit fidelity parameters commonly used to specify simulator Sys-
tem requirements. We will thus distinguish between "explicit" and
"implicit" fidelity in this section.
f 13
v ý!_4
AN OPERATIONAL DEFINITION
than how the pilot responds. This is understandable since the mechanical,
electrical, and computational specifications must ultimately be defined
Key, Ibid.
14
would correspond to a one-to-one duplication of inertial-based displace-
ments, velocities, and accelerations in each axis of freedom.
__iI To the extent that the human operator's perception can be explained inl
515
~ 1r
while learning to perform the task in the aircraft. CarterII suggests the
use of the term "error fidelity" to formalize the idea of evaluating the
fidelity of a simulator for a specific task in terms of the similarity
between the errors occurring in the simulator and those occurring in the
aircraft. This concept appears to provide an additional framework for the
systematic determination of fidelity requirements which has not been ex-
plored in past studies. According to this concept a simulator has high
error fidelity for a given task when:
16
oI
"- -, - . . -.
0 Objective fidelity
* Perceptual fidelity
* Error fidelity
Our approach is to quantify each of the above to the extent possible and
to form a model of the combined pilot-simulator which can then be compared
to a. like model of the combined pilot-aircraft." We shall explain this
approach in several steps.
-A
17
- -,*
f.q
CC
[ IK
Lo
-A 4-
4-' C'n
_r I - -
r4-
- 18
Relevant training simulator examples include the Army's UH-l requirement
12
document and UI1-60 specification1 3 .
of the pilot is accounted for in the model. Functions include the sensory
(perceptual) mechanisms, the central elements (cerebrospinal system),
neuromuscular actuation system, and the controlled element (parts of the
aircraft which are closely tied to the human operator dynamics such as the
cockpit manipulators). It is important to note that, having considered
19
I Iz
I CL
14 o
F Iz
Io w
It INo
II _Z_
L--
20 I -
the structural isomorphic model, we are able to reduce its complexity to a
lower level by using an effective system pilot model.
15e
S15 Ormsby, C. C., Model of Human Dynamic Orientation, MIT Ph.D. Thesis,
January 1974.
16 Hofmann, L. G.,
and Susan A. Riedel, Manned Engineering Flight
Simulation Validation. Part I: Simulation Requirements and Simulator
i Motion System Performance, AFFDL-TR-78-192, February 1979.
17
Hennessy, Robert T., Dennis J. Sullivan, and Herbert D. Cooles,
Critical Research Issues and Visual System Requirements for a V/STOL
Research Simulator, Canyon Research Group, Inc., Final Technical
Report No. RTH-0180 40-3053, 14 June 1979.
21
tasks 1 8 . Additional background and insights into visual perception, its
measurement, and its modeling will be discussed in the section on pilot
modeling; but Table 1 gives several examples of how motion and visual
perceptual mechanisms can be modeled for use in determining simulator
fidelity.
-
19
control theory1, the pilot tends to adapt his control behavior in a way
which complements the dynamics of the vehicle simulator or aircraft.
The specific rules for adaptation are reasonably well understood and have
been verified experimentally on many occasions. We shall dwell upon this
matter also in a subsequent section.
For now, however, we would propose that one measure of fidelity is how
well the simulator induces pilot control adaptation suitable for the ac-
tual aircraft. -This would be evident from direct measurement of features
involved in the pilot model and by analytically comparing features so
measured in a simulator to features so measured in an actual aircraft as
shown in Fig. 3 (for motion stimuli) and Fig. 4 (for visual stimuli).
There are several techniques available for measuring pilot psychomotor
behavior. A general discussion of these techniques is given
19
Memorandum, 1981.
22
~~;y
- -22•/
9
TABLE 1
2 0
0 Perception of Rotational Motion
Response of Motion
Semicircular Canals Threshold Subjective
-Velocity s Velocity
'• + 0.)0 s 1s 1 b.
(Task Dependent)
2
* Perception of Translational Motion l
Response Motion
of Utricles Threshold Subjective
Force (s + 0.08) Acceleration
tis ý0•.o2)(o.ls 1)
(Task Dependent)
ITransformation
Perception Unight
Perspective Threshold Scaling
Sub ective
Height
Actual Height, h a" p
'- h
hActual Subjective
Runway Width, w Runway
Width, wp
22
* Perception of Visual Range
Subjective
Landing Area
Size, A
21 Ibid.
23
--2 ,
-b ------
TABLE 1 (Concluded)
Subjective
Rotation Angle-to-
Angle Lateral
Perspective Perception Deviation
Transformation Threshold Scaling Subjective
S~Subjective
Actual Range, R Height, UP
Actual Depression
From Horizon, 7
and Actual Angle of
Centerline With Respect
to Vertical, v
Rotation
Angle
Perception
Threshold
Actual Perceived
Bank Angle Bank Angle
Subtended
Angle
Perception
Actal Threshold Subjective
Pitch or Pitch Attitude
S~Subjective
Scaling
• Factor, k
-i _I
23 Clement, Beffley, and Jewell,.1978, op cit.
S~24
0I w
4J 4
4r,
0 0) 0
4
0 -
0
94 0.d im
43 0.0
00
10 HO cs0
J
0..
0
14 0 0$-c
0 A Z
13
p
0V
. $J4
>.4
toE34
~ 04 0 0 04'0 4 0
40
ý42
r- 0
0 -
O
.2: 0
4 '
0z w 0
t OW0
c5 A.
C5~
34;*4
25.-0
L4I
,4)04 1
LO 43 4 4
k 04.k
o0,-
0 C) 0
0 W 0 W
U)
00
C-O>
0 1 c. to 04
A 1 ) g,4 .
- C0 0
0'0 0 W 43 0 /
z) u Id VC) U
43 04..,
2 443 - t 4 4
E $4
0) 0- k 05 0
0 ~
as 0
O0 0
> 0
0 c 0 04 4J
0 0
W '.4 .dW
0i1 rz.
< 41~ o
t
riw 0 9:
C0 C26
csi
Si
Note that-the means of viewing the simulator and the pilot, which is
described above, allows for extensive and direct quantification. Our
objective regarding fidelity is to establish a working definition which '
27
. - - - - - <1 - - - - - - - --. _ -
No'
•~I~i
inferred perception) of necessary cues. Hence we would construct a quan-
titative comparison between simulator and the actual flight situation of
the combined induced strategy and pilot perception. This frees us from
the notion that perfect fidelity is a one-to-one correspondence between
simulator systems and the actual aircraft 2 5 _ a practical impossibility
anyway. Rather, perfect fidelity is characterized by the simulator pilot
behaving in a manner appropriate to the aircraft situation. These ideas
do not, in essence, vary from the various concepts of simulator fidelity
mentioned earlier.
28
I
Second, this implies the requirement that:
- -- - 29 1
1. Can be closed with pure gain equalization by the pilot.
Given the perceptual abilities of the pilot, there are four additional
requirements regarding dynamic changes in cues corresponding to dynamic
changes in the essential feedbacks. The change in cues or states must: V
S~30
L
L V.,
I Be large enough to exceed the perceptual thresholds
(e.g., vestibular thresholds or visual acuity)
Threshold
Quickness }
Distortion
Signal-to-noise ratio
31
~~1
* States
0 Transfer relationships P
"32
- J
4 A. A
TABLE 2
SIMILATOR FIDELITY:
33
WI
-•-- ~33 •;
--------------------------
-------------------------
include attitude, airspeed, altitude, heading, and track along with their
34
C *•J."- .
fidelity parameters in that pilot perception is included in the analysis
of simulator motion.
35
E-1
rq 43
S• .- • oto-
o-4 4V
.g ILI la)
O-ý C
00
0
t-I-
o
H4
E-IH co
to a,
4--'
o0
4,,
Tr-40
CIS 01)as )
00 :->- r- :0
E2 to o0
0 _ -- 36
a a)__________
PL4 ca .
+6
+
States "
The term "state" refers to any of the variables which describe air-
craft (or simulator) operation. This would naturally include attitude,
heading, airspeed, altitude, angle of attack, engine torque, etc., along
with various flight and engine controls. "State" could also refer to. time
derivatives or integrals of each variable above.
The states which relate to fidelity are those which are involved in a
particular mission phase or task. To find these states we would consider
"a block diagram of the piloting technique loop structure implied by, say,
"a training manual task description. Examples will be considered shortly.
-- "37
[ TABLE 3
SIMULATOR-FIDELITY-RELATED STATES
-three components
(sensed states are subject to threshold,
washout, and lag)
0 TRANSLATIONAL RATES A
horizontal and vertical transverse rates
plus range rate
38
#; *-
useless, and visual motion less than the visual system resolution or the
pilot's visual acuity is likewise useless. It may be necessary to either
enhance the system (e.g., by adequate motion travel or increased visual
system resolution) or enhance the pilot's perceptual power (e.g., by pro-
viding a g-seat or corrective lenses).
39
0 Pitch and Roll Attitudes 1 rad/sec
II
additional motion or visual system delays, Hence, of the groups listed
above, attitude control would be expected to be more susceptible to a lack
of quickness in the simulator. Lateral flight path and airspeed would be
less critical.
The sources of lags and delays in simulator systems are numerous and,
to a degree, generally additive in their net effect. The effects of var-
ious sources of delay within real-time digital computing systems for
flight simulators have been examined analytically by Heffley, et al., 5
The objective of the examination was to explore useful simulation fidelity
metrics and procedures for obtaining them. Emphasis is placed therein on
digital computing systems involving two computers operating in series or
within feedback loops in which there may be several forms of delays along
with the complications arising from multirate or multiloop operation.
Particular examples of delay discussed by Heffley, et al., include the
following:
• Transport delay
• Algorithmic delay
40
- -
-- -- --.--- -IIIII__I__I__ ..... . .
* Frame slip (two or more digital processors operating at
37 Ibid.
4I
gyro. The net effect Is to reduce tne effective whole-
task time delay by about 0.15 sec (which also happens to
be the time delay incremen% required to develop a first-
order low-frequency lead)."
e41
Figue
borowedfro
6 Sinco ) shows the effective pilot time delay
versus motion drive bandwidth. Note that lack of motion response can
contribute as much as a 2:1 effect in determining effective pilot delay.
42
Baron et al. also addresses this aspect of simulator fidelity.
42
-'- -S.-,'
4-f-,
XFi.'Cd
base 0.7
0.4
I(D C.3
(sec)
~ . rad/sec'
0.1-
0 10 30 6o
(rad/sec)
43
+. . . . m+ • +, . . 1
-. -4 C
ý-simulator fidelity. As was pointed out earlier, simulator performance can
compare well with actual aircraft performance although the pilot may be
using differing control techniques or may have differing perceptions. A
better application of statistical analysis is perhaps in characterizing
how individual pilot technique, or pilot perception parameters (time or
frequency domain), are distributed for various subjects, levels of skill,
or over the period of a given task. Statistical parameters have a great .4
potential if applied to piloting technique as well as the usual leasures
of system performance. 4<
lator fidelity.
Perhaps the most prevalent example of distortion is the level of mo-F
tion washout necessary for working within a restricted motion travel. Due
to washouts, sustained motion (angular rates or specific forces) is sub-
ject to gross distortion. As a result it is also necessary to scale down
motion commands and to use gravity forces via pitch and roll angles to
simulate long term specific forces. 4 5
Sinacori addresses motion dis-
tortion in a systematic way which includes presentation of supporting
experimental results. We shall shortly discuss further experimental
results which serve to define the respective roles of motion and vis-al
modalities. ,
Visual distortion aspects appear to be far more subtle than for mo-
tion. It is fair to say that the simulator system characteristics which
contribute to visual state distortion are not well understood at this
time. It is possible, however, to infer the amount of distortion by ob- 1.
serving pilot behavior, and an example involving range perception will be
44
presented in a later section. The work of Roscoe, et al, 4 6
concerning
visual accommodation effects on apparent size and hLight should be con-
sidered with regard to the issue of distortion.
Domains
46i
46 Roscoe, Stanley N., "When Day is Done and Shadows Fall, We Miss the
Airport Most of All," Proceedings of the llth NTEC/Industry
Conference, NAVTREQtIPCEN 11-306, Naval Training Equipment Center,
Orlando, Florida, November 14-16, 1978.
45
TA BLE 4
Quickness (lag,
delay, initial
response
bandwidth)
Motion bandwidth effective
throughput, delay, effective
lag
Visual update,
effective lag Li
Distortion (shape Flatness of frequency response, Perceived range,
of response, washout time constant size, height factors
correlation among
multiple stimuli)
imp1 ies Morion travel, velocity, Field of view,
sufficient acceleration depth of field,
amplituide maximum brightness,
contrast
S~I
46
I....,.
Each of the above domains offers potential benefits in terms of conven-
ience of measurement, availability of data, and compatibility of
description between the human operator and the simulator system,
4_I %r
simulator. Carter 4 9 illustrated the hypothetical relationships between
these parameters and stressed the importance of including both parameters
in pilot training system cost-effectiveness models, noting that transfer
percent is a measure of training effectiveness while CTER is a measure of
50
training efficiency. More recently, Bickley has shown that both of
these parameters and the interaction between them can be modeled by means
48
given simulator will provide the essential cues required to permit a high
degree of transfer is, in itself, a worthwhile objective.
49
and cataloging pilot behavior in full task situations rather than in per-
forming partial tasks. Furthermore, the process of making such
measurements - both in flight and in simulators -will itself expose and
illuminate concepts of simulator fidelity. Specific suggestions for mea-
eurements will be the objective of a later section.
- -N - -
50
'i
The value of understanding the training mission and piloting task was
emphasized in the earlier section. The goal now is to develop further
that understanding in specific terms which are useful for the ultimate
objective prescribing levels of simulator fidelity needed to train.
The procedure for accomplishing the above begins with a review of the
various Army training missions and piloting tasks to be considered. This
determining and de-
will be followed by a discussion of guidelines for
scribing task loop structure. Finally we shall consider a number of
specific task analyses which pertain to Army flight training missions.
All of this will be preparatory to the subsequent section dealing with
analytic tools for describing motion and visual fidelity.
The scope of this study covers a wide spectrum of Army training mis-
sions including both rotary- and fixed-wing aircraft with the level of
training spanning undergraduate through continuation training. Of
51
LI
51 Anon., Aircrew Training Manual. Utility Helicopter, Department of the
Army, TC 1-135, October 1978.
52
77ill
( TABLE 5
APrROACH/LANDING EMERGENCIES
53
'.- * *-
Army rotary-wing pilots 5 4 and, for more specialized tasks, the helicopter
gunnery manuals 5 5 and the NOE task analyses of Gainer and Sullivan5 6 .
The major groupings for Table 5 imply either a common piloting loop
structure (as in cases of basic flight or hovering tasks), a class of
maneuvers (such as takeoff and approach and landing), a basic environment
(low altitude operations or weapon delivery), or a basic limitation on
modality of pilot information (as in instrument flight tasks). This
classification system is not a rigid, well-defined form; but it is a com-
plete and convenient checklist of flight training objectives to be
considered in our analyses.
4 54
Anon., Program of Instruction for 2C-15A/2C-10OB-B, Officer/WO Rotary
Wing Aviator Course, U. S. Army Aviation Center, Ft. Rucker, Alabama,
SSI/MOS: 15A/100B, February 1978.
54
TABLE 6
55
14[I
TABLE 6 (Continued)
state also)
56
IY
TABLE 6 (Concluded)
Pursuit Level Techniques
(Feedforward loop structure involving coitrols,
states, errors, and disturbances)
I A. Pitch
avoidance, etc.)
B. Roll
C. Yaw
B. Lateral
A. Speed
l."Pitch-to-Throttle" Crossfeed (frontside operation - to
counter speed change)
B. Engine State
C. Symmetry
57
i-1
A review of the existing manual control literature reveals that a
quantification in terms of piloting tasks and piloting technique is lack-
ing. Where pilot techn'que has been directly measured in manual control
theory ten,'s, it has generally been associated with: (a) inner loop pi-
loting task'.. such as regulation of pitch and/or roll attitude, by means of
their respective controls; (b) simultaneous roll attitude and yaw rate
regulation by means of two controls57; (c) inner- and outer-loop regula-
tion of height by means of a single control 58
; or (d) inner and outer loop
*regulation of speed and position by means of a single control9, al based
* on instrument flight techniques. 60
A few measurements have been taken for
* an overall piloting task in which inner loops, euch as attitudes, are used
in direct support of outer loops, such as position, altitude, and head-
ing. Thus, even though the inner loop measurements which do exist are
* widely applicable to many of the training missions and piloting tasks
listed in Table 6, we lack the important quantification of the outer loop
behavior, especially in critical tasks such as nap-of-the-earth operation,
approach and landing, or air-to-air combat; and these are areas where
58
__ _ __ t
i
defhintion of visual and motion simulator fidelity requirements is prob-
ably of most interest.
Let us now consider a simple basic set of guidelines and rules for
describing task loop structure for a given statement of the task and some
understanding of the vehicle dynamics and operating environment. We shall
gain from this a useful point of perspective for viewing the specific task
analyses to be presented in the subsequent subsection and beyond that, to
lead into the discussion of analytic tools for describing fidelity in the
next section.
Simply stated, the primary rationale for describing task loop struc-
ture is first to define the overall piloting objective and, second, to
discover how the piloting objective might be successfully executed working
within (a) the constraints of the information available, (b) the aircraft
dynamics, and (c) the workload limitations of the pilot. In general, to
satisfy all of these constraints, the e'cecution of a primary task objec-
tive requires the use of a multiloop structure for which there are nested
intermediate loops (series loop structure) accompanied by a similar set of
nested loops for each additional axis of control (parallel loop struc-
ture). Samples of this general structure are shown in Figs. 7 through
11. Figure 7 shows an acceleration/deceleration maneuver in a utility
helicopter. The basis for this control structure is the task description
599
"M- -
=
1 4j
> ?I
-4
45.
"04
0 r-
00
-I
60-
zii
-~ j ~---~-"--~
A
-X * . -U
- -;-
~~~~~rn
0I
o0 0o~ 000
0.
0 > v
CL~
V) 0
$.4
CL
.5.
V4
0
15-
0 0
d .4-d
100
Z1.4
go
-z) 00
"J
-2-2 -
Li L 0
.4~ ~~~ Ld
.
-a
v5Ii05
E
61 o 0 L
oc
z$4
0~
0
CC I
a 00
- 40 -
4J.
to
I 0
I~I-
Il o
p zz.
zz-4
445 t- ZA z '. .4 ;r
a -.
~
.4 - 0 6 2 . >
- I
-l
o
E!-1
000
ci 5-
41
( bD
100
SAS
IL'
63
412
:r4•
I to
Li i °
* .1-i
0
o ii
** - * W 0
-• * cc
• o0
F4
cc
N.-I
~I-IE-I6:
__.......___________________________________
64•
• I
64
provided for Army aviators in the aircrew training manual. Figure 8
similarly describes the pilot-vehicle loop structure which can be inferred
from a literal interpretation of another training manual task descrip-
6
tion. 5 In this case an approach to hover is considered. Note that t;he
structure is similar to the previous case except that range perception is
involved and manual control of rotor rpm is addressed. Figure 9 shows the
inferred loop structure for a straight climb on instruments 6 6 which is
equally applicable to both helicopters and fixed-wing aircraft. Addi-
tional examples of common loop structure are shown in Figs. 10 and 11 for
helicopter hovering and fixed-wing frontside operation, respectively.
In each of the above examples only the general loop structure is de-
scribed - there Is no quantification of the various pilot or vehicle
elements. Quantification is required, however, if these diagrams are to
be used for determination of simulator fidelity requirements. Pilot-
vehicle loop structure can be quantified by direct measurement of piloting
technique and aircraft response; however, estimates can be made by direct
literal interpretation of pilot training manual descriptions. 67 Some
examples are shown below.
tJ
i
Literature, AIAA Paper No. 81-1822, 1981.
65
To illustrate the concept of flight task quantification, consider a
simple example - heading regulation during cruising flight. The in-
structions for executing a small amplitude level turn are:
This implies the following piloting technique relating bank angle command,
ý0 to heading error, le:
ýc =•e
(Commanded or (Heading
Desired Error) (Commanded
Heading) + *e Bank Angle)
4m (Measured Heading)
mI
Note that this piloting technique is not dependent upon vehicle dynamics
or flight condition; it is invariant.
Combining the above piloting technique with the vehicle dynamics in-
volved in turns (i.e., turn rate, ', equals bank angle, 4, times the
kinematic ratio of gravity acceleration to airspeed, g/U) we obtain the
68 Anon., Instrument Flying, Air Force Manual, AFM 51-37, 15 August 1979.
66
-71
Studying the errors in the performance of actual flight tasks can also
be a useful technique in identifying cues used by the pilot. more spe-
cifically, errors frequently occurring in the simulator which never or
rarely ever occur in the aircraft can lead to the identification of real
world cues to correct performance that is missing or distorted in the
simulator. For example, the tendency of students to always pull up late
in a Northrop simulation of the F-4J barrel roll attack was traced to a
probable target minification effect, apparently caused by the eye's ten-
dency to return to a resting accommodation in the absence of textural
detail, as desribed70
as described by Roscoe0. Conversely, the faf ture of a simulator
67
-~ N'
IA
Z 4-') W
44J
CH !d
0 to
oW
mJ 0
4' -Uq
0H
00
41
:ii
cicis
om o ctj csj4
0'~ CJS ) 4t
4
0\ -4O
0 O'\*
0- PA 04V
~d0 0 3
t
00
bo 0~O 4' r
4-') ;9 0
4-)'
+2IL me~ 0o
Uq
r 0 0
68i
W M WO"
to induce an error which normally occurs in the aircraft can help pinpoint
artifactual cues which do not exist in the real world or to identify mis-
sing or inadequately simulated distracting cues and workload factors which
make the task unrealistically easy in the simulator in order to correct
; performance.
Important clues to how piloting technique might be adjusted in terms
69
r - --
Verbal Description
70
___________________________
77-7
Verbal Description7 1
• jI, I._ T 1 1L
Des LUiL-------
S71 Anon.,
_1
"71
SFliht T!ask
Turn:i Technique
,,
then `roout
•~ o.,
I
1
+
-o,,i
s I
| "I
1
5
I' I
;F~llul.'Headin~g
il
e dg
seadingI
72 Ibid, p. 2-13.
72
Verbal Description
Stick Deflection
Stick Force Elevator Deflection
_ _ _g S Flight Task
a(Longitudinal 12 Swasbplato)
. . [.'IC
I * V
O+ Oe OU1T~s)e- s K
j tfue I 1 /-j e _ _1 _ ..
Desired s)kIto-surtual
°" 73
+ • + +~~ -- - - + _•- - . +• +
7
Verbal Description 3
/
Compensation gain
_ - / _
Ibid, p. 2-11.
74
Verbal Description'74
Before entering the climb or descent, decide on a power setting and estimate the amount of
pitch attitude change required to maintain the airspeed. Normally, the pitch and power changes
are made simultaneously.
The power change should be smooth, uninterrupted, and at a rate comensurate with the rate
of pitch change. In some aircraft, even though a constant throttle setting is maintained, the
power may change with altitude. 1herefore, it may be necessary to occasionally cross-check the
power indicator(s).
While the power is being changed, rcfer to the attitude indicator and smoothly accomplish
the estimated pitch change. Since smooth, slow power applications will also produce pitch
changes, only slight control p:essures are needed to establish the pitch change. Additionally,
very little trim change is raquired since the airspeed is constant. With a moderate amount of
will remain
practice, the pitch and power changes can be properly coordinated so the airspeed
within close limits as the climb or descent is entered.
Ten
Upon approaching the desired altitude, select a predetermined leveloff lead point.
percent of the vertical velocity in feet is a good estimate for the level-off lead point. At
the level-off lead point, smoothly adjust the power to an approximate setting required for level
flight and simultanecusly change the pitch attitude to maintain the desired altitude.
Piloting Technique
c 5O~sgn(b)
•cc
airb/P-VInnerLoops Vebicle Outer Loop
descent*,-I- - 1
If~~h~5Ott desce..11
them leveloff 1 +Tlj - J•i S A.ld
t-, If :hJ<--
eOf
Altua ft i !~ - I . II I A
L-------------------------.. ..- _
Compensatoryr levcloff
based cn smoth tnsiton Effective vertical velocity lag
o cimb/descent T 2 sec
Decision to h
start leveloff
= climb/descent
fmAec
I7 Ibid, p. 2-11.
75
i,
Verbal Description
75
h~~ II . .SActual
Hegh '2 Height
76
77-1
Verbal Description
Flight Ta-k
P-V ._ er Loop Vehicle!Outer Loop
1
| _ otITeeI.ni ue
. ,__
U + j
1T
_]
1sec
s 0
_____
1
+/TI I S e pi ,
U
Pilot Co=pensat.-on
1z =.o - xg e
(efciv-uerlo Efecie=
Ef.0(1c ic Lage
K,.3-)u!e Speed Damping
Xu= .020I I-iU
(effective outer loop
crossover frequency
.07 rad/sec)
Crossfeed to
collective
"control
77
- 'a
Verbal Description
7
Feedback Control Description 7
Desired R**2~ I i + Te -T
R Pilot Perception
I f
1
Perceived1+ /
Range
"a 3
78
r-
-a- . -- - - - -
7i
Verbal Descriptin
-rapidly
•' rotor) orasexcessive
possible without ground contact
increase in height (of the tail
--
Desre Acts _I
(Ix;•.reieIntrlo
Raage cL anthe d Ra
I deg/rt SpCcZ Ccu:pensation I
Ku = deg/kt zffective
Pitch Attitude Lrg
T9 =.3 se
(Im-~ol~ms precise control of
Though not shown, the piloting attitude and is critical to
technique for controflng height success of Lr.ný=er,)
is as crucial as the deceleration
per SO.
79
- - - -- _________________-----------~---------~-'----I-
__. _____..... _______________I . . . .. ..... ..
PILOT MODELING TECHNIQUES
Pilot control refers not only to the general piloting technique (as I
discussed already) but also ty the adjustment of compensation, selection
of available cue information, and adoption of higher levels of control" •-1
organization. Pilot control can involve a whole "bag of tricks" which is
aimed at accomplishing the given task with adequate performance and rea-
sonable workload. The -topics which are discussed in- the following pages
K
will touch, upon some of the more important ideas of pilot control and
bring us to a point at which perceptual effects can be addressed.
human
linear,
psychomotor
sometimes
and cognitive
80
UAI
tions is that simple models frequently describe pilot behavior within the
limits of measurement. Further the same simple models follow the common-
sense dictates of good control design, minimization of effort, economy of
essential information transfer, and realization of acceptable (but not
necessarily optimal) performance to accomplish the task objectives.
81
system lag which is related to aveiage intervals of discrete activity
(e.g., the discrete behavior of a digital computer can often be accounted
for using a first-order lag having a time constant equal to the computer
frame time). It is also feasible to treat the human controller as a low-
rate sample data system for low frequency outer loops such as control of
airspeed with throttle. 8 0
-83
PF~
84
- . -2
0 0
0 E
lb c +
I
0
W
+
44,
CC
0 E-0
C ~Cc
0
o
IL H
Cs 0
0 0
* "0
CA % CO
rw T
0+
020
E --
P4
U00
00
2-
- .1-4
86
! model expresses the human operator's adaptive control (motor) behavior as
an optimal controller which makes use of all system states and controls in
such a way as to minimize some form of cost function. Those state var-
iables which are assumed to be perceived are operated on by an optimal
estimation process (Kalman filter) in order to generate the needed states
for the control process.
Three areas of difficulty of the modern control theory algorithmic :
83
model approach regarding psychomotor behavior are given in McRuer
These are, briefly stated:
83 Ibid.
87
Nevertheless algorithmic models may be appropriate y and successfully
applied to represent the human operator's cognitive perceptual organizing
and activity-supervising processes. The process is the Successive Organi-
zation of Perception 84
(SOP) developed by Krendel and McRuer and McRuer,
86
et al. ,85 and summarized in McRuer and Krende1 . Most of the observed
"continuous" manual control behavior falls into relatively few arche-
typical categories from which logical criteria zan be employed to select
the most appropriate organizational category, given a task, an environ-
ment, and the circumstances attending the operator. One model for the
perceptual organization process would be an active off-line supervisory
monitor which identifies the conditions that currently exist, selects and
activates some most appropriate organizational structures for behavior,
monitors the result, and reselects a new or modified- organizational struc-
ture when necessary or when further information is identified as a result Y
of the initial operations. Appropriately this has been termed the meta-
87
coto 88
control system8. A simplified diagram of such a metacontroller is
given in Fig. 25. Other preliminary work on algorithmic models for these
cognitive perceptual organizing and activity-supervising processes are
89
given in McRtier, et a1 . Thus algorithmic models should be used where
they are best suited (for logical functions), while classical isomorphic
structural models are used where they are most efficient (for well-defined
184
88 T:
i•-
SELECTION OF APPROPRIATEPUT-
PATHWAY(S)
'II
SELECTION OF
RESPON{SE
UNIT FROM
REPERTOIRE
'I1
[EXECUT.ION
S~OF:RESPONSE
1
-• OFF-LINE SUPERVISOR;
MONITOR RESULTS,
, I
89
1
continuous tracking or stimulus-response situations which are subject to
divided attention).
The firsc ae. of raquirements comprises those whicn depend upon the
task being performed and are independent of the fact that the pilot is
human. Aircraft angular and linear motions are sensed and used in closed H
feedback loops to develop, through appropriate weighting and equalization,
the throttle, control stick, pedal, and collective (if the aircraft is a
helicopter) deflections that guide and control the aircraft. The choice
of which aircraft motion quantities to use and what their relative
weighting and equalization must be- to achieve system stability and path
following capability are all guidance and control requirements which would
, •'exist even if the pilot were to be replaced by a complex and elaborate
machine.
The second set of requirements comprises those--which arise because the
90
S1
certain limitations- he can perform only a limited number of tasks at
the same time. The kind and degree of equalization applied to the
perceived vehicle motions is likewise limited. Further his control
activity is not completely correlated with the aircraft motions - it
contains uncorrelated activity- noise to the control engineer, remnant
to the human performance researcher. In short the pilot-centered
requirements are largely those which are imposed by human limitations9 0 .
90:
90 Exceptions are those pilot-centered requirements which are imposed by
human capabilities such as adaptation and learning, both of which are
at present hardly imitated by machines in their infancy. Learning
expresses a human organizational capability in successive encounters
with the same environment, whereas adaptation expresses a human
organizational capability in an encounter with a new environment.
91 Gordon, D. A., "Perceptual Basis of Vehicular Guidance," Public Roads,
34, 1966, pp. 53-68.
91
thereby relieving the pilot of this burden by matching, in a limited
sense, the requirements of the task to the pilot's capabilities. At the
same time this change In the display might obscure from the pilot that
which he calls status or situation information. In this example the state
of the motion variable is obscured to an extent depending upon the quick-
ening added to the signal.
trates most of the concepts involved. For the moment we shall ignore the
vestibular aspects and consider visually-perceived information only.
92
I 4
The term describing function comes from automatic control theory. De-
scribing functions are used to express the linear properties of nonlinear J
13
elements. The term's use in the context of hunan response description
serves to emphasize that a complex, nonlinear element is being approxi-
mated linearly for purposes of quantification in the particular situation.
Croscover Model
~ &93
mw~
CL
YpYc
yy s c- z
(1)
S|
in the vicinity of the gain crossover frequency, wc' for that loop 9 3 This
form should be more correctly written to include a pure time delay, Te)
which represents the pilot's effective neuromuscular lag as well as any
high frequency vehicle response lags.
To accomplish this end result, the pilot must tailor his describing
function, Yp, to the specifics of the control situation. Y is expres-
I: p
sible in terms of the following parameters:
94
r
the least and Te the greatest when the pilot must generate low-frequency
lead. In fact the major "cost" of equalization is this increase in time
delay. Both Te and wc will vary with pilot skill and level of attention
to the control task. Experimentally-based values for the crossover model
parameters for several dynamic forms and input bandwidths are given in
94
McRuer and Krendel as well as elsewhere.
The complete pilot description includes the remnant. The major
A
The simple crossover model given above is diagrammed as shown in
Fig. 26. It describes pilot behavior in single-loop compensatory control
"tasks and in the outermost loop of multiloop compensatory -tasks. In this
system structure, the pilot is controlling a single task-related response
variable with a single control by operating only on the perceived error.
•, A typical example of such behavior would be regulation of vehicle attitude
Ibid.
95 -
TIM
ControlledIj
Pilot Element
I-i. *1
rv
Figrure 26. Compensatory Pilot Model
It
tii 96
motion). The pilot cannot preview what is coming and can only respond to
the developing error as it becomes manifest on his displays or in the
simulated visual scene.
The data base which quantifies the single loop compensatory pilot
model consists primarily of describing function measurements of well-
97
Figure 27 is a simplified diagram of the pursuit model, so-called be-
cause it typifies the classic situation where target motion (the input
command) is perceived as separate and distinct from the aiming error. If
ouite familiar with the response properties of the controlled element (his
own aircraft), he can operate only on this input and generate control
activity which will result in responses having very little error. In
effect his describing function (operating only on the command) is given by
1/Y. such that YPiYcSY~1. At the same time he will respond to the
residual errors with the appropriate compensatory behavior, represented in
Fig. 27 by Y
98
__ __ __ _
Pilot
{ Controlled,
S~Element
1"1
Command + 1 Error CotorRsos
II
99
- -S-99-
equalization the effect of the motion is to reduce the pilot time delay
and increase the available crossover frequency. The continuous task
performance is thereby improved (increasing crossover frequency, W., means
At this point let us make two observations which relate to the aboveI
pursuit-level symptoms. First, optimal control pilot models, when used to
97
predict pilot behavior such as described by Curry, et al., , tend to
yield a pursuit-level describing function signature. Second, the role of
cockpit motion discussed by Hess98, whether predicted by optimal or "[
classical methods, tends to be related to low frequency pursuit behavior
and not to basic compensatory actions at or near the crossover frequency.
100
-7j
4 0r - COMPENSATORY
--- PURSUIT
20
>- 0
-201I Ii i
1'; -.100
-200
-300
-400j.
-500
.1 1 10
w..,radls
Ibid.
101 -
Finally, the highest level of psychomotor skill is described by the
precognitive model. This model represents those skilled control activi-
ties which resemble preprogrammed or self-generated behavior. The pilot
acts as though the only information needed, presuming full familiarity
with the input command and the controlled element, is the triggering
stimulus which tells him when to begin and his sensations of his control
movements.
102
a. Physical Model for a Typical Perceptual Channel Showing Separation
of Display, Perceptual, and Equalizing Pareme-ters
c~~-PERCEPTION
DISLA EQUALIZATION
Yd()Cotinpulrg DcIk~' -
PAR~AM.ETER
RSeqc
(Parcmielcr~s c/cpcncl ota J;5?6y charneferstac,. actn parafovcal oingle)Clcac.s
ractims Delcciion Process Delays I-C Control C-aeiuas K (4)
Lo~qs S4ai;c, 0 Dyramwijk Art.41 ThrelcsI-r-, A Cornuirfin5 Delays -re~f)
Resolui~orn (QoaMIZ04ion Blur 4 Saiurofioni Limits, B Processin.g Noisr and
Lu~aieObservcttiok1 ?4osc 'rime V~ir.yinq Poroameicrs
S Rollo
Dsl~r.er.Aýi htMc~
Foveal LUný%s
Nlonc.et 1RTl:6IAL(Le. 9j
Yh (A,;
Percraved
S'I.ates
I Pzcpual
Remnant
Penceptuol
0-:r~b~ncS FUICtiDoa
'klivsResidusal
Pe~rbVnC RAGOai Central ahcA NevromuLsculatr
I(Threskliz. LrWaili) (Lca4*,jLgs.,eC.) Tvrar:.3.?WiS.SIci
103
understood at present. But the lack of determinism in portions of the
human operator models under certain conditions is reflected by correspond-
ing variations in the stochastic "remnant" (e.g., observation and motor
noise). The "remnant" forms an-essential part of the contemporary, quasi-
linear pilot models. It is attributable to time-varying time delays,
When passed around a tracking control loop, the high frequency noise
components in the control signal are usually attenuated by the controlled
element, and any quasi-periodic components are dispersed, so that the
resulting perceptual remnant appears broadband and has stochastically
stationary properties. This fact permiits a great economy of computa-
iton. Because most of the display and perceptual effects tend to have
f,'equency invariant describing functions, and because we can determine
only overall effects from any one measurement, it is customary to lump
these perceptual remnant effects with the operator's adjustable state-
~4
104
__________ _____________________
weightings (e.g., rate, displacement, integral) so as to -yield an
effective set of gains for the operator as shown in Fig. 29b. For our
purposes here, however, we will keep the perceptual describing function
and remnant separated from the equalization, so as to permit a closer tie-
in with associated displayed variables; and because we plan to include -the
pilot's scanning, sampling, and- reconstruction effects on the remnant as
well.
1
PERCEPTUAL MODELING
Notion Perception
below the threshold level will not be perceived by the pilot. There are
two types of thresholds modeled here. The first is a sensory threshold,
105
it arises from physical limitations of the organ itself. This zhreshold
type is evident for the semi-circular canals they are unable to sense
angular velocities of magnitude less than 0.035 rad/sec1 02
.
106
The perceptual model includes the sensory threshold for angular
velocity and the indifference threshold for specific force in the appro-
priate paths. Figure 30 depicts the model used to implement these
thresholds as a mathematical functions. Also the threshold level for each
axis is indicated.
[ An alternate,
perception is
but indirect, method of treating thresholds in motion
to use a "divided attention" model of the pilot's sensors.
This technique is involved in the optimal control model employed by Baron,
et al., 105 and Levison and Junker 0 to match rms performance of experi-
mental data obtained from motion simulators. The technique assumes that
the pilot spends fractions of time sampling various sensors (e.g., visual,
vestibular, tactile). These time fractions are then used to scale noise
models of the sensor outputs, and thus to predict rms performance. The
method is indirect in that rantlom noise alone is not a sufficient model
for a threshold either physically or mathematically. It is also not clear
how one systematically sets the values for the various time fractions.
-1,.-I0"
#2
2 "PT
Up
T
PT PT
2
T(Ps) P PT sin (p /PT)
AXIS
Pitch
THRESHOLD VALUE
2 deg/sec
K
Surge 0.1 g
Roll
Sway 2 deg/sec
0.1 g
Yaw 2 deg/sec
Heave 0.0 g
108
It is convenient to categorize the kinds of flight motions in three
The first comprises those low level, typically high frequency motions
which are not manifest in visual displays or simulations of an outside
visual scene, and which make modest demands on linear motion base
travel. These motions provide a "background" and contribute a sense of
realism to the simulated task but are not used in the closed loop control K
sense. They typically are related to the simulated flight environment:
rotor slap, buffet, very low level turbulence (ignorable from the stand-
point of control), etc. These motions may be used intermittently as
indicators of the simulated flight condition. In any event the fidelity
109
requirements are relatively eacily met. Because visual display cor-
110
simulator. The model is based upon data taken for well trained pilot
subjects performing in continuous control tasks. It is somewhat less
* suitable for predicting adaptations requiied for the performance of
d iscrete maneuvers; although, even here, the same modeling principles can
be applied for analysis - only the data base is deficient.
L * Pilot background
Plight
M Task (including controlled element dynamics).
__ _ __ III _ __II
A suggested task for such a research program is that of hovering a
Visual Perception
112
were presented by GibsonIII in narrative form with propositions, axioms,
and geometrical graphics which can be tested experimentally. Apparently
motivated in part by a concurrent (circa 1950) survey of the status of
12
research in visual perception by GrahamI , Gilinsky1 13
developed and
validated a quantitative formulation of visual size and distance percep-
tion. Gordon discussed human space perception -mathematically in the
"context of the environmental geometry around a moving eye114 and set forth
the perceptual -basis of vehicular guidanceI15 in the vicinity of fhe
16
ground plane. RoscoeI has more recently addressed size perception as a
function )f visual accommodation and found that, in general, eyes focus
only well enough for the required discrimination.
113
"Raish'1 9 , and Grtn-aI And Merhav 1 2 0 These visual cues include, among
others, apparent motion of single points and apparent size, orientation,
and motion of groups of points which may be fixed in the external pan-
orama, but which appear framed by the windshield boundary; perspective
distortions of known geometrical shapes on the ground; perceived rates of
change of lineal and areal dimensions of individual objects and their
differences from the corresponding rates of change of their apparent.back-
ground; and perceived changes in uniform ground textures.
Notion Perspective
The apparent motion of single points which are fixed in inertial space
gives rise to the "streamers" 1 2 1 of point images, and the apparent notion
of inertially-fixed point sets gives rise to the phenomenon of "motion
pese lie"22 123.
perspective,1 created by the streamers of the point set
S123 Ibid.
114r
£r to true range S~related
with the aid of a characteristic measure of
S R
S R 1 + R/A
115
- ---
closure rate, vertical velocity, and relative angular velocity of the line
of sight, respectively.
Streamers offer the observer the means to discern the point toward
which a vehicle is moving (aiming point) from a two-dimensional abstrac-
tion of the outside world as projected on the retina. The probable basis
for the perception of visual directional information is the expansion
pattern of the panorama about the "fixed point" toward which the aircraft
is moving. In the parafoveal streamer theory1 2 5 directional information
is obtained by the observer, in motion, by making use of the objects in
the field of view which appear to move along paths radiating from the
fixed point called the "focus of expansion" in the perspective. nhe
vectors tangent to these paths are the "streamers," and the pattern formed
116
1-4
z
aA
to I.
C C -, , ..
0. 4 *0 -L v'
41 0. .4 0 0 - :ý -,
S 0t0 - C6a C Vc I
E- 0.~ 1 C . .- . C ~ C 0
.4IL 5. to5.4
t4 9C W C -C
117H
* TABLE 8a.
VISUAL FIELD INFORMATION REQUIREM~ENTS: P(6SITION
x C ~ees~az for
FPositio
U
Lon--g
al
Positio _________
AT-tu-
be
-
e
eW; aqr
_____
SO~~
a
.h
(0
Ig
S
________landing_
-x
apprzazh vzd-
ance, se
z:ia
ly at IOU
speeda, in
hovering, and
% recessaty7 for
be R. pproach,
-- 0-15Tbabr
beb Izhovering and
launding guid-uJ
lvert~al 66~
0. or CO 2&6 wher 0aviergu atdg idacel ln
Position B.x ;qArgCo 70
1(o)
orluin bT '6.6 gP Cos 78e6
A -0go.6 - N , - e TL
ST'
118c-s LI-f-
- ~ 7:;:C;~>-p'...~ -
4-
64
v, 4,
.4' vi v~
2 S O
x
to 0 w
E-4
0~rd
E4
0 L
0 ~/0
J44
H 0
8450
>~ 4,4'
-4.
' C
g- 0,
00 ý 9. "'-4
0
Ut
4,
N 0
VC CV
~ 4
0 r-04
0 "
0 4, 4
ao oj
-. Ns
I:4,
dD
,,4,
Z.4 4,>
4,1 , v
4 Q4a
Q 4
41 w~
c0 4
__
___ __
119
ft 141
Ci)
&-4
CC
1
rj0 cu
-0
I
o".30
c t*
10
I.,~~~ CC CC11-~
-- 9
v 0., C1 .0 Z.
'-40
a..-
01
f VIf itffII
0 I 11 c
zz
>u IC
1-I C. *120
by them is the point toward which the observer is moving. Sometimes this
point can be observed directly as when the observer's speed is high,
weather is clear, and objects are close. Otherwise the location of the
point must be inferred by extrapolation of the streamer pattern character-
istics of various types of objects and ground texture.
S. Ringland, et al., 126
presents an analysis showing how the interpre-
tation of motion perspective geometry will enable the observer to
anticipate changes in the future course of his motion. When present and
recognized, these essential visual elements from motion perspective, in
turn, will enable an observant cont-ller to provide first- and second-
order visual lead compensation127 of his controlled element without the
customary intensive psychomotor workload which accompanies visual antici-
Pattern Recognition
126 Ringland, Robert F., Warren F. Clement, and Henry R. Jex, Factors in
Determining Visual and Motion Fidellt- Requirements for Trainins.
Simulators, Systems Technology, Inc., Working Paper No. 1162-4,
December 1980, Revised March 1981.
127 Second order lead compensation here is akin to the adoption of
pursuit behavior measured in: Allen, R. Wade, and Duane McRuer, The
Man/Machine Control Interface - Pursuit Control, Systems Technology,
Inc., Paper No. 243, April 1979.
121
j$ . . - 5oS -g
_4
1) Memorizing a pattern i. the process of constructing 1) Closed cyclic nature of feature network.
oan Internal representation of the pattern In.memory,
"
in the form of a sequential feature network. a
closed network of memory traces recording the fee-
tures of the pattern and the attention shifts
required to pass from feature to feature across Zhe
visual field.
5) The directed ;sture of the matching process (note in 5) Consistent with Fackvorth's fings that
4) sa the key to the recornition of patterns in the visual noise causes tunnel vision *
6)
presence of noise and clutter. The feature network
directs attention to the features of the pattern,
while avoiding the noise and clutter.
Memorizing and recognizing a pattern are seen to be 6) Consistent with succe s i ve organization of
U
closely analogous to memorizing and repeating a perception (SOP) theory
conventional sequence of behavior, each being an
alternating sequence of sensory and motor
activities.
8) Under conditions in which attention shifts must take 8) Contrast these findings with the apparent lack
the form of eye movements, the development of the of determinism in Instrument scanning under
scan-path during memor~zation of a pattern has been IFR reported by Fitts, et al.; Veir aTIn6,
1 eln;
experimentally demonstrated. Its use In subsequent Clement, et al., and Clement, et al.,3 end
recognition awaits co.afirmation. their antecedents. L
129 Sanders, A. F., 'Some Aspects of the Selective Process In the Functional "leual Field,. Ergonomics, 13,
1970, pp. 101-117.
130 Sperling, C.. 'Successive Approximations to a Model for Sh-vt Term Memory,' Act& Psy-hologic, 13.,1970.
131 Ihcktorth, N. R., -Visual Noise Causes Tunnel Visior., Psychone'jrological Sýiences, 3_ 1965, pp. 67 and 68.
Clement, W. F., at 4l., Aplication of lknual Control Teor, to the Development of P1ight Director
viThlaw
5,mtrms for STO0. Aircraft, fart 11: )W/ti-As Sampllng, P!Ilot Wtrkload, and Diploy Integration- System
-thnology, Inc., lhchnlcel isport No. 1011-2, 1974.
COemnt, U. F., at al., A Meesmination of Pilot Ee -Movement Data, Systems Technology. Inc., Technical
Memorandum No. 263-A, iev. 28 February 1967.
122
I
"Noton134 together with some remarks and connections with the SOP theory
and other findings which have been found to be useful in characterizing
visual perception and cognitive activities. In terms of the successive
organization of perception theory 135 and the sequential theory of pattern
perception in Noton1 3 6 , the experienced pilot is probably always operating
at least at the pursuit level of pattern perception. At the pursuit level
of pattern perception, it may be reasonable to assume that recognition is
conditioned only upon detection with fairly high probability, say 0.95,
and insignificant additional time delay over that already accounted for in
the effective perceptual-motor delay, Tel of the crossover model for the
A; compensatory level of control, unless head movements are required to scan
the functional visual field as defined, for example, by Sanders' selective
process 137
In contradistinction, the student pilot may not yet have memorized all
ofthe changing geometric patterns in the visual elements which are neces-
A4 123
•-i
Sanders has shown in a decade of visual research at the TNO in the
13
Netherlands • that a "scanning controller" function exists which directs
the motibns of the eye and head to points of interest in the visual
i ,
138
138 Ibid.
124
140.
saccades.140 Consequently our divided or
intermittent attention models
141
for perception , coupled with GilirF~ky's models for the perception of
size and distance 1 4 2 and streamer theory1 4 3 , should be adequate for
application in the guidance and control Analysis for the present study.
S140
The details oi the intentional movement dynamics of both the head and
• eye are fairly well known and are quite complex in detail (e.g.,,
Ibid). However, because the saccades are rapid (less than 0.06 sec
for eye-only motionr), for purposes of analysis and evaluation the
effect can be represented by a delay in perception of displayed
• &tates. This delay can be lumped with a typical detection delay of
•:• About 0.02 sec.
S142Gii-insky,."Perceived
Size and Distance in Vir~ua! Space," 1951,
Op cit.
• Gilinsky, "The Effect of" Attituie Upon the Perception of Size," 1955,
op cit.
Goron 1965, op
ct.
S•
144Middleton, W. E. Knowles, "Vision Through the Atmosphere,2' Canada:
S• 1'952.
University of Toronto,
? :12'5
Visibility Effects
Allen and McRuer 1 4 8 have coupled the modified Koschmeider theory with
the modified Blackwell-Davies contrast thresholds149 in studies of how the
145 5Ibd.
C - Cexp (-R- XR .)
where CO = inherent source or target contrast
So
W Koschmeider's meteorological extinction coefficient
X - backsca&tering coefficient
226
visual segt.'et can affect perceptual cues for guidance and control of
surface vehicles. The same technical approach ha. been applied by Clement
1 50
and Heffley to estimate the minimum effective visual range for guidance
and ccntrol of VTOL operations at night.
15 1
Jewell, et al., describe a simple method for measuring and comr-
paring, in the closed-loop context involving the human operator, various
techniques of obtaining computer-generated images (CGI). The proposed
method is independent of the manufacturers' hardware and software specifi-
cations and allows different CGIs to be compared in situ on an absolute
scale as well as back to back. Furthermore the method also offers a ra-
tional means for evaluating hardware and/or software changes to extant CGI
systems and to their host computation systems which provide the aircraft
mathematical model. For exa=ple a carefully designed experiment using the
method described in Jewell, et al,152 can reveal how the effects of asyn-
chronous data transfer and lead prediction or 9moothing compensation in
the CGI software affect the performance of the human operator in accom-
plishing a specific training objective.
111
S150 Clement, Warren F., and Robert K. Heffley, Some Effects of Adverse
* Visibility on Threshold Properties of the Pilot's Perception in VTOL
Approaches to Non-Aviation Ships, Systems Technology, Inc., Working
Paper No. 1115-3, May 1978.
151 Jewell, Wayne F., and Warren F. Clement, A Simple Method for
Measuring the Effective Delay in Simulator Digital Computing Systems
Involving Manual Control, Systems Technology, Inc., Working Paper
127
means of calculating the relationships between the geometric pattern
details and the quality of state information used by the pilot. Care must
be exercised, however, to insure that the "optimal" solutibns really do
reflect correct or realistic piloting technique and task performance, It
may be advisable to simply use such methods to establish likely starting
points for pilot-vehicle models which are then studied in order to reveal
the fundamental first-principles effects. The concern is that the mathe-
matical complexity usually associated with optimal control and estimation
procedures might cloud the insight necessary to Identify basic cause-
effect relationships. (These same comments also apply to use of optiral
control techniques in conjunction with motion fidelity.)
156 See Roberts, Edward 0., External Visibility Criteria for VTOL
Aircraft, AFFDL-TR-67-27, March 1967, for the method of
representation.
157 A geometric construction for inferring the approximate location of
the moving aiming point is illustrated in Fig. 32. Notice that the
aiming point will always pursue the pad from a shallower depression
angle, i.e., 'from "above" the apparent pad. until arrival over the
spot ior hover.
128
g-*- - -~
Polar picture plane representation of forward hemispheric field of view
-Irate of descent
=
Zenith' a. M horizontal deceleration
Pad at Pad a
one-half hover
hover . altitude
129
Zenith i
-1
I
atorepndn topaoajrers E
~one-a~lf
a~tit~de / *A
ta a
,~& A
ligure3.Ineec
Foeh ofAmnPitLoainFo te
130
task until the pad is occAx'ed by the nose. in this aircraft, which has a
rather generously depres:4d field of view, the pad is marginally visible
throughout the critical portion of the conversion to hover. This profile
is most prone to pad overshoot when a transitio-v from instrument flight
rules to visual flight rules is required.
sec, and the pad radius in Figs. 31, 32, and 33 will be 40 ft. Note that
for this case the maximum slant visual range will be 680 ft, which cor-
responds roughly to Runway Visual Range for ICAO Category III-A. Notice
how little time (5 sec) Is available after acquiring visual contact with
the pad to establish pursuit tracking of the inferred aiming point with
respect to the pad before the pad is occulted by the nose. Thereafter
inference of the aiming point from motion streamers is hampered by the
occultation unless other ground texture besides a partial glimpse of pad
outline becomes visible. Yet the pilot must continue to pursue the
inferred center of the pad with the inferred aiming point until coinci-
dence is achieved, whence the pure descent can be arrested in hover over
the pad.
131
;Polar picture plane representation of forward hemispheric field of view
h - rate of descent
a- horizontal deceleration
_na ft h/ax = normalizing unit of time
132
Melt-
Figure 34 shows a short runway approach viewed from inside the same
aircraft. A constant glide slope angle of 9 deg is maintained. Notice
how the depression angle of the runway threshold at a constant 9 deg below
the horizon coincides with the fixed aiming point and offers a .compensa-
tory cue for maintaining position on the glide slope. Furthermore the
perspective angle of the line-up centerline or the asymmetry of the
(lighted) runway outline will provide a compensatory lateral displacement V
cue as shown in the inset at the lower left corner of Fig. 34. The tran-
sition to a level of skill higher than compensatory is thus postponed
until the flare point is approached or until the minimum go-around deci-
sion altitude is reached. The runway appears in the pilot's tunnel field
of view throughout most of the approach. This constant-angle profile
offers superior potential for compatible instrumefit-to-visual transitions. '
133
py,~
(I
ZI'?ithi-
iV
Hortixon
4A
i/
a\ ay ..
at one-half £jdi"
"flare altitude
borison .
havay
b
C. approach
1314.
* -mw
,.
_Zenith
GA
' /I KA
Uiway
at one-half
flare altitude J..
1 kzdir
approach
135
- __ _ -
:+ •++•... . .. . . --.. . " -.- -
V
Hover position sensitivity perceived by the pilot falls
136
- - -- ~ -~ - ~ ::-7
7 Nowinal Glide Slope Angle
y = Lateral Displacement
h = Altitude
F F Line Up Centerline
12
Observer; 0 Horizon
JfF 7t~
Observer's View of
S~F.F2 Ground Range, Foresho:rtened Line Up C
Line Up F2
137
sensitivity of the perspective angle, V, to changes in lat-
eral displacnment, y (with the latter normalized by the
ground range, S), is equivalent to the partial derivative,
i.e.,
3V 2
7 -cot Y sin V
138 .11
.•'I'
1
:•-f 139
' I
f^ .
ARALYTIC PRO(ZDURE FOR DETEIMINIOG NECESSARY
VISUAL AND MDTION CUE STIMULI
The pilot uodel concepts outlined in the preceding section form the
basis for the fidelity requirements analysis. The methodology uses the
analytical techniques of the control engineer to formulate plausible sys-
tem structures consistent with human pilot characteristics expressed in
Carter and Semple 163 point out, pilots also should be queried about the
140
ANALYTIC PROCEDURE
Te third S165
"lateral-directional" degree of freedom is roll and,
although not stated, is always coupled with the lateral and
directional motions.
141
~ i-:
"4-
TABLE II
(5) For each essential loop, look up the catalogued fidelity potential
for a specific candidate medium and determine whether this potential
permits the required loop closure.
142
--
X I
In executing this first step, it is exceedingly helpful to have the
benefit of experience in selecting information. Some areas are well de-
fined and documented while others will rzquire extrapolation of data or
estimations requiring good engineering judgment. Perhaps the most crucial
aspect is strict economy of system parameters without losing the ability
to include all critical system effects. Two approaches can be taken:
The latter is preferred because it will most quickly provide some degree
of "solution" in order to get the analysis process underway. The hazard
in this may be the willingness to accept immediately the first solution
without qualification.
The next step is to establish the likely system loop structure re- I
quired to regulate against disturbances assuming information for the
outermost loops. This is usually visually-derived information only. This
may be relatively easy (pilot input strictly governed by explicitly dis-
played quantities) or more difficult (visual information derived from the
pilot's out-of-cockpit view in a multiple loop task). Past experience
plays an important role here; but clearly the pilot cannot respond to
0Z- something if he c ann•o t perceive it; and to control a particular degree of ,'
143
In most cases the command (outermost) loops will consist of either
displacements or velocities. The only acceleration command loop which is
obvious is normal acceleration command for fixed-wing air combat man-
euvering, and even this may not be appropriate for helicopters.
144
pilot opinion or workload for the task under consideration. Even when
less extreme, the need for lead equalization signals for potential utility
of motion inputs to the pilot. In either case this classification by lead
equalization requirements using the compensatory pilot model identifies
those flying tasks whenever additional visual information or motion is
potentially useful - particularly in the first two phases of the SOP
sequence.
Depending upon the task and the skill level of interest, there may be
various solutions to this step. For example it may be found that the
addition of an intermediate visual velocity feedback loop is all that is
necessary to provide adequate pilot-vehicle damping for the execution of a
task at the compensatory level. Or it may be necessary to utilize ves-
tibular feedback in order to boost the task execution to a pursuit
level. Unfortunately we presently lack adequate quantification of tasks
and piloting technique versus skill level to perform this step with con-
fidence, but it is a crucial step to discovery of simulator fidelity needs
and therefore will be addressed under research objectives.
The evidence for such structure is most substantial for the angular
acceleration motion inputs, somewhat less so for linear accelerations.
This is primarily because control of an aircraft's angular motions is
fundamental to virtually any flying task and considerable experimental
data have been gathered. On the other hand, the pilot's use of proprio-
ceptively perceived linear accelerations appears to be quite task-specific
(e.g., flare of the aircraft prior to touchdown). The pilot's awareness
145
of linear accelerations (and by tentative inference, his use of these
sensations) depends in part on his past experience. A background of heli-
copter flying generally implies a greater awareness of linear
accelerations than does the background of the fixed-wing pilot.
146
attested to by the performance variability among the several experimental
conditions. Normally system performance remains approximately fixed and
pilot opinion (interpreted as subjective workload) changes. One result,
surprisirg at first, was that the elimination of linear motion improved
performance relative to the case where both linear and angular motion
K (presumably most "representative" of flight) were present. The elimina-
tion of the linear motion resulted in unrealistic tilting sensations which
nevertheless helped the pilot control the simulated vehicle, thus
constituting what training psychologists term an "irrelevant cue to
correct performance." (Sometimes an irrelevant cue to correct performance
is a good thing if it doesn't become a crutch.) Tight attitude control
was required by the nature of the task for system stability, and, without-
these sensations, attitude information was available only visually and
more difficult to obtain because of other scanning demands.
the pilot.
170 Ibid.
1.47
• ....
.- . .
of the pilot's vestibular senses for much of the time. Thus the perfor-
mance differences due to motion were considerably attenuated.
These results are quite important for motion simulation. The princi-
pal means of reducing the linear travel requirements in motion simulators
is the use of one or more variants on the "residual tilt" scheme, a
tilting of the simulator cab at sub-vestibular threshold rates to produce
linear acceleration sensations while avoiding the linear travel otherwise
required. This technique amounts to an intentional distortion of the
motion sensations. Whether or not it is acceptable for training in a
particular task depends upon the magnitude of the distortion relative to
148
LLI
K aircraft!
greater than 0.1
The pilot will not be able to tolerate time discrepancies much
sec without complaint. This is
tasks demanding maximal performance on the part of the pilot, e.g.,
particularly true for
target
tracking, where the crossover frequencies to be attained are relatively
high.
The jet transport flare and landing study cited previously 1 7 1 provides
us with a relevant case illustrating some aspects of the foregoing proce-
dure as they relate to visual fidelity. The model of the landing task, as
determined from flight measurement, indicated a level of closed-loop damp-
ing which was more than a simple pure gain feedback of altitude (the
command loop) alone could provide. A feedback of vertical velocity or its
kinematic equivalent was also required. The large amplitude vertical
motion simulator involved in this study did not induce the required level
of closed-loop damping. This analysis was a good example of failure to
149
i! i
reject the hypnthesis that the simulator differs from the aircraft in some
property essential to the flare and lending. He~ice there was an obvious
question of simulator fidelity. What essentidl cue was either lacking or
incorrect?
Exceosive sivulator
E delays or lags (visual update,
algorithmic, etc.,
1722
172 For example, the focus of expansion (of streamers) itself may be
diffuse, the reduced field of view may inhibit extrapolatiou of
peripheral streamers to define the focus of exnansion, or perspective
distortion may contribute to misjudgment of the direction of flight.
150
I
TABLE 12
REDUCED DATA: A
I •. •, PILOT-VERICLE INFLUENCES, ,
1
predTctable quantity which can be estimated or measured
directly.
(Negligible)
B -B /+ Bpilot
a/c
In the case of the landing maneuver
Good fidelity
•!Bflt pilot
_S Basn
pilot
Godfieit
in terms of
height --
o attitude
~ aim Good fidelity in terms of
Aflt Apim direction of flight jor sink
pilot
•= pilot rate) -. attitude
151
173
The results of the landing study can be extrapolated to other
height control tasks, piloting techniques, and aircraft types if we pre-
serve several constraints. These include basic control theory, the
physical laws of vehicle dynamics, and the perceptual pathways implied in
the landing situattmn, i.e., height and direction of flight. Figure 37
shows the results of such an extrapolation for ranges of Army aircraft,
both rotary- and fixed-wing, over their respective speed ranges and con-
trol techniques. Thn boundaries shown indicate the amount of height
.174 above4 which direction of flight cues are needed.
response lag1 The
Sconclusions from this plot would be that direction-of-flight cues are
required for low-speed helicopter flight (below translational lift) and
for low-speed fixed-wing aircraft with relatively high-wing loadings. On
the other hand, only altitude cues would be required for fixed-wing air-
craft with low-wing loading. Further the boundaries shown could be
adapted to any simulator for which direction-of-flight feedbacks were
required yet not exhibited by the pilot.
152
TASK: PRECISE CONTROL. OF HEIGHT
LAG
IN
HEIGHT
RESPONSEY
(SEC)
2'2
DIRECTION-OF-FLIGHT CIJ'ES NEEDED FOR
:% EIGHT CONTROL VIA SQ.jk~TIVE
OH-6
DIRECTION-OF-LHT US
A4 NEEDED FOR HEIGHT CONTROL
VIA PITCH ATTITUDEi
ROTARY-
WING
RANGE OV-1 (high wing loading)
44
0-2(o iglaig
00 100 200
AIRSPEED QrT)
Our treacment of simulator fidelity thus far has prepared us for the
next step - to establish a system~atic bookkeeping scheme for simulator
fidelity with respect to the array of Army training objectives. This step
will take full advantage of our definition of fidelity, our task analysis,
the modeling approaches for pilot behavior and perception, and the an-
alytic approach for cue stimulus needed to train.
0 Basic information
0 Constructed information
Basic information must be acquired from past research and analysis and
o 154
£1
TABLE 13
155
155
I . . .. .j
consider each of the entries in Table 13 and suggest examples of their
form and content.
156
0
041
0
041
4E-
CD
0 ba
1-40
0o0
~~Cl C)) C) C -4 4 .
E- C C C
0 CC) C)co V) CC
Q.~CC 44 4. . -4- 1-3 ý4- C
Co~0 . 0 0
4-3.1 u- mC ca1
Q) 0 -- 4C 4
0) Q)
-
4r4
~CCAi1) > U4 co
- > 4- ,* w-
uo -4 44 -HW 14 4J
'I4-1 ca CC 41 4
w-41-J .1 CC
v4~ ý ) 01 to
00 .4 H-.- 44 41 4-IS-
rC rC. , 0CCU0 0 C: (1
0. v-i410 0. 00 '4
H -T3
C-Z
F44 157C 4
cc e ) 1 $
:3 >
to c-v4
AERODYNAMIC FEEDBACKS VERSUS AIRCRAFT TYPE
f While
pilot feedback,
most command loops
a number
depend almost entirely
and
axis, are described in Table 15. Note that the table identifies specific
aircraft stability derivatives which can be evaluated for any given air-
craft and analyzed to find the net contribution of each derivative to the
task in question. To an extent, this kind of analysis has already been
Refer to Table 6, pp. 55, 56, for examples of pilot control techniques
which are more or less independent of the specific Army Flight Training
the end of the section entitled "Analysis of Army Flight Training Object-
ives" for examples of more task-specific piloting techniques and
speed-change maneuvers shown in Figs. 19, 20, and 21. .ch case some-
a-
173 Heffley, Robert K., Wayne F. Jewell, John M. Lehman, and Richard A.
Van Winkle, A Compilation and Analysis of Heliccpter Handling
Qualities Data. Volume One: Data Compilation, NASA CR 3144, August
1979.
158
TABLE 15
"AERODYNAMIC EFFECTS
Roll Bank Angle -Ground effect, (Lf(h)) Usually stabilizing but negligible
ix 3u
Poll Rate Roll damping, Stabilizing, a function of wing loading and, for
av
3i helicopters, rotor type
Pitch Pitch Rate Pitch damping, Mq Stabilizing, a function of tail geometry for
1 36 airplane and helicopter at high speed and rotor
type for helicopter at low speed
Surge Velocity I 2-X Speed damping, Xu Parasite drag, negligible effect for airplane
m au at low speed
Sway Sway Velocity 7- Drift damping, Yv Always negl.gible, function of vertical fin
ýu
159hr, 4--
Table 16 provides another example for landing a fixed-wing light-twin
engine aircraft using attitude to control the direction of flight.
I174
feeds from one axis of control to another.
indicator of pursuit-level piloting technique.)
Heffley,
development
et al.,
in
and Jewell 175
the Navy carrier landing task in
(In fact this is
cK
174 Heffley, Robert K., Warren F. Clement, and Samuel J. Craig, "Training
Aircraft Design Considerations Based on the Successive Organization
of Perception in Manual Control," Sixteenth Annual Conference on
Manual Control, MIT, ay 5-7, 1980, pp. 119-127.
160
"_ it
U!!
"U,TABLE 16
Landing Altitude 2 ft
(fixed-wing,
light-twin) Direction of Flight (Vertical) I deg @ 0.25 rad/sec
using attitude
to control Drift 4 ft
flight path
Direction of flight (lateral) ?
1L
"'7-v
,p - r
The next set of features which should be cataloged are the various
essential cues which might be candidates for each essential loop in a
given task scenario. This information would be valuable in analyzing any
deficiencies in essential loop behavior. Table 17 presents some examples
for direction-of-flight and attitude.
162
TABLE 17
Visual Motion
2) Derived rate of
change of altitude
3) Vertical velocity
163
T 77>-
TRAINING CAPABILITY BY TASK
VERSUS TRAINING DEVICE
One known example of this kind of data is the airline landing maneuver
analysis previous described177.
I- 164
Ii________________
results must be validated or refined on the basis of experimental data
involving pilot subjects performing realistic flight tasks.
This matter is discussed in several places in this report. For ex-
ample motion cues in general are discussed on pp. 105-112. Motion cues in
support of lateral tracking are discussed on pp. 194-206, Visual range
perception is discussed on pp. 187-191. Hypotheses for deficiencies in
visual direction-of-flight cues are discussed on p. 150 and in Fig. 37,
1 9
p. 153. For a given training mission ' (e.g., helicopter NOE), training
objective (e.g., "attack target"), and training medium (e.g., ground-based
simulation) - it is possible to determine the tradeoffs among various
visual and motion fidelity measures and their training value. Other ex-
amples of this form of tradeoff would be as follows:
A
0 Target tracking range versus visual system resolution:
:& r
TanksA
fine coarse
Visual Field Resolution
165
0 Allowable sideslip alignment maneuvers versus motion system
lateral travel:
1C'O'f Sidestep Distance Desired
Spurious 2Oft
Motion
Cues .1MEE/ / Indff d'e re nc e
Threshold
Y \ While
Tracking
2,40 60 80 100
ALWAYSCINERAMA
_________(160*)
OFTEN •
CAR Sim
OCCASIONAIM. B ,enkA
UCLA
CAR S I U
OCCASIONALLY TV SANrRE
NEVFZ H ME
166
5 -
Except for the kinetosis tradeoff plot, the above sketches are not
based on actual calculations, but have the correct trends and are typical
of numerous tradeoffs involving macro detail cues available and micro
detail features required.
iI
tA
167
RESEAICH NEEDED TO DEFINE SIMULATOR REQUIREMENTS
The fact that there are presently gaps in the data necessary to define
simulator fidelity requirements does not mean that those data cannot be
acquired. Various means do exist for obtaining the required information
and one of the main goals in this section will be to outline the various
methods for collecting empirical data needed to determine simulator
fidelity requirements. The examples given are based on some of the Army
aviation training missions, although the material presented is never-
theless applicable to many other areas of aircraft operations.
168
TABLE 18
Helicopter
I! VMC and IMC
Primary Through Continuation Training-
I TAKEOFF/APPROACH/LANDING
{
• Fixed Wing and Helicopters
VMC
Primary Through Continuation Training
1 83
LOW ALTITUDE OPERATIONS
Helicopter
VMC Day and Night
1
WEAPON DELIVERY 84
{ •Helicopter
-- VMC Day and Night
"EMERGENCIES
182
S182 See Table 5 for further breakdown into specific piloting tasks.
183 High priority.
169
?I ..
* Measurement tools
170
_ _ _ _ __ _ _
PIEPARAMTOX ANALYSIS OF THE PILOTING TAS
171
structures relates to the primary objective in that axis, and the inner
series loops act to support that primary objective. For example, in the
lateral axis, the primary objective is to maintain a track along the ap-
proach centerline; and this -is reflected in the outermost loop around
lateral position. In direct support of this is regulation of heading and,
in direct support of heading, Ps regulation of bank angle. A similar
organization can be observed in other axes and in other piloting' tasks.
Systematic recognition of such structure is thus the first step in analy-
sis of any given piloting task or mission.
The second step in our task aaalysis is to estimate the likely ranges
of aircraft dynamics or kinematic relationships involved in the task.
This is not inordinately difficult, since most fixed-wing or rotary-wing
aircraft of a given size and type tend to have very similar dynamic prop-
erties, especially if these are aircraft in an operational status and not
unusual research or experimental aircraft. For example, in an analysis of
the typical properties of single-rotor helicopters by Heffley1 8 6 , it is
shown that the major variation in dynamic features among various helico'p-
ters is primarily in attitude control characteristics. However, even this
aspect is highly predictable with knowledge of the basic rotor type (tee-
tering, articulated, or rigid). The main point to be made is that an
estimation of likely ranges of vehicle dynamics is not unduly tied to any
particular helicopter model. For the purposes of a preparatory analysis,
the differences bet-'een, cay, an OH-6 and a UH-l are not great.
The third step in our task analysis is to estimate the likely ranges
of bandwidth for each of the loops involved in the task. This can be
accomplished on the basis of relatively simple rules of manual, multiloop
control theory in combination Vith available rules of thumb or stated
performance objectives. Ir fact, a dependence on manual control theory
ideas is likely to be reletively low. A very good estimation of loop
bandwidths is possible by an engineering interpretation of nominal rules
172 f
rV
fr
of thumb for each pil6ting task. Also bandwidths, once established for
-I one loop, are likely to be applicable to the same loop in other tasks. As
with the other steps in the preparatory analysis, it is really, only neces-
sary to obtain "ballpark". estimates for the various loop bandwidths.
173
•lz .- i~i~~ ..
Increasing perceptual
motor load
Increasing rehearsal
HI Increasing bandwidth
::72 0 1
•tW • o40 CS 4:
Q) rq2
I• a 0 Ell
40
Er4
0 0'
to 04
0 0
0)0 )
vt, a
" U
a)
V z0~0
FOS
Z,
430)-
(1) 1C
0 0 lod
0) 0 a)
0 0) 0D
"0'-4iO Ow-'P
4-1
CO
W 0 a) 14 4CPJCJ0
E- t 0 I I,
E-4~
VH
I
pr
TABLE 20
F! TASK BREAKDOWN
Airspeed
Altitude
Heading
-Maintain mask
Maintain/monitor obstacle clearance
Determine position/performance intersection
-Engagement -Maneuver
J,
Attack
fire SHovex
-Running fire
. Mask
S"• 17 5
TABLE 21
As the pilot ascends toward the top of the xask, The task scenario shou•ld Involve a terrain or vegetation
whether a stand of trees or steeply sloping terrain, feature which induces forward translation during vertical
there is a tendency to translate forward. Hence there ascent and unmasking, e.g., a pyramidal shaped hill, bar-
is an absolute requirement to translate rearward by an rner, or tree. In order to gauge position relative to the
appropriate amount during the descent. Also, the terrain, a tip-path plane edge should be providei in the
pilot is likely to gauge position and ve:tical .path of outside visual scene. Direct measurements should be applied
the helicopter using the tip-path plane to help evalu- to control of fore and aft position vhich reveal the a.ount
ate tree-top clearance. of pilot regulation (bandwidth. Phase margin) and coordina-
tion with height (crossfeed of x-positlon comand with
vertical position).
Control of Vertica•l Position:
The pilot needs the atility to make a crisp bob-up Direct measurements should be applied to cou.mand and control
a bove the mask. hold altitude precisely for 5 to of vertical position and should indicate bandwidth and level
10 sec while perforn-ing the observation or weapon of compensation while unmasked.
delivery task, and descend smartly belou the aask.
Woring the vertical ascent nd descent there is likely Measurement of lateral command and control should likewise
to be little or no lateral movement. However, follow- be applied but will focus on the time interval following
Ing rems~.ng, the pilot will want to translate remask and descent.
quickly to a new lateral position below the mask.
Regulation Against Atmorpheric Disturb~ances:
AtAospheric disturbances introduce a significant A simple random free-air turbulence model would probably
.ompllcation In the performance of the maneuver, suffice in forcing a position dispersion on the sirframe/
Turbulence can of" course, affect the attitude and control system combination. The major effect on pilot
position regulation task through direct action on the behavior, however, will be the introduction of a determinis-
vehicle. More Insidious is the effect of wind shear tic wind shear at tree-top height. The shear should be
a-, the tree top level or ridrellne. On one hand, a applied in bot.: directions, but it can-be asssumd that the
Stsalvlwnd forces the-helicopter toward the vegetation pilot has prior knowledge of which direction. Position
Sor terrain sask and thus creates a hazard during the bandwidth and compensation should be measured for varying
desacent. Perhaps a more serious condition is a head- amplitudes and directions of disturbances. Position of the
ind. however. During ,he unmasking the pilot might aircraft relative to the obstacle should be plotted for the
-trim Into the headina; but during remasking the vertical plane.
headwind quickly disappearci, forcing the vehicle into
the trees or ground. (A Navy counterpart to this
hazard has been observed for landing on the stern of a
ship.)
176
I
177
.,- --- . .
COOPER-HARPER RATING
PILOT
u
ORAL OR WRITTEN COMMENTARY
SI
MAGNETIC
TAPES
TA-ES- ---- OFF-LINE REVIEWOR
"DATA ANALYSIS
PARAMETER IDENTIFICATION
SOLUTIONS OF PILOTING
TECHNIQUE
z
•. VIDEO REPLAY FOR
S , • -PILOT DEBRIEFING
-• z APESOR ENGIN~EERIN~G ANlALYSIS
178
II
Pilot-centered analysis tools can consist of the standard Cooper-
189
Harper rating and oral or written pilot commentary. Cooper-Harpe*.
ratings are really not of any particular value unless a pilot is exper-
lenced in giving them, and this usually requires a research or flight test
189 Cooper, George E., and Robert P. Harper, The Use of Pilot Rating in
the Evaluation of Aircraft Handling Qualities, NASA TN D-5153, 1969.
179 j
f -'-
-- - •' -. ' 74$
pilot will simply apply more effort to increase attitude bandwidth and
thereby retain the same standard of pitch attitude dispersion as was en-
joyed in calm air with relatively little effort. Thus the fact that
attitude dispersions are about the same in rough air as in calm air im-
plies that the pilot has substantially modified his behavior and is
working harder, but these dispersion statistics do not provide a direct
measure of that crucial difference. However there are ways to make that -
important direct measurement using additional statistical measures or raw
time history recordings of variables.
180
1[91
191
181
DISTURBANCE
INPUT
MEASUREMENTS/
FROM
HE /HUMAN OPERATOR_
SUBJECT MODEL STRUCTURE{
INTERPRETATION
jITERTIVE IJOF
-IDENTIFICATION~
RESULTS
SEARCH-MTO NUMERICAL
S PROCEDURE DEFINITION
) I___ -OF MODEL
STRUCTURE
LNG SOLUTION-j
-182 {
Another analysis tool not routinely used iE direct plotting of phase
planes, control-state portraits, and control-control portraits. Thes.f are
graphic representations of the relationships between selected pairs of
state variables in which time is not necessarily represented as a basic
independent paraueter. For example the closed-loop behavior of the pilot
performing a landing flare maneuver has been quantified using a direct
193
plot of sink rate versus altitude and pitch attitude versus altitude
The first of these plots can be interpreted in order to obtain the
effective closed-loop frequency and damping of the landing maneuver and
the second plot shows the specific control law being used by the pilot to
perform this maneuver. This kind of scheme involves minimal instrumen-
tation of either a simulator or aircraft and has, in fact, been used to
analyze the differences which result from training for the landing flare
194
maneuver in a simulator as opposed to an actual aircraft9.
The final and perhaps simplest of the non-routine analysis tools de-
scribed here is provision -for a video replay of a simulated or actual
flight maneuver for the purpose of either debriefing the pilot or for
direct engineering analysis. It has been observed that pilots, when re-
viewing a replay of their own flight, can adopt a more objective point of
view for self-analysis and may even substantially change commentary given
before that replay. This idea could be extended to include replay of the
simulator rather than just. video replay.
183
9.
~1
of this activity has been founded upon the pilot-in-the-loop models which
have come about through the application of control system theory to the
[
qaency outer loops whose char3cteristics can change within a fraction of a
cycle of the predominant outer loop frequency.. (For example, on a final
approach segment the regulation of pitch attitude can involve tens of
cycles without significant change in piloting technique while the landing
195L
Hofmann ane Riedel, 1979, op cit.
184
TABLE 22
4; ~
REPRESENTATIVE EXAMPLES OF' AVAILABLE DATA
* Attitude and position In hover (simulator - cockpit reference with and without motion references)
196
i
-
0 Height, pitch, and roll attitude regulation in cruise (simulator -. cockpit rteference)197
198
* Glide slope regulation (STOL dircraft simulator ~-cockpit reference)
202
* Foroatidn-keepinj In forwiard flight under IFR (helicopter - csckpit reference)
*Landing flare (in flight an 0 ~n jet transport training simulatcr, outside night visual field, cockpit
L
display, motion references)5
196i
16Ringland, Stapleford, and Hagdaleno, 1971, op cit.
197 Stapleford, McRuer, and Magdaleno. 1966, op cit.
.Weir, D. H., and Di. T. -McRuer, Pilot LDynamics for Instrumnent Ap1 roach Tasks: PFull-ranel Mtultiloop and
Flight Director Operations NASA CR-2019, May 1972.-
202 Clement, Allen, and Graham, 1971, op cit.
203 Heffley, A Model for Manual DELcelerating Approaches to Hover, 1979, op cit.
185
_ _ _ _ _ __ _ .f
EIAMPLES OF PILOTING TECHNIQUE AND
PERCEPTUAL MEASUREMENTS AND INTERPRETATION
OF EXPERIMENTAL RESULTS
_ _ _ __ _ _-
CL) 0 0
r P4 4)40C
00C (\1
01 0 -0 CO
4-) 0J
4) Je It U
o r9
0Cd-
04,
04
4)
.
CN 0
K
-A
I3 r 4i
/d 0
C)a 0
1870
d)~
measurements of the parameters A and k in the deceleration guidance law
((Fi8 . 40) while the helicopter is under visual manual control, In
addit"on the same ideas applied to the deceleration task in Fig. 40 can
alco be extended to vertical and lateral flight path guidance.
The key to describing (and measuring) the fidelity of the visual per-
spective (Fig. 40) is provided in Gilinsky20 9 where the psychological
measurements of apparent range and apparent size of essential cues in the
visual field are related to varl-as metrics of visual perspective. There
it is shown that perceived range, Rp, is related to t-rue range, R, by:
R
p 1+RIX
Sp Rp.
._ 1
S R I + R/A
Opcit.
188
21 1
Moen are given in Fig. 40 for eventual comparison with corresponding
measurements from simulator tests.
A+ r r R
S A +R 1 AAL 1 ~A
0
189
-L ! -,
The length A is thus the subjectively perceived range at which the size
ratio s/S 0 tends to vanish. The mean out-of-doors field value of A ex-
trapolated from the measurements 2 1 3 was 300 ft.
These results for A imply that the collimation tended in part to com-
pensate for the distortion of the visual perspective associated with
I I
21
213 Ibid.
214 Palmer, Everett, Tamara Mitchell, and John Pettit, Angular Size-
Estimation: A Measure of -Simulator Visual Fidelity, (unpublished
-paper), No date, airca January 1979.
190
2 7 - '
tA
Other analogous measurement:s have been derived from tests wherein the
subjects viewed computer-generated imagery (CGI) consisting of calli-
graphic night visual scenes of an airport runway beside which the standard
and variable triangles were alternately presented for comparative judg-
21 6
ment. These results are reported in Palmer and Petitt , also for
collimated and uncollimated viewing; Again the results for A have been
calculated and are listed below based on data from Palmer and Petitt2i7 .
Since the comparable out-of-doors night scene was not -tested for compari-
son, one is left- to speculate among hypotheses for the much lower ranges
215 See Kibort, Bernard- R., and Fred. J. Drinkwater III, A Flight Study of
Manual Blind Landing Performance Using Closed Circuit Television
Displays, NASA TN D-2252, May 1964, for results of flight tests of
blind landing performance using closed-circuit TV displays with
iconoscope lenses -having different focal lengths. The average error
in touchdown point varied- in linear proportion to the focal length of
the lens. Thus:
217 ibid.
191 1
I,- -
. - 4- s-. -
for values of A. Again, however, the beneficial contribution of callima-
tion is apparent in increasing the range for A.
192
Notion Perception in Target Tracking2 1 8
"* Tracking
have the rotational cues than the translational ones. If tracking per-
formance were the sole criterion, the translational motions might even be
eliminated altogether as long as the task did not require a translational
cies. For the low frequency limit, it does not appear necessary to go as
low as the vestibular sensor washout, roughly 0.1 rad/sec. A conservative
193
lower frequency limit would be 0.5 rad/sec and even 1 rad/sec would be
reasonable.
220 Jewell, Wayne-F., Robert L. Stapleford, and Robert Xt. Hef fley, rI
Computed Responses of Several Aircraft to Atmospheric Turbulence and
Discrete Wind Shears, NASA CR-152,181, February 1977, 1
"194
an aircraft, rotorcraft, or system failure, such as an engine or stability
augmentation failure, motion cues can •play an especially important
role. 2 2 1 The motions accompanying a failure can help greatly in the
pilot's timely detection of the failure. This is especially -true if the
aoticeable effects were displayed on the flight instruments (such -as the
artificial horizon).
At the, present no general -requirements based on failure detection are
available. As a minimum, the motion should be enough to provide an un-
ambiguous clue to the failure. For example, to simulate a hardover yaw
damper malfunction, the simulator should have enough lateral travel so
that -the pilot can clearly separate the lateral acceleration cue accom-
panying the failure from those due to gusts. In many cases failure
detection may put the most stringent- requirements. on translational
motions.
S-221
Caro, 1977, op cit.
195
÷ - 7 -- -
S~12
formed using roll and sway motions of the Large Amplitude Multimode
Aerospace Research Simulator (LAMARS) of the Flight Dynamics Laboratory at
Wright-Patterson Air Force Base, Ohio. The objectives of these experi-
ments were:
K s
Ybeam
2
Yfree flight s2+ 2wyaS + W
yy y
196
where K. = attenuation factor, w - high-pans break frequency (rad/sec),
Reshaping the forcing functions was also investigated and shown to reduce
travel requirements.
-223 Ibid. 11
224 Jex, Jewell, and Magdaleno, 1979, op cit.
197
iteratively fine-tuned to avoid hitting stope while
minimizing spurious washout artifacts. Additionally,
It should be especially useful during training, where
motion cue usage ic changing.
We shall now present some of the results which characterize the pilots'
judgments of "realism."
Although the pilots were encouraged to use their own words tc describe
* the effects of the motion cues, there was a certain amount of commonality
in the terms used by all the pilots. These are summarized below:
198
'.{{
~~ -
7 77T IFMP,
disconcerting. (Note here the conditional dependence of
the utricular threshold on task workload.)
199
? : - J-- -
61"Roli-Swoy
-1 s + Washout Filters
- cbI.iJ..4
g- +
andsmall oy attenuation
to eth
e- •..'•;."ACCEPTABLE"••.;"'"•';.••t
Fr toJexo
it) and the loans or dhch
yed side forces
>pressionso eaicsn predominat y o
22 IbTid..
SI 200 ii
4 77 ¾.'L'CZq'ý'''
7 "
201
A
0 Bank and stop maneuvers.
0 Roll tracking.with "reduced" input
Ayp
[3 "About the same"
"Very Uncoordinated"'
(large delayed side forces) K
Change" =-JCoordinated"
0-
0 .5 ~Kvlrad/ sec) L
Finally one other important comment was the pilots' universal dis-
pleasure with hitting, the sway displacement limits. The adverse effects
of hitting displacement limits have been observed in other simulators
(e.g., Jewell, et al.,2)
229 and should be prevented by adopting nonlinear
motion drive logic.
229 Jewell, Wayne F., Warren F. Clement, Lt. Col. Thomas C. West, USA
(Ret.), and Dr. S. R. M. Sinclair, Powered-Lift Aircraft Handling
Qualities in the Presence of Naturally-Occurring and Computer-
Generated.Atmospheric Disturbances, FAA-RD-•79-59, May 1979.
203
"AI
R7----,- ---
clue to the failure. In many cases failure detection may put the most
demanding requirement on translational motions.
Roll and sway motion cues have recently been investigated with the aid
204
41-
I.V.
displacement within the 110 ft of LAMARS travel. The main results from
this investigation show that:
•il •205
. - -
SUMMARY FOR rLAflKENG AND COLLECTING MEASUREMENTS
themselves but also the preparation for their use and the Judicious in-
terpretation of their results.
U
Table 23 is offered as a check-list for setting out to obtain empirical
data whether from simulator or -flight.
2i
0
" I 206
l(
TABLE 23
20V
,- _ 'n.-t2 --
- - -•---
TABLE 23 (Continued)
full-mission simulation
- Closed-circuit video
-Audio communications
"-Hard
copy (e.g., subjective ratings and observers' notes)
- Operator-centered measurements
0 General recommendations
CI
209
~-_A
C0NCLUSIMOWS AND RECOMMENDATIONS
this potential, however, must await the gathering of crucial data which
describe piloting technique and pilot perception during specific flight
tasks and operating environments.
The term "explicit fidelity" has been tied to the combination of pilut
perception and piloting technique exhibited in the simulator as compared
to that exhibited in an actual flight situation. Thus explicit fidelity
carries with it the ideas of perceptual fidelity. A more incidental kind
of fidelity can be associated with the actual simulator software and
hardware characteristics, those features which are normally considered to
come nnder the heading of objective or engineering fidelity. One
important result of this report has been to show how the explicit
simulator characteristics of piloting technique and pilot perception can
be q..antified.
210
The first step in establishing simulator fidelity requirements is the
quantitative description of the piloting task or training objective. This
constrains the pilot-aircraft scenario so that control theory analysis
tools can be systematically applied. Much of this quantitative
description can be obtained from existing training documents such as the
ATMs.
The first two elements above would permit a systematic accounting of the
fidelity potential which is available in existing simulator or training
device motion and visual systems. With the addition of the third item
frcm the above list, it would be theoretically possible to predict
requirements for given training objectives.
from essential loops; and, in many cases, these loops are easy to identify
essential.
211
to the mote obvious and essential altitude loop. When viewing thir same
maneuver on a particular training simulator there was little or no
evidence of this important direction-of-flight loop in most cases, and it
wa.3 preýsumed that there was a deficiency in the direction-of-flight cue
available to the pilot. While It has not yet been possible to discover
the exact source of the cue deficiency in this case (either motion or
visual) it has been possible to tabulate a bounded list of candidates for
the fidelity problem. The most plausible of the various alternatives in
this case was a deficiency in visual eirection-of-flight information due
to the restricted field of view. For thie fidelity potential exhibited in
this particular simulator, a sample extrapolation was made from the
airline jet transport to various Army fixed- and rotary-wing aircraft and
their respective piloting techniques.
212
.x
i: blown, intensive research program intended to fill out large portions of
I--
II
r!I
--' ' ..2-r_
--=; .F
- ,. _b
GENERAL ALPHABETICAL BIBLIOGRAPHY
214
Anon., Rotary Wing Flight, U. S. Army Field Manual No. 1-51,
4 16 April 1979.
Anon., Instrument Flying, Air Force Manual, AFM 51-37, 15 August 1979.
Calvert, E. S., "The Theory of Visual Judgments in Motion and Its Appli-
cation to the Design of Landing Aids for Aircraft," Transactions.
of the Illuminating Engineering Society, 22, 1957.
Clement, Warren F., R. Wade Allen, and Dunstan Graham, Pilot Experiments
"for a Theory of Integrated Display Format (Final Report),
JANAIR Report No. 711107, October 1971.
215
Clement, W. F., D. T. McRuer, and R. H. Klein, "Systematic Manual Control
Display Design," Guidance and Control Displas, AGARD CP-96, Feb-
ruary 1972.
Cooper, George E., and Robert P. Harper, The Use of Pilot Rating in the
Evaluation of Aircraft Handling _Qalities, NASA TN D-5153, 1969.
Davies, E. B., Contrast Tresholds for Air to Ground Vision RAE Technical
Report 65089, April 1965.!
Eddowes, Edward E., and Wayne L. Waag, The Use of Simulators for Training
In-Flight and Emergency Procedures, AGARD-AG-248, June 1980.
101
216
-I
Gilinsky, A. S., "Perceived Size and Distance in Visual Space," Psycho-
logical Review, 58, 1951, pp. 460-482.
Gordon, D. A., "Static and Dynamic Visual Fields in Human Space Percep-
tion," Journal of the Optical Society of America, 55, 1965, pp.
1296-1303.
Heffley, Robert K., Closed Loop Analysis of Manual Flare and Landing,
AIAA Paper 74-834, August 1974.
Heffley, Robert K., "A Model for Manual Decelerating Approaches to Hover,"
Proceedings of the Fifteenth Annual Conference on Manual Control
AFFDL-TR-79-3134, 1979, pp. 545-354.
Heffley, Robert K., Wayne F. Jewell, John M. Lehman, and Richard A. Van
Winkle, A Compilation and Analysis of Helicopter Handling
Qualities Data. Volume One: Data Compilation, NASA CR 3144,
August 1979.
217
- 11,
Heffley, Robert K., Ted M. Schulman, Robert J. Randle, Jr., and Watren F.
Clement An Analysis of Airline Landing Data Based on Flight and
Training Simulator heasurements, Forthcoming NASA Technical
Memorandum, 1981.
Hess, Ronald A., "Pursuit Tracking and Higher Levels of Skill Development
in the Human Pilot," Sixteenth Annual Conference on Manual Con-
trol, MIT, Cambridge, Massachusetts, May 5-7, 1980.
"Jewell, Wayne F., Warren F. Clement, Lt. Col. Thomas C. West, USA (Ret.),
and Dr. S. R. M. Sinclair, Powered-Lift Aircraft Handling Quali-
ties in the Presence of Naturally-Occurring and Computer-..enerated
Atmospheric Disturbances, FAA-RD-79-59, May 1979.
4J
218
Wit
Jewell, Wayne F., and Ted M. Schulman, A Pilot Control Strategy Identifi-
cation Technique for Use in Multiloop Control Tasks, Systems
Technology, Inc., Technical Report No. 1153-2, August 1980.
Jewell, Wayne F., and Warren F. Clement, A Simple Method for Measuring the
Effective Delay in Simulator Digital Computing Systems Involving
Manual Control, Systems Technology, Inc., Working Paper
No. 1156-4, December 1980.
Key, David L., (Ed.), Fidelity of Simulation for Pilot Training, AGARD
Advisory Report LNo. 159, October 1980.
Kibort, Bernard R., and Fred J. Drinkwater III, A Flight Study of Manual
Blind Landing Performance Usiný Closed Circuit Television Dis-
plays, NASA TN D-2252, May 1964.
219
- .- - - -- - - - ---- - - --
McRuer, Duane, Irving Ashkenas, and Dunstan Graham, Aircraft Dynamics and
Automatic Control, Princeton University Press, Princeton, New
Jersey, 1973.
McRuer, Duane T., Warren F. Clement, and R. Wade Allen, A Theory of Human
Error, Systems Technology, Inc., Technical Report No. 1156-1,
May 1980.
Moen, Gene C., Daniel J. DiCarlo, and Kennetn F. Yenni, A Parametric An-
alysis of Visual Approaches for Heli-opters, NASA TN D-8275,'
December 1976.
January 1974.
Palmer, Everett, Tamara Mitchell, and John Pettit, Angular Size Estima- 41
tion: A Measure of Simulator Visual Fidelity, (unpublished paper)
No date, circa January 1979.
(' Randle, Robert J., Stanley N. Roscoe, and John C. Petitt, Effects of
Magnification and Visual Accommodation on Aimpoint Estimation in
Simulated Landings with Real and Virtual Image Displays, NASA
TP 1635, October 1980.
220
-SZ
Ringland, R. F., R. L. Stapleford, and R. E. Magdaleno, Motion Effects on
an IFR Hover Task - Analytical Predictions and Experimental Re-
sults, NASA CR-1933, November 1971.
Ringland, Robert F., Warren F. Clement, and H1enry R. Jex, Factors in De-
"termining Visual and Motion Fidelity Requirements for Training
Simulators, Syetens Technology, Inc., Working Paper No. 1162-4,
December 1980, Revised March 1981.
"Roark, Marvin, and Andrew Junker, "The Effects of Closed Loop Tracking on
a Subjective Tilt Threshold in the Roll Anis," Proceedings of the
Fourteenth Annual Conference on Manual Control, University of
Southern California, April 25-27, 1978.
Roscoe, Stanley N., Halim Ozkaptan, and Aaron Hyman, Review of Flight
Training Technology, U. S. Army Research Problem Review 76-3,
July 1976.
Roscoe, Stanley, N., "When Day is Done and Shadows Fall, We Miss the
Airport Most of All," Proceedings of the 11th NTEC/Industry
Conference, NAVTRAEQUIPCEN IH-306, Naval Training Equipment
Center, Orlando, Floridai November 14-16, 1978.
L
Roscoe, Stanley N., Ground-Referenced Visual Orientation with Imaging
Displays: Final Report, AFOSR-79-4, November 1979.
221
- -< - N "
Spring, W. G., "Simulatlon Development and Preparation," Volume 4 of
Weir, D. H., and R. H. Klein, The Measurement and Analysis of Pilot Scan-
ning and Control Behavior During Simulated Instrumented
Approaches, NASA CR-1535, June 1970.
Zacharias, Greg L., and William H. Levison, "A Model-Eased Procedure for
Determining .Visual Cue Requirerents," 1981 IMAGE Ii Conference
Proceedings, AFHRL, Scottsdale, Arizona, June 10-12, 1981. I
C 222 :
BIBLIOGRAPHY OF PILOTING TECHNIQUES
' ~223
Anon., Aircrew T'raining Manual. (bservation Helicopter, Department of the
Army, TC 1-137, October 1978.
Anon., Rotary Wing Flight, U. S. Army Field Manual No. 1-51, 16 April
1979.
Anon., Instrument Flying, Ai: Force Manual AFM 51-37, 15 August 1979.
Anon., "Keep That Tail Up!" U. S. Army Aviation Digest, January 1980,
pp. 24-26.
Anon., Airman's Information Manual (AIM), Basic Flight Information and ATC
Procedures, Federal Aviation Administration, Jaiuary 1981.
224
i -
§ M __F_ _'IM
Kershner, William K., The Instrument Flight Manual, The Instrument Rating,
•,. Second Edition, 1976.
a.I-
.•1.
9F 77 I _M
22
RIBLIOGRAPHY OF V•'3IJAL PERCEPTION
Baron, Sheldon, Ramal Muralidharan, and David Kleinman, Closed Loop Models
for Analyzing Engineering Requirements for Simulators,
NASA CR-2965, February 1980.
Bynum, James A., Brian D. Shipley, and William C. White, "Helicopter Night
Low-Level Navigation: Study 1 - Checkpoint Identification," Pro-
ceedings of the 32nd Annual National V/STOL Forum of the American
Helicopter Society, 1976 (AHS Preprint 1024).
Calvert, E. S., "The Thecry of Visual Judgments in Motion and Its Appli-
cation to the Design of landing Aids for Aircraft," London,
Transactions of the Illuminating Engineering Society, 22, 1957.
Gibson, J. J., The Perception of the Visaal World, Boston, Houghton Mif-
flin, 1950, Chaps. 6-7.
Gordon, D. A., "Static and Dynamic Visual Fields in Human Space Percep-
tion," Joucnal. of the Optical Society of America, 55, 1965, pp.
1296-1303.
226
g - -- k _ _ __ _ _ __ _ _- _ _ _ __ _ _
Graham, C. H., "Visual Perception," In A Handbook of Eicperimental Psy-
chology, (Ed. S. S. Stevens), John Wiley, New York, 1951, pp. 868-
920.
Linton, Paul M., and Warren F. Clement, "Predicting Field of View Require-
ments for VSTOL Aircraft Approach and Landing," AGARD-CP-:268,
January 1980, pp. 40-1 through 40-17.
Semple, C. A., R. Hennessey, Mark Sanders, Bart Cross, Barry Beith, and
Michael McCauley, Aircrew Training Device Fidelity Peatures, Can-
yon Research Group, CRG TR-3041 B, February 1980.
"227
S'- '"'-"'--.-
---- %:-7- -ý*t
GLOSSARY
Control (variable) Any of the variables which the pilot must man-
ipulate in order to fly the aircraft (or
simulator).
228
Essential Cue A cue which is required to close an essential
loop.
[ Explieit or
equations of motion such as Newton's second law
of motion, aerodynamic effects,
chomotor and cognitive behavior.
behavior of a
Intrinsic Fidelity pilot which, if exhibited in a simulator
situation for a given task, would lead to suc-
cessful execution uf the same task in an actual
flight situation. Specifically, this involves
both the piloting technique and pilot perceptual
transfer functions essential for the task and is
therefore intrinsic to the task.
229
230
Perceptual Fidelity Characteristics of those aspects of a sitiulator
which the pilot uses to obtain essential cues or
characteristics of those behavioral aspects of
the pilots themselves.
231