Effects of Task Goal Orientation On Learner Engagement in Task Performance
Effects of Task Goal Orientation On Learner Engagement in Task Performance
Dao, Phung (2019) Effects of task goal orientation on learner engagement in task performance.
International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching, 59 (3). pp. 315-334. ISSN
0019-042X
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1515/iral-2018-0188
Publisher: Walter de Gruyter GmbH
Version: Accepted Version
Downloaded from: https://e-space.mmu.ac.uk/623404/
Enquiries:
If you have questions about this document, contact [email protected]. Please in-
clude the URL of the record in e-space. If you believe that your, or a third party’s rights have
been compromised through this document please see our Take Down policy (available from
https://www.mmu.ac.uk/library/using-the-library/policies-and-guidelines)
Accepted: 14/01/2019 Published on line. 31/01/2019 1
Cited as:
Dao, P. Effects of task goal orientation on learner engagement in task performance. International Review
of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching.
Abstract
The study investigated the potential impact of task goal orientation on cognitive, social
and emotional aspects of task performance through the lens of learner engagement. Sixteen EFL
task. Their audio-recorded interactions were transcribed and coded for evidence of engagement,
engagement). To determine the effects of task goal orientation on learner engagement, scores for
engagement types were compared between two tasks. To understand learners’ perception about
their engagement, posttask exit questionnaire responses were analyzed using content-analysis
approach. Findings showed that learners showed greater cognitive and social engagement in the
engagement. Results are discussed in terms of the role of task goal orientation in promoting
Key words: task goal orientation, convergent, divergent, learner engagement, task-based
interaction
Accepted: 14/01/2019 Published on line. 31/01/2019 2
Cited as:
Dao, P. Effects of task goal orientation on learner engagement in task performance. International Review
of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching.
Introduction
Tasks are often used as the main means to get second language (L2) learners to interact
with each other in the classroom. L2 research has investigated various task features in order to
inform L2 instructors about how to select and design tasks that encourage learners to engage in
interaction (e.g., Baralt, Gurzynski-Weiss, & Kim, 2016; Lambert, Philp, & Nakamura, 2017;
Skehan, 2014). Among the numerous features of tasks examined in previous research, task goal
orientation has drawn much attention among L2 researchers (Lambert & Engler, 2007; Pica,
Kanagy & Falodun, 1993). Task goal is an important feature of a task because the ultimate
purpose of getting L2 learners to carry out tasks is to achieve a non-linguistic task goal through
interaction (Ellis, 2003; Erlam, 2016; Long, 2015; Skehan, 2014). Based on Pica et al.’s (1993)
taxonomy, task goal can be manipulated along the communication purpose and is classified as
having either convergent or divergent goals. Convergent tasks require learners to arrive at a
consensus in order to achieve the task goal. In contrast, divergent tasks diverge learners towards
the task goal during task performance. Two typical tasks representing this classification include
Previous research has shown that learners’ orientation towards the convergent and
divergent task goals affected task performance differently in terms of qualitative and quantitative
uses of language (see Bygate & Samuda, 2009; Jackson, 2007; Keller-Lally, 2006) and learners’
opportunities to receive input, provide feedback, and modify language production (Duff, 1986;
Pica et al., 1993). While a majority of studies on convergent/divergent tasks have focused on the
impact of task goal orientation on learners’ linguistic behavior of language production and
cognitive processes (e.g., negotiation of meaning), little is known about whether the task goal
orientation affects emotional and social aspects of task performance. The current study,
Accepted: 14/01/2019 Published on line. 31/01/2019 3
Cited as:
Dao, P. Effects of task goal orientation on learner engagement in task performance. International Review
of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching.
therefore, took social and emotional aspects into consideration by investigating the potential
effects of task goal orientation on multiple aspects of task performance (i.e., cognitive, emotional
Previous research has showed that task goal orientation manipulated along convergent
and divergent goals impacted the occurrence of negotiation for meaning, an interactional feature
central to L2 learning (Mackey, 2012; Long, 1996). Tasks with a convergent outcome enhanced
turn exchanges, encouraged learners to engage more in negotiation for meaning (Duff, 1986;
Jackson, 2007; Keller-Lally, 2006), and promoted learners’ collaboration when they worked
toward a single task goal (Skehan, 2001; Wegerif, Mercer, & Dawes, 1999). However, divergent
tasks were more likely to induce learners to use more complex syntactic structures in their
language production than were convergent tasks (Skehan & Foster, 2001). Despite providing
insights into the different kinds of interaction that each task goal promoted, this body of research
examined only the impact of convergent/divergent task goals on negotiation for meaning (i.e.,
cognitive aspect) and language production such as words and turns (i.e., behavioral aspect).
2010; Swain, 2013; van Lier, 2002), recent task research has expanded to explore several
different aspects of task-based interaction. One of the research lines in response to this trend is
research that has used the multidimensional methodological framework of engagement (Philp &
Duchesne, 2016; Svalberg, 2009, 2017), which is discussed in the next section.
Task research has recognized the importance of considering various aspects of interaction
when investigating the impacts of task features on task performance (Authors, XXXX; Baralt et
al., 2016; Philp & Duchesne, 2016; Svalberg, 2009, 2017). Much of the recent research on task
engagement has therefore followed the multidimensional framework of engagement. One of the
Accepted: 14/01/2019 Published on line. 31/01/2019 4
Cited as:
Dao, P. Effects of task goal orientation on learner engagement in task performance. International Review
of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching.
influential frameworks is Philp and Duchesne’s model (2016) that conceptualizes task
engagement as “a state of heightened attention and involvement” (p. 51), consisting of four sub-
described as learners’ sustained attention, mental effort, and self-regulation strategies. Emotional
engagement refers to learners’ affective responses during task interaction, with indicators
(Skinner, Kindermann & Furrer, 2009). Behavioral engagement is learners’ on-task or off-task
participation that could be measured through language output. Social engagement reflects
Task research that followed Philp and Duchesne’s model has documented the effects of
task design and implementation condition on engagement. For example, comparing tasks with
teacher-generated content and those with learner-generated content, Lambert et al. (2017) found
that learners produced greater elaborative talk and negotiation for meaning (i.e., cognitive
engagement), more backchannels (i.e., social engagement), more language production and time-
on task (i.e., behavioral engagement) in the learner- than teacher-generated content tasks. Using
similar measures of task engagement, Phung (2017) reported that learners were more
cognitively, socially and behaviorally engaged in tasks that they preferred compared to less-
preferred tasks. Regarding task implementation condition, previous research showed that
learners were less engaged in tasks that were administered repeatedly, but showed greater
cognitive and behavioral engagement in tasks that have familiar topics (Qiu & Lo, 2017). In
addition, paring low proficiency learners with higher proficiency partners promoted greater
production of ideas units (i.e., cognitive engagement) and responsiveness (i.e., social
Following previous research that has emphasized the multifacetedness of interaction and
between the behavioral engagement and other types of engagement (Oga-Baldwin & Nakata,
2017; Reeve, 2012; Reeve & Tseng, 2011), engagement has been conceptualized as consisting of
cognitive, social and emotional dimensions that were described through behavioral indicators
(Authors, XXXX). In addition, indicators of learner engagement may vary depending on task
types, suggesting that more measures are needed to capture fully different aspects of engagement
(Philp & Duchesne, 2016). Thus, different measures of engagement are used, including idea
units and language-related episodes (LREs) as measures of cognitive engagement (Baralt et al.,
2016; Helme & Clarke, 2001; Toth, Wagner & Moranski, 2013), instances of responsiveness as a
measure of social engagement (Authors, XXXX), and instances of explicit task enjoyment and
Furrer, 2009).
negotiation for meaning and language production (Duff, 1986; Jackson, 2007; Keller-Lally,
2006; Skehan & Foster, 2001; Smith, 2003). Little research has investigated whether learners’
orientation towards these convergent/divergent task goals affect emotional and social aspects of
interaction. Thus, the current study explores the potential impact of task goal orientation on
orientation has been shown to promote different kinds of interaction. It was proposed that learner
engagement would possibly differ as a function of learners’ orientation toward these task goals.
2. What are learners’ perceptions about their task engagement in relation to task goal
orientation?
Accepted: 14/01/2019 Published on line. 31/01/2019 6
Cited as:
Dao, P. Effects of task goal orientation on learner engagement in task performance. International Review
of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching.
Method
Participants
The participants were sixteen dyads formed by 32 Vietnamese undergraduate students (26
females and 6 males) at a university in Vietnam. Although they were recruited from two English
as a Foreign Language (EFL) classes, they knew each other well because they were classmates in
other courses. They ranged in age from 20 to 25 years old (M=22.44; SD= 1.13). They were
enrolled in the same undergraduate program at the time of data collection. Their average English
proficiency based on paper-based TOEFL test was 479.82 (SD = 58.84). They reported to have
studied English at a mean of 8.72 years (SD = 1.98), and did not travel or study in any English-
speaking countries.
Design
A within-groups design was used to examine the effect of task goal orientation on learner
engagement. The independent variable was the task goal orientation operationalized in terms of
convergent versus divergent outcomes. While the convergent outcome was manipulated by
divergent outcome required learners to defend their opinions and argue against partner’s
viewpoint on the topic of shopping online versus at the store. The dependent variable was learner
engagement (Authors, XXXX). Cognitive engagement was learners’ attention and discussion
about task content and language aspects. Social engagement was degree of learners’
aroused during interaction, for example, enjoyment, interest, excitement, enthusiasm or boredom
The tasks
Accepted: 14/01/2019 Published on line. 31/01/2019 7
Cited as:
Dao, P. Effects of task goal orientation on learner engagement in task performance. International Review
of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching.
Two different tasks were used in the study. The decision-making task with a convergent
goal orientation asked the learners to discuss with their partners in order to identify the problems
existing in their university and decide on solutions to these problems. At the end of the task, the
learners submitted a list of problems and solutions that they agreed on, which they used later to
write a report. The opinion-exchange task with a divergent goal orientation asked the learners to
store-based system for their newly co-owned business. That is, one learner needed to defend the
opinion that “online shopping is more advantageous and convenient than shopping at the store,
and therefore investment in an online shopping website for their newly co-owned business would
be more profit-beneficial”, while the other student had to defend the opinion that “shopping at
the store is more advantageous and convenient than online shopping, and therefore investment in
a store-based system for their newly co-owned business would be more profit-beneficial”. At the
end of the task, the learners wrote down a list of reasons to defend their opinions and explanation
to address their partner’s counter-arguments. The reasons and explanation were then used in
order to write a report that suggests why investment in either an online-shopping website or a
store-based system is a good proposal. More details regarding the convergent decision-making
and the divergent opinion-exchange tasks following Pica et al.’s (1993) task typology are
presented in Table 1.
Table 1.
to address counter-arguments)
Accepted: 14/01/2019 Published on line. 31/01/2019 8
Cited as:
Dao, P. Effects of task goal orientation on learner engagement in task performance. International Review
of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching.
The convergent decision-making and divergent opinion-exchange tasks were selected because of
both theoretical and practical reasons. In terms of theoretical reasons, the convergent decision-
making and divergent opinion-exchange tasks shared both similarities and differences in task
parameters as shown in Table 1. Based on Pica et al.’s (1993) task taxonomy, both convergent
requiring interlocutors to exchange information during the interaction. The outcome options of
both tasks involved a range of acceptable task outcomes (i.e., possibility for many opened
outcomes instead of a single and predetermined answer). However, the two tasks differed in
terms of the goal orientation (i.e., achieve consensus on the shared outcome versus diverge to
meet the task goal). In addition to the different goal orientation, the convergent decision-making
task was likely to promote discussion-based interaction with the inclination toward agreement,
whereas the divergent opinion exchange task was supposed to promote argumentation-based
With regard to practical reasons, both tasks were included in the learners’ syllabus and
course materials, and the teachers of the participants reported to have used them frequently in
their previous teaching activities. The two task topics (university issues and shopping) matched
the themes covered in the learners’ theme-based course materials. To reduce a possibility that
task topic might have impacted learner engagement, the two topics were selected based on the
informal survey that reported university and shopping topics as the learners’ two most favorite
topics.
Accepted: 14/01/2019 Published on line. 31/01/2019 9
Cited as:
Dao, P. Effects of task goal orientation on learner engagement in task performance. International Review
of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching.
Questionnaire materials
The exit questionnaire required learners to provide descriptive answers to ten open-ended
questions adapted from Baralt et al.’s (2016) study. The goal of the exit questionnaire was to
gain insight into learners’ perceptions about their own engagement (cognitive, social, and
emotional) in relation to the perceived task goal orientation and the task topics. All questions
were piloted and revised accordingly, and the pilot participants reported that questions were clear
and easy to understand. To facilitate comparison of the effects of task type on learner
engagement, the instructions emphasized that the participants had to compare the two tasks when
providing written responses. Thus, each question included two answer boxes next to each other
so that the participants could compare their answers to the same question for each task.
few instances of explicitly expressed emotions in the interactions (Authors, XXXX). The
emotional engagement questionnaire consisted of five Likert scale questions that investigated
learners’ reported emotions during their interaction with partners. The five questions asked
learners to indicate, using a 10-point scale, how much they felt enjoyable, interested, excited,
enthusiastic and bored. Questionnaire items were ‘I felt enjoyable when interacting and doing the
task’, ‘I felt interested when interacting and doing the task’, ‘I felt excited when interacting and
doing the task’, ‘I felt enthusiastic when interacting and doing the task’, and ‘I felt bored when
interacting and doing the task’. The reliability of the questionnaire items using Cronbach’s alpha
was .88.
Procedure
The learners carried out the two tasks during their regularly scheduled English class
meetings. An equal number of 16 pairs had their class in the morning and in the afternoon. First,
the researcher introduced the research project and answered questions from the participants (5
Accepted: 14/01/2019 Published on line. 31/01/2019 10
Cited as:
Dao, P. Effects of task goal orientation on learner engagement in task performance. International Review
of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching.
minutes) who completed the consent form and the background information questionnaire (10
minutes). Then, the participants selected their partners and formed dyads to carry out the tasks
within 10 minutes. Each pair’s interaction was audio-recorded using a portable voice recorder.
To counterbalance task sequence, pairs from the morning class were asked to do the decision-
making task first and then the opinion-exchange task, whereas pairs in the afternoon class did the
two tasks in the opposite order. The learners completed the emotional engagement questionnaire
twice (after each interaction), and filled out the exit questionnaire once. The researcher also
talked informally to learners about their answers in the exit questionnaire with the main goal of
clarifying their answers in order to gain better understanding of their perceptions about their
Analysis
To address the first research question that asked whether there was difference in learner
engagement between two tasks, the audio-recordings were first transcribed by a research
assistant and verified by the researcher who later coded all dialogues for three types of
and formal aspects of languages, consequently measured by idea units and LREs, respectively.
Idea units that taped into learners’ production of task content were defined as a segment of
information, idea or comment about the theme under discussion (Ellis & Barkhuizen, 2005;
Lambert et al., 2016; McCarthy, 1991; Shin, Lidster, Sabraw, & Yeager, 2016). An example of
idea units taken from Pair 02 in the divergent opinion-exchange task is shown in Excerpt 1
1 P1: For shopping at the store you have a chance to touch the material of the
products and also you can fit on your body whether it fit with your
measurement or not and also you have a chance to purchase –uh can
2 P2: I think shopping online you can also purchase price with the sale off
Excerpt 1 has four idea units. Learner 1 produced three idea units to argue for the benefits of
shopping at the store or the market: (1) you have a chance to touch the material of the products,
(2) you can fit on your body whether it fit with your measurement, and (3) you have a chance to
purchase–uh can reduce the cost with the seller. Learner 2 generated one idea unit to provide a
rationale for the benefits shopping online: you can also purchase price with the sale off.
Following Swain and Lapkin’s (1998), LREs were defined as talk episodes where
“learners talk about the language they are producing, question their language use, or correct
themselves or others” (p. 326). Excerpt 2 taken from Pair 10 in the convergent decision-making
Excerpt 2. An LRE
1 P1: Actually you know we are last year student so I think that there are
many of our friends they drop out of out this school can you can you
3 P1: Oh it means they cut they cut the class and they don’t want to learn any
more.
In Excerpt 2, Learner 2 did not understand the lexical item drop out, so asked for clarification
drop out what do you mean (line 2). Learner 1 responded by explaining the meaning of the
lexical item it means they cut the class and they don’t wan to learn any more (line 3). Learner 2
acknowledged ah and continued the task I think maybe they have their own (line 4). Since both
learners were involved in the LRE, it was counted that each learner had one instance of LRE in
this excerpt.
Accepted: 14/01/2019 Published on line. 31/01/2019 12
Cited as:
Dao, P. Effects of task goal orientation on learner engagement in task performance. International Review
of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching.
instances of explicit task enjoyment (Authors, XXXX). Excerpt 3 taken from Pair 01 in the
In Excerpt 3, when learner 2 had difficulties to explain why shopping at the traditional market is
more suitable to people in the area, learner 1 laughed and commented that her partner did not
have strong rationales for his opinion (line 1). This comment also made the learner 2 laugh
accordingly (line 2). Because both learners expressed having fun when doing the task, it was
instances of responsiveness that was based on the concept of mutuality (Storch, 2001). An
instance of responsiveness was a talk episode in which learners responded and engaged with
developing each other’s idea or providing backchannels. Excerpt 4 taken from Pair 10,
convergent decision-making task, illustrates an instance of learner 2’s social engagement with
learner 1.
In Excerpt 4, the learners discussed about the teachers at their university. When learner 1 said
that the teachers were unfair (line 1), learner 2 responded by repeating unfair to show agreement
For inter-reliability of the coding, a second rater coded independently 25% of the data.
Pearson correlation r was .92 for idea units, .86 for LREs, .96 for task enjoyment episodes, and
.97 for responsiveness instances. Identified instances of idea units, LREs, positive emotions, and
responsiveness were summed per interaction across two tasks. Although time allotted for each
interaction was restricted to ten minutes, to further control the effect of difference in speech
quantities, a ratio of instances for each engagement measure to total turns was calculated by
dividing the total number of instances of idea unit, LREs, positive emotions, and responsiveness
by total turns. Due to non-normal distribution of the data, Wilcoxon signed-rank tests were
performed to compare the ratios of instances in all coding categories between the two tasks. For
the emotional engagement questionnaire, a mean score for each learner was obtained by
averaging the five items on each questionnaire, and then compared between two tasks. To
answer the second research question, which asked learners about their cognitive, social and
emotional engagement in relation to task goal orientation, learners’ responses in the exit
questionnaire were qualitatively analyzed using content-analysis method (Braun & Clark, 2006).
Results
decision-making task and divergent opinion-exchange task, instances of idea units, LREs, task
instances for each engagement type per interaction across the two tasks were conducted and
presented in Table 2.
Table 2
M SD M SD M SD M SD z p d
Cognitive:
Idea units 42.22 18.76 1.60 1.01 30.97 14.04 .97 .68 4.64 .01 .82
LREs 2.44 2.257 .09 .08 1.63 1.56 .07 .06 2.33 .02 .41
Emotional:
Task enjoyment 3.56 3.23 .11 .10 2.59 2.93 .08 .14 1.57 .13 .27
Social:
Responsiveness 24.91 13.97 .89 .56 14.22 9.59 .42 .33 4.73 .01 .83
As shown in Table 2, the descriptive data showed that the learners demonstrated greater
cognitive, emotional and social engagement in the convergent decision-making task than the
divergent opinion-exchange task across all engagement measures. The Wilcoxon signed-rank
tests showed that there were significant differences in the two measures of cognitive engagement
(i.e., idea units and LREs) and in the measure of social engagement (i.e., responsiveness).
However, no significant difference was observed in the measure of emotional engagement (i.e.,
Table 3 presents the summary of quantitative results from the posttask emotional
engagement questionnaire. The dependent t-test showed no difference in the learners’ reported
Table 3
M SD t df p d
decision-making and the divergent opinion-exchange tasks, the learners’ responses from the exit
questionnaire were qualitatively analyzed using the content analysis approach. The results
showed that learners reported differences in their attention level to each other’s ideas (an
indicator of cognitive engagement) due to the task goal orientation. For instance, in the
convergent decision-making task, learners were asked to identify problems and converge to
decide on solutions to these problems. They appeared to have paid more attention to their
‘It is important for us to listen to each other’s ideas in order to evaluate the
that we could agree on the list in the end…we also had to reason whether the
However, when the learners were asked to defend their opinions in the divergent opinion-
exchange task, they did not seem to pay much attention to each other’s ideas, but just focused on
focused on my reasons and did not care much about whatever she [my
sometimes I ran out of ideas to argue against her…this task was difficult”
Learner’s reports in the exit questionnaire also suggested that the convergent decision-making
task encouraged them to engage more socially with each other. Comment 3 from one learner in
that both of us shared the same view’ [Comment 3, Pair 10, Decision-making
task].
The other participant of this pair also commented positively on their social engagement:
“In this task, I often used expressions that I learnt to show my agreement,
helped and supported my friend when she prompted an idea but could not
finish it… my friend also jumped to help me when I did not know how to
In contrast, the divergent opinion-exchange task tended to lower the learners’ social engagement
my partner always rejected and argued for his preference [Comment 5, Pair
Another learner also reported the difficulties in connecting socially with her partner in the
divergent opinion-exchange task when they were required to defend their opinions and address
ignored her opinions. That’s why I felt bored and wanted to end the
Accepted: 14/01/2019 Published on line. 31/01/2019 17
Cited as:
Dao, P. Effects of task goal orientation on learner engagement in task performance. International Review
of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching.
Learners’ responses in the exit questionnaire also revealed information about their emotions. The
majority of the learners reported that they had positive emotions when carrying out the two tasks.
Only two learners such as the learner in Comment 6 expressed explicitly her negative emotions
due to the divergent task goal. That is, she felt bored when defending her opinions and argued
against her partner. When asked about their perception towards the task topics, all of the learners
reported positive emotions toward the topics of the two tasks. All the learners used positive
adjectives to describe the task topics: university topic (e.g., very fun and hilarious, exciting,
interestingly ‘hot and realistic’ topic) and shopping topic (e.g., exciting, curious, familiar topic
but interesting). In sum, the qualitative data showed that task goal orientation affected how the
Discussion
The goal of the current study was to investigate whether task goal orientation had an
impact on learner engagement in terms of the cognitive, social and emotional dimensions. The
quantitative results showed that the learners produced more idea units, and were engaged in
more LREs and responsiveness instances, showing that they were more cognitively and socially
engaged in the convergent decision-making task than the divergent opinion-exchange task.
However, no significant difference was observed for instances of task enjoyment (an indicator of
emotional engagement) and scores of reported emotions between two tasks. The qualitative
analysis also revealed similar results that the learners were more cognitively and socially
engaged in the convergent decision-making task than the divergent opinion-exchange task.
The learners’ higher generation of idea units in the convergent decision-making task
suggests that when the learners converged on the same task goal, they were more likely to pay
attention each other’s ideas and produced task contents (see Comment 1). This finding supports
Accepted: 14/01/2019 Published on line. 31/01/2019 18
Cited as:
Dao, P. Effects of task goal orientation on learner engagement in task performance. International Review
of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching.
previous research suggesting that tasks with a convergent goal orientation encouraged learners to
talk, negotiate for meaning, and exchange more turns (Jackson, 2007; Keller-Lally, 2016; Smith,
2003). The smaller number of idea units generated in the divergent opinion-exchange task
suggests that the divergent goal orientation tasks tended to encourage less production of idea
units than the convergent goal orientation tasks. This corroborated Pica et al.’ (1993) suggestion
that the divergent opinion-exchange task is the least effective type of task for promoting
learners’ interaction, as compared to other task types such as the convergent decision-making
task. In sum, tasks that are manipulated along the task goal orientation affected learners’
attention and mental effort (i.e., cognitive engagement) when producing task content.
The greater production of LREs in the convergent decision-making task also showed that
convergent goal orientation tasks promoted greater learners’ attention to language form than
divergent goal orientation tasks. However, it should be noted that the number of LREs observed
in both tasks was small (i.e., fewer than three instances per interaction). This finding supports
previous research suggesting that learners tended to focus on conveying the messages rather than
attending to language form in meaning-focused tasks (Authors, XXX; Storch & Aldosari, 2013;
Young & Tedick, 2016). The small number of LREs in both tasks also corroborates previous
research findings that learners rarely generated LREs in purely communicative tasks (Philp,
Another finding was that the learners demonstrated greater responsiveness to partners in
the convergent decision-making task than the divergent opinion-exchange task. These results
suggest that the learners were more socially engaged with each other in the tasks with
convergent goal orientation than those with the divergent goal orientation. The learners’ greater
instances of responsiveness – also corroborated with the learners’ self-report (see Comments 3, 4
and 5). These comments highlight that the convergent decision-making task promotes learners’
Accepted: 14/01/2019 Published on line. 31/01/2019 19
Cited as:
Dao, P. Effects of task goal orientation on learner engagement in task performance. International Review
of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching.
social engagement such as their willingness to listen to each other (Baralt et al., 2016; Svalberg,
2009), reciprocity (Author, XXXX; Damon & Phelp, 1989), and mutual support (Philp &
In contrast, the divergent opinion-exchange task did not seem to encourage learners’
social engagement as reflected in fewer instances of responsiveness and the learners’ Comment
6. It seems that the learners in the divergent opinion-exchange task did not perceive the divergent
task goal to be meaningful when asked to defend their opinions and argue against each other.
When task goals are not perceived to be meaningful, the learners might not feel encouraged, thus
invested less in doing the task (Egbert, 2003; Lambert & Minn, 2007; Maehr, 1984), and failed
to use all available resources to complete the task (Bygate & Samuda, 2009). Therefore, the
learners’ negative perception toward the meaning of the task goal orientation in the divergent
opinion-exchange task might have affected the degree of their willingness to interact with the
partners (Baralt et al., 2016; Svalberg, 2017), suggesting the low mutuality (Author, XXXX;
Galaczi, 2008) and the mechanic interaction in which learners passively received
With regard to the learners’ emotions, the learners reported positive emotions by using
positive adjectives to describe the tasks. The quantitative results also showed that there were no
differences between the two tasks in terms of instances of task enjoyment and learners’ reported
emotions. The learners’ positive reactions to both tasks suggest that task goal orientation did not
affect learners’ emotional engagement significantly. However, it should be noted that the learner
boredom, which suggests that there was a case where the divergent opinion-exchange task
Finally, one may argue that task topics might influence the extent to which learners are
engaged in tasks (Phung, 2017). The learners in the current study reported that that task topics
Accepted: 14/01/2019 Published on line. 31/01/2019 20
Cited as:
Dao, P. Effects of task goal orientation on learner engagement in task performance. International Review
of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching.
did not affect their interaction. It was possible that the topics for the two tasks were selected
based on learners’ preference, so they did not appear to affect their engagement in tasks.
The results suggested some pedagogical implications. First, since task goal orientation
affected learners’ cognitive and social engagement, it is important to take this factor into
consideration when designing tasks for language classroom activities. Second, the learners
reported positive emotions about task topics that were selected based on their suggestion or
topics in order to create positive impacts on their performance (Egbert, 2003; Lambert et al.,
2017; Phung, 2017). One possible way to elicit information about the task topics that learners
The study has limitations that need to be kept in mind when interpreting the results. First,
although the tasks used in the present study represented the typical tasks that are used frequently
in the participants’ EFL classes, they did not reflect a wide range of tasks that teachers used in
the program. Thus, it is worth exploring different types of tasks to determine the impacts of each
particular type of task in relation to task goal orientation on learner engagement. Second, the
study did not explore the individual differences such as learners’ belief or mindset (Sato, 2017)
as well as the contextual factors that may also play a role in affecting how they engage in tasks,
particularly for those tasks that require the interactants to defend their arguments (i.e., divergent
goal or debate tasks). Thus, future research may need to explore the impact of these factors in
combination with task goal orientation to shed light on their possible combined effect on learner
engagement.
Conclusion
The current study provides evidence that task goal orientation operationalized as
divergent versus convergent task outcomes affected the learner’s cognitive and social
engagement. Findings suggest that designing tasks with a convergent goal orientation is
Accepted: 14/01/2019 Published on line. 31/01/2019 21
Cited as:
Dao, P. Effects of task goal orientation on learner engagement in task performance. International Review
of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching.
potentially effective in enhancing greater cognitive engagement (i.e., production of idea units
and LREs) and promoting better responsiveness between learners (i.e., social engagement). The
results also point to the importance of considering task goal orientation, when selecting and
designing tasks for effective language learning activities. To conclude, the study provides insight
into peer task-based interaction in light of learner engagement, with task goal orientation
References
Baralt, M., Gurzynski-Weiss, L., & Kim, Y. (2016). The effects of task complexity and
Ballinger (Eds.), Peer interaction and second language learning: Pedagogical potential
perspective on language use and language learning, (pp. 3–23). Oxford: OxFord
University Press.
Braun, V. & Clarke, V. (2006). Using thematic analysis in psychology. Qualitative Research in
Psychology, 3, 77-101.
Bygate, M., & Samuda, V. (2009). Creating pressure in task pedagogy: The joint roles of field,
purpose, and engagement within the interaction approach. In A. Mackey & C. Polio (Eds.),
Damon, W., & Phelps, E. (1989). Critical distinctions among three approaches to peer education.
Duff, P. A. (1986). Another look at interlanguage talk: Taking task to task. In R. R. Day (Ed.),
Egbert, J. (2003). A study of flow theory in the foreign language classroom. Modern Language
Ellis, R. (2003). Task-based Language Learning and Teaching. Oxford, New York: Oxford
Applied Linguistics.
Ellis, R., & Barkhuizen, G. (2005). AnalyzingLlearner Language. Oxford: Oxford University
Press.
Accepted: 14/01/2019 Published on line. 31/01/2019 23
Cited as:
Dao, P. Effects of task goal orientation on learner engagement in task performance. International Review
of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching.
Erlam, R. (2016). I’m still not sure what a task is: Teachers designing language tasks. Language
Galaczi, E. D. (2008). Peer–peer interaction in a speaking test: The case of the First Certificate in
Helme, S., & Clarke, D. (2001). Identifying cognitive engagement in the Mathematics
Jackson, D., (2007). Another look at convergent and divergent tasks: evidence from synchronous
Keller-Lally, A. (2006). Effect of task-type and group size on foreign language learner output in
Lambert, C., & Engler, S. (2007). Information distribution and goal orientation in second
Lambert, C., & Minn, D. (2007). Personal investment in L2 task design and learning: A case
Lambert, C., Philp, J., & Nakamura, S. (2017). Learner-generated content and engagement in
Leeser, M. (2004). Learner proficiency and focus on form during collaborative dialogue.
Long, M. (1996). The role of the linguistic environment in second language acquisition. In W. C.
Ritchie & T. K. Bahatia (Eds.), Handbook of Research on Language Acquisition (pp. 413–
Chichester: Wiley-Blackwell.
Mackey, A. (2012). Input, interaction and corrective feedback in L2 classroom. Oxford: Oxford
University Press.
Ames, & C. Ames (Eds.), Motivation in Education: Student Motivation (pp. 115–144). San
University Press.
Oga-Baldwin, W., & Nakata, Y. (2017). Engagement, gender, and motivation: A predictive
Philp, J., & Duchesne, S. (2016). Exploring engagement in tasks in the language classroom.
Philp, J., Walter, S. & Basturkmen, H. (2010). Peer interaction in the foreign language
classroom: what factor foster a focus on form? Language Awareness, 19, 261-279.
Pica, T., Kanagy, R. & Falodun, J. (1993). Choosing and using communication tasks for second
language instruction and research. In G. Crookes & S. M. Gass (Eds.), Tasks and
language learning: Integrating theory and practice (pp. 9-34). Clevedon: Multilingual
Matters.
Qiu, X. & Lo, Y. Y. (2017) Content familiarity, task repetition and Chinese EFL learners’
engagement in second language use. Language Teaching Research, 21, 681 –698.
Accepted: 14/01/2019 Published on line. 31/01/2019 25
Cited as:
Dao, P. Effects of task goal orientation on learner engagement in task performance. International Review
of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching.
Reeve, J., & Tseng, C. (2011). Agency as a fourth aspect of students' engagement during
Shin, S-Y., Lidster R, Sabraw. S., Yeager, R. (2016). The effects of L2 proficiency differences
Benjamins.
Skehan, P. & Foster, P. (2001). Cognition and tasks. In P. Robinson (Ed.), Cognition and Second
Skinner, E., Kindermann, T., & Furrer, C. (2009). A motivational perspective on engagement
Storch, N. (2001). How collaborative is pair work? ESL tertiary students composing in pairs.
Storch, N. (2008). Metatalk in a pair work activity: Level of engagement and implications for
Accepted: 14/01/2019 Published on line. 31/01/2019 26
Cited as:
Dao, P. Effects of task goal orientation on learner engagement in task performance. International Review
of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching.
Storch, N., & Aldosari, A. (2013). Pairing learners in pair work activity. Language Teaching
Swain, M. (2013). The inseparability of cognition and emotion in second language learning.
Swain, M., & Lapkin, S. (1998). Interaction and second language learning: Two adolescent
French immersion students working together. Modern Language Journal, 82, 320–337.
Toth, P. D., Wagner, E., & Moranski, K. (2013). Co-constructing explicit L2 knowledge with
high school Spanish learners through guided induction. Applied Linguistics, 34, 255-278.
London: Continuum.
Wegerif, R., Mercer, N., & Dawes, L. (1999). From social interaction to individual reasoning:
Williams, J. (2001). Learner-generated attention to form. Language Learning, 51, 303- 34.
In Sato, M. & Ballinger, S. (Eds.) Peer interaction and second language learning:
Pedagogical potential and research agenda, (pp. 135 –160). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Accepted: 14/01/2019 Published on line. 31/01/2019 27
Cited as:
Dao, P. Effects of task goal orientation on learner engagement in task performance. International Review
of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching.
Appendix
Instruction: Read the questions and provide answers in the boxes next to each question
Conversation 1 Conversation 2
Questions
What was your thinking about the task goal/outcome? And how
What was your overall perception of the task that you just did with
your partner?
What features of language did you notice during the task? Apart
Do you think that you and your partner were both equally willing
Provide three adjectives to describe how you felt during the task?