framework for analysis
framework for analysis
framework for analysis
DOI 10.1007/s10857-005-0852-6
INTRODUCTION
Geometric Knowledge
Students’ understandings of geometry have received considerable
attention in most curriculum documents, and this area is regarded as
providing important foundations for appreciation of other mathemat-
ics topics such as algebra. K-12 mathematics curriculum documents
(Board of Studies, 2002; National Council of Teachers of Mathemat-
ics, 2000) have identified geometric knowledge under various themes
such as spatial concepts, attributes of 2-dimensional (2-D) and 3-
dimensional (3-D) shapes, plane geometry, deductive geometry and
coordinate geometry. All these areas involve spatial thinking and use
of conventions in geometry diagrammatic representations. Vinner and
Dreyfus (1989) suggested that formal concept definitions and images as
provided in the curriculum may be different from images of the con-
cept that individual students develop. In a traditional classroom,
teachers may focus on the former at the expense of the latter, which
200 MOHAN CHINNAPPAN AND MICHAEL J. LAWSON
METHOD
Participants
The teachers whose geometric knowledge is the subject of this report
were two of five experienced teachers recruited for a larger project,
along with five novice teachers. The experienced teachers were selected
using two criteria: first, they had at least 15 years of mathematics
teaching experience at the high school level; and second, they were all
recommended by their peers or professional subject associations as
exemplary teachers. The maps developed for this report were based on
the knowledge exhibited by two teachers, Gary and Sue. In addition
to having 20 years of teaching experience, Gary was the head of the
mathematics department at his high school in an Australian capital
city and was also involved in the writing of mathematics textbooks for
high school students. Sue was one of the senior mathematics teachers
in a private girls’ school in a different capital city.
The teachers were interviewed individually during school hours and
their responses were audio-taped and videotaped for later transcrip-
tion. They were told that the purpose of the study was to find out
what teachers know about topics in geometry and about the teaching
of these topics.
Procedure
Three interviews were conducted with each participant, each lasting
about 1 h. The interview schedule allowed each participant multiple
opportunities to access knowledge and provided a range of activities
for prompting such access. Throughout the interviews, the teachers
were reminded also to consider geometry knowledge that was relevant
to their teaching. During the first interview the teacher was asked to
talk about a list of focus schemas in the areas of geometry, trigonome-
try, and coordinate geometry that were relevant to the school curricu-
lum and their teaching. In the first instance teachers were asked to talk
about the concept of square (focus schema 1) and their understandings
about the teaching and learning a square. As this was a free recall ses-
sion we asked questions such as: ‘Tell me what you know about
GEOMETRIC KNOWLEDGE FOR TEACHING 205
square’ and ‘Tell me how you would teach square to your students’.
The teachers were invited to use diagrams to explain their thoughts if
they wished to do so. Following teachers’ responses to square, the
teachers were asked similar questions about 12 other focus schemas
(squares, rectangles, lines, similar triangles, congruent triangles, paral-
lel lines, area, coordinates, triangles, right-angled triangles, regular
hexagons, regular octagons, circles). All 13 focus schemas were
required to solve the four problems given to the teachers during the
second interview.
During the second interview, the teachers were asked to think aloud
as they solved four problems. The problem-solving activity was in-
cluded because we anticipated that the application of knowledge to a
problem might lead to activation of knowledge additional to that
accessed in the first free-recall interview. Two of the problems involved
the use of knowledge related to the focus schema square. When the
teachers indicated that they had completed a solution they were asked
if the problem could be solved in any other ways. They were also asked
to comment on any feature of their solution that could be related to
the way their students would solve these problems. For example, teach-
ers were asked, ‘How would you expect your students to tackle this
problem’, ‘What type of difficulties would you expect your students to
experience if they are given these problem, Why?’ The above prompts
were expected to elicit teachers’ further understandings about how their
students would approach the problem thus providing data about how
teachers integrate knowledge about focus schemas (from Interview 1)
and the use of these schemas in a problem situation.
The format for the final interview was a series of probing ques-
tions designed to give the teacher the opportunity to access relevant
knowledge that had not been activated in the previous two inter-
views. For example, in a discussion with a teacher who had not yet
mentioned symmetry, we might have commented; ‘You haven’t said
anything about symmetry of a square yet’. Or if a particular rela-
tionship had not been discussed we asked: ‘How are these two con-
cepts related?’
The activities in the three interviews were designed to provide good
estimates of the teachers’ knowledge in the area of geometry and
knowledge for teaching geometry. We can never be certain that we
have tapped all that a teacher might have constructed and decon-
structed about a specific topic. However, we argue that the use of the
free-recall, problem solving and detailed probing activities did provide
a good estimate of the functionally available knowledge of geometry
and for teaching geometry.
206 MOHAN CHINNAPPAN AND MICHAEL J. LAWSON
Map Structure
We adopted a simple form of representation for the node-link struc-
ture for the maps that can be used to identify teachers’ knowledge of
geometry (KG) and knowledge of geometry for teaching (KGT). The
boxes and ovals, or nodes, in the maps indicated schemas and fea-
tures, and the lines joining the boxes/ovals showed that a relationship
was expressed between a schema and features, or between schemas.
We identified four areas of KG and KGT about the focus schema and
the relations that had been built around that schema.
1. The defining features of the focus schema (Defining Features)
The term ‘defining features’ is used here to refer to necessary prop-
erties of the focus schema. For example, in the case of square, these
features are that the sides are equal, all the interior angles are equal,
and the opposite sides are parallel.
2. The related features of the focus schema (Related Features)
Related features of the focus schema include information that one
could derive by going beyond the basic defining properties of the
schema. For instance, information about the formula for determin-
ing the area of a square would fall into this category of related
features.
3. Relationships between the focus schema and other schemas (Other
Schemas)
In addition to information that was activated about a particular fo-
cus schema, teachers would also make links between the focus sche-
ma and other geometric figures, such as the connection between a
square and a rhombus.
4. Other representations of the focus schema (Applications)
In this section of the map we considered the different representations
of the focus schema, such as analogies, metaphors, illustrations or
GEOMETRIC KNOWLEDGE FOR TEACHING 207
real life examples. For example, the idea that some floor tiles are
square in shape, or that tessellation of tiles is used in tiling an area,
falls into this category of information.
The above-mentioned four areas reflect the complex structure of fo-
cus concepts such as that of square that evolves from single words
that Gray et al. (1999) refer to in their analysis of role of language in
perceptions about shapes. Defining and related features are related to
the notion of concept images (Vinner & Dreyfus, 1989). The frame-
work shows emergence of hierarchies that emanate from use of key
words such as square. Gray et al. (1999) suggest that the mental ima-
ges and physical objects that are supported by the linguistic descrip-
tions could evolve in a ‘more pure and imaginative way’.
A1 A2 S1 S2
Other
Applications
Schemas
FOCUS
SCHEMA
Defining Related
Features Features
D1 D2 R1 R2
Quantity
We interpreted quantity as having two sub-categories: (a) Number of
nodes, and (b) Number of links. The concept maps were analysed in
two ways in order to generate values for ‘Number of nodes’ and
‘Number of links’ listed in Table I. First, we considered nodes that ap-
peared on the first layer away from the focus concept. For example, in
the case of Gary’s concept map (Figure 2), nodes with labels ‘Angles’
and ‘Sides’ were deemed to be located in the first layer, as these had
the first direct link to our focus concept, ‘Square’. We then counted
the number of nodes that evolved from each of these Layer 1 nodes,
and added one more for the starting node. This procedure yielded the
result 5 for both ‘Angles’ and ‘Sides’ for Gary. Only nodes at the first
layer were considered in this analysis. A similar procedure was fol-
lowed for the counting of links, which included the link from the focus
concept to the layer one node in question. Again, for Gary we ob-
tained 5 links for ‘Angles’ and ‘Sides’.
Cross-links made between Defining and Related features were also
identified. In Gary’s map there were three cross-links between Related
features and the Sides node and the score of 3 is shown under Cross-
linking in Table I. This analysis of the concept map in order to gener-
ate values for quantitative indicators of knowledge connectedness was
TABLE I 210
Within-Schema Indicators of Knowledge Connectedness
Integrity Connectedness
Teachera G S G S G S G S G S G S G S G S G S G S
Defining features
Angles 5 4 5 4 L3 L3 M M 0 0 0 5 4
Sides 5 5 5 5 L3 L3 M M 3 1 0 5 5
Subtotal 10 9 10 9 H H H H 0 0 10 9 0 0
Related features
Area 2 2 2 2 L2 L2 L L 0 0 0 0 2 2
Perimeter 2 2 L2 L 0 0 2
Symmetry 10 3 10 3 L3 L3 H L 0 0 0 0 9 3 1
Tessellation 1 1 L1 L 0 0 1
MOHAN CHINNAPPAN AND MICHAEL J. LAWSON
Diagonals 1 2 L1 L 0 0 0 0 1 1
Subtotal 15 6 15 7 H H 14 6 1 1
Applications 0 0 0 0
Total 25 15 25 16 3 1 0 0 24 15 1 1
a
G=teacher Gary; S=teacher Sue.
b
For Integrity and branching H=High rating.
c
For depth L1, L2, L3=Level 1 ratings, Level 2 ratings, Level 3 ratings.
GEOMETRIC KNOWLEDGE FOR TEACHING 211
is a
is half of Rectangle
Equal is a
length is a subset of is a subset of
is not a
SQUARE
has
has has
has 2
has Reflection
Angles symmetry has
has Area
Sides
lines of
is symmetry number
has
are
are sum formed by
Rotation
symmetry side
number Equal squared number
number length formed by
has Perpendicular
Perimeter lines
Equal
360
degrees is 4
order
bisect
each Diagonals
4 4
Opposite Tessellates
sides
90 are 4 times number Opposite
degrees side 4 sides
parallel
2
Quality
The quality of the concept maps was analysed for the two broad
dimensions of integrity and connectedness. Integrity was analysed in
terms of (a) completeness and (b) accuracy. A judgement of integrity
here is not an absolute judgement about what a teacher knows, but
was seen more functionally as a rating of the knowledge that the tea-
cher was prompted to access by the range of specific research proce-
dures used here. A rating of ‘High’ for completeness indicated the
presence of all defining features. A rating of ‘Moderate’ for complete-
ness was assigned if one defining feature was missing and a rating of
‘Low’ was given if more than one defining feature was missing.
Accuracy refers to the degree of correctness of the information pro-
vided by teachers. Information that is not correct may be manifested in
various forms. An incorrect piece of knowledge could be a misconcep-
tion (McKeown & Beck, 1990) or it could be ‘garbled’ (Perkins & Sim-
mons, 1988). For example, if a teacher could not differentiate between
the lines of symmetry in a square and a rhombus this would be a case
of a misconception. If there was no evidence of garbled knowledge or
TABLE II 212
Between-Schema Indicators of Knowledge Connectedness
Integrity Connectedness
Teachera G S G S G S G S G S G S G S G S G S
Layer 1 Schema
Quadrilateral 3 3 L3 1 3
Rhombus 4 1 7 2 L3 L1 L 1 3 1 4 1
Polygon 1 1 L1 1
Isoc rt triangle 3 4 L2 L 1 3 1
Right angle
Rectangle 1 1 2 2 L1 L1 1 2 1
Right triangle 3 3 L2 L 1 2 1
Face 2 2 L2 2
Total 12 7 17 9 H H 3 1 3 2 7 5 7 2
MOHAN CHINNAPPAN AND MICHAEL J. LAWSON
a
G=teacher Gary; S=teacher Sue.
b
For Integrity and branching H=High rating.
c
For depth L1, L2, L3=Level 1 ratings, Level 2 ratings, Level 3 ratings.
GEOMETRIC KNOWLEDGE FOR TEACHING 213
Cube
can be
Face
Rectangle
is not a
is a is related to is
is a
SQUARE
has
has
has
has
Angles
Sides Line/
reflection
has symmetry
are number are Area
has
is
Equal
has
Side Lines of
Equal 4 has bisects squared symmetry
each number
4 Diagonals
90 Opposite 4
degrees sides
are
Parallel
Within-Schema Ratings
Results of the analysis for the focus schema of square in terms of
quantity and quality are provided in Tables I and II. Table I con-
tains data on indicators that were internal to the concept of square,
links that indicated within-schema organisation. Gary’s concept map
contained a total of 10 nodes related to the defining features of the
focus schema, and 15 nodes associated with the related features.
Analysis of the number of links among these nodes also showed sim-
ilar scores for defining and related features. The number of nodes
and links in Sue’s map was similar for the defining features, but low-
er for the related features. In terms of the estimates provided by our
analysis Gary’s knowledge of square was more extensive than Sue’s.
However, both teachers have constructed KG that shows the links
between language and images that children need to learn about 2-D
shapes.
In the qualitative analysis there were high ratings for integrity in
the within-schema sections of both teachers’ maps and the ratings for
depth were similar for the two maps. In Gary’s representation of sym-
metry there was a greater extent of branching than that produced by
Sue. This suggests that this is a more elaborated chunk of knowledge
for Gary indicating a higher level of deconstruction of content knowl-
edge to KGT. However, the lack of cross-linking between these two
branches of symmetry indicates that there seems to be little integration
between knowledge about line symmetry and rotational symmetry.
Overall, Gary’s map showed more cross-linking, though none of the
cross-links was explicitly labelled by either teacher. The complexity of
the labels used by the teachers to describe within-schema relationships
was also similar, with most relationships being rated as moderate,
indicating lack of elaboration of these relationships. Here one can de-
tect room for further developments of Gary’s KGT.
Between-Schema Ratings
Table II shows the ratings given to the teachers’ knowledge about
relationships between square and other schemas. Scores were derived
216 MOHAN CHINNAPPAN AND MICHAEL J. LAWSON
for each of the schemas in the first layer of nodes extending from the
focus schema (Layer 1 Schemas). The quantitative analysis showed
that Gary again accessed a larger number of nodes than did Sue and
established a greater number of links among these nodes. Of these, the
highest number of links and nodes accessed by Gary involved the
rhombus schema, suggesting the development of mature KGT in this
area. For both teachers the relationships established were accurate.
With regard to between-schema connectedness, with one exception, the
ratings for both teachers were generally lower than was the case for
their within-schema representations. For both, the depth ratings in the
between-schema analysis were lower on average and there was less evi-
dence of branching. In Gary’s map the links with greatest extent of
depth and branching were those between square and rhombus, rhom-
bus and parallelogram, and rhombus and quadrilateral, thus indicating
the clustering of these schemas.
Gary provided more instances of complex labelling of relationships
than did Sue, though there was no instance where such descriptions in-
volved extensive elaboration. For example, Gary mentioned that an
isosceles right-angled triangle is half of a square. However, he did not
go further and discuss the implications of such relationships in using
or deriving, say, Pythagoras’ theorem, or trigonometric ratios. These
extended connections and elaborations constitute features of well-
developed KGT. Both teachers made explicit links between features of
square and other schemas, with Gary making more of these links than
Sue. In addition, Gary explicitly linked the square with quadrilateral
and rhombus schemas, and linked the latter to the isosceles right-an-
gled triangle schema.
The scoring of the maps provided the detail supporting the inter-
pretation derived from visual inspection of the maps. The specifica-
tion of qualities of the maps allowed us to make judgements that
were not just quantitative ones. Thus, through use of the scoring
system, we were able to make clear that not only did Gary’s output
show a more extensive network of linked nodes, but that more of
the nodes in his map were linked by complex relationships. This
relational information contained in related features and related sche-
mas might be called upon during teaching for problem solving. Our
analysis suggests that in a problem where relationships between, say
a square and isosceles triangles, or between a square and a quadri-
lateral, needed to be ascertained, Gary would more readily access
such knowledge than would Sue because of the better state of his
KGT.
GEOMETRIC KNOWLEDGE FOR TEACHING 217
CONCLUSION
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
NOTES
In the top right-hand corner of Figure 2, two attributes of Other Schemas appear in
circles, while the remaining ones are in ovals. This is due to the technical feature of
the software which was used to construct the map. We do not attribute any signifi-
cance to the ovals and circles.
REFERENCES
Anderson, J.R. (2000). Cognitive psychology and its implications 5th edn. New York:
Worth Publishers.
Ball, D.L. (2000). Bridging practices: Intertwining content and pedagogy in teaching
learning to teach. Journal of Teacher Education, 51(3), 241–247.
Ball, D.L., Lubienski, S.T. & Mewborn, D.S. (2001). Research on teaching
mathematics: The unresolved problem of teachers’ mathematical knowledge. In V.
Richardson (Ed.), Handbook of research on teaching (pp. 433–456). 4 Washington:
American Educational Research Association.
Beissner, K.L., Jonassen, D.H. & Grabowski, B.L. (1993). Using and selecting
graphic techniques to acquire structural knowledge. ERIC Document No.
ED362151.
Board of Studies (2002). K-10 Mathematics Syllabus. Sydney: Department of
Education and Training.
Bruner, J.S. (1966). Toward a theory of instruction. New York: Norton.
Chinnappan, M. (1998a). Schemas and mental models in geometry problem solving.
Educational Studies in Mathematics, 36, 201–217.
Chinnappan, M. (1998b). The accessing of geometry schemas by high school
students. Mathematics Education Research Journal, 10, 27–45.
220 MOHAN CHINNAPPAN AND MICHAEL J. LAWSON
Clark, C. & Peterson, P.L. (1986). Teachers’ thought processes. In M.C. Wittrock
(Ed.), Handbook of research on teaching (pp. 255–297). New York: Macmillan.
Coleman, E. (1993). Inducing a shift from intuitive to scientific knowledge with
inquiry training. In M. Polson (Ed.), Proceedings of the fifteenth annual conference
of the Cognitive Society (pp. 347–352). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.
Feiman-Nemser, S. (1990). Teacher preparation: Structural and conceptual alterna-
tives. In W.R. Houston, M. Haberman, & J. Sikula (Eds), Handbook of research on
teacher education (pp. 212–233). New York: Macmillan.
Gray, E., Pinto, M., Pitta, D. & Tall, D. (1999). Knowledge construction and
divergent thinking in elementary and advanced mathematics. Educational Studies
in Mathematics, 38, 111–133.
Grossman, P.L. (1995). Teachers’ knowledge. In L.W. Anderson (Ed.), International
encyclopaedia of teaching and teacher education (pp. 20–24). 2 Kidlington Oxford,
UK: Elsevier Science Ltd.
Jonassen, D.H., Beissner, K. & Yacci, M.A. (1993). Structural knowledge:
Techniques for conveying, assessing, and acquiring structural knowledge. Hillsdale,
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Knapp, M.S. (1997). Between systemic reforms and the maths and science classroom:
the dynamics of innovation, implementation, and professional learning. Review of
Educational Research, 67, 227–266.
Laturno, J. (1994). The validity of concept maps as a research tool in remedial
mathematics. In D. Kirshner (Ed.), Proceedings of the sixteenth annual meeting of
the North American Chapter of the International Group for the Psychology of
Mathematics Education (Vol. 2, pp. 60–66). Baton Rouge: Louisiana State
University.
Lawson, M.J. (1994). Concept mapping. In T. Husén & T.N. Postlethwaite (Eds.),
The international encyclopedia of education (2nd ed., Vol. 2, pp. 1026–1031).
Oxford: Elsevier Science.
Lawson, M.J. & Chinnappan, M. (1994). Generative activity during geometry
problem solving: Comparison of the performance of high-achieving and low-
achieving students. Cognition and Instruction, 12, 61–93.
Leinhardt, G. (1987). The development of an expert explanation: An analysis of a
sequence of subtraction lessons. Cognition and Instruction, 4, 225–282.
Markman, K.M., Mintzes, J.J. & Jones, M.G. (1994). The concept map as a research
and evaluation tool: Further evidence of validity. Journal of Research in Science
Teaching, 31, 91–101.
Mayer, R.E. (1975). Information processing variables in learning to solve problems.
Review of Research in Education, 45, 525–541.
McKeown, M. & Beck, I. (1990). The assessment and characterisation of young
learners knowledge of a topic in history. American Educational Research Journal,
27, 688–726.
Munby, H., Russell, T. & Martin, A.K. (2001). Teachers’ knowledge and how it
develops. In V. Richardson (Ed.), Handbook of research on teaching (pp. 877–904).
4 Washington, DC: American Educational Research Association.
National Council of Teachers of Mathematics. (1989). Curriculum and evaluation
standards for school mathematics. Reston, VA: The Council.
National Council of Teachers of Mathematics. (2000). Curriculum and evaluation
standards for school mathematics. Reston, VA: The Council.
GEOMETRIC KNOWLEDGE FOR TEACHING 221
Novak, J. (1990). Concept mapping: A useful device for science education. Journal of
Research in Science Education, 27, 937–949.
Novak, J. & Gowin, D.B. (1984). Learning how to learn. New York: Cambridge
University Press.
Novick, L.R. & Hurley, S.M. (2001). To matrix, network or hierarchy: That is the
question. Cognitive Psychology, 42, 158–216.
Perkins, D.N. & Simmons, R. (1988). Patterns of misunderstanding: An integrative
model for science, math and programming. Review of Educational Research, 58,
303–326.
Robinson, N., Even, R. & Tirosh, D. (1992). Connectedness in teaching algebra: A
novice-expert contrast. In W. Geeslin & K. Graham (Eds), Proceedings of the
psychology of mathematics education conference, PME-XVI (pp. 258–265).
Durham, New Hampshire: PME.
Schoenfeld, A.H. (1988). When good teaching leads to bad results: The disasters of
‘well taught’ mathematics classes. Educational Psychologist, 23, 145–166.
Schoenfeld, A.H. (1992). Learning to think mathematically: Problem solving,
metacognition, and sense making in mathematics. In Grouws A. Douglas (Ed.),
Handbook of research on mathematics teaching and learning (pp. 334–370). New
York: Macmillan.
Shavelson, R.J., Lang, H. & Lewin, B. (1993). On concept maps as potential
‘authentic’ assessments in science: Indirect approaches to knowledge representa-
tion of high school science. ERIC Document, No. ED367691.
Shulman, L.S. (1986). Paradigms and research programs in the study of teaching. In
M.C. Wittrock (Ed.), Handbook on research in teaching (pp. 3–36). 3 New York:
MacMillan.
Vinner, S. & Dreyfus, T. (1989). Images and definitions for the concept of function.
Journal for Research for Mathematics Education, 20, 356–366.
Watson, K.L. & Lawson, M.J. (1995). Improving access to knowledge: The effect of
strategy training for question answering in high school geography. British Journal
of Educational Psychology, 65, 97–111.
Williams, C.G. (1998). Using concept maps to assess conceptual knowledge of
function. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 29, 414–421.
Wittrock, M.C. (1990). Generative processes of comprehension. Educational
Psychologist, 24, 345–376.