978-3-031-53414-0
978-3-031-53414-0
978-3-031-53414-0
Second Language
Instruction
A Context-Driven Multimedia
Learning Approach
Ian Pemberton
Usage-Based Second Language Instruction
Ian Pemberton
Usage-Based Second
Language Instruction
A Context-Driven Multimedia
Learning Approach
Ian Pemberton
Warwick Foundation Studies
University of Warwick
Coventry, UK
© The Editor(s) (if applicable) and The Author(s), under exclusive licence to Springer Nature
Switzerland AG 2024
This work is subject to copyright. All rights are solely and exclusively licensed by the Publisher,
whether the whole or part of the material is concerned, specifically the rights of translation,
reprinting, reuse of illustrations, recitation, broadcasting, reproduction on microfilms or in any other
physical way, and transmission or information storage and retrieval, electronic adaptation, computer
software, or by similar or dissimilar methodology now known or hereafter developed.
The use of general descriptive names, registered names, trademarks, service marks, etc. in this
publication does not imply, even in the absence of a specific statement, that such names are exempt
from the relevant protective laws and regulations and therefore free for general use.
The publisher, the authors, and the editors are safe to assume that the advice and information in
this book are believed to be true and accurate at the date of publication. Neither the publisher
nor the authors or the editors give a warranty, expressed or implied, with respect to the material
contained herein or for any errors or omissions that may have been made. The publisher remains
neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
This Palgrave Macmillan imprint is published by the registered company Springer Nature Switzerland
AG
The registered company address is: Gewerbestrasse 11, 6330 Cham, Switzerland
1 Introduction 1
1.1 Rationale 1
1.2 Outline 3
References 4
v
vi Contents
Index 255
1
Introduction
1.1 Rationale
As a reaction to the shortcomings of earlier pedagogies, Communica-
tive Language Teaching has come to dominate language education. In
fact, communicative methods are so well-established amongst language
educators and in teacher training programmes that it is difficult to voice
criticism of them. To teach communicatively is to be a progressive and
enlightened educator. Brown (2001: 44) asserts that ‘no one these days
would admit to a disbelief in principles of CLT; they would be marked as
a heretic.’ In fact, to question the validity of Communicative Language
Teaching can raise concerns about an educator’s ability to teach. Such
an ill-advised admission has the potential invoke capability procedures,
re-education, and, in the worst-case scenario, even redundancy.
The word ‘communicative’ is a strongly emotive concept in a similar
way to concepts such as freedom, justice, and truth. It embodies sharing,
mutual understanding, and tolerance. So much so that when applied to
language teaching, it is difficult to imagine that there could be any other
way. Harmer (1982: 164) notes that ‘no self-respecting teacher, materials
designer, or applied linguist would think of teaching English as anything
else.’
© The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer Nature 1
Switzerland AG 2024
I. Pemberton, Usage-Based Second Language Instruction,
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-031-53414-0_1
2 I. Pemberton
1.2 Outline
The main aims of this book are to build a case for change in the field of
language teaching and to explore in-depth how people learn languages
to better understand what that change might be.
The book begins by building the case for methodological change. It
examines in detail the history of English Language Teaching to firmly
ground its case in the accumulated knowledge and wisdom of those
scholars, educators, and innovators who have searched for answers to the
same questions in the past. It looks at what we can learn from the histor-
ical language teaching methods of the Classical, Reform, and Scientific
Periods, then evaluates the methods of the Communicative Period from
1970 up to the present. This analysis of the history of ELT considers
the benefits and drawbacks of each of the major methods and identifies
important elements of a language teaching approach.
At the same time, it identifies critical weaknesses in the current ortho-
doxy, Communicative Language Teaching, which cast doubt on whether
its methodology actually leads to fluency. Having built a case for change,
the book then considers the question: If CLT is not the hallowed ground
that language educators have been searching for, then what is?
Throughout history, innovative educators have looked to first language
acquisition to answer the question of how to teach second and addi-
tional languages. Thus, a natural starting place in a search for an answer
is to investigate theories of first language acquisition. The book exam-
ines evolutionary biology, making a distinction between primary and
secondary biological skills. It also looks at the classical first language
development theories of Constructivism and Sociocultural Theory, and
their influence on second language pedagogy. Then, it introduces Social-
Pragmatic Theory, a usage-based theory, which offers a cogent explana-
tion of first language acquisition.
Advocates of usage-based theories assert that they apply equally to
second language acquisition. To gain a more detailed understanding of
usage-based linguistics, the book examines its theoretical foundations.
First, it explores a general complexity theory known as Dynamic Systems
Theory. Then, it looks at constructionist grammars. After examining
4 I. Pemberton
References
Brown, H. D. (2001). Teaching by principles: An interactive approach to language
pedagogy (2nd ed.). Prentice Hall Regents.
Hall, G. (2018). Exploring English language teaching: Language in action (2nd
ed.). Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group.
Harmer, J. (1982). What is communicative? ELT Journal, 36 (3), 164–168.
Prabhu, N. S. (1990). There is no best method—Why? TESOL Quarterly,
24 (2), 161–176.
Part I
Making the Case for Change
2
Historical Teaching Approaches
The major teaching pedagogy associated with the Classical Period has
been known by several different names. According to Brown (2001: 18),
until the nineteenth century, it was referred to as the Classical Method.
More recently, Cook (2008: 239) refers to it as the Academic Teaching
Style.
The Grammar-Translation Method has its roots in the study of
the Classics which were written in Latin and Ancient Greek, and the
methods used to study these texts were subsequently applied to the
teaching of foreign languages. According to Cook (2008: 239), its
syllabus consists of grammatical rules and vocabulary items. Instruction
takes place in the students’ mother tongue. In a typical class, the teacher
will go through a written text line-by-line with students, explaining
2 Historical Teaching Approaches 9
starts with the whole language which acts as a basis for imitation
and later analysis. This analysis was particularly ahead of its time,
and the terms re-emerged over a hundred years later in Dave Wilkins’
Notional-Functional Syllabus.
In some respects, Prendergast was also ahead of his time. His Mastery
System was founded on his observations of young children. According
to Richards and Rodgers (2001: 7), he noticed ‘children use contex-
tual and situational cues to interpret utterances.’ Howatt (1984: 157)
explains that these cues included ‘what people do, how they look, their
gestures and facial expressions, and so on.’ Furthermore, he noticed
that children ‘learn ready-made “chunks” of language, “pre-fabs” as they
have been called in recent times, and weave them into their utterances.’
Prendergast mistook these formulaic sequences for sentences, and this
led him to conclude that ‘an efficient foreign language teaching system
would consist entirely of memorized sentences, practised to the point of
instant recall’ (Howatt, 1984: 158). This led to the development of ‘the
first “structural syllabus,” advocating that learners be taught the most
basic structural patterns occurring in the language’ (Richards & Rodgers,
2001: 7). When viewed retrospectively, we can see that his observa-
tions prefigured developments over a hundred years later in language
acquisition theory and teaching pedagogy.
2.3 Summary
A knowledge of the history of ELT is important for teachers. It provides
teachers with information about procedures and techniques that they can
use in their own practice. In addition, knowing the history of a field helps
to avoid repeating its mistakes. The history of ELT is generally divided
into four periods. The first period, the Classical Period, was dominated
by a single approach known as the Grammar-Translation Method. In
the Grammar-Translation Method, learners were taught the grammar of
a language in their mother tongue, and they were expected to translate
the target language into their first language. It has received strong criti-
cism from modern-day advocates of Communicative Language Teaching
because it does not teach ‘communicative competence’. However, its
benefits include lower cognitive demands for beginning students, the
ability to develop reading skill, and the ability to develop an appreciation
of foreign cultures. In addition, critics overlook the fact that a continuous
text creates an authentic context for the language it contains.
The second period, the Reform Period, was a very active historical era.
It was so-called due to attempts by more innovative educators to reform
the way languages were taught. There are four main methods associated
with this period and they are the Rational Method, the Mastery System,
the Series Method, and the Direct Method.
To begin with, Claude Marcel’s Rational Method proposed the idea
of four language skills, the division of language skills into impression
(reception) and expression (production), and that receptive skills should
precede productive skills. Thomas Prendergast’s Mastery System was
based on his observation that children watched what people did and their
facial expressions to interpret language. He was significantly ahead of
his time in noticing that children learnt ready-made chunks of language
14 I. Pemberton
2.4 Implications
A number of points can be distilled from historical teaching methods.
The drawbacks and benefits of each method give an indication of
what language educators might include in an approach, and what they
might avoid. The innovators of the Reform Period observed that the
Grammar-Translation Method did not imbue learners with the ability to
communicate. They recognized that first language acquisition was vastly
more successful than second language learning and developed innovative
methods which drew upon features of first language learning. In their
view, an effective language teaching method would:
• emphasize listening
• map language onto real-world events
• teach prefabricated chunks
• focus on use
2 Historical Teaching Approaches 15
References
Bayley, S. N. (1998). The Direct method and modern language teaching in
England 1880–1918. History of Education, 27 (1), 39–57.
Brown, H. D. (2001). Teaching by principles: An interactive approach to language
pedagogy (2nd ed.). Prentice Hall Regents.
Cook, G. (2003). Applied linguistics. Oxford University Press.
Cook, V. (2008). Second language learning and language teaching (4th ed.).
Hodder Education.
Hall, G. (2018). Exploring English language teaching: Language in action (2nd
ed.). Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group.
Hill, L. A. (1967). Selected articles on the teaching of English as a foreign language.
OUP.
Howatt, A. P. R. (1984). A history of English language teaching. Oxford
University Press.
Howatt, A. P. R., & Smith, R. (2014). The history of teaching english as
a foreign language, from a British and European perspective. Language &
History, 57 (1), 75–95.
Hunter, D. (2009). Communicative language teaching and the ELT Journal: a
corpus-based approach to the history of a discourse. Ph.D. thesis, University of
Warwick.
Krashen, S. D., & Terrell, T. D. (1983). The natural approach: Language
acquisition in the classroom. Pergamon.
Kumaravadivelu, B. (2006). Understanding language teaching: From method to
post-method . Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Lewis, M. (1997). Implementing the lexical approach. Thomson Heinle.
Lopez-Sanchez, A. (2009). Re-writing the goals of foreign language teaching:
The achievement of multiple literacies and symbolic competence. Interna-
tional Journal of Learning, 16 (10), 29–38.
Pennycook, A. (1989). The concept of method, interested knowledge, and the
politics of language teaching. TESOL Quarterly, 23(4), 589–618.
Richards, J. C., & Rodgers, T. S. (2001). Approaches and methods in language
teaching (2nd ed.). Cambridge University Press.
3
The Scientific Period
The Scientific Period is thus called because it was an era during which
there was a trend for linguists to justify their methods in terms of
‘the new social sciences: particularly linguistics but also, increasingly,
learning theory derived from psychology’ (Howatt & Smith, 2014: 85).
The major method that emerged during this era was the Audio-Lingual
Method. While not strictly a teaching method, an analysis of Noam
Chomsky’s Universal Grammar is included here as it is generally consid-
ered to have had a direct influence on the emergence of Communicative
Language Teaching.
Wilkins (1976: 16) points out that it is not easy to define all situa-
tions in which learners will need to use language or their language forms.
He (ibid.: 17) makes the point that while learners have agency over the
language that they produce, they have no control over what the other
participants will say. While he concedes that a situational approach might
be adequate for the limited needs of a tourist, waiter, or telephonist, it
3 The Scientific Period 21
memorize it, and then, they may act it out (Kumaravadivelu, 2006: 106).
After the presentation phase, the class moves on to the practice phase. In
Audiolingualism, language learning is a process of habit formation: repe-
tition leads to greater learning. Therefore, a basic feature of the controlled
practice phase is the extensive use of drills. Kumaravadivelu (2006: 102)
citing Hockett (1959) comments that learners ‘require drill, drill, and
more drill, and only enough vocabulary to make such drills possible.’
These drills can include ‘repetition, backward build-up, chain, substitu-
tion, transformation, and question-and-answer’ (Larsen-Freeman, 2000:
44). A key feature of Audiolingualism is zero-tolerance of errors. As errors
are thought to lead to the formation of bad habits, the teacher seeks to
eliminate them when they occur. Additionally, a class may use a language
laboratory for practice in rhythm, stress, and intonation. Work in the
language lab is then followed by reading and writing practice. Finally, in
the production stage, learners roleplay similar dialogues. Kumaravadivelu
(2006: 106) comments that ‘they are supposed to modify the language
they have memorized in order to vary their production.’
Audiolingualism enjoyed some popularity with both teachers and
learners because it provided a well-structured learning environment.
Kumaravadivelu (2006: 113) notes that it was ‘a theory-driven, system-
atically organized, and teacher-friendly language-centered pedagogy.’
According to Cook (2008: 246), teachers were clear about what they
had to do, and learners were able to relax as they took part in familiar
classroom activities and knew what they were expected to do.
While its goals were practical and communicative, its results were
disappointing. Cook (2008: 244) asserts that its main objective was to
get students to communicate in the real-life situations they were going to
experience such as interacting at the railway station or in a supermarket.
However, it failed to achieve its objectives. Richards and Rodgers (2001:
65) point out that ‘students were often found to be unable to transfer
skills acquired through Audiolingualism to real communication outside
the classroom.’ Krashen and Terrell (1983: 13) explain that ‘methods in
which students never engage in real communication cannot be expected
to produce students able to communicate using the language they study.’
Another criticism is that the nature of its activities failed to engage
learners. Krashen and Terrell (1983: 14) state that teachers and learners
3 The Scientific Period 23
3.6 Summary
The third historical epoch in English language teaching is known as the
Scientific Period. It was so-called because of efforts by educators to link
language teaching methodology to scientific theory. A major milestone
in the Scientific Period was the development of the structural syllabus.
A structural syllabus is an inventory of syntactic structures chosen on
the basis of learnability, utility, and teachability. Structural items can be
such things as sentence patterns, grammar words, tenses, modal verbs,
3 The Scientific Period 27
Specifically, even for motivated learners its drills could be boring, and
there was no engagement in real communication, so it did not prepare
learners for real life.
The 1960s also saw the arrival of Noam Chomsky’s Universal
Grammar. Chomsky came to prominence when he criticized the
behaviouristic view of language learning for being too simplistic. He
argued that the difficulty of learning a language was so great that it
could only be achieved if grammar were an innate property of the
human mind. He theorized that evolution had resulted in a genetically-
based grammar which contained principles which were common to all
languages and parameters which varied within a narrow range. Only
minimal language input was necessary to set the parameters. Chomsky’s
Universal Grammar was praised for re-focusing attention on human
cognition, but it has had a negligible impact on language teaching. It
has also been noted that no principle or parameter is common to all
languages.
In his Universal Grammar, Chomsky made a distinction between
linguistic competence and performance, valorizing competence and
dismissing performance. As a reaction to this, American linguist
Dell Hymes argued that communicative competence, not linguistic
competence should be the objective of school language education.
He considered competence to be based not only on grammatical,
but also psycholinguistic, sociocultural, and probabilistic competen-
cies. His concept of communicative competence was developed into a
more detailed framework by Canale and Swain with four categories,
namely: grammatical, sociolinguistic, discourse, and strategic compe-
tence. However, teaching intercultural competencies may be unnecessary
and may take up time which could be used to learn language.
3.7 Implications
The strengths and weaknesses of the methods of the Scientific Period
further indicate what language educators might embrace in a method and
what they might eschew. The structural syllabus was praised for focusing
attention on language patterns but criticized for not dealing with
3 The Scientific Period 29
References
Canale, M., & Swain, M. (1980). Theoretical bases of communicative
approaches to second language teaching and testing. Applied Linguistics, 1,
1–47.
Chomsky, N. (1959). Review of B. F. Skinner’s verbal behavior. Language, 35,
26–58.
Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the theory of syntax. M.I.T. Press.
Cook, G. (2003). Applied linguistics. Oxford University Press.
Cook, V. (2008). Second language learning and language teaching (4th ed.).
Hodder Education.
Cook, V., & Newsom, M. (1996). Chomsky’s universal grammar: An introduction
(2nd ed.). Blackwell.
Ellidokuzoglu, H. (1996). Decline and fall of communicative approach. KHO
International ELT Conference: From diversity to synergy, Ankara, Turkey.
Ellis, R. (1997). Second Language Acquisition. Oxford University Press.
30 I. Pemberton
Following on from the Scientific Period, there was a marked shift in how
educators regarded language—from a system of rules which could be
systematically introduced and taught to the perspective that the main
function of language was communication, and therefore this should be
the focus of classroom activity. Thus, Howatt and Smith (2014) refer to
the era from 1970 to the present as ‘the Communicative Period.’
Unlike most historical descriptions, this chapter does not deal with
the designer methods of the 1970s (Total Physical Response, The Silent
Way, Community Language Learning, and Suggestopedia). Although
interesting, these methods were limited in scope and while elements of
each method can be used as classroom techniques, they do not represent
complete solutions to the language learning conundrum.
The following chapters are not simply intended as a description of the
history of modern ELT methods as a number of these already exist (e.g.,
Cook, 2008; Howatt & Smith, 2014; Howatt, 1984; Larsen-Freeman &
Anderson, 2011; Richards & Rodgers, 2001). Their purpose is to analyse
modern ELT methods to assess their efficacy and identify important
elements of an effective language teaching approach.
Along the way, it also raises important questions about each of the
major methods. These include:
together the whole language structure (Wilkins, 1976: 2). Yalden (1983:
22) comments that a synthetic approach results in a structural syllabus.
On the other hand, an analytic approach can be taken to syllabus
design. This is a more holistic approach where a language educator
identifies the circumstances in which learners will use a language and
then considers the linguistic forms that they need. It produces either a
situational or a notional syllabus. According to Wilkins (1976: 16), a
structural syllabus considers language to be a general system, but in fact,
language choices are always linked to social context and those choices
are likely specific to the situation, so a situational syllabus is a more
profitable approach. As discussed earlier, this involves identifying the
situations where native speakers use language which results in a more
relevant learner-centred syllabus.
Wilkins (1976: 16) explains the shortcomings of a situational syllabus,
noting that it is limited to physical situations with easily identifiable
language interactions and associated language forms. While these might
be adequate for a person with limited aims such as a tourist, waiter, or
switchboard operator, a situational syllabus takes away the agency from
the language learner and what they want to communicate, and this might
not be language that is readily identifiable with the particular situation.
Similarly, the reactions of other participants in the situation cannot be
perfectly predicted, and the situational syllabus does not provide the
learner with a means of coping with such situations.
In place of either a structural syllabus or a situational syllabus, Wilkins
(1976: 18) proposes a notional syllabus. According to Wilkins, a notional
syllabus focuses not on how learners communicate nor where they
communicate but on what they communicate. In other words, it is a
content-based syllabus. However, the content is general and thus appli-
cable to different situations. Hall (2018: 221) describes this as a syllabus
design with a meaning focus. Wilkins (1976: 21) sets out three cate-
gories of meaning which he labels: semantico-grammatical, modal, and
communicative functions.
34 I. Pemberton
4.1.1 Semantico-grammar
Category Concepts
Time Points in time, duration, time relations (e.g., past, present,
and future), sequence, and frequency
Quantity Countable and uncountable items, numerals, and
mathematical operations
Space Dimensions, locations, and motion
Relational 1. Semantic roles such as agent, initiator, object,
meaning beneficiary, and instrument
2. Prediction and attribution such as John is a pilot. A man
with a long face
Deixis The time, place, and person involved in the communicative
act
The Natural Order Hypothesis argues that people learn grammar rules
in a particular order, irrespective of the order in which items are taught.
Some simple items, such as third person singular and plurals, emerge late,
which means that learners only master them after years of study even if
they are taught early on.
The central component of the theory, the Input Hypothesis, claims that
language is learnt only by understanding comprehensible input. The
hypothesis argues that the input should be at the level i + 1, which
means at a level slightly higher than a learner’s existing language level.
Krashen (1985: 2) explains that ‘we are able to understand language
containing unacquired grammar with the help of context, which includes
extra-linguistic information, our knowledge of the world, and previously
acquired linguistic competence.’
38 I. Pemberton
4.3 Summary
The Notional-Functional Syllabus represented a move away from rule-
based and structure-based approaches to linking language with the
contexts in which it is used. While this might suggest a situational
syllabus, Wilkins was critical of this as being too specific and limiting
to be of widespread application. Instead, he proposed a content-based
approach using general concepts and functions which would apply across
different situations with similar features. He proposed three general cate-
gories: semantico-grammatical, modal, and communicative. Semantico-
grammatical items described common concepts, modal items expressed
beliefs and feelings, and communicative items expressed functions. A
number of criticisms have been made of notional-functional syllabi: there
are too many functions to specify, the approach downplays lexis, and it
neglects receptive skills.
The late 1970s saw the appearance of Steven Krashen’s Natural
Approach. It is no understatement to say that the Natural Approach
has been one of the most controversial methods of all time. While it is
largely eschewed by academics and has been for some decades, it is still
being discovered and debated by practitioners even today. The Natural
Method was based on a number of hypotheses, collectively referred to as
the Input Hypothesis. These hypotheses were the Acquisition-Learning
Hypothesis, the Natural Order Hypothesis, the Monitor Hypothesis, the
Input Hypothesis, and the Affective Filter Hypothesis. The Acquisition-
Learning Hypothesis states that language is acquired not learnt. The
Natural Order Hypothesis states that grammar items are learnt in a set
order that cannot be influenced by instruction. The Monitor Hypothesis
states that learners can use conscious learning as a monitor when they
have enough time to do so, such as when writing. The Input Hypoth-
esis states that comprehensible input is necessary for acquisition, and
44 I. Pemberton
the Affective Filter Hypothesis states that acquisition can only take place
when learners have a low affective filter, in other words, when they are
relaxed.
To operationalize the Natural Approach Krashen and Terrell describe a
detailed set of classroom procedures. These are divided into activities for
beginners and post-beginners. The main source of input for beginners
is teacher talk. They are expected to go through a pre-speech or silent
phase before progressing to an early production phase where they ask and
answer questions. Next is an extending production phase, where a teacher
uses realia and asks more complex questions. In the post-beginners’
phase, comprehensible input is increasingly generated by student inter-
actions. Students work together to complete information gap activities,
information gathering activities, role plays and simulations, and discus-
sions. This being the case, a Natural Approach classroom in the early
1980s must have looked very similar to a modern-day CLT classroom.
A number of criticisms were made of the five hypotheses of the
Natural Approach. While the Natural Order, Input, and Affective filter
hypotheses are generally accepted, the Acquisition-Learning and Monitor
hypotheses have come in for criticism. Taken together, Krashen’s posi-
tion has been that explicit instruction in grammar (learning) cannot
become implicit acquisition. When viewed through a usage-based lens,
all language patterns, whether grammatical or lexical in nature, exist
alongside each other in cognition. The teaching of grammatical rules
imparts a knowledge of grammatical rules. If acquisition is taken to mean
automatization, it doesn’t result in acquisition. That requires a further
process of entrenchment—repeated processing and production of actual
instances of language. However, a knowledge of grammatical structures
can certainly act as a sociocultural tool which can mediate understanding
and lead ultimately to acquisition.
4 The Communicative Period 45
References
Cook, G. (2003). Applied linguistics. Oxford University Press.
Cook, V. (2008). Second language learning and language teaching (4th ed.).
Hodder Education.
Corder, S. P. (1973). Introducing applied linguistics. Penguin.
Hall, G. (2018). Exploring English language teaching: language in action (2nd
edn). Routledge/ Taylor & Francis Group.
Howatt, A. P. R. (1984) A history of English language teaching. Oxford
University Press.
Howatt, A. P. R., & Smith, R. (2014). The history of teaching english as
a foreign language, from a British and European perspective. Language &
History, 57 (1), 75–95.
Krashen, S. D., & Terrell, T. D. (1983). The natural approach: Language
acquisition in the classroom. Pergamon.
Krashen, S. D. (1985). The input hypothesis: Issues and implications. Longman.
Larsen-Freeman, D. & Anderson, M. (2011). Techniques and principles in
language teaching (3rd edn.). Oxford University Press.
Randall, M. (2007). Memory, psychology and second language learning. John
Benjamins Publishing Company.
Richards, J. C., & Rodgers, T. S. (2001). Approaches and methods in language
teaching (2nd ed.). Cambridge University Press.
Skehan, P. (1998). A cognitive approach to language learning. Oxford University
Press.
Swain, M. (1995) Three functions of output in second language learning.
In Cook, G. and Seidelhofer, B. (eds.), Principles and practice in applied
linguistics. Oxford University Press.
Wilkins, D. A. (1976). Notional syllabuses: A taxonomy and its relevance to
foreign language curriculum development. Oxford University Press.
Yalden, J. (1983). The communicative syllabus: Evolution, design and implemen-
tation. Pergamon.
5
Communicative Language Teaching
language teaching’ which ‘suggests that the term has no clearly under-
stood and received meaning when it is applied to language teaching.’
Richards and Rodgers (2001: 155) assert that ‘there is no single text
or authority on it, nor any single model that is universally accepted as
authoritative.’ Harmer (2007: 69) makes the point that ‘the real problem
when attempting to define CLT (or the Communicative Approach as
originally called) is that it means different things to different people.’
The combination of enthusiasm for the Communicative Approach and
a lack of clarity about what it represented led to the emergence of both
strong and weak versions of the approach.
5.3.1 Aim
However, even with Ellis’s modifications, the hypothesis still faces two
serious criticisms. The first is the limitations of working memory. While
processing input for meaning, a listener needs to recognize that a speaker
is adapting their language, hold the adapted version in working memory,
recall their current cognitive representation, compare the two, and send
the new form for storage in long-term memory. This represents a signif-
icant cognitive overload of working memory. The second, and perhaps
more serious criticism, is that learners have to learn a huge number of
language items, and it is infeasible that they can negotiate them all.
According to Ellis (1991: 4), in his later publications even Long concedes
that the hypothesis can’t account for the bulk of acquisition.
To sum up, a major weakness of Communicative Language Teaching
is that its theory of learning is vague at best. While adherents believe
that the pressure to communicate motivates learners to use their existing
language knowledge resulting in cognitive changes taking place, there
has been no explanation of how this happens. Richards & Rodgers have
proposed that its language learning theory can be inferred from commu-
nicative principles, but these represent nothing more than a specification
of the conditions under which acquisition can occur, not the process of
acquisition. Michael Long’s Interaction Hypothesis has been suggested as
an account of this process, but it is too rudimentary to account for the
bulk of acquisition that takes place.
54 I. Pemberton
5.3.3 Inputs
While the heavy emphasis on output activities in the literature can make
it a challenge to identify what the inputs might be to learners in the
communicative paradigm, one way to consider the types of input is to
look at the proposals for communicative syllabi. The two main syllabus
types that have been suggested for communicative courses are structural
syllabuses and functional syllabuses.
Several well-known educators have suggested that communicative
courses should retain a structural core. According to Littlewood
(1981: 77), taking a communicative approach is not a reason for aban-
doning a structural syllabus. He asserts that a mastery of structural
features is still necessary for communication. His only proviso being that
language examples are meaningful to learners. In the early 1980s, Sandra
Savignon also underscored the importance of grammar in the commu-
nicative paradigm. She analysed communicative curricula into five
components: language arts, language for a purpose, personal language
use, theatre arts, and beyond the classroom. Savignon (2001: 237–238)
defined the first component, language arts, as follows: this ‘represents
what language teachers do best–most often because it is all they have
been taught to do. Language arts focuses on rules of usage and provides
explanations of how language works.’ In her book, The Communicative
Syllabus, Yalden (1983: 110) argues that learners should be taught the
structural elements of a language before moving on to functions. Celce-
Murcia et al. (1997: 141) comment that communicative language classes
apply instruction in grammar to the teaching of conversation.
The retention of a structural syllabus has proven unpopular with
some CLT advocates. According to Allwright and Hanks (2009: 47),
Littlewood’s publication Communicative Language Teaching ‘solved the
commodity problem by offering much less challenging ideas and
returning to carefully chosen syllabus content.’ Richards and Rodgers
(2001: 155) also take a critical stance, commenting that some consider
that Communicative Language Teaching represents little more than
integrating grammar and functions.
While some educators recommended maintaining a structural
syllabus, a hallmark of CLT is its adoption of a functional syllabus. Berns
5 Communicative Language Teaching 55
5.3.6 Interaction
5.3.7 Meaningfulness
She makes the point that each learner knows something the other does
not.
In favouring meaningful activities over meaningless drills, commu-
nicative educators argue that they facilitate language learning. Johnson
(1982) states that language use in meaningful tasks promotes learning
(cited in Richards & Rodgers, 2001: 161). Richards (2006: 22) asserts
that interaction involving meaningful communication facilitates second
language learning adding that the driving force of learning is meaning
(ibid: 25). Harmer (2007: 69) notes that an essential belief of CLT is
that if learners do communicative tasks with a focus on meaning, then
‘language learning will take care of itself.’
limited. Another point is that if the language they use is not correct, it is
not clear how they will develop accurate representations of language.
5.4.2 Jigsaw
Savignon (2001: 240) suggests that in situations where the learners are
living in the area where the language is spoken, they can source adver-
tisements from local newspapers to find out where they can buy products
cheaply before actually going out into the community to ask about the
prices of such products as second-hand cars, watches, and cameras, and
then report back to the class.
Richards (2006: 20) defines role plays as activities where the teacher
assigns students roles and asks them to improvise a scene or provides
cues and information as the basis for an exchange. Harmer (2007: 69)
comments that they are useful activities as the communicative classroom
places emphasis on real or, at least, realistic communication where task
completion is as important as accurate language use. On a basic level,
Littlewood (1981: 70) suggests that the teacher instructs learners to
perform actions or mime them. Savignon (2001: 239) notes that fantasy
and play-acting are a natural part of growing up which provide opportu-
nities for language use. Thus, theatre arts are an important component
of second language learning. However, she cautions that they need to
be set up carefully to be useful. While the criticism could be made that
learning lines is not real language use, Littlewood (1981: 60) suggests
that learners improvise. They can be presented with a stimulus and make
their own language choices. In addition, learners can be asked to adopt
certain identities or act out certain personality types.
5.4.6 Discussion
are going on a three-day hike in the mountains and have a list of gear,
but they can only carry 11 kilograms each. They have to decide what to
take. A less challenging activity might be to rank important qualities in a
partner (Richards, 2006: 19). As opposed to just being a free exchange of
opinions, Littlewood (1981: 57) also explains that learners can be given
roles in a discussion. While the discussion can focus on either a real or
simulated issue, the use of roles ensures that the participants introduce
different arguments which leads to a lively exchange of views. At the end
of the discussion, the participants have to come to a consensus or report
back to the class.
5.5 Criticisms
While it is widely accepted, Communicative Language Teaching has its
critics. One common criticism is that it provides insufficient input for
language learning to occur. According to Field (2008: 2) the second half
of the twentieth century has seen instructors place great emphasis on
speaking in the belief that it is the most important skill for language
learners. He contends that the overemphasis on speaking is the result of
‘muddled thinking’ and makes the point that communication is a two-
way process in which it is as important to listen as it is to speak. Howatt
(1984: 287) observes that CLT rests on an unstated assumption that
learners already know English and the instructor’s job is to make sure that
they use their knowledge to communicate. Similarly, Field (2007: 33)
notes that what learners are actually doing is using what they already
know rather than extending their language knowledge. Ellidokuzoglu
(1996: 4) agrees, pointing out that while proponents of Communicative
Language Teaching do not assert that input is not important for second
language acquisition, CLT coursebooks underemphasize input, focusing
instead on output. Even CLT advocate H. Douglas Brown (2001: 377)
concedes that ‘in the hustle and bustle of our interactive classrooms,
sometimes we get so caught up in lively groupwork and meaningful
communication that we don’t pause to devote some attention to words.’
Finally, Skehan (1998: 40) notes research shows that ‘second language
use, in itself, does not reliably lead to second language change.’
5 Communicative Language Teaching 63
5.6 Summary
This chapter has considered Communicative Language Teaching which
has been the orthodox teaching approach for more than four decades.
It is widely considered to have appeared as a reaction to mechanistic
approaches to language teaching such as Audiolingualism and Struc-
turalism. The 1970s saw both strong and weak versions of the approach
emerge. Strong versions were favoured by some academics, but commer-
cial considerations and the practicalities of the classroom eventually saw
weaker versions become mainstream.
As Communicative Language Teaching acts as an umbrella term for
a wide variety of different methods, it is necessary to examine their
common features to ascertain its nature and scope. To begin with,
communicative methods share common principles. The main aim of
communicative methods is to teach communicative competence, in
other words, to teach learners how to communicate. All communica-
tive methods share a common theory of learning which is that people
learn language by communicating with others. A majority of main-
stream communicative methods use structural and functional syllabi
or some combination of these to specify inputs. A fourth principle
shared by communicative methods is the use of authentic materials such
as signs, magazines, advertisements, and maps. These are thought to
promote engagement and to provide accurate language models. A fifth
principle is active learning or learning by doing. This is thought to
be more effective than didactic teacher-centric instruction. In order to
realize active learning in the classroom, a communicative teacher sets
up interactions in dyads, triads, and small groups. To promote language
acquisition, the interactive activities must be meaningful. In contrast to
the drilling and rote learning of earlier approaches, they concentrate on
the exchanging meaning. A final common feature is the communica-
tive approach to error correction. In the early days, it was thought to
be unnecessary to correct mistakes, but in contemporary communicative
language teaching, there has been a renaissance in error correction.
In addition to common principles, communicative methods share
common activities. The archetypal communicative activity is the infor-
mation gap. This is a pairwork activity where each person has the same
5 Communicative Language Teaching 65
References
Allwright, D., & Hanks, J. (2009). The developing language learner: An
introduction to exploratory practice. Palgrave Macmillan.
Berns, M. S. (1983). Functional approaches to language and language teaching:
Another look. Studies in Language Learning, 4 (2), 4–22.
Brandl, K. (2008). Communicative language teaching in action: Putting principles
to work. Pearson Prentice Hall.
Brown, H. D. (2001). Teaching by principles: An interactive approach to language
pedagogy (2nd ed.). Prentice Hall Regents.
Celce-Murcia, M., Dornyei, Z., & Thurrell, S. (1997). Direct approaches in L2
Instruction: A turning point in communicative language teaching? TESOL
Quarterly, 31(1), 141–152.
Cook, G. (2003). Applied linguistics. Oxford University Press.
Cook, V. (2008). Second language learning and language teaching (4th ed.).
Hodder Education.
Dornyei, Z. (2009). Communicative language teaching in the 21st century:
The ‘principled communicative approach.’ Perspectives, 36 (2), 33–43.
Ellidokuzoglu, H. (1996). Decline and fall of communicative approach. KHO
International ELT Conference: From diversity to synergy. Ankara, Turkey.
Ellis, R. (1982). Informal and formal approaches to communicative language
teaching. ELT Journal, 36 (2), 73–81.
Ellis, R. (1991). The interaction hypothesis: A critical evaluation. Paper presented
at the Regional Language Centre Seminar (Singapore, April 22–28, 1991).
Ellis, R. (1997). Second language acquisition. Oxford University Press.
Field, J. (2007). Looking outwards, not inwards. ELT Journal, 61(1), 30–38.
Field, J. (2008). Listening in the language classroom. Cambridge University Press.
Hall, G. (2018). Exploring English language teaching: Language in action (2nd
ed.). Routledge/ Taylor & Francis Group.
Harmer, J. (1982). What is communicative? ELT Journal, 36 (3), 164–168.
Harmer, J. (2007). The practice of English language teaching (4th ed.). Pearson
Longman.
Howatt, A. P. R. (1984) A history of English language teaching. Oxford
University Press.
Larsen-Freeman, D. (2000). Techniques and principles in language teaching (2nd
ed.). Oxford University Press.
Littlewood, W. (1981). Communicative language teaching: An introduction.
Cambridge University Press.
5 Communicative Language Teaching 67
on the language.’ And this ‘enables learners to map new language onto
meaning and thought’ (Snow & Brinton, 2017: 4).
In its purest form, Content-Based Instruction is realized in immer-
sion language programmes that occur in secondary and tertiary settings.
Krashen (1985: 16) describes these as public-school programmes where
majority language students study in a minority language (e.g., English-
speaking students studying in Spanish in the USA).
An important aspect is the amount of second language input that
learners receive. Brinton et al. (2003: 7) explain that early successes with
a 1965 French immersion project led to the emergence of ‘a mass educa-
tional movement’ where Canadian school children received ‘much or all
of their school instruction through a second language.’ They also describe
bilingual secondary programmes where students took forty to fifty per
cent of their classes in French.
Immersion programmes are considered to be highly successful.
Brinton et al. (2003: 9) report that ‘immersion students achieve a high
level of functional ability in the second language with near-native profi-
ciency in receptive skills by the time they graduate.’ Skehan (1998: 12)
notes that ‘immersion educated children reach much higher levels of
achievement than do children educated by traditional “core” methods.’
Krashen (1985: 16) comments that immersion programmes have shown
teaching subject-matter is language teaching when it is made compre-
hensible.
However, some educators point out that they are not an unqualified
success. Lyster (2019: 12) draws attention to Swain’s concern ‘about the
L2 development of French immersion students, stating that “some of the
errors do not disappear” even after many years in immersion.’ Skehan
(1998: 12) notes that ‘although the children concerned perform at levels
of comprehension close to native speakers, the same cannot be said of
their productive skills.’ In investigating why this may be the case, Swain
and Lapkin (1986, cited in Lyster, 2019: 14) observed that there was
little information exchange in immersion classes and there was not much
discussion of ideas. In addition, Brinton et al. (2003: 48) point out that
‘students with either too low or too advanced second language profi-
ciency do not benefit fully from sheltered courses. If their skills are not
adequate for understanding the lectures and readings with reasonable
6 Other Communicative Approaches 71
efficiency, they will fail at the subject matter and not learn very much
language either.’
One early attempt to define tasks often cited in the literature is Breen:
simple exercise type to more complex and lengthy activities such as group
problem solving or simulations and decision making. (Breen, 1987, in
Willis & Willis, 2007: 12)
Willis and Willis (2007: 12) make the point that this definition is
of limited usefulness as it could cover anything that takes place in the
classroom. A number of linguists offer more focused definitions. Cook
(2008: 257), citing Bygate (2001), explains that ‘a task is an activity
which requires learners to use language, with emphasis on meaning,
to attain a goal.’ Jane Willis (1996) provides a very similar defini-
tion: ‘a goal-oriented activity in which learners use language to achieve
a real outcome.’ These definitions are still considered to be vague,
so consideration has also been given to what is NOT a task. Willis
(1996) explains that any activity which has a primary focus on practising
specific language forms such as a pre-scripted dialogue or an information-
gathering activity intended to practise a single structure (e.g., Do you
like -ing?) would not count as a task. Skehan (1998: 96) further clar-
ifies that form-focused ELT practices such as transformation exercises
and students discovering the rules of grammar inductively would also
not count as tasks. Richards (2017: 175) adds drills, cloze activities, and
controlled writing activities to the list. He states that these activities are
better described as exercises. Usefully, Willis and Willis (2007: 13) set
out a number of questions to help teachers evaluate whether an activity
counts as a task or not:
Finally, Skehan (1998: 96) notes that even with a set of questions,
judgement may sometimes be subjective and that activities may be better
thought of as points on a cline from less task-like to more task-like in
character.
74 I. Pemberton
Tasks can be based on texts, or they can be topic-based. A topic can act
as an umbrella for different types of tasks (Willis, 1998). Nunan (2004:
131) expresses a preference for a topic-based approach, commenting that
‘it affords maximum flexibility and allows me to bring in a wide variety
of content that can be tailored to learner needs.’ To further clarify, Willis
and Willis (2007: 66) provide a typology of tasks which are based on
different cognitive processes.
6.2.3 Terminology
The first step is the task itself. During this stage, commentators agree
that it is important that learners are free to make their own language
choices. Richards (2006: 33) notes that learners work in groups or pairs,
and it provides them with the opportunity to use whatever language they
wish to fulfil the task. Similarly, Nunan comments that learners ‘are free
to use any linguistic means at their disposal to complete the task.’ Skehan
(1998: 127) comments that the target task has the potential to develop
learners’ fluency, accuracy, and complexity as they use language and test
their hypotheses about it.
After completing the main task, Willis (1996) suggests a planning
phase and then a reporting phase. In the planning phase, learners
consider how they are going to report the outcome of the task. When
planning is complete, at least some of the learners will report back
on their task performances to the whole class. Skehan (1998: 148)
comments that the reporting phase might actually be more appropriately
grouped together with post-task activities.
Willis (1996) suggests one further activity after the reporting has taken
place. Whereas learners listened to a recording of native speakers doing
a parallel task before they attempted their own task, they now listen to
a recording of native speakers doing the same task and compare it with
their own performance.
The Post-Task Stage: In the final phase in the three-stage task cycle,
learners focus on form. If students have used written or spoken texts
earlier in the cycle, these are re-introduced for language analysis. Willis
(1996) suggests various activities including choral drilling of phrases,
work on recalling them, or matching verbs with their subjects and
objects.
A further reason relates to learnability. Long and Crookes (1992: 31)
point out that ‘where syntax is concerned, research has demonstrated that
learners rarely, if ever, move from zero to target-like mastery of new items
in one step.’ Thus, Willis and Willis (2007: 18) argue that when struc-
tures are introduced at the beginning of a lesson, learners will end up
using them inaccurately leading to a sense of failure.
78 I. Pemberton
6.2.9 Criticisms
occurrence, Allwright and Hanks (2009: 98) point out that it is diffi-
cult to establish its impact on acquisition. Aston (1986) cited in Skehan
(1998: 20) considers that such negotiations can be irritating for students,
and not representative of what happens in normal spoken interaction.
Its importance is brought into question by Nation (2001: 124) who
reports that as the main goal is to complete a task, this necessarily limits
the amount of language learners negotiate. In fact, Skehan (1998: 20)
cites research by Foster (1998) who found that ‘most individual students,
whatever the task or participation pattern, engage in the same amount
of negotiation of meaning – nil.’
6.3 Summary
This chapter has looked at two process-based communicative teaching
approaches: Content-Based Instruction and Task-Based Instruction.
Content-Based Instruction has been divided into three broad categories:
theme-based instruction, sheltered instruction, and adjunct instruction
which are appropriate for EFL, EMI (English as a Medium of Instruc-
tion), and EAP, respectively. Content-Based Instruction promotes high
levels of language proficiency, but it does not result in first language
levels of proficiency in the productive skills of writing and speaking. This
led Canadian linguist Merrill Swain to formulate her Output Hypoth-
esis which argued that output is a necessary condition for acquisition.
Swain suggested three roles for output: noticing a gap, hypothesis testing,
and discussing grammatical form. However, none of these three func-
tions guarantee the presentation of correct forms to the learner, and this
seriously weakens the ability of the Output Hypothesis to explain how
acquisition takes place.
Task-Based Instruction means different things to different teachers.
However, advocates agree on some points. They are agreed that a task is
a meaning-focused activity in which learners are free to use any language
that they know to complete the task. Additionally, they agree that a
task should have a clear goal and, ideally, a relationship to the real
world. While tasks can be classified in different ways, an influential
typology is Willis’s classification according to the cognitive processes
82 I. Pemberton
References
Allwright, D., & Hanks, J. (2009). The developing language learner: An
introduction to exploratory practice. Palgrave Macmillan.
Brinton, D., Snow, M. A., & Wesche, M. B. (2003). Content-based second
language instruction (Michigan classics ed.). University of Michigan Press.
Brown, H. D. (2001). Teaching by principles: An interactive approach to language
pedagogy (2nd ed.). Prentice Hall Regents.
Cook, V. (2008). Second language learning and language teaching (4th ed.).
Hodder Education.
Krashen, S. D. (1985). The input hypothesis: Issues and implications. Longman.
Long, M. H., & Crookes, G. (1992). Three approaches to task-based syllabus
design. TESOL Quarterly, 26 (1), 27–56.
6 Other Communicative Approaches 83
One of the few approaches that has emerged since the arrival of the
Communicative Approach is the Lexical Approach. While it has not been
widely adopted, there is almost universal agreement on its significance
for language teaching and learning. In contrast to structural approaches,
a Lexical Approach considers lexis as the main organizing principle
of language teaching and learning. Richards and Rodgers (2001: 132)
state that ‘lexical approaches in language teaching reflect a belief in the
centrality of the lexicon to language structure, second language learning,
and language use.’ Advocates of the Lexical Approach do not draw a
distinction between grammar and lexis, rather they see language as occu-
pying a continuum from concrete items at one end to fully abstract
expressions at the other. Author of the Lexical Approach, Michael Lewis
(1997: 3), argues that the components of language are not grammar and
vocabulary but prefabricated multi-word chunks.
In the early 1990s, evidence of the existence of these multi-word
chunks emerged from the analysis of language corpora in the field of
corpus linguistics. This led John Sinclair (1991: 110) to formulate his
idiom principle which states that ‘a language user has available to him or
her a large number of semi-preconstructed phrases that constitute single
choices, even though they might appear analyzable into segments.’ The
existence of preconstructed phrases highlights the scale of the task faced
by language learners. Lewis (1993: 92) asserts that a language consists
of hundreds of thousands of multi-word chunks. Harmer (2007: 74)
provides some familiar examples:
How are you? See you later. You must be joking. I’ll give it my best shot.
Changing the subject slightly. Might as well …. If it’ll help ….
1. Words
2. Collocations
3. Fixed or institutionalized expressions
a. Fully fixed expressions
b. Semi-fixed expressions
7.1.1 Words
Lewis (1997: 8) remarks that the first category will be familiar to teachers
as vocabulary but it also includes contractions, compound nouns, and
polywords. Lewis (1997: 22) holds the view that contractions should
7 The Lexical Approach 87
7.1.2 Collocations
The next category, which will probably be the most familiar to language
teachers are collocations. Richards and Rodgers (2001: 133) explain that
collocations are words which regularly occur together. Lewis (1997:25)
adds that collocations are words which occur together at a frequency
greater than random. Sinclair (1991: 112) points out that while collo-
cations vary in strength, there are many strong collocations such as hard
luck, hard work, hard evidence, hard facts.
However, collocation is an arbitrary phenomenon and different
languages will collocate words in different ways; for example, an English
speaker will talk about a strong wind , heavy rain and heavy or deep snow.
On the other hand, a Japanese speaker will talk about a strong wind but
strong rain and deep but not heavy snow. Whereas strong rain is marginally
possible in English, we don’t say strong snow. The fact that we generalize
some adjectives to particular nouns but not others clearly indicates that
we experience and store language as lexical chunks but that these do not
necessarily translate into other languages.
Lewis (1997: 30) continues that while collocation is a powerful way
to learn language, learners require assistance to identify them in a text.
Also, the fact that two words may be adjacent in a text does not guarantee
88 I. Pemberton
that they collocate. On the other hand, many collocations are spatially
separate.
This category can be divided into those expressions which are fully fixed,
and others which are semi-fixed ‘frames’ with ‘slots’ which may be filled
in a limited number of ways (Lewis, 1997: 9)
Fully fixed expressions:
There is general agreement amongst Lexical Approach advocates
about the nature of fully fixed expressions. Early advocates of a Lexical
Approach, Pawley and Syder (1983: 191), refer to fully fixed multi-
word expressions as ‘institutionalized’ expressions. While their language
is somewhat rigidly formal, their intention here is to indicate that they
are conventionalized through use and that the same or very similar
expressions are employed by almost all other speakers of a language.
Lewis (1993: 95) picks up on their terminology and describes fully fixed
expressions as complete sentences with a clear pragmatic meaning and
easily identifiable as such. Nattinger and DeCarrico (1992: 39) agree
with Lewis that institutionalized expressions are of sentence length and
add that they are invariable and mostly continuous. They (1992: 40)
agree with Pawley and Syder that some may be phrases in general use
in a speech community (e.g., How do you do? How are you? ) but point
out that some might be idiosyncratic phrases that individuals have found
effective in getting their meaning across (e.g., give me a break, have a nice
day). By way of illustration, Pawley and Syder (1983: 206–7) provide
the following examples:
Can I come in? Can I help? Need any help? Need a hand? Are you all
right? Is everything OK? Are you ready? What did you say? What’s for
dinner? … I see what you mean. I’ll believe it when I see it. That’s easier
said than done. I thought you’d never ask. He never has a bad word to
say about anyone. If I’d known then what I know now. He’s not the man
he used to be.
7 The Lexical Approach 89
Semi-fixed expressions
Advocates agree that by far the more interesting category is semi-fixed
expressions. Lewis (1997: 43) considers that this category is important
for understanding how language is learnt and its representation in the
lexicon. Pawley and Syder (1983: 205) provide a ballpark estimate of
how many of these expressions there may be. They estimate that while
an adult English speaker knows many thousands of complete clauses and
sentences, phraseological expressions which are not completely specified
clauses are much more numerous.
Lewis (1997: 34) explains that ‘in general, semi-fixed expressions
consist of a pragmatic (or functional) frame, which is completed by a
referential slot filler.’ Pawley and Syder (1983: 205) describe these expres-
sions as ‘sequences which contain a nucleus of fixed lexical items standing
in construction with one or more variable elements (often a grammatical
inflection), the specification of the variables being necessary to complete
the clause.’
The category can be broken down further. Nattinger and DeCarrico
(1992: 41) analyse semi-fixed expressions into phrasal constraints and
sentence builders. They explain that phrasal constraints are short- and
medium-length phrases which allow for variation of both lexical and
phrasal (grammatical) categories. Examples include:
A _____ ago. To _____ this up. As I was _____. See you _____. _____
as well as _____. As far as I _____. The _____er the_____ er. What with
_____ (and all).
I think that X. Not only X but also Y; my point is that X; I’m a great
believer in X; The _____er X, the _____er Y.
90 I. Pemberton
Lewis (1993: 95) explains that ‘it now seems plausible that an impor-
tant part of language acquisition is the ability to produce lexical phrases
as unanalyzed wholes or “chunks”, and that these chunks become the raw
data by which the learner begins to perceive patterns, morphology, and
other features of language traditionally thought of as grammar.’ While
Lewis is not particularly explicit, the basic proposal here is that learners
create an inventory of concrete linguistic items and then analyse these
concrete examples of language to identify grammatical categories and
create more abstract syntactic patterns.
7.4 Methodology
Lewis (1993: 104) argues that, historically, we have analysed language
into words and grammatical structures but that this analysis is incorrect,
or at best, unhelpful. A language is more usefully analysed into lexis; that
is to say, meaningful phrases of, generally, more than one word.
He (1997: 86) asserts that languages are best learned by doing a
large amount of listening to acquire lexis, and then complementing that
with an equally large amount of reading. This sounds very similar to
Krashen’s Natural Approach, but in contrast to Krashen, Lewis asserts
that comprehensible input alone is not enough, and that learners benefit
from direct instruction. According to Lewis (1993: 104; 1997: 117),
effective instruction should focus on three things:
1. identifying chunks
2. matching
3. completing
4. categorising
5. sequencing
6. deleting
Lewis (1993: 122; 1997: 70) explains that the identification of lexical
chunks (including but not limited to collocations) is central to the
Lexical Approach. He suggests a range of pedagogical activities. The first
of these is underlining chunks as they occur in texts (Lewis, 1997: 108).
This assumes that learners are able to do so but Lewis provides a caveat
that even where they are unable to, it will give the teacher an idea of what
learners regard as chunks. A second activity offers more guidance. In this
case, teachers can provide a list of words which collocate in a text (e.g.,
verbs or headword nouns) and ask learners to scan the text to identify
the collocates. A third activity might involve asking students to pick out
all the nouns in a text and then review the text to find their collocates.
A fourth activity is based on the green cross code:
When you see a word, even if it is a word with which you are familiar,
STOP, LOOK LEFT, LOOK RIGHT, LOOK LEFT AGAIN, AND,
WHEN SATISFIED, PROCEED. (Lewis, 1997: 111)
94 I. Pemberton
7.4.2 Matching
7.4.3 Completing
7.4.4 Categorizing
7.4.5 Sequencing
7.4.6 Deleting
Lewis (1997: 91) suggests that deletion can be used as a form of negative
evidence to prevent overgeneralization errors. For example, learners may
be asked to decide which of a group of collocates is the odd one out, i.e.,
does not collocate with a particular word (Lewis, 1997: 94), or they may
be asked which of a number of words that fit in a slot is different from
the other words, e.g.,
You look a bit ……………….
7.4.7 Drills
While language drills fell out of favour due to their association with the
behaviouristic approach of Audiolingualism, Lewis (1997: 127) advo-
cates asking students to practise repeating longer expressions to master
their phonological features.
7.4.8 Discussion
How are you? How’s things? How’s tricks? What have you been up to
recently? What’s new with you? How’s life been treating you?
Don’t worry – I’ll pick you up / It’s the sort of thing you think can never
happen to you / Can I give you a hand? Do we have to ask your mother?
Not tonight. I’ve got a headache.
7.4.9 Production
7.5 Notebooks
A key pedagogic strategy of the Lexical Approach is to get learners to
record new vocabulary that they hear or read in a notebook. In Imple-
menting the Lexical Approach, Lewis (1997: 67–75) suggests ten different
principles for organizing the notebook. He comments that there is no
best way to organize lexis and that different ways are appropriate for
different lexical areas.
7.5.2 Formats
1. Collocation boxes
2. Pattern Displays
3. Discourse Structures
7.6 Criticisms
Lewis recognizes some of the limitations of his own approach. He admits
that it means that the amount of study learners have to do increases.
He (1997: 33) concedes that ‘the lexicon is somewhat larger than we
suspected, and the learning load is in some ways consequently greater.’
In addition, he loses the courage of his convictions somewhat, conceding
that a Lexical Approach alone is insufficient. After arguing passionately
that there is no distinction between grammar and lexis, Lewis (1997:
41) states that the Lexical Approach ‘recognizes clearly that lexis is
not enough and that courses which totally discard grammar are doing
learners a disservice.’
7 The Lexical Approach 101
7.7 Summary
A Lexical Approach divides language into a number of categories, and
common categories are words, collocations, fully fixed expressions, and
semi-fixed expressions. Proponents of a Lexical Approach don’t differen-
tiate between vocabulary and grammar. They believe these things exist
on a cline, and they assert that much more focus needs to be placed
on teaching and learning lexis, as opposed to grammar. Advocates of
a Lexical Approach believe that pre-formed units of language above a
word level are essential to language understanding and use. They assist
language processing and production by reducing cognitive load for both
listeners and the speakers. Analysis of corpus data by researchers has
confirmed that people generally communicate using conventional expres-
sions, and the existence and importance of lexis are widely accepted by
both theorists and practitioners.
In his book Implementing the Lexical Approach, Michael Lewis
discusses methodology for teaching lexis, and he highlights the impor-
tance of identifying it in spoken and written texts and recording it in
102 I. Pemberton
References
Harmer, J. (2007). The practice of English language teaching (4th ed.). Pearson
Longman.
Lewis, M. (1993). The Lexical approach: The state of ELT and a way forward .
Language Teaching Publications.
Lewis, M. (1997). Implementing the Lexical Approach. Language Teaching
Publications.
Nattinger, J. R., & DeCarrico, J. S. (1992). Lexical phrases and language
teaching. Oxford University Press.
Pawley, A. & Syder, F. (1983). Two puzzles for linguistic theory: Nativelike
selection and nativelike fluency. In J. C. Richards & R. W. Schmidt (Eds.),
Language and communication (pp. 191–226). Routledge.
Richards, J. C., & Rodgers, T. S. (2001). Approaches and methods in language
teaching (2nd ed.). Cambridge University Press.
Sinclair, J. (1991). Corpus, concordance, collocation. Oxford University Press.
Wray, A., & Perkins, M. R. (2000). The functions of formulaic language: An
integrated model. Language & Communication, 20 (1), 1–28.
Part II
Establishing a Theoretical Basis
language than a first language, and some academics speculate that the
formal educational setting of most second language learning may have a
negative impact on its effectiveness.
Throughout the history of language education, the intuitions of many
language educators have been that the processes of first language acqui-
sition hold the key to successful second language acquisition. Howatt
(1984: 295) comments that ‘the success of informal learning, and partic-
ularly of the child acquiring its mother tongue, has always impressed
language teachers, and attempts to reproduce the same effect by creating
the same causes have been a regular feature of language teaching history.’
This was particularly evident during the Reform Period. Richards and
Rodgers (2001: 7) note that early exponent of input-based learning,
Claude Marcel, considered that L1 learning by children was a good
model for L2 teaching, and that Francois Gouin attempted to build his
methodology based on observations of children learning language (ibid.:
11). In addition, Howatt (1984: 157) reports that Thomas Prendergast
also started from the example provided by child language acquisition.
All of this suggests that an examination of the processes of first language
acquisition may prove to be a fruitful place to start in the development
of an approach to second language learning. An analysis of first language
acquisition can be performed in two ways: by considering the evolution
of language over human history and by considering how language evolves
over a person’s lifetime.
8.1 Evolution
While there is much debate and disagreement about the origins of
language and in particular, when this actually took place (estimates
range from somewhere between 3.5 million and 50,000 years ago),
there is some agreement on how language came into being. Feeney
(2018: 78) points out that traditionally the mainstream has assumed
a Darwinian account based on natural selection. MacWhinney (2015:
20) notes that Darwin demonstrated competition between organisms
for survival and reproduction leads to evolution. According to evolu-
tionary education psychologist, David C. Geary (2002: 319), natural
8 First Language Acquisition 107
8.3 Constructivism
The constructivist theory of learning is most often associated with Swiss
psychologist Jean Piaget. In his theory of cognitive development, Piaget
set out that children’s thought processes change as they get older and that
their language develops in parallel to these changes in their thinking.
According to Piaget, as children experience new things, they develop
schema. A schema is a basic building block of memory which encodes
physical experiences, for example, with objects and actions, as well as
more abstract cognitive ones, e.g., theories.
A child’s schema can be altered in one of two ways: assimilation or
accommodation. Assimilation is the process of integrating new knowl-
edge with existing cognitive structures. In the case of assimilation, the
new information does not alter existing beliefs and understandings of
the world. Pound (2014: 48) explains that the new knowledge is stored
in parallel to existing knowledge.
Not all new information can be assimilated into existing cogni-
tive structure. In some cases, the new knowledge conflicts with
current understanding leading to accommodation. Accommodation is
the process of incorporating new knowledge into existing schematic
understanding where the new information results in a revision of the
existing understanding.
In the course of his research, Piaget identified four distinct stages that
a child progresses through in their cognitive and linguistic development.
Piaget’s stages are:
To take this argument a step further, it appears that even error correc-
tion is not necessary for language learning. Firstly, explicit correction
does not seem to have a major influence. Bloom (2000: 1) reports that
children do not receive consistent feedback on the accuracy of what they
say. Krashen (1985: 35) cites research which shows that parental approval
(e.g., a parent saying ‘That’s right’) does not improve accuracy. Secondly,
implicit correction does not appear to change childish forms. Yule (2020:
207) states that even subtle correction, such as when an adult repeats
what they have said with a correction, they continue to use their own
language. Tomasello (2003: 177) comments that few linguists think that
recasts constrain children’s tendency to overgeneralize.
A final point to note is that Vygotsky envisaged the Zone of Proximal
Development as a place where secondary, not primary learning would
take place, and in fact, as has been set out above, language learning
draws upon primary not secondary biological skills. In other words,
learning language and learning conceptual knowledge, particularly tricky
threshold concepts, are qualitatively different processes.
To begin with, Bloom (2000: 61) states that children use naive
psychology (theory of mind) to determine what words refer to.
Tribushinina and Gillis (2017: 32) also point out that theory of mind
and language develop in parallel, as theory of mind enables children
to understand things from the perspectives of others and grasp their
communicative intent.
American developmental and comparative psychologist, Michael
Tomasello, outlines the set of key skills which children possess. These
include the abilities to:
These skills are also domain general. Dolgova and Tyler (2019: 942)
state that language learning involves general thinking skills like cate-
gorization, pattern finding, and noticing frequency. Tomasello (2003:
4) comments that children use general cognitive abilities to parse
language and store the whole utterance together with its parts. Langacker
(2000) states that children represent the form and meaning of words
in cognition using general learning skills and these include association,
categorization, and abstraction (cited in Verspoor & Nguyen, 2015:
312).
118 I. Pemberton
8.6 Summary
This chapter has examined accounts of first language acquisition as a
basis for second language teaching and learning pedagogy. It looked at
language acquisition from both an evolutionary perspective and from
explanations of first language acquisition during childhood.
From an evolutionary perspective, language has arisen through the
processes of natural and sexual selection. Early humans who could use
language to understand and communicate gained greater access and
control of essential resources, improving their survival rates and those
of their kin. On the basis of the emergence of language through evolu-
tionary forces, a distinction has been proposed between primary and
secondary biological skills. Primary biological skills, specifically, the skills
of observing and listening, allow all normal human beings in all normal
environments to acquire their first language. That is to say, we are biolog-
ically adapted to learn language. Secondary biological skills are those
we use in the classroom to learn culturally important knowledge for
participation in society, for example, reading and writing. We are not
biologically adapted for this, and it is much more difficult. Second
languages taught through biologically secondary skills are much more
difficult to learn. This suggests that if we are able to employ our primary
biological learning abilities, we may be able to learn languages more
successfully.
Two well-known language learning theories are Constructivism and
Sociocultural Theory. The first of these theories, Constructivism, is
associated with Jean Piaget. This is a cognitive theory of knowledge
acquisition that states that recurrent perceptual experience results in the
creation of knowledge frameworks known as schema within memory.
A schema is a general representation of real-world phenomena that a
child experiences. Schema are used in processing to understand, and in
production to articulate the phenomena that they represent.
In terms of child language development, Piaget drew a distinction
between ego-centric speech and socialized speech. Whereas up until
around seven years of age, children may appear to be communicating,
they are essentially thinking out loud and providing a commentary on
what they are doing. They have no particular expectation that listeners
120 I. Pemberton
will understand or respond to what they are saying. Piaget’s view was that
when a child matures sufficiently, they become communicatively compe-
tent enough to talk to their listener in a way that they will understand
and respond. Piaget calls this the move from ego-centric to socialized
speech.
The second theory, Sociocultural Theory, is associated with Lev
Vygotsky. Vygotsky believed that learning took place as a result of social
interactions between a learner and a more knowledgeable other in what
he termed the Zone of Proximal Development (ZPD). Like Piaget,
Vygotsky recognized the phenomenon of private speech (ego-centric
speech) and also regarded it as thinking out loud rather than an attempt
to communicate but he felt that its disappearance at around seven years
of age was not due to the emergence of social interactional skills but
represented the movement of externally verbalized thought to internally
verbalized thinking or ‘inner speech.’
While advocates of CLT hold that language is learnt through inter-
action and draw upon Sociocultural Theory as a justification for pair
and groupwork activities, research shows that adult speech may have an
impact, but it is a minor one, and caretaker speech is not a necessary
condition for language learning anyway. Moreover, research into error
correction demonstrates that even when correction is subtle, such as
when parents use recasts, it does not change a child’s erroneous language
forms.
In contrast to classical theories of language acquisition, Social-
Pragmatic Theory provides a detailed account of first language acqui-
sition. Social-Pragmatic Theory claims that language is learnt via the
application of general learning skills to concrete examples of language.
Children employ three types of general learning abilities. The first set
are intention-reading skills which are also known as theory of mind.
These include the ability to share attention, to follow attention, and
to direct attention. Intention-reading skills allow children to interpret
the communicative intent of adults and thus understand what they
are saying. Secondly, children employ cultural imitation to internalize
language. This involves copying both the language and the intent of a
speaker. A third set of skills are general pattern finding skills including
8 First Language Acquisition 121
8.7 Implications
We learn our mother tongue using the primary biological skills of obser-
vation and listening, but we use secondary skills to learn conceptual
knowledge for use in society. When these are applied to second language
learning, they are much less effective. This implies that if primary biolog-
ical skills can be applied to second language learning, outcomes might be
better.
The broad interpretation of Constructivism for many educators is as a
call for Active Learning. Active Learning implies learning which is inde-
pendent of the teacher, and this usually means putting learners into pairs
or groups to discuss things together. However, neither independence
from the teacher nor pair or group work are sufficient conditions for
learning. This pedagogical strategy can lead to behavioural activity, for
example, learners chatting to each other, whereas, cognitive activity is
necessary for learning, in other words, comprehending, selecting, orga-
nizing, and integrating information with existing knowledge, and this
can be done with or without a partner.
Sociocultural Theory, with its concept of a more knowledgeable other,
is also used to justify the use of pair and group work but interaction
in the Zone of Proximal Development can’t account for the bulk of
language learning. Thus, implementing activities that involve extensive
amounts of negotiation of meaning may not be a productive use of time
and energy.
The less well-known Social-Pragmatic Theory of first language acqui-
sition offers a compelling account of how children learn their first
language. That is, children learn through intention-reading, imitation,
and general pattern finding skills. This implies that learners can learn
without explicit instruction or correction from a more knowledgeable
other.
122 I. Pemberton
References
Allwright, D. & Hanks, J. (2009) The developing language learner: An introduc-
tion to exploratory practice. Palgrave Macmillan.
Bloom, P. (2000). How children learn the meanings of words. MIT Press.
Bybee, J. (2010). A usage-based perspective on language. In J. Bybee (Ed.),
Language, usage and cognition (pp. 1–13). Cambridge University Press.
Dolgova, N., & Tyler, A. (2019). Applications of usage-based approaches to
language teaching. In X. Gao (Ed.), Second handbook of english language
teaching (pp. 939–961). Springer International Publishing.
Ellidokuzoglu, H. (2017). Towards a receptive paradigm in foreign language
teaching. Turkish Online Journal of English Language Teaching (TOJELT),
2(1), 20–39.
Eskildsen, S. W. (2009). Constructing another language-usage-based linguistics
in second language acquisition. Applied Linguistics, 30 (3), 335–357.
Feeney, A. (2018). 5. Language evolution and the nature of the human faculty
for language. In C. Wright, T. Piske, & M. Young-Scholten (Eds.), Mind
matters in SLA (pp. 72–90). Multilingual Matters.
Flowerdew, J. (2015). John Swales’s approach to pedagogy in Genre Analysis: A
perspective from 25 years on. Journal of English for Academic Purposes, (19),
102–112.
Fosnot, C. T. & Perry, R. S. (1996). Constructivism: A psychological theory
of learning. In Constructivism: Theory, perspectives, and practice (2nd ed.).
Teachers College Columbia University.
Garhart-Mooney, C. (2013). Theories of childhood: An introduction to Dewey,
Montessori, Erikson, Piaget, and Vygotsky (2nd ed.). Redleaf Press.
Gass, S. M. (1997). Input, interaction, and the second language learner. Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates.
Geary, D. C. (2002). Principles of evolutionary educational psychology.
Learning and Individual Differences, 12(4), 317–345.
8 First Language Acquisition 123
Gopnik, A., Meltzoff, A. N., & Kuhl, P. K. (2001) How babies think: The
science of childhood . Phoenix.
Houston, S. H. (2019). A survey of psycholinguistics. De Gruyter Mouton.
Howatt, A. P. R. (1984). A history of English language teaching. Oxford
University Press.
Krashen, S. D. (1985). The input hypothesis: Issues and implications. Longman.
Lantolf, J. P. (2007). Sociocultural theory. In J. Cummins & C. Davison (Eds.),
International handbook of English language teaching (pp. 693–700). Springer
US.
Lantolf, J. P. (2000). 1. Introducing Sociocultural theory. In J. P. Lantolf (Ed.),
Sociocultural theory and second language learning. Oxford University Press.
Lantolf, J. P. & Thorne, S. L. (2006). Sociocultural theory and the genesis of
second language development. Oxford University Press.
Lewis, M. (1993). The Lexical approach: The state of ELT and a way forward .
Language Teaching Publications.
Mayer, R. E. (2014). Introduction to multimedia learning. In R. E. Mayer
(Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of multimedia learning (2nd ed., pp. 1–24).
Cambridge University Press.
MacWhinney, B. (2015). Multidimensional SLA. In T. Cadierno & S. W.
Eskildsen (Eds.), Usage-based perspectives on second language learning. De
Gruyter, Inc.
Nation, I. S. P. (2001). Learning vocabulary in another language. Cambridge
University Press.
Nunan, D. (1997) Listening in language learning. The Language Teacher.
Available at: http://www.jalt-publications.org/tlt/files/97/sep/nunan.html.
Accessed May 11, 2021.
Pound, L. (2014). How children learn: Educational theories and approaches.
Practical Pre-School Books.
Renandya, W. A. (2012). Five reasons why listening strategy instruction might
not work with lower proficiency learners. ELTWorldOnline.com, Vol. 4.
Richards, J. C., & Rodgers, T. S. (2001). Approaches and methods in language
teaching (2nd ed.). Cambridge University Press.
Shipley, K. G. & McAfee, J. G. (2019) Assessment in speech-language pathology:
a resource manual (Sixth ed.). Plural Publishing, Inc.
Skehan, P. (1998). A cognitive approach to language learning. Oxford University
Press.
Sweller, J. (2015). In Academe, what is learned, and how is it learned? Current
Directions in Psychological Science: A Journal of the American Psychological
Society, 24 (3), 190–194.
124 I. Pemberton
Half a century ago, British linguist Pit Corder (1973: 109) observed that
‘until we have a much better idea of what (the learning processes) are
we cannot, on a systematic and principled basis, create the necessary
conditions for optimal learning; we can only do what we have largely
been doing, that is, working on a hit and miss basis.’ Corder pointed
out that sceptics doubted the study of first language acquisition had any
relevance for second language education as there were too many differ-
ences between the conditions under which first and second languages are
learnt.
In the intervening five decades, second language acquisition has
become a research field in its own right. Even so, its impact on class-
room teaching is somewhat limited. According to Lantolf (2012: 63),
SLA researchers express concerns about the applicability of their research
and most recognize the importance of making their theory and findings
more accessible and comprehensible for classroom teachers.
A main aim of this book is to bridge the research-practice gap by trans-
lating complex language acquisition theory into classroom pedagogy. The
previous chapter examined the Social-Pragmatic Theory of first language
acquisition. This theory posits that children learn their first language
9.1.1 Complexity
9.1.1.1 Subsystems
• age
• aptitude
• motivation
• attitude
• personality factors
• cognitive style
• hemisphericity
• learning strategies
• gender
• interests
9.1.2 Dynamism
A dynamic system is one which changes over time. The state the system is
in the moment before a change occurs determines how it develops. The
outcomes of interactions between its components are unpredictable as
each interaction alters the environment in which further interactions take
place. Baicchi (2015: 14) points out that this means a complex system
cannot be understood in simple mechanical ways. While parts of the
9 Foundations 131
9.1.3 Openness
explain that while a closed system arrives at a steady state and does not
change, energy flows into an open system which allows it to change. A
similar point is made by Larsen-Freeman (2012) who observes that an
open system will interact with systems outside of itself, which allows it
to receive feedback as a basis for change.
All living languages are also open to and interact with their envi-
ronments. According to Verspoor and Nguyen (2015: 307), a language
is an open system which interacts with ‘cognitive, historical, pedagog-
ical, economic, social, and a number of other systems.’ According to De
Bot et al. (2007: 14), a language learner also represents an open system
which interacts with a larger social system. A learner also has their own
ecosystem which consists of cognition, intelligence, motivation, aptitude,
intentionality, mother tongue, knowledge of the target language, and so
on. This ecosystem is affected by maturity, education, the amount of
exposure to the target language, etcetera.
9.1.4 Self-Organization
9.1.5 Adaptation
9.1.6 Non-linearity
9.1.7 Emergence
9.2.1 Constructions
9.2.2 Continuum
Substantive Schematic
Complex Idioms Syntactic rules
Atomistic Words Syntactic categories
9.2.3 Constructicon
9.3 Summary
The purpose of this chapter was to consider the foundations of usage-
based linguistics. One cornerstone of usage-based linguistics is Dynamic
Systems Theory which is drawn upon to explain language develop-
ment. The seven dimensions of DST are complexity, dynamism, open-
ness, self-organization, adaptation, non-linearity, and emergence. These
dimensions can each be related to second language acquisition.
A key feature of DST is complexity. A complex system has a large
number of components which interact with each other. In addition, a
complex system is hierarchically organized into multiple subsystems at
different levels of granularity. A language meets the criteria to be thought
of as a complex system. First, it has a large number of components
and possesses multiple subsystems which interact with each other. These
include semantics, syntax, lexis, morphology, phonology, and pragmatics.
Each one of these subsystems is complex in its own right. To take lexis as
an example, experts are agreed that the lexicon contains tens of thousands
and possibly hundreds of thousands of concrete examples of language.
In addition, the process of language learning can also be thought of
as complex. It is dependent on many interacting factors such as first
language, target language, degree and quality of input, and learner factors
such as age, gender, aptitude, personality type, motivation, and interests.
A complex system is dynamic and changes iteratively over time. Any
change that takes place is always dependent on the initial condition
of the system, which makes anything other than short-term outcomes
unpredictable. A language is also dynamic. Either the language can
change, or a person’s idiolect can change. While languages are often
thought of as static entities, they change gradually over time as people
use them to interact. In addition, a learner’s knowledge of a language
140 I. Pemberton
References
Baicchi, A. (2015). Construction learning as a complex adaptive system: Psycholin-
guistic evidence from L2 learners of English. Springer.
Beckner, C., Blythe, R., Bybee, J., Christiansen, M. H., Croft, W., Ellis, N.
C., Holland, J., Ke, J., Larsen-Freeman, D., & Schoenemann, T. (2009).
Language is a complex adaptive system: Position paper. Language Learning,
59 (s1), 1–26.
Bloom, P. (2000). How children learn the meanings of words. MIT Press.
Brysbaert, M., Stevens, M., Mandera, P., & Keuleers, E. (2016). How many
words do we know? Practical estimates of vocabulary size dependent on word
definition, the degree of language input and the participant’s age. Frontiers
in Psychology, 7 (1116).
Bybee, J. (2010). Language, usage and cognition. Cambridge University Press.
Corder, S. P. (1973). Introducing applied linguistics. Penguin.
Croft, W. (2010). Construction grammar. In D. Geeraerts & H. Cuyckens
(Eds.), The Oxford handbook of cognitive linguistics (1st ed.). Oxford Univer-
sity Press.
Croft, W., & Cruse, D. A. (2004). Cognitive linguistics. Cambridge University
Press.
De Bot, K., Lowie, W., & Verspoor, M. (2007). A Dynamic Systems Theory
approach to second language acquisition. Bilingualism, 10 (1), 7–21.
De Bot, K., Lowie, W., Thorne, S. L., & Verspoor, M. (2013) Dynamic
Systems Theory as a comprehensive theory of second language development.
In: García-Mayo, M. d. P., Gutierrez-Mangado, M. J., & Martínez-Adrián,
M., (Eds.). Contemporary Approaches to Second Language Acquisition. John
Benjamins Publishing Company.
Diessel, H. (2017). Usage-based linguistics. In M. Aronoff (Ed.), Oxford
research encyclopedia of linguistics. Oxford University Press.
9 Foundations 143
10.1 Fundamentals
10.1.1 Constructions
• The simplest of these are formulas which are chunks learnt by rote,
for example, ‘lemme see,’ ‘I wanna do it’ and ‘I dunno.’ These are
also referred to as ‘multi-word expressions’ (MWEs) or, in the Lexical
Approach, ‘fixed expressions.’
• A low-scope pattern (known in the Lexical Approach as a ‘semi-fixed
expression’) is a structure that includes both open and fixed positions
(Bybee, 2010: 9). For example, I wanna VERB, I don’t VERB.
• Finally, Diessel (2017: 3) provides an example of a fully abstract
construction, explaining that ‘player’ is a construction consisting of
a verb ‘play’ and a suffix ‘er’ denoting an actor. This likely gives rise
to abstract patterns such as ‘action-actor’ or the even more abstract
‘verb–er.’
10.1.2 Continuum
10.1.3 Constructicon
the brain and its response to experience. De Bot et al., (2013: 209) go
further, asserting that the brain does not contain an abstract grammar. In
its place, a language user has a network of constructions, and these are
conventions that have emerged over time as a consequence of language
use.
All usage-based theories of representation subscribe to the principle
of redundancy. That is to say they consider that concrete examples exist
alongside schematic structures. Tummers et al. (2005) describe the usage-
based model of language representation as one which includes not only
abstract grammatical patterns of multiple levels of schematicity but also
their lexical instantiations (cited in Eskildsen, 2009: 335). Ellis (2017:
41) alludes to the scope of the constructicon in commenting that it
contains not thousands but tens of thousands of constructions.
10.2 Entrenchment
According to Dynamic Systems Theory, a language is a complex, open,
and adaptive system. An open system exchanges energy with its environ-
ment and an adaptive system changes in response to its environment.
At the same time, the environment changes in response to the system.
In other words, the system and environment co-adapt to each other. In
terms of a language, when the language spoken by the speech community
changes, it is known as conventionalization. When a speech community
influences an individual, it is known as entrenchment.
The process of entrenchment is the key mechanism in the establish-
ment of constructions. Schmid (2017: 9) comments that the term was
first introduced in 1987 by Ron Langacker. According to Langacker
(1999: 93), entrenchment is also referred to in the literature as routiniza-
tion, automatization, and habit formation. He points out that the delib-
erate use of entrenchment implies a gradual learning of a unit over many
instances (Langacker, 2017: 40). In relation to this, Cordes (2017: 269)
comments that entrenchment is a lifelong process of adaptation and reor-
ganization of a speaker’s language knowledge. Entrenchment occurs as a
result of the confluence of a wide variety of learner external factors and
internal mechanisms.
10 Usage-Based Linguistics 149
(2011: 55), learners notice frequent items in input, and language which
is perceptually salient has a better chance of gaining their attention. A
further factor which affects uptake is how clear the meaning of a language
item is. De Bot et al. (2013: 210) cite MacWhinney’s competition model
which sets out that in addition to frequency, cue availability and relia-
bility also influence acquisition. While cue availability refers to frequency
of occurrence, cue reliability refers to the transparency of a language item.
This is a function of how often it is used with the same meaning. For
example, the use of ‘is’ after he or she. This will be frequently present
with the same meaning. On the other hand, the English articles ‘a’ and
‘the,’ although frequent will be used with different meanings, reducing
cue reliability.
The notion that frequency underpins fluency begins to reveal the
scale of the task. Larsen-Freeman (2011: 56) explains that once learners
have assigned a meaning to language items, they can categorize them
and begin to form prototypes. Ellis (2019: 48) cautions that learning
a language is like developing expertise in any other skill. As a rule of
thumb, skill learning requires up to ten thousand hours of practice.
In contrast to generative grammar which posits that learners use
language specific learning skills to acquire a language, usage-based theo-
rists assert that learners use general cognitive mechanisms. Diessel (2017:
1) notes that these learning mechanisms are also employed in the fields
of vision, memory, and thought. A wide variety of cognitive mecha-
nisms are used to learn new language, such as association, categorization,
abstraction, and schematization. The major mechanisms are outlined
below.
10.3 Association
Schmid (2020: 44) argues that the fundamental mechanism of language
learning is association. He defines associations as the ability of one expe-
rience to evoke another. Associations are organized into neural networks
in the brain, which constantly shifts its attention between collections of
neurons firing either in sequence or at the same time. Schmid (2017:
10 Usage-Based Linguistics 151
10.4 Categorization
Categorization is a fundamental cognitive skill which we employ from
early childhood to understand our surroundings. It involves grouping
items and gives rise to hypernyms (group names) and hyponyms (group
members). The basis for categorization is perceptual similarities. For
very young children, this will mean characteristics such as size, shape,
and colour. Categorization is a general ability which allows us to make
predictions about unknown entities based on their similarity to known
entities. Historically, judgments about other individuals, things, and
circumstances would have been important for human survival. The cate-
gorization process is fundamental to generalization and schema forma-
tion. Cordes (2017: 276) explains that the basis of categorization is a
prototypical member of a category which embodies most of the shared
elements of other members of the category. Members of a category will
either be closer to or further away from the prototype.
10.5 Analogy
A similar process but one which is distinct from categorization is analogy.
A categorization is made when two things have clear physical similari-
ties, and thus, they can be put in the same class. On the other hand, an
analogy is made when two things may not be obviously similar but may
have functional similarities. One or more elements of the known thing
(the source) are aligned with and mapped onto the unknown thing (the
target). Cordes (2017: 280) gives the example of comparing the solar
system to a nuclear atom. At its centre, an atom has a nucleus, and elec-
trons revolve around the nucleus. Similarly, at its centre, the solar system
has the sun, and the planets revolve around the sun.
154 I. Pemberton
10.6 Schematization
The cognitive processes of categorization and analogy lead to the
process of schematization. This involves the abstraction (or extraction) of
commonalities from two or more exemplars to create a linguistic proto-
type. These commonalities then form a higher level of abstraction. In
usage-based linguistics both the concrete forms and the more abstract
constructions are retained. Eskildsen (2009: 336) notes that learners
progress through increasing levels of schematization: they begin by
employing formulas (unanalysed multi-word units with specific mean-
ings) in familiar situations. They then progress to the use of low-scope
patterns. A low-scope pattern is a schema which is partially fixed and
partially productive; for example, ‘I don’t VERB.’ Usage-based theorists
then hypothesize that learners progress to fully abstract constructions,
e.g., noun phrase-auxiliary-negator-verb.
10.7 Summary
Usage-based linguistics draws upon Dynamic Systems Theory, consid-
ering language to be non-linear, self-organizing, and emergent. With
echoes of Steven Krashen’s Natural Approach, it puts a strong emphasis
on the importance of input, but it goes beyond the Natural Approach
as it does not consider comprehensible input to be a sufficient condition
for acquisition. In fact, it does not even consider comprehended input to
be a sufficient condition. In order to acquire, a learner also has to attend
to the form of the input.
In addition to the dimensions of Dynamic Systems Theory, usage-
based linguistics draws upon constructionist grammars. In particular,
it adopts the three key concepts of constructions, continuum, and
constructicon. Advocates assert language knowledge consists of construc-
tions. A construction is a symbolic representation of syntactic form
and semantic meaning. They may be found at all levels of specificity:
morpheme, phrase, clause, sentence, or even at a suprasentential level.
Secondly, usage-based linguists make no distinction between grammar
and vocabulary. They consider grammar to be nothing more than a
10 Usage-Based Linguistics 155
10.8 Implications
Drawing upon complexity theories and constructionist grammars, usage-
based linguistics presents a compelling account of first, second, and
additional language acquisition which has the power to explain a range
of language phenomena. To begin with, it explains why teaching and
practising grammar doesn’t lead to fluency. While the abstract slot and
filler patterns of grammar may be used to create original language, they
only represent a fraction of the patterns that exist within a language and
cannot imbue a learner with the ability to produce conventional ways of
saying things, or for that matter to understand them when they are used
by other speakers particularly where the language is idiomatic.
In addition, usage-based linguistics explains why over-focus on spoken
output activities in pairs or groups (as advocated by Communicative
Language Teaching and Task-Based Instruction) doesn’t lead to fluency.
A fluent language user has a tacit knowledge of language patterns of
all levels of specificity, from the highly abstract to the highly concrete,
including their relative frequencies and contexts of use, and can deploy
this knowledge to process and produce conventional ways of saying
things which identify them as legitimate language users in a speech
community. This knowledge is acquired through repeated exposure to
language in its contexts of situation and culture. This kind of knowledge
cannot be learnt through classroom activities which focus on production.
Furthermore, the concept of emergence explains why learners plateau
for significant periods of time and then experience breakthroughs as they
build a critical mass of language knowledge and then move to the next
level of proficiency.
This begs the question. What then, should learners do to develop
the proficiency to do well in language examinations, or to interact as
legitimate members of a target language community? The foregoing
suggests that to attain fluency, language learners need to experience and
understand language patterns repeatedly in their situational and cultural
10 Usage-Based Linguistics 157
contexts and that this kind of practice may allow them to build a crit-
ical mass of language knowledge which will precipitate phase shifts in
their language systems. But what does the research say about usage-based
linguistics? This is the focus of the next chapter.
References
Baicchi, A. (2015). Construction learning as a complex adaptive system. Springer
International Publishing.
Bybee, J. (2010). Language, usage and cognition. Cambridge University Press.
Cordes, A. K. (2017). The roles of analogy, categorization, and generalization
in entrenchment. In H. J. Schmid (Ed.), Entrenchment and the psychology of
language learning. De Gruyter Mouton.
De Bot, K., Lowie, W., Thorne, S. L., & Verspoor, M. (2013). Dynamic
Systems Theory as a comprehensive theory of second language development.
In M. D. P. García-Mayo, M. J. Gutierrez-Mangado, & M. Martínez-
Adrián (Eds.), Contemporary approaches to second language acquisition. John
Benjamins Publishing Company.
Diessel, H. (2017) Usage-Based Linguistics. In: Aronoff, M., ed. Oxford
Research Encyclopedia of Linguistics. New York: Oxford University Press:
Dolgova, N., & Tyler, A. (2019). Applications of usage-based approaches to
language teaching. In X. Gao (Ed.), Second handbook of English language
teaching (pp. 939–961). Springer International Publishing.
Ellis, N. C. (2007). Dynamic systems and SLA: The wood and the trees.
Bilingualism, 10 (1), 23–25.
Ellis, N. C. (2017). Cognition, corpora, and computing: Triangulating research
in usage-based language learning. Language Learning, 67 (S1), 40–65.
Ellis, N. C. (2019). Essentials of a theory of language cognition. The Modern
Language Journal, 103(S1), 39–60.
Eskildsen, S. W. (2009). Constructing another language—Usage-based linguis-
tics in second language acquisition. Applied Linguistics, 30 (3), 335–357.
Goldberg, A. E. (2009). The nature of generalization in language. Cognitive
Linguistics, 20 (1), 93–127.
Ibbotson, P. (2013). The scope of usage-based theory. Frontiers in Psychology,
4 (255), 1–15.
158 I. Pemberton
The previous chapter set out how usage-based linguistics explains second
language acquisition. It explained that usage-based linguistics is founded
upon Dynamic Systems Theory, a general complexity theory. It also
draws upon the tenets of constructionist grammars. It considers the
fundamental elements of a language to be symbolic pairings of form and
meaning called constructions. Usage-based theories do not dichotomize
between grammar and lexis and constructions can be placed upon a cline.
The cline has two dimensions: from atomicity to complexity, and from
specificity to schematicity. Furthermore, the constructions exist within
a constructicon, which is theorized to be a hierarchically structured
taxonomy of language items. Within the constructicon, there is consid-
erable redundancy brought about by the inclusion of concrete language
items and the more abstract schemas that are created from them.
On a theoretical level, usage-based linguistics exhibits considerable
explanatory power, but what does the empirical research suggest about its
efficacy? In comparison to other areas of linguistics, it is a relatively small
field but one which is growing in importance. A review of the existing
literature reveals multiple research approaches as well as a variety of areas
of inquiry. Approaches to research include case studies, cross-sectional
studies, and corpus analyses. Among the most prominent areas of inquiry
are learner variation and variability, as well as pedagogical approaches,
including the use of autonomy, and the employment of multimedia.
The key points here are that the constructions are language patterns that
describe specific phenomena and are completely fixed. The constructions
in the second stage of development are variously referred to as:
The key difference here is that these constructions are partially fixed. For
example, they may contain the same verb, but the object of the verb may
be an open slot which can be filled by various objects. The constructions
in the final stage of the process are variously referred to as:
The key points here are that these constructions are abstract, and that
they can be generalized to unrelated phenomena. While this might be
confusing to the novice reader, usage-based linguistics envisages a move-
ment from concrete examples of language to expressions with a fixed
element and an open slot, to entirely abstract representations. A number
of case studies have investigated this developmental trajectory.
666) concludes that Ana ‘began with the ability to use individual items,
perhaps as limited scope patterns, and later developed abilities that may
be characterized as grammaticalized linguistic constructions.’
upon Talmy’s Motion Event typology. Talmy analysed motion events into
four components: the Figure (the thing that moves), the Motion (the
act of moving), the Path (the trajectory followed), and the Ground (the
destination). A Path is encoded using either a preposition or a satellite
and a preposition, e.g., go out to the garden.
Li et al. analysed 120 hours of video footage of one individual (Carlos,
the Spanish-speaking learner of English from Eskildsen’s study) span-
ning a time period of three and a half years. The results showed that he
initially used a limited number of motion verbs, primarily ‘go’ and ‘come’
and that the Path was limited to prepositions (e.g., to, in) or missing
(e.g., go the bank). Over time, Carlos began to use a wider range of
motion verbs together with a wider range of prepositions and satellite
plus preposition combinations. Li et al. conclude that Carlos’s acqui-
sition of verb-argument constructions was item-based, and these items
formed the basis of more complex constructions which emerged over
time. However, they caution that in the period under investigation, it was
not possible to assert that he began to produce abstract constructions.
This data suggests that at the beginner stages, lexical knowledge acts as
a precursor to the emergence of syntax, but by the time learners have
reached an intermediate level, their syntactic system stabilizes, allowing
them to focus on expanding their lexis further.
Referential Phrasemes
1. Compounds sunbathing, dressing rooms, deep
blue, forest fire, after-sun cream,
two-week holiday, ice-cream
2. Lexical collocations heavy rain, closely linked, apologize
profusely; the sun goes down, take
a dive, strong current, pretty hard,
real close, went wrong, hurt badly
3. Particles Aim at, afraid of, involved in, at
school, in English
4. Complements avoid –ing; necessary to; want/going/
have/manage to; go –ing; keep –ing;
would like to; be able to; know +
clause; say that + clause
5. Phrasal verbs blow up, make out, crop up
6. Idioms to spill the beans, to let the cat out of
the bag, to bark up the wrong tree
7. Similes as old as the hills, to swear like a
trooper
(continued)
166 I. Pemberton
(continued)
Referential Phrasemes
8. Irreversible bi-and trinomials bed and breakfast; kith and kin; left,
right and centre
9. Structures even ADJ + er than; as ADJ as, it is
easy to do, a year ago, two metres
high, so happy that
10. Variable idioms think nothing of –ing; pay a price for
–ing; end up –ing
11. Constructions The sooner we are finished, the
sooner we can go
12. Conventionalized sentence stems one thing I know for sure is…; all
they can do is…
13. Conventionalized sentences It‘s hard to explain. I‘m just who I am.
I (really) like her as a friend
Textual phrasemes
14. Textual prepositions with respect to, in addition to, apart from,
irrespective of
15. Textual conjunctions so that, as if, even though, as soon as,
given that
16. Textual adverbs in other words, last but not least, more
accurately, what is more, to conclude, the
reason for, however
17. Textual sentence stems the final point is …; another thing is …; it
will be shown that …; I will discuss …
Communicative Phrasemes
18. Communicative speech act formulae good morning; take care; you‘re
welcome; suggesting (why don’t
we), concluding (that’s all)
19. Communicative attitudinal formulae in fact, to be honest, it is clear
and sentence stems that, I think that…
20. Commonplaces it‘s a small world; we only live
once; the sky is the limit
two groups: a low input group or a high input group. The low input
group received approximately two hours of input per week of conven-
tional foreign language instruction giving a total of 220 hours over two
years. The high input group were part of a semi-immersion programme
and were taught half of their school subjects (History, Mathematics,
Science, etc.) in English. They received 15 hours of English input per
week providing a total of 1320 hours over two years. The researchers
collected writing samples at the beginning and end of the study. The
writing samples were hand-coded for types of chunk and tokens. They
then calculated how many examples of each type of chunk were present
in every hundred words. In the second part of the study, the researchers
analysed the writing samples of two individual learners. In order to do
this, they collected a greater number of writing samples.
The results of the cross-sectional study were contrary to the
researchers’ expectations. While the analysis showed that the high input
group used more chunks by the end of the study, they did not use signif-
icantly more. There was only one significant difference in the type of
chunk—they used four times as many conventionalized sentence stems,
e.g., ‘one thing I know for sure is’ and ‘all they can do is’ (Verspoor &
Smiskova, 2012: 41).
With regard to the individual learners, the researchers investigated
development in chunk tokens and types. To begin with tokens, neither
learner showed a steady increase in chunk use. Using a moving correla-
tion and min–max graphs, Verspoor and Smiskova (2012: 37) looked
for shifts in learner proficiency. The data for the lower input learner
exhibited no clear signs of development showing consistent bandwidth,
neither widening nor narrowing throughout the time period. On the
other hand, for the high input learner, the analysis indicated clear signs
of development. The variability band both widened and narrowed: there
were peaks in the use of chunk types followed by plateaus as the learner’s
language system oscillated and stabilized.
In addition to investigating the number of tokens used by the learners,
the researchers also analysed the number of chunk types. This anal-
ysis showed that the high input learner employed a greater range of
types. Furthermore, they looked at the development of fully fixed chunks
168 I. Pemberton
and partially schematic chunks. They categorize fully fixed chunks and
partially schematic chunks as follows:
For both FFCs and PSCs, the low input learner was consistent in using
three types each throughout the period of investigation. Furthermore,
there appeared to be no correlation between development in the use of
these two types of chunks. On the other hand, the high input learner
began with using two to three FFCs at the beginning of the period but
increased their use to a maximum of ten types. With regard to PSCs,
the high input learner began by using three types but progressed to use
five types later on. Also, there was a clear correlation in the patterns of
development with peaks and plateaus.
All in all, these research results indicate that the acquisition of chunks
is a slow process. While the researchers were able to demonstrate a
developmental pattern for the high input learner, the cross-sectional
study suggests that, in a shorter time frame, exposure to the target
language alone may not be adequate to build enough language knowl-
edge to support phase shifts in learners’ proficiency, and that it might be
necessary to combine other strategies with an input-based approach.
A 2019 study by Romer and Berger set out to measure the development
of verb-argument constructions (VACS) across a wide range of learners.
In order to do this, they used the EFCAMDAT corpus (English First
Cambridge Open Language Database). This allowed them to analyse
samples from more than 12,000 learners. The researchers looked at
texts written by German learners (28,000 texts, 2.8 million words) and
Spanish-speaking Mexican learners (40,000 texts, 3.2 million words) at
five proficiency levels from beginner to advanced. They used a concor-
dancer to investigate the development of the 19 frequent VACS. All have
a verb-preposition-noun pattern.
1) type-token comparisons
2) construction growth analysis
3) correlation analysis of verb-VAC associations
over three years. The researchers collected 54 texts written by the subject.
They analysed the texts for accuracy and complexity. The Finnish system
of case was selected for analysis of accuracy as it has 15 different cases.
To measure complexity, they looked at word, noun phrase, and sentence
level complexity. They also examined whether there was a correlation
between accuracy and complexity.
In terms of accuracy, the learner demonstrated greatest variability in
the early stages. From the mid-point of the study there was a decrease
in variability in the learner’s accuracy as it appeared to stabilize. In
terms of complexity, it was observed that there was a correlation between
word, noun, and sentence complexity which all increased over time. The
learner began by using a greater number of shorter items which became
progressively longer. With regard to the relationship between accuracy
and complexity, examination of the data did not show a correlation.
Overall, the analysis highlighted that development in all of the measures
was characterized by progress and regress, as well as peaks and plateaus.
class in the same school, the developmental trajectories of the girls are
different.
In a 2020 study, Bulte and Housen drew upon data from Verspoor et al.’s
(2012) corpus of student writing for their investigation of learner devel-
opment across multiple levels of proficiency. Bulte and Housen’s analysis
was neither a cross-sectional nor a case study but sought to make a
detailed comparison of the development of ten Dutch learners of English
over a period of two years. The main purpose of the study was to inves-
tigate the relationship between different components of complexity. In
particular, the existence of a relationship between lexical and syntactic
complexity: whether one of these precedes the other, whether they
develop in parallel, or whether their relationship is a random one.
The researchers examined eleven texts per learner according to four
different metrics. Two of these were syntactic. One was the level of
clausal subordination measured by the Sub-Clausal Ratio (SCR). The
other was the Mean Length of T-Units (MLTU). The other two metrics
were lexical: one was the level of phrasal elaboration, measured by the
Mean Length of Noun Phrases (MNLP), and the other was the Guiraud
Index which is used to measure lexical diversity.
As expected, the study found a strong positive relationship between
the two measures of syntactic development (SCR and MLTU). Similarly,
there was a positive relationship between lexical diversity, as measured
by the Guiraud Index, and the MNLPs. In other words, both pairs
of metrics run in parallel to each other. In addition, the researchers
note that all four metrics show an increasing trend over time. They
also found a competitive relationship between clausal subordination and
phrasal elaboration. That is to say, learners either use more subordinate
clauses or more phrasal elaboration, but not both. Unlike some earlier
studies, they didn’t find any evidence of one component acting as a
precursor to another. They concur with other researchers that individual
learners demonstrate substantial variability and thus, general trends from
cross-sectional data cannot be applied to individual learners.
11 Empirical Research 179
In 2017, Kusyk repeated Toffoli and Sockett’s (2010) study but with a
different set of students. A questionnaire designed to investigate online
habits was distributed to 953 students: 538 French students and 415
German students. The participants were non-English majors. The results
of the questionnaire were as follows. Both groups had similar frequency
186 I. Pemberton
11.7 Summary
The usage-based linguistics literature reveals a wide range of macro-
approaches to usage-based research. One common research approach
is to use case studies. A case study can focus on a range of language
phenomena. For example, a study can look at specific items. One case
study examined the development of relative clauses. The child participant
demonstrated a developmental pattern from the use of individual items
to low-scope constructions. Another study looked at the use of negation
by adult learners. This study demonstrated a developmental trajectory
from item-based expressions to increasingly abstract representations. A
further case study examined the use of verb-argument constructions to
express motion. This study showed that the learner began with a limited
set of general verbs and prepositions to express motion events. Over time
the learner began to use an increasingly specific set of verbs and prepo-
sitions. These studies lend weight to the hypothesis that learners use
concrete examples of language to form more abstract representations.
Whereas case studies can be used to track individual developmental
patterns, cross-sectional studies can be used to look at general develop-
mental trends. In one study, texts written by learners were categorized
into five levels of proficiency, then analysed to track the use of different
language phenomena. This analysis indicated that at lower levels of profi-
ciency, lexical development precedes syntactic development, and that
syntax stabilizes at an intermediate level of proficiency allowing for the
further development of lexis. Another area which usage-based linguists
have investigated cross-sectionally is the use of chunks. An initial study
compared two groups of learners: a high and low input group. Although
the learners in the high input group used more tokens by the end of the
11 Empirical Research 187
study, they did not use significantly more. In a later reanalysis of the data,
the high input group were shown to use significantly more types, longer
chunks, and to use a greater proportion of chunks per text.
Cross-sectional studies can also be useful to compare different teaching
approaches. In order to compare usage-based instruction to form-focused
instruction, researchers have experimented with delaying grammar input,
and not teaching grammar at all. A study assessing the effect of delaying
grammar instruction found that learners who received an intensive
grammar course towards the end of their second year of instruction
did as well on a grammar test as learners on a conventional structure-
based course. A pair of studies investigating the effect of not providing
any explicit instruction in grammar compared the attainment of two
groups of learners: one group followed a conventional structure-based
course, whereas the other group were given a usage-based course but
not taught any grammar. In both cases, the learners on the usage-based
course became more proficient in general English and did as well on
a grammar test. What these studies show is that even without explicit
instruction in grammar, learners can implicitly acquire the underlying
abstract patterns of the language system.
As a language is a complex system consisting of a huge amount of
data, corpus techniques are well-suited to usage-based linguistic anal-
ysis. A pair of studies examined texts produced by learners at different
levels of proficiency. The specific focus was the use of high frequency
verb-argument constructions. In both cases, the researchers were able to
identify which constructions were used at which levels of proficiency,
giving a plausible account of learner development. The studies showed
that learners began by using a small set of general verbs in a narrow range
of constructions. As they became more proficient, they increasingly used
more specific verbs in a wider range of constructions.
A major area of interest within usage-based linguistics is variation
and variability. A number of studies have examined the variability of
specific linguistic phenomena. One study looked at the development
of future expression. The results of the study showed that development
differed from learner to learner. Other research has examined variability
in general measures of language proficiency. One study looked at the
188 I. Pemberton
References
Bardovi-Harlig, K. (2002). A new starting point? Studies in Second Language
Acquisition, 24 (2), 189–198.
Bulté, B., & Housen, A. (2020). A DUB-inspired case study of multidimen-
sional L2 complexity development: Competing or connecting growers? In
W. Lowie, R. Steinkrauss, M. Keijzer & M. Michel (Eds.), Usage-based
dynamics in second language development. Multilingual Matters.
Bylund, E., & Athanasopoulos, P. (2015). Televised whorf: Cognitive restruc-
turing in advanced foreign language learners as a function of audiovisual
media exposure. The Modern Language Journal, 99 (S1), 123–137.
Dahl, A. (2015). Input and language competence in early-start foreign language
classrooms. In T. Cadierno & S. W. Eskildsen (Eds.), Usage-based perspectives
on second language learning (pp. 125–152). De Gruyter Mouton.
Erman, B., & Warren, B. (2000). The idiom principle and the open choice
principle. Text (The Hague), 20 (1), 29–62.
Eskildsen, S. W. (2012). L2 negation constructions at work. Language Learning,
62(2), 335–372.
Gustafsson, H., & Verspoor, M. (2017). Development of chunks in Dutch L2
learners of English. In J. Evers-Vermeul & E. Tribushinina (Eds.), Usage-
based approaches to language acquisition and language teaching. De Gruyter,
Inc.
Irshad, F. M. (2015). Second language development through the lens of a dynamic
usage-based approach. University of Groningen.
Jo, R., & Oh, S.-Y. (2021). A Usage-based study of L2 constructional develop-
ment: Combining learner corpus and experimental data. English Teaching,
76 (1), 3–31.
Koster, D. E. S. (2015). A dynamic, usage-based approach to teaching L2
Dutch. Dutch Journal of Applied Linguistics, 4 (2), 257–264.
Kusyk, M. (2017). The development of complexity, accuracy and fluency in
L2 written production through informal participation in online activities.
CALICO Journal, 34 (1), 75–96.
Larsen-Freeman, D. (2006). The emergence of complexity, fluency, and accu-
racy in the oral and written production of five chinese learners of english.
Applied Linguistics, 27 (4), 590–619.
Li, P., Eskildsen, S. W., & Cadierno, T. (2014). Tracing an L2 learner’s motion
constructions over time: A usage-based classroom investigation. The Modern
Language Journal, 98(2), 612–628.
11 Empirical Research 191
Lowie, W., Verspoor, M., & van Dijk, M. (2018). The acquisition of L2
speaking: A dynamic perspective. In R. A. Alonso (Ed.), Speaking in a second
language. John Benjamins Publishing Company.
Mellow, J. D. (2006). The emergence of second language syntax: A case study
of the acquisition of relative clauses. Applied Linguistics, 27 (4), 645–670.
Nguyen, T. P. H. (2013). A dynamic usage-based approach to second language
teaching. DPhil University of Groningen.
Piggott, L., Tribushinina, E., & de Graaff, R. (2020) 11. The icing on the
cake? Effects of explicit form-focused instruction after two years of implicit
EFL learning. In W. Lowie, M. Michel, M. Keijzer & R. Steinkrauss (Eds.),
Usage-based dynamics in second language development. Multilingual Matters,
249–270.
Roehr-Brackin, K. (2014). Explicit knowledge and processes from a usage-
based perspective: The developmental trajectory of an instructed L2 learner:
Explicit processes from a usage-based perspective. Language Learning, 64 (4),
771–808.
Römer, U., & Berger, C. M. (2019). Observing the emergence of construc-
tional knowledge: Verb patterns in german and spanish learners of english
at different proficiency levels. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 41(5),
1089–1110.
Rousse-Malpat, A., & Verspoor, M. (2012). Measuring effectiveness in Focus
on Form versus Focus on Meaning. Dutch Journal of Applied Linguistics,
1(2), 263–276.
Rousse-Malpat, A., & Verspoor, M. (2018). Foreign language instruction from
a dynamic usage-based perspective. In A. E. Tyler, L. Ortega, M. Uno &
H. I. Park, (Eds.), Usage-inspired L2 instruction: Researched pedagogy. John
Benjamins Publishing Company:
Rousse-Malpat, A., Koote, L., Steinkrauss, R., & Verspoor, M. (2021).
Parlez-vous francais? Effects of structure-based versus dynamic-usage-based
approaches on oral proficiency. Language Teaching Research, 1–25.
Sockett, G. (2013). Understanding the online informal learning of English as
a complex dynamic system: An emic approach. ReCALL : The Journal of
EUROCALL, 25 (1), 48–62.
Sockett, G., & Toffoli, D. (2012). Beyond learner autonomy: A dynamic
systems view of the informal learning of English in virtual online commu-
nities. ReCALL : The Journal of EUROCALL, 24 (2), 138–151.
Spoelman, M., & Verspoor, M. (2010). Dynamic patterns in development of
accuracy and complexity: A longitudinal case study in the acquisition of
Finnish. Applied Linguistics, 31(4), 532–553.
192 I. Pemberton
12.1.1.1 Authenticity
linguists Verspoor and Nguyen (2015: 308) state that it is better to teach
language in meaningful contexts which approximate the norms of social
and cultural usage. When learners are exposed to sufficient authentic
input, it helps them to establish links between form and meaning (ibid.:
315). Secondly, authentic texts promote retention. Hill (1967: 118)
points out that ‘it is much easier to learn a word or structure if it grows
naturally out of a context than if it is taught without such a context,
or if the context is a forced, artificial, classroom one.’ In summary,
authentic contexts supply the raw data that becomes the building blocks
for construction of learner interlanguage. They are concrete evidence of
how language is actually used. As with any inputs to a system, and partic-
ularly a complex one, the higher the quality of the inputs, the greater the
chance of creating a well-constructed and functioning system.
learning vocabulary but that many learners will need a teacher’s help in
identifying co-text.
12.1.1.5 Frequency
12.1.1.6 Connotation
language like a first one. This is certainly true. But it misses the point. A
first language is not taught, rather, it is learnt. And the good news is that
psycholinguistic theory is reporting what the reformists and other more
enlightened educators have known all along—that second and additional
languages are learnt like first ones.
Multimedia materials create ideational and linguistic contexts for
learners which activate their primary biological skills. They provide expo-
sure to concrete examples of language which can be schematized to
create linguistic prototypes. This is a very exciting idea but even today,
despite their availability, most educators and learners don’t use multi-
media resources extensively in the classroom. And even when they are
used, they are not necessarily used in ways that will lead to language
acquisition. Thus, a second set of principles is needed that relate to how
to use multimedia materials.
the learners from the teacher. This definition places learners in self-
determining groups rather than individuals deciding what they will do.
A contextual approach interprets autonomy in a stricter sense—not the
autonomy of the group but personal autonomy. That is to say, learners
work by themselves, not all the time, but enough to develop a language
base that they can subsequently draw upon in their interactions with
teachers and other learners and depending on their context, perhaps
friends, family, or members of the public.
Autonomy can relate to behaviours inside or outside the classroom.
Modern day classrooms tend to be dominated by speaking activities.
However, a contextual approach favours learner autonomy in the class-
room based on the observation that languages are, for the most part,
not learnt while speaking to others. They are learnt while listening and
mapping acoustic data onto features of physical and cognitive environ-
ments. Working autonomously crucially places a learner’s emphasis on
processing not production.
It is sometimes suggested that learners provide input to each other,
and this is an important element of Krashen’s Natural Approach and
Communicative Language Teaching but there are questions of whether
less proficient learners can provide language input to each other of a suffi-
ciently high quality to promote acquisition, and whether learners can
process input for form while they are attending to meaning.
Autonomy can also relate to behaviours outside the classroom.
Allwright and Hanks (2009: 2) make the point that they want students
to learn how to learn and continue to do so after their educational
programmes finish. Field (2008: 4) agrees, stating that a central goal of
language teaching should be to prepare learners for life outside of the
classroom. This recognizes the limitations of classroom teaching and that
for many learners, most of the language learning is going to occur in the
outside world through the use of reading and listening materials, and
where learners are living in the target language community, spoken inter-
actions. Training learners to learn autonomously turns them into lifelong
learners who can continue to study once they have graduated from their
educational programmes.
12 Principles 207
In his ELT Journal article, Skills and strategies: towards a new methodology
for listening, Field (1998: 110) contrasts classical and modern approaches
to the teaching of listening. He begins by setting out a typical format for
listening lessons in the 1960s and 1970s which he calls ‘the compre-
hension approach.’ A typical lesson would proceed according to the
following stages:
Field (1998: 111) makes the point that the lesson format has been
modified, whereas it actually needs rethinking completely.
Field (2008: 45) suggests giving learners control over the pace of the
recording. This would:
it decays rapidly. Schmidt (2001: 16) notes that stimuli which a person
doesn’t attend to will only persist for a few seconds. The second feature is
a process of articulatory rehearsal. Articulatory rehearsal means repeating
items (e.g., a phone number) using inner speech. The process of articu-
latory rehearsal keeps items in memory for longer periods of time (Ellis,
2001: 33).
Articulatory rehearsal in phonological working memory is an impor-
tant driver of both first and second language acquisition. To begin with,
children learning their L1 use rehearsal to internalize new language. Wen
(2015: 47) reports that cognitive psychologists in Europe have found
rehearsal facilitates the acquisition of vocabulary and the development of
a learner’s first language. Furthermore, an increasing number of linguists
view working memory as playing an equal or greater role in the acquisi-
tion of an additional language. Schmidt (2001: 10) cites Baddeley et al.
(1988) who contend that rehearsal in short-term memory is a neces-
sary condition for learning new language. According to Randall (2007:
161), rehearsal in working memory is not only a necessary condition for
learning but actually leads to fluency. Hulstijn (2001: 261) reports on the
results of a number of studies which have shown that briefly repeating
the target language in working memory leads to better retention. These
studies indicate that repetition can be either vocal or subvocal. More-
over, Hulstijn (ibid.: 66) cites research by Ellis and Sinclair (1996)
which found that when second language learners listen to and use the
target language, they learn about the frequency of its phonemes and
their probable sequences, and repetition consolidates words in long-term
memory.
In summary, while the rate of incidental learning is insufficient, and
either asking learners to focus on form by teaching them grammar or
during productive speaking exercises isn’t effective, research is increas-
ingly showing that attending to linguistic form and rehearsing it in
working memory provides better retention. Thus, the sixth and final
principle of a contextual approach is that attention to form is a necessary
condition for language learning.
12 Principles 213
12.2.2.3 Shadowing
12.3 Summary
This chapter introduced six principles for realising a contextual approach
inside and outside the classroom. The first four principles strive to
create the optimum conditions for language learning to take place.
The first principle of a contextual approach is that context is vital for
learning. Notwithstanding its contribution to language learning, histor-
ical teaching methods have tended to underestimate its importance.
Theorists and educators of all stripes are agreed that contexts should be
authentic. This is because they provide the building blocks for learner
interlanguage. The facilitating features of context are considered key
12 Principles 215
References
Alba, J. W., & Hasher, L. (1983). Is memory schematic? Psychological Bulletin,
93, 203–231.
Allwright, D., & Hanks, J. (2009). The developing language learner: An
introduction to exploratory practice. Palgrave Macmillan.
Baddeley, A. (2015). Working memory in second language learning. In Z. Wen,
M. Borges-Mota, & A. McNeill (Eds.), Working memory in second language
acquisition and processing (pp. 17–28). Multilingual Matters.
Barclay, S., & Schmitt, N. (2019). Current perspectives on vocabulary teaching
and learning. In X. Gao (Ed.), Second handbook of English language teaching
(pp. 799–819). Springer International Publishing.
Bybee, J. (2010). Language, usage and cognition. Cambridge University Press.
Chapelle, C. A. (2009). The relationship between second language acquisi-
tion theory and computer-assisted language learning. The Modern language
journal (Boulder, Colo.), 93(s1), 741–753.
218 I. Pemberton
However, there are many expatriates who live in a country for decades
who still fail to learn the target language to a degree of fluency. So, being
in a country where a language is spoken is not a sufficient condition for
learning the language. There are other important factors such as moti-
vation and effort which determine engagement with the target language,
and these are things that a learner can draw upon whether in the country
or not.
The third hypothesis is that use of multimedia resources promotes
deeper learning. This statement may divide opinion. However, it was
argued earlier that watching and listening are the primary biological skills
that children use to acquire their mother tongue, and that reading and
writing, the skills that are generally applied in second language learning
classrooms, are much less effective. And being in the country of the
target language provides a rich visual and auditory context within which
learners can employ their innate capacities to watch and listen, and to
map sound onto image.
But do we actually need to be there? We live in the twenty-first
century, an era when most everyone in higher resource contexts has access
to the Internet and can vicariously travel through cyberspace to pretty
much anywhere they wish. Many of us have or can get access to any
number of films, documentaries, dramas, and animations written, acted,
and narrated by expert users of the target language. Things have changed
a lot since 1625 when Francis Bacon wrote, ‘if the mountain won’t come
to Mohammad, then Mohammad must go to the mountain.’ It seems
that, on this occasion, should we wish to do so, we can bring the moun-
tain, whether it be the Matterhorn, Mount Fuji, or Mont Blanc, into our
homes, or for that matter, anywhere we happen to be.
But surely, it is still better to be in the country? The fourth hypothesis
is that autonomous learning results in deeper learning. This is a position
many will disagree with this. Any teacher schooled in Communicative
Language Teaching will have been exhorted by teacher trainers, teacher
managers, and even fellow teachers to use pairwork and groupwork activ-
ities on the basis that language is learnt through interaction with others.
They might have come to believe that this is true. Admittedly, while
learning autonomously, you can’t have conversations with people, you
can’t test hypotheses, and you can’t negotiate meaning with people.
13 Materials Design 223
But you can do better than that. The fifth hypothesis is that itera-
tion promotes language acquisition. This is more difficult to disagree
with. Put simply, watching a movie, documentary, drama, or animation,
learners can stop the programme when they don’t hear something, they
can take as long as they like, and they can replay it as many times as they
like. If they can’t catch something, they can use target language subtitles,
or if there are no target language subtitles, they may be able to use subti-
tles in their own language. Once a learner has identified the language,
they can check the meaning in a dictionary, or dictionaries, or by using
a free or paid version of an online AI translator. Now, learners can’t
interrupt a target language speaker every time they don’t hear, or don’t
understand something. If they did, the target language speaker would
quickly get frustrated with them.
The sixth and final hypothesis is that attention to form promotes
language acquisition. I think most people would agree with this state-
ment too. It was pointed out earlier that language learners have difficulty
simultaneously listening for meaning and attending to form due to
the limitations of working memory. As already mentioned, a learner
can’t keep stopping an L1 interlocutor while they focus on the form of
what they are saying. But with a video recording, learners can stop the
recording, repeat, and imitate speakers as much as they like.
To summarize, if someone asserted that a person could learn a
language better outside of a target language community than within it—
most people would disagree. But we live in an era where we have easy
access to multimedia materials which can provide rich cultural, situ-
ational, conceptual, and ideational contexts for a target language. So,
with motivation and effort, there is really no reason why you can’t learn
a language to a higher level of fluency even outside a target language
community. Of course, a learner can have the best of both worlds. That
is, live in the country where the language is spoken, but at the same time
make systematic and principled use of multimedia materials to attain
higher levels of proficiency. This is certainly what expatriates focused on
mastering a target language should do.
This chapter describes how to realize the principles of a contextual
approach, not only for the individual language learner, but also in the
design of classroom materials for learners (of an intermediate level and
224 I. Pemberton
of climate change, they can learn arguments for and against alternative
energies such as solar, wind, and wave power, or they can learn about the
steps in a process such as the greenhouse effect.
13.3.1 Title(s)
To begin with, use any titles to identify main ideas. These can be
converted to content questions to pinpoint the most important informa-
tional content. In some cases, this will be straightforward, as illustrated
by the following examples.
Semantic
Video Title Category Content Question
How playing sports benefits Factorial How does playing
your body and brain sports benefits your
body and brain?
Competitive sports: harmful or Discussion Are competitive sports
healthy? harmful? Why? Are
competitive sports
healthy? How?
Why languages die Factorial Why do languages die?
Five things we can do to Solution What are five things
control climate change we can do to control
climate change?
Make great coffee with a Process How do you make a
moka pot great coffee with a
moka pot?
228 I. Pemberton
13.3.2 Introductions
On other occasions, the title and the introduction might still not
provide enough information to identify the most important informa-
tional content, but it may be identifiable from the text type. While
the topic may be clear, the specific content may not be. An inspection
of the video transcript in terms of the semantic categories introduced
earlier, might reveal the text type, for example, a discussion (benefits and
drawbacks) structure.
Topic sentences are generally associated with written texts and a video is
a spoken text. However, many videos are scripted. They are not purely
spontaneous spoken interactions but are organized into spoken para-
graphs in much the same way as a piece of writing. Furthermore, these
paragraphs often have a conventional structure and include elements
such as topic sentences and supporting sentences. This raises a related
question of how to identify the breaks between different paragraphs.
There are a number of possible cues: a pause, a discourse marker, a
change of speaker, a change of location, or a return to a main presenter.
as ‘The future of language’ has a clear topical focus, but it is still broad,
and requires narrowing down. An analysis of the transcript might indi-
cate the logical division of ideas, for example, the script writer might
organize the text using different languages such as Chinese, French,
English, Spanish, and Arabic.
13.4 Constructions
A context facilitates the acquisition of conceptual and ideational knowl-
edge. On a language level, a context does more than disambiguate the
meanings of words. It also facilitates the acquisition of the language
necessary to articulate the concepts and ideas that the content expresses.
Furthermore, a context helps learners to connect concepts within the
same sentence, across sentences, paragraphs, and even whole texts. It
allows learners to develop knowledge not only of the semantic meaning
of words and their lexico-grammatical patterns, but also to begin to build
up an intuitive knowledge of the frequency with which specific words
and expressions occur when they are used to talk about particular topics,
subjects, and fields. This raises a question of how to choose vocabulary
from context for learners. A range of criteria can be applied to help do
this.
13.6.2 Captions
To see or not to see? That is the question. The use of captions is a contro-
versial issue in language teaching and learning. There are arguments both
13 Materials Design 233
for not using them, and for using them. Let’s think about this in detail
and also review relevant research.
The sixth principle of a contextual approach is the Attention Principle.
This hypothesizes that attention to the form of an utterance promotes
deeper learning. Most experts agree that memory is divided into long-
term and working memory. Wen (2015: 41) explains that working
memory allows us to maintain a small amount of information in our
minds while we manipulate it to do such things as understand language,
do mathematical calculations, or solve problems. Working memory has
two components: a phonological loop which processes sounds, and
a visuospatial sketchpad which deals with images. Another feature of
working memory is that it has a limited capacity, which is generally
considered to be seven bits of information plus or minus two. However,
more recent research indicates that its capacity could be four plus or
minus one. In addition to having a limited capacity, information held
in working memory decays within seconds unless refreshed by rehearsal
(ibid:43).
A number of studies have shown that parallel auditory and visual
inputs support learning. A series of studies by Mayer (2014: 6) consis-
tently found that learning is enhanced when people use both their
auditory and visual channels simultaneously to process information. He
offers two explanations: One is quantitative—it doubles the amount
of information a learner processes. A second explanation, which Mayer
favours, is qualitative—a learner must go through the cognitive process
of integrating the auditory and visual information, which leads to deeper
learning. Clark and Mayer (2016: 71) add that ‘presenting words alone
may encourage learners - especially those with less experience or expertise
- to engage in shallow learning, such as not connecting the words with
other knowledge.’
Some investigations have looked at the parallel use of visuals, audio
narration, and subtitles. While it might be expected that increasing the
number of modalities from two to three would lead to better under-
standing, Clark and Mayer (2016: 131) summarize research that shows
that people learn better from graphics and audio narration, than from
graphics and audio narration plus onscreen text. This is because the
presentation of both images and the pictorial form of words overloads
234 I. Pemberton
the visual input channel as people try to focus on both of them at the
same time. This seems to suggest that when designing classes, it might
be better to limit the use of subtitles.
However, the participants in the studies were L1 users. There is
a significant amount of research that looks at the effectiveness of
using closed captions for second language learners. A meta-analysis by
Montero-Perez et al. (2013: 720) found that the use of subtitles had a
large effect on both the comprehension of L2 learners and their retention
of vocabulary. A meta-analysis by Kanellopoulou et al (2019) also found
that bimodal subtitles (L2 audio and L2 subtitles) improved listening
skills and long-term vocabulary retention. In a study of caption use,
Vanderplank (2019: 421) reports that participants made use of captions
to increase enjoyment, improve understanding, develop their listening
skills, and build vocabulary knowledge. While captions might have a
negative impact on the understanding of L1 viewers, studies show that
they have a positive effect on the understanding and vocabulary retention
of second language learners.
Individual Study
While it is more important to increase exposure to the target language
than spend time reviewing what has already been heard and understood,
there is a value in recording vocabulary. When studying online, an elec-
tronic medium, such as an excel spreadsheet, is convenient to record
vocabulary. Have one column for the target vocabulary, and a hidden
column for meaning, which allows for review.
An important reason for recording vocabulary is that while listening,
checking meaning, and writing it down, learners attend to form. A
listener holds the vocabulary in short-term memory while moving
between the video, dictionary or translation software, and spreadsheet.
While it is useful to review the vocabulary, it is not critical because
learners have a lot to learn, and as they watch more videos, they meet
the same words and phrases in different contexts and gain an intuitive
understanding of the frequency of each construction.
Format
It is useful to record vocabulary in the order that you hear it. While
watching an instructional video, documentary, drama, or animation, a
learner builds up a cognitive representation of the content (a content
schema) and, at the same time, builds a parallel linguistic schema. The
content schema helps to remember and recall the language.
An important point is to record new words or new patterns together
with their co-text to associate a word not only with its conceptual
context, but also its lexico-grammatical context. The objective is not only
to remember constructions but to relate them to other constructions that
form a part of the surrounding conceptual and ideational context.
13.7.1 Flashcards
13.7.2 Pronunciation
13.7.3 Recounts
Write a summary
Discuss the pros and cons of social media. You should explain:
• the benefits of social media
• why young people are at greater risk
• the dangers of social media
And finish by making a conclusion
The prompts for the spoken recount will follow a similar pattern:
Give a 2-minute talk about the pros and cons of social media
You should discuss:
• the benefits of social media
• why young people are at greater risk
• the dangers of social media
And finish by making a conclusion
13.7.4 Transcript
Once students have written a summary, they tend to want the teacher to
check whether their work is correct or not, but this misunderstands the
purpose of the activity. Provide a transcript at this point and tell students
to check what they have written against the transcript. This will allow
them to compare their own language with the actual language. In other
words, they will be able to notice the gap between what they can produce
and the target language, which will allow them to begin the process of
13 Materials Design 239
bridging the gap. In some ways this might seem similar to suggestions
made in Merrill Swain’s (1995: 26) Output Hypothesis:
This final activity type is consistent with the Produce phase of a conven-
tional language class. However, students are not expected to attempt far
transfer. Rather, it maintains and extends the existing context while rein-
forcing the language. This activity requires students to integrate new
knowledge from two or more lessons with their background knowledge.
For instance, if students have seen two videos about marriage rates such
as Rise in singletons in South Korea and Average age of marriage for women
in South Korea hits 30, they can be asked to do the following activity.
It can be seen that this particular essay does not require far transfer of
knowledge. Both videos refer to the Korean context. While maintaining
the original context of marriage rates, it requires learners to combine
knowledge and language of the Korean context with their own experience
and understanding.
13.7.6 Assessment
13.8 Limitations
Unlike some of the major methods of the twentieth century, a contex-
tual approach is not intended for all learners in all situations. Thus, it is
important to recognize some of the limitations of the approach.
13.8.1 Level
13.8.2 Resources
13.8.4 Motivation
References
Clark, R. C., & Mayer, R. E. (2016). E-learning and the science of instruction:
proven guidelines for consumers and designers of multimedia learning (4th ed.).
John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
Kanellopoulou, C., Kermanidis, K. L., & Giannakoulopoulos, A. (2019).
The Dual-coding and multimedia learning theories: Film subtitles as a
vocabulary teaching tool. Education Sciences, 9 (3), 210.
Lewis, M. (1997) Implementing the Lexical Approach. Language Teaching
Publications.
Mayer, R. E. (2014). Introduction to multimedia learning. In R. E. Mayer
(Ed.), The Cambridge Handbook of Multimedia Learning (2nd ed., pp. 1–
24). Cambridge University Press.
Montero Perez, M., Van Den Noortgate, W., & Desmet, P. (2013). Captioned
video for L2 listening and vocabulary learning: A meta-analysis. System
(linköping), 41(3), 720–739.
Nation, I. S. P. (2001). Learning vocabulary in another language. Cambridge
University Press.
O’Keeffe, A. (2021). Data-driven learning—a call for a broader research gaze.
Language Teaching, 54 (2), 259–272.
Swain, M. (1995). Three functions of output in second language learning. In
G. Cook & B. Seidlhofer (Eds.), Principle & practice in applied linguistics:
Studies in honour of H. G. Widdowson. Oxford University Press.
Vanderplank, R. (2019). ‘Gist watching can only take you so far’: Atti-
tudes, strategies and changes in behaviour in watching films with captions.
Language Learning Journal, 47 (4), 407–423.
244 I. Pemberton
CLT out in the cold light of day. A close examination led to the revela-
tion that CLT lacks a coherent theory of learning. Despite this serious
flaw, somehow it has continued to thrive unabated. But lacking the guid-
ance of a theory, it overemphasizes productive speaking activities in the
belief that these lead to fluency, misspending class time, and preventing
learners from achieving their full potential.
the second type will result in learning. This can take place irrespective of
whether a learner is working with others or working alone.
A further rationale for learner interactions in the communicative class-
room is drawn from Sociocultural Theory. On the face of it, the theory
suggests that children learn their first language with the assistance of a
more knowledgeable other who listens and corrects them. That is to say,
learning takes place as a result of social interaction. However, this is not
borne out by research findings. These show that input from adults and
older siblings plays only a peripheral role. A final point is that a more
knowledgeable other need not even be a sentient being. It can also be a
computer, a textbook, a dictionary, or even a pencil and paper. Thus, it
appears that Sociocultural Theory does not provide a strong rationale for
pair and group activities.
The classical theories of child cognitive development provide some
guidance for second language learning. However, their main emphasis
is on the learning of concepts, and they lack specifics on the cognitive
processes of language acquisition. An understanding of these processes
can be gained from a more up-to-date theory of language learning—
Social-Pragmatic Theory. This theory defines language acquisition in
terms of three processes. First of all, it argues that children use inten-
tion reading to understand what adults mean. In other words, they don’t
have to go through the time-consuming process of forming hypotheses
and negotiating the meaning of each utterance. Secondly, the theory
explains that children imitate what they hear in order to internalize it.
Thirdly, children encode knowledge in memory using general pattern
finding skills including categorizing, analogy, and schema building.
Social-Pragmatic Theory is a usage-based theory of first language
acquisition. Usage-based theories claim that all languages, first, second,
and additional are acquired in the same way. For its theoretical foun-
dations, usage-based linguistics draws upon Dynamic Systems Theory
(DST) and constructionist grammars.
Dynamic Systems Theory is a general complexity theory which posits
that a complex system is non-linear, self-organizing, and emergent. A
language is clearly a complex system consisting of tens or even hundreds
of thousands of lexico-grammatical patterns which range from general,
in the case of grammar, to specific, in the case of concrete examples
14 Conclusion 249
of its message, whether main ideas are easily identifiable, and whether it
contains clear examples of constructions for the learners to focus on.
The Context Principle also guides the design of activities. There are
two objectives for while-watching activities: focus learners on content
and focus them on language. To focus learners on content requires
specific types of activity. One of these activities is content questions.
These are distinct from comprehension questions in that they focus
learners on main ideas. Other types of activities which will focus
learners on the informational content of a video are note and summary
completion activities.
The Context, Iteration, and Attention Principles guide the design of
post-watching activities. These can include (digital) flashcards to increase
frequency and promote the stability of constructions in memory. To
encourage learners to make sense of the content, written recount, where
learners write down the main ideas, and spoken recount, where learners
articulate those ideas in speech, can be used. In addition to helping
learners build content and language schemata, these activities increase
the frequency with which learners use constructions, and they increase
the stability of those constructions in learners’ minds.
In keeping with ELT tradition, it may be useful to sketch out what
a contextual approach classroom looks like. Classroom procedures are
guided by the Autonomy, Iteration, and Attention Principles. To study
a multimedia context which provides visual and auditory input, and
to work autonomously, learners will need a device—either a laptop
or tablet—and an internet connection. A mobile phone is marginally
acceptable but difficult to manipulate. Alternatively, they could work in
a room equipped with personal computers. Headphones or earphones
are also a requirement.
As learners study primarily autonomously, they control the pace with
which they work through the materials, what they focus on, and the
number of times they review language that they have difficulty with. The
task-based conundrum of when to focus on form becomes a non-issue.
This is integrated with the activities. It is in part determined by what the
teacher selects for deletion from the materials, but also learners have the
agency to decide whether or not to focus on form at other points while
viewing a video. While learners work independently to watch and listen
14 Conclusion 253
to the video, the teacher monitors what they are doing, intervening only
when necessary to keep learners on track.
The one thing some teachers will struggle with, and particularly those
trained in communicative teaching, is the idea of having a primary focus
on input. Does this mean there is no output? A contextual approach
does not preclude output, rather, in line with the tendencies of cognitive
linguistic theories to identify input as the main driver of acquisition, it
backgrounds output to foreground input. There are a number of ways
that learners can output, and learners are not precluded from working
together. To begin with, the use of flashcards as an immediate self-test
requires them to recall the language. Similarly, writing down the main
ideas requires learners to make sense of the input and to use the language.
At this point, having perceived, attended to, and produced language,
learners can work together to recount the main ideas in pairs. Teachers
can also employ free practice activities such as topical discussions and
debates, particularly where learners are studying a foreign language, but
this might be less important when they are studying for exams or are able
to use what they learn in a target language community.
Assessment in a contextual approach is assessment for learning. That
is, testing represents spaced repetition of learnt vocabulary. Teachers
can test learners’ ability to correctly apply learnt vocabulary to gapped
constructions.
14.5 Conclusion
Some fifty years ago, the appearance of Communicative Language
Teaching caught the mood of language teaching professionals the world
over and rescued the field from the totalitarianism of Audiolingualism. In
what is now known as the Communicative Period, CLT has become the
new status quo, not a new totalitarian regime but more a protectorate
ruled over by prominent educators in positions of power and influ-
ence. But Communicative Language Teaching is an approach that lacks
a coherent language learning theory. Without the guidance of a theory,
it overrelies on spoken production, a naïve strategy which is inconsistent
254 I. Pemberton
A Kusyk, M. 185
Abstract constructions 161 Sockett, G. 184
Abstraction 117 Toffoli, D. 182, 183
Acquisition-Learning Hypothesis 36
Active Learning 56, 111
Adaptation 133 B
Affective Filter Hypothesis 38 Behavioural Principles 207–214
Analogy 153 attention principle 209
Assessment 240 iteration principle 207
Association 117, 150–152
paradigmatic association 152
pragmatic association 152 C
symbolic association 151 Case studies 160–164
syntagmatic association 151 Eskildsen, S.W. 162
Attention Principle 209 Li et al. 162
Audiolingual Method 21 Mellow, J.D. 161
Audio materials 203 Roehr-Brackin, K. 163
Authenticity 197 Categorization 117, 153
Autonomy Principle 205 Chunks 92, 93
Autonomy studies 182–186 Classical Period 8–9
© The Editor(s) (if applicable) and The Author(s), under exclusive 255
licence to Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2024
I. Pemberton, Usage-Based Second Language Instruction,
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-031-53414-0
256 Index
Emergence 135 H
Empirical Research 159–190 Historical Teaching Approaches
autonomy studies 182 7–15
case studies 160 classical period 8
corpus analyses 172 reform period 9
cross-sectional studies 164
multimedia studies 179
variation and variability studies I
175 Incidental learning 209
Entrenchment 148–150 Individuals 235
Environmental Principles 196–206 Inner speech 113
autonomy principle 205 Input Hypothesis 37
context principle 196 Input Principle 201
input principle 201 Intention reading 116, 184, 248
modality principle 203 Interaction Hypothesis 51
Evolution 106–108 Introductions 228
Iteration Principle 207
F
K
Fillmore, C. 136, 141 Knowledge Integration 239
First Language Acquisition 105–122 Krashen, S. 32, 35, 71, 92, 154,
constructivism 109 198, 201
evolution 106
primary biological skills 107
social-pragmatic theory 115 L
sociocultural theory 112 Langacker, R.W. 117, 136, 137,
Fixed (Institutionalized) expressions 141, 145, 148
88 Language Schemata 199
fully fixed expressions 88 Larsen-Freeman, D. 21, 22, 31,
semi-fixed expressions 89 127–135, 149, 150, 176, 204,
Flash cards 236 205, 209
Formulas 118, 154, 160 Level 241
Frequency 148, 200 Lewis, M. 85, 108, 234
Fully abstract constructions 118, 154 Lexical Approach 85–102
Fully fixed expressions 88 learning theory 91
lexical categories 86
methodology 92
G notebooks 97
Grammar-Translation Method 8 theory of language 90
258 Index
Lexical Categories N
collocations 87 Natural Approach 35–43, 92, 154
fixed (Institutionalized) acquisition-learning hypothesis 36
expressions 88 affective filter hypothesis 38
words 86 input hypothesis 37
Lexicon 128 monitor hypothesis 37
Limitations 241–243 natural order hypothesis 37
Logical division of ideas 228 teaching procedures 39
Long, M. 51 Natural Order Hypothesis 37
Low-scope patterns 118, 154, 161 Negotiation of meaning 52, 79, 114
Non-linearity 134
Note-completion 232
Notional-Functional Syllabus 32–35
M
communicative functions 35
Mastery System 11
modal meaning 34
Materials Design 221–243
semantico-grammar 34
constructions 229
Nunan’s Task Cycle 78
limitations 241
multimedia materials 224
post-watching activities 235
O
pre-watching activities 231
role of the teacher 224 Openness 131
while watching activities 232 Other Communicative Approaches
Mental flexibility 242 69–82
Modality Principle 203 content-based instruction 69
Modal meaning 34 task-based instruction 72
Monitor Hypothesis 37
More Knowledgeable Other 112,
114, 224 P
Motivation 242 Pairwork 114
Multimedia materials 204, 224–227 Paradigmatic association 152
Multimedia studies 179–182 Paralinguistic information 198
Athanasopolous, P. 179 Pattern finding skills 117, 185, 248
Bylund, E. 179 Piaget, J. 109
Irshad, F.M. 181 Post-watching activities 235–240
Koster, D.E.S. 181 Pragmatic association 152
Nguyen, T.P.H. 180 Pre-watching activities 231
Primary Biological Skills 107
Principles of a Contextual Approach
195–217
Index 259