Investigative Interviewing
Investigative Interviewing
Investigative Interviewing
Interviewing
Rights, research, regulation
Edited by
W IL L A N
PUBLISHING Tom Williamson
Investigative In te rv ie w in g
Investigative In te rvie w in g
Rights, research and regulation
E d ite d by
T o m W illia m s o n
WI L L A N
PUBLISHING
Published by
Willan Publishing
Culmcott House
Mill Street, Uffculme
Cullompton, Devon
EX15 3AT, UK
Tel: +44(0)1884 840337
Fax: +44(0)1884 840251
e-mail: [email protected]
website: www.willanpublishing.co.uk
Willan Publishing
c /o ISBS, 920 NE 58th Ave, Suite 300
Portland, Oregon 97213-3644, USA
Tel: +001(0)503 287 3093
Fax: +001(0)503 280 8832
e-mail: [email protected]
website: www.isbs.com
The rights of the editor and contributors to be identified as the authors of this book have been
asserted by them in accordance with the Copyright, Designs and Patents Act of 1988.
All rights reserved; no part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system,
or transmitted in any form or by any means, electronic, mechanical, photocopying, recording
or otherwise without the prior written permission of the Publishers or a licence permitting
copying in the UK issued by the Copyright Licensing Agency Ltd, 90 Tottenham Court Road,
London W 1P9HE.
Hardback
ISBN-13: 978-1-84392-124-0
ISBN -10:1-84392-124-3
A catalogue record for this book is available from the British Library
In dex 357
vi
F o re w o rd
If society did not already have grave concerns for the well-being of
society, recent events have given rise to still greater concerns relating
to the administration of justice. Let us consider but a few of these
events.
To mention terrorist activity leads to thoughts of the alleged
terrorists confined at Guatanamo Bay for extended periods of time
while investigations are undertaken. For all this time they are held
in captivity without knowing the charge or charges that they may
eventually face. This seems to violate the provisions of Magna Carta,
perhaps the earliest declaration of human rights in the western
world. How does such an extended detention affect the prisoner and
his response to interrogation? W hat safeguards, if any, should be
imposed on the manner of interrogating the prisoners?
W hat of false confessions. Case studies have determined that false
confessions leading to the conviction of innocent prisoners have all too
often been made. Was it the manner of interrogation that led to these
confessions and, if so, how should an interrogation be conducted of
those who it appears are likely to make such confessions?
As well, studies have demonstrated that tunnel vision of
investigators can affect the investigation of a crime and those involved
in the interrogation of suspects. Once again, what can be done to
correct tunnel vision during the interrogation of suspects?
A great deal of the investigation of crimes and terrorist activities
will be done by means of interrogation. This excellent book deals with
methods of interrogation and aspects such as tunnel vision which
may affect the manner in which an interrogation is conducted which
in turn may result in false confessions and wrongful convictions.
Investigative Interviewing
This book has its origins in the presentations given at the first
International Conference on Police Interviewing, held at the Ecole
nationale de Police du Quebec, Quebec, Canada from 9 to 11 February
2004. The conference was the brainchild of Michel St-Yves, a forensic
psychologist with the Surete du Quebec, and of his colleagues Jaques
Landry and Michel Pilon who are police officers with the Surete, and
they all share a particular interest in investigative interviewing. In
pursuit of this interest they have visited several countries and closely
examined various models of teaching people skills in interviewing.
They observed that there was much in common with the various
approaches and also areas of considerable disagreement. It was their
vision to hold a conference that would allow the various interested
parties to explain and defend their respective positions. To this end
the conference can be considered a success in that, although creative
tension was built into the design of the conference and disagreements
were discussed, the atmosphere was constructive and never became
disagreeable. Those of us who were privileged to take part in the
conference owe a debt of gratitude to the Surete du Quebec for the
vision to hold the conference and for their excellent arrangements. It is
hoped that this will be the first of a continuing series of international
conferences on investigative interviewing, with others planned for
Portsmouth, England, in 2006 and Hong Kong in 2008.
In tro d u c tio n
Tom Williamson
P a rt I: D e ve lo p m e n ts in rig h ts
xi
Investigative Interviewing
P a rt II: D e ve lo p m e n ts in research
xiii
Investigative Interviewing
P a rt III: D e ve lo p m e n ts in re g u la tio n
xiv
Introduction
XV
N o te s on c o n trib u to rs
xvii
Investigative Interviewing
His book Telling Lies: Clues to Deceit in the Marketplace, Politics and
Marriage, first published in 1991, has been widely acclaimed.
xviii
Notes on contributors
xix
Investigative Interviewing
XX
L is t o f fig u re s and ta b le s
F ig u re s
Ta ble s
xxi
Part I
Developments in Rights
C h a p te r I
Tom Williamson
In tro d u c tio n
3
Investigative Interviewing
4
Investigative interviewing and human rights in the war on terrorism
5
Investigative Interviewing
6
Investigative interviewing and human rights in the war on terrorism
7
Investigative Interviewing
The issue at stake is respect for the sovereignty, equality and legal
parity of nation-states, regardless of their wealth or power, in order
to establish the rule of 'right' over 'm ight' in regulating interstate
affairs (Chandler 2000: 55).
One result is that investigators in many countries find themselves
caught betw een the narratives of crim e and w ar (Hayward and
Investigative interviewing and human rights in the war on terrorism
Some lessons can be learnt from a natural experiment that has been
taking place in Britain which has a recent history of dealing with
terrorism. One of the first responses to terrorism is for the state to
bring in the military. Between 1966 and 1999 there were 3,636 deaths
in Northern Ireland from political violence, in a population of 1.5
million. By 1971 the scale of the violence grew beyond the capabilities
of the para-military police force, the Royal Ulster Constabulary, and
the army became the prime security force and legislation was passed
permitting internment without trial. These emergency powers also
led to the ill-treatment of detainees and lethal confrontations between
the army and civilian population which directly contributed to the
continuation of terrorist activity.
Legislation was often event driven in response to the latest terrorist
attack. In a review of the legislation in 1996 four principles were set
out against which terrorism legislation should be judged:
9
Investigative Interviewing
10
Investigative interviewing and human rights in the war on terrorism
12
Investigative interviewing and human rights in the war on terrorism
13
Investigative Interviewing
14
Investigative interviewing and human rights in the war on terrorism
15
Investigative Interviewing
officers and the intelligence com m unity face in dealing with global
terrorism. But accepting evidence obtained through torture appears a
bit like saying you did not steal anything you only received it from
som eone who, you had reason to believe, had stolen it. Is this really
an essential part of the solution to international terrorism?
Three consequences flow from the use of torture or inform ation
obtained from other people using torture. One is that it could affect
the treatm ent of US, UK and Coalition soldiers, police officers and
civilian hostages captured in future armed conflicts. Secondly, it would
remove the restraints of hum an rights law on other states that would
be able to invoke the precedent of US policy at Guantanam o Bay and
in British tribunals. Thirdly, it would be likely to m ake m artyrs in the
m oderate M uslim world, with whom the West m ust work to ensure
peace and stability, of the prisoners treated in this way.
16
Investigative interviewing and human rights in the war on terrorism
terrorism is to create a gap betw een those countries that have reacted
by restricting hum an rights and liberal dem ocracies which have not.
It m ay be sym ptom atic of a divergence of values betw een the USA
and Europe (Hutton 2003: 101-2).
The m ilitarization of crim inal justice is a strategy that fails on
moral, legal and utilitarian grounds, and bitter experience. The
response to the threat of terrorism and the legal fram ew orks chosen
ultim ately defines the kind of country we each live in and the means
adopted to ensure our security.
G o v e rn m e n ts ’ c h o ic e o f legal fra m e w o rk s fo r th e w a r on
t e r r o r is m : s e c u rity sta te o r d e m o cra c y?
17
Investigative Interviewing
C o n c lu s io n
The choice betw een a security state where personal freedoms are
inevitably eroded and a dem ocracy achieving a proportionate balance
between individual rights and threats to national security is not a false
dichotomy. It may be that w e are at a watershed in term s of a new
approach to managing the threat of third-m illennium terrorism and
other international crimes. The world of law enforcem ent is rapidly
changing. Instead of the public police there is now a m ultitude
of other public and private providers. M any people em ployed as
interrogators in Iraq are private contractors. According to N ewburn
(in Jones and N ew burn 2002):
18
Investigative interviewing and human rights in the war on terrorism
19
Investigative Interviewing
N o te
R e fe re n c e s
20
Investigative in terview ing and human rights in the w a r on te rro rism
21
Investigative Interviewing
22
C h a p te r 2
In tro d u c tio n
National security and public safety are primary concerns for all
professionals who investigate terrorism (White 2003; Borum 2004).
These investigations frequently involve questioning subjects, either
for purposes of intelligence gathering or for investigations that may
lead to criminal prosecution. The objective of these interview s/
interrogations is to gather accurate and reliable information that
furthers security, safety, intelligence and investigative interests. The
current threat environment, however, poses some particular challenges
for law enforcement professionals investigating terrorism (Hoffman
1999; Laqueur 1999; Simon 2003; Borum et al. 2004).
Many 'adversaries' in the global war on terrorism have beliefs,
ideologies, cultures and life experiences that differ markedly from
those of their interrogators - and often differ from those of criminals
with whom law enforcement professionals more typically interact.
Terrorist groups and networks affiliated with al-Qaeda, in particular,
pose ominous threats and present enormous challenges to the
investigative and intelligence personnel who pursue them. Al-Qaeda
and related operatives are committed to a cause: not just to their
own personal interests, but to the interests of the 'brothers' (group)
of Islam. They may be trained to withstand questioning and to utilize
counter-interrogation techniques (Gunaratna 2001).
Various interrogation strategies have been employed with success
against al-Qaeda-affiliated subjects since the first attack on the
World Trade Center in 1993. These approaches were refined over the
23
Investigative Interviewing
24
Al-Qaeda-related subjects: a law enforcement perspective
25
Investigative Interviewing
26
Al-Qaeda-related subjects: a law enforcement perspective
27
Investigative Interviewing
28
Al-Qaeda-related subjects: a law enforcement perspective
29
Investigative Interviewing
30
Al-Qaeda-related subjects: a law enforcement perspective
easier if the subject has lived in the West for a considerable period of
time; in other cases, more difficult, based on the subject's expectation
of what will occur and his knowledge of the laws.
31
Investigative Interviewing
32
Al-Qaeda-related subjects: a law enforcement perspective
Developing rapport
33
Investigative Interviewing
34
Al-Qaeda-related subjects: a law enforcement perspective
Gathering information
35
Investigative Interviewing
D eveloping themes
36
Al-Qaeda-related subjects: a law enforcement perspective
37
Investigative Interviewing
M anaging resistance
The interview team needs to prepare for resistance. The team should
have a plan for dealing with subjects who refuse to answer questions.
For example, a subject who is supported by his network in a detention
facility is likely to be prepared and to have several strategies that he
plans to employ as resistance in the initial phases of the interview.
The interviewer and support team need to be prepared to work
through these resistances.
38
Al-Qaeda-related subjects: a law enforcement perspective
C o n clu s io n
39
Investigative Interview ing
N o te
1. See Dore G old's Hatred's Kingdom for the accounts of the plundering
of and killing in Shia cities of N ajaf and Karbala c. 1799-1803 by the
descendents of the Muhammad Ibn 'Abd al-Wahhab and the Muhammad
Ibn Sa'ud alliance. This early period of 'W ahhabism ' was characterized
by death and destruction in the name of God, and not just for the worst
of the apostates. According to this belief system , those deserving of death
included Shia and Sunni 'brothers' in Mecca and Medina who would
challenge the Wahhabi brand of Islam.
R e fe r e n c e s
40
A l-Q a e d a -re la te d subjects: a law enforcem ent perspective
Navarro, J. (2002) 'Interacting with Arabs and M uslim s', FBI Law Enforcement
Bulletin, 71: 20.
Nydell, M. (2002) Understanding Arabs: A Guide fo r Westerners (3rd edn).
Yarmouth, MA: Intercultural Press.
Riddell, P. and Cotterell, P. (2003) Islam in Context: Past, Present, and Future.
Grand Rapids, MI: Baker Academic.
Schafer, J. and Navarro, J. (2004) A dvanced Interviewing Techniques: Proven
Strategies fo r Law Enforcement. Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas.
Walters, S. (2002) Principles o f Kinesic Interviezv and Interrogation (2nd edn).
Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press.
W hite, J. (2003) Defending the Homeland: Domestic Intelligence, Law Enforcement,
and Security. New York, NY: Wadsworth.
41
C h a p te r 3
David Rose
42
American interrogation methods in the war on terror
43
Investigative Interviewing
Two days after Yoo wrote his m em o, G uantanam o accepted its first
prisoner transport from Afghanistan, where the Taliban regim e had
recently been crushed. O ver the follow ing three weeks, an intense
debate raged within the adm inistration. Eventually, the Pentagon and
Justice D epartm ent were able to defeat the argum ents m ade by Colin
Powell and his colleagues at the State Departm ent, who said that to
abandon Geneva w ould place captured US troops at risk in future
conflicts. On 7 February 2002, President Bush formally announced
that Am erica did not consider itself bound by Geneva in respect
o f detainees at G uantanam o, nor in its treatm ent of Taliban and al-
Qaeda detainees elsewhere. H aving been 'captured on the battlefield'
of the war on terror, these prisoners were to be designated 'unlaw ful
com batants',2 and therefore did not deserve the convention's shield.
The USA would, Bush said, respect the 'spirit' of the convention,
but even this w as qualified - this indefinable quality would only
be observed 'so far as m ilitary necessity allow s'. There would be no
tribunals to determ ine w hether detainees really had been terrorists
or unlaw ful com batants, as G eneva's Article 5 appears to require in
the case of irregular fighters of all kinds. At the stroke of a pen, the
international law of war had effectively been cast aside.
The im plications of this decision, and of Yoo's doctrine of
unrestricted presidential power, were developed over the next few
m onths by a broad interagency group chaired by Jay S. Bybee,
then Assistant Attorney-General. Its classified report was issued on
1 A ugust 2002. W hen this w as leaked alm ost two years later, and
its frank readiness to countenance torture revealed, the W hite
House claim ed the docum ent was 'irrelevant', saying it would now
be 'rew ritten'. No rewritten version has, at the time of writing in
44
American interrogation methods in the war on te rror
Decem ber 2004, emerged. Large parts of its text and analysis were
to be reproduced verbatim in subsequent memos drawn up at the
Pentagon.
Bybee and his colleagues appear not to have questioned the
prem ise that torture and coercion w ill be more effective at finding
out what a prisoner know s than other forms of interrogation.
Having accepted this, the m em orandum sought to establish an
extrem ely narrow view of what torture meant. If the pain inflicted
were physical, it 'm ust rise to the level of death, organ failure, or
the perm anent im pairm ent of a significant bodily function'. If it were
m ental, 'it m ust result in significant psychological harm of significant
duration, e.g., lasting for m onths or even years'. According to the
m emo, A m erica's dom estic law against torture, a Congressional Act
of 1994 and the UN Convention against Torture to which the USA
w as a signatory 'prohibit only the m ost extrem e forms of physical
or mental harm '. Anything less would be merely 'cruel, inhum an or
degrading treatm ent'. W hilst this too was banned by the convention,
those responsible for such treatm ent would not be liable to criminal
penalties.
Bybee and his colleagues based this definition - which was
reproduced in m any subsequent docum ents - on the fact that both
the Am erican statute and the UN convention described torture
as the infliction of 'severe' pain, but did not specify what 'severe'
m eant. They went on to adopt a curious reading of the definition
in W ebster's dictionary, which lists the m eanings of 'severe' pain as
'hard to endure; sharp; afflictive; distressing; violent; extrem e'. They
were clearly being highly selective, for pain m ight well be sharp,
afflictive and hard to endure without rising to the level of organ
failure or death - indeed, there would be little point in adm inistering
any coercive technique if it were easy to endure. Bybee's team
therefore tried to buttress their definition from a strange source: US
laws governing the paym ent of health insurance benefits. These laws,
the m em o stated, 'treat severe pain as an indicator of ailm ents that
are likely to result in perm anent and serious physical damage in the
absence of im m ediate m edical treatm ent'. If insurance com panies had
to pay out in cases where patients were at risk of organ failure or
death, their logic ran, then to count as torture the m ethods used by
interrogators 'm ust rise to a sim ilarly high level'.
Echoing the argum ent m ade by Yoo, the memo w ent on to state
that it w ould be im proper to rule any interrogation method out,
because any attem pt to apply the law in a way which would interfere
with the President's right to determ ine the conduct of a war would
45
Investigative Interviewing
46
American interrogation methods in the war on terror
47
Investigative Interviewing
earlier Bybee document, and echoed both its definition of torture and
the claim that the President had a free hand in wartime. It suggested
that torture could be justified as a form of self-defence:
The m em o's authors were w'ell aware of the dark territory into w'hich
their arguments led: the w'ar criminal's claim that he w'as 'only
following orders', and thus could not be held accountable. They even
quoted the charter of the Nuremburg Nazi War Crimes Tribunal:
'The fact that the defendant acted pursuant to the order of his
government or of a superior shall not free him from responsibility.'
But an interrogator accused of torture could argue that his or her
orders 'm ay be inferred to be lawful', they suggested, and wrould
have been 'disobeyed at the peril of the subordinate'. In other words,
an interrogator ordered to inflict torture could justify his or her
actions by saying he or she was frightened of the consequences of
disobedience.
The group's last memorandum on interrogation methods at
Guantanamo emerged six w'eeks later. It took a pragmatic approach,
arguing that the 'choice of interrogation techniques involves a risk
benefit analysis in each case'. When assessing wfhether 'exceptional'
methods were appropriate, 'consideration should be given to the
possible adverse effects on US armed forces culture and self-image,
which at times past may have suffered due to perceived law of war
violations'. There was also a risk that extracting a confession by
harsher means 'may produce a statement that might be argued to be
involuntary for purposes of criminal proceedings... the more coercive
the method, the greater the likelihood that the method will be met
writh significant domestic and international resistance'.
On 16 April 2003, Rumsfeld issued his revised menu of 'counter
resistance techniques' for interrogators. In addition to psychological
methods, detainees could be placed in 'less comfortable' settings
and deprived of proper meals for long periods. All comfort items,
including the Koran, could be confiscated. The use of 'sleep adjustment'
48
American interrogation methods in the war on te rror
was perm itted - this m eant 'adjusting the sleeping times of the
detainee [e.g. reversing sleep cycles from night to day]'. Rumsfeld
claimed: 'This technique is N OT sleep deprivation.' Also included
was 'environm ental m anipulation: altering the environm ent to create
m oderate discom fort [e.g. adjusting the tem perature or introducing
an unpleasant sm ell].' This, Rum sfeld acknow ledged, would be
regarded by some nations as 'inhum ane'.
Finally, and perhaps m ost im portant, came isolation in solitary
confinem ent. Rum sfeld placed no lim it on the length of time prisoners
might have to endure this. 'Those nations that believe detainees
are subject to POW protections may view use of this technique as
inconsistent with the requirem ents of Geneva III,' he admitted. But
since 'the provisions of Geneva are not applicable to the interrogation
of unlaw ful com batants', this w as no obstacle.
Before exam ining what have been the operational consequences
of this official lead, it is worth considering the radical nature of
this policy's fracture with the recent past. In Europe, a sense that
torture is both m orally abhorrent and ineffective dates back to the
Enlightenm ent and beyond. Friedrich Spee, a Jesuit academ ic from
Trier, issued his polem ic against the use of torture in witch hunts, the
Cautio Criminalis, in 1631. The point at which different individuals will
break will vary, he wrote: the end of endurance to pressure and pain
is a subjective, not an objective phenom enon. Eventually, however,
all would: 'It is incredible what people say under the com pulsion of
torture, and how m any lies they w ill tell about them selves and about
others; in the end whatever the torturers want to be true, is true.' If
he were an inquisitor, Spee wrote, he could exact confessions from
priests and bishops. He had met an inquisitor who boasted that he
could w ring a confession to devil-w orship out of the Pope himself.
Cesare Beccaria also knew that torture is not only repugnant,
it doesn't 'w ork', in the sense of providing discernibly accurate
information. 'The only difference betw een torture and trials by fire
and boiling water is, that the event of the first depends on the will
of the accused, and of the second on a fact entirely physical and
external: but this difference is apparent only, not re a l/ he wrote in
his On Crimes and Punishments in 1764. 'A man on the rack, in the
convulsions of torture, has it as little in his power to declare the
truth, as, in form er tim es, to prevent w ithout fraud the effects of
fire or boiling water.' The greater the pressure, the less reliable the
testim ony: 'The very means em ployed to distinguish the innocent
from the guilty will m ost effectually destroy all difference between
them .' Frederick the G reat of Prussia had banned torture even before
49
Investigative Interviewing
50
American interrogation methods in the war on te rror
did possess such know ledge, the stronger such a defence to claims
of torture would be. But like others who have explored this moral
swam p, he did not pause to exam ine the case which corresponds
more closely with reality: the captive who might know som ething
about terrorism, even a deadly and im m inent attack - but equally,
m ight not.
As the adm inistration's lawyers developed their thinking in
2002-3, the effects of their approach were rapidly apparent on the
ground. The full truth may not emerge for years to come. But it is
evident that physical and psychological coercion in varying degrees
becam e the norm in num erous locations. As the Taliban collapsed
in Afghanistan in the autumn of 2002, interrogators began to use
coercive techniques at the US bases at Bagram and Kandahar. Human
Rights Watch has reported:5
51
Investigative Interviewing
52
American interrogation methods in the war on te rror
trickle. O f course, there were two possible reasons for this: either the
prisoners may have know n very little about terrorism and al-Qaeda,
or they were not being questioned with sufficient skill. But Baccus
told the task force's interrogators not to scream at detainees, and in
other w ays did his best to prevent abuse.
By the time of my visit in O ctober 2003, M ajor General M iller had
been in post for alm ost a year. For him, intelligence was a m atter of
volum e, of productivity, much like the w ork of the artillery corps
w here he had spent m ost of his career: 'Since the beginning of 2003,
the am ount of intelligence extracted from detainees each m onth has
increased by 600 per cent,' he told me. N or did this consist of mere
tidbits:
53
Investigative Interviewing
54
American interrogation methods in the war on te rror
A fterwards, he would be taken back to his cell for about three hours.
Then, he said, the guards would reappear, and the process begin
again.
Som etim es, Dergoul said, the interrogators also used heat: 'The air
conditioning control would be turned so it was blow ing out air even
hotter than w hat was outside. And som etim es, if you budged from
your position, they'd take the chair away, so you'd keel over, tipped
in agony on to the floor.' In periods of especially heavy interrogation,
he would be given no clean clothes or bedding, or garm ents which
were too small. Another technique was to refuse toilet paper, 'so you
can't clean yourself after using the toilet. Or they'd give you like
four sheets - not enough to blow your nose'.
A sif Iqbal w as also left chained in a booth for m any hours, and
like Dergoul, was eventually com pelled to soil himself. Prisoners were
forced to urinate in the booths so often, he said, that it becam e norm al
for the interrogators to have their plastic chairs hosed down after
each session. Som etim es, music was played at a deafening volume:
he rem em bered having to listen to Em inem , Bruce Springsteen and
'techno' dance m usic, accom panied by flashing strobe lights. Once,
he said, an interrogator showed him explicit pornography, saying:
'Look at that, it's the last time you'll ever see pussy again.' Raised
in Britain, he was relatively immune to sexual taunts of this kind: 'I
55
Investigative Interviewing
just laughed.' But Arab detainees told him and Dergoul of being left
chained in the booths with their underpants around their ankles, a
cause of profound hum iliation.
Dergoul also described the use of what w'as known as the 'short
shackle,' in which the bonds of the three-piece suit were pulled tight
to keep the subject bunched up, whilst chained to the floor. 'A fter a
w hile, it was agony. You could hear the guards behind the mirror,
m aking jokes, eating and drinking, knocking on the walls. It w asn't
about trying to get inform ation. It was ju st about trying to break you.'
Sleep deprivation was also deployed regularly. Rhuhel Ahmed told
how the detainees came to speak of 'frequent flyers' - prisoners who
were forced to don their chains and m ove cells, day and night, every
two hours. The freed detainees said G itm o's interrogators also used
psychological methods. Am ongst the m ost com m on was a claim by
an interrogator that he had proof of the suspect's 'gu ilt', a technique
w hich Shafiq Rasul encountered time and again. For exam ple, he was
told that photographs of him on an 'al-Q aeda m em bership form ' had
been found in a raid on an Afghan cave:
General M iller's 'ca rro t/ the incentives available in return for co
operation, som etim es provoked false allegations by one prisoner
against another, w'hich would then becom e the subject of further
intensive interrogation. Shafiq Rasul told me:
They kept taking us and taking us, show ing us photos saying:
'This guy says you've done this, this guy says you've done that'
- w'hat they m eant was that other detainees desperate to get
out of there were m aking allegations, m aking stuff up that they
thought would help them get out of the camp.
His own interrogators told him: '" I f you w ant extra com fort items,
get us some info on the people on the block." I refused.' After this
session, Rasul said, he was placed on the lowest-level regime as a
punishm ent: 'You only got a thin m at instead of a m attress, and a
blanket only betw een 11 pm and 5 am. During the days in the cell
you had ju st your clothes and the Koran.'
56
American interrogation methods in the war on te rror
My heart is beating, beating, I'm saying it's not me, it's not
me, but I'm thinking, 'I'm going to be screwed, I'm on an
island in the middle of now here, there's nothing I can do'. This
w om an had com e down and she plays me the video. I say, 'Are
you blind? That doesn't look anything like m e'. But it makes
no difference. I'd got to the point where I just couldn't take
anymore. 'D o what you have to do,' I told them. I'd been sitting
there for three m onths in isolation so I says 'yes, it's me. Go
ahead and put me on trial'.
57
Investigative Interviewing
At around the sam e time, Ahm ed and Iqbal made sim ilar confessions.
But the three men from Tipton were lucky. Som e time in Septem ber
2003, British officials from MI5 cam e to G uantanam o, arm ed w ith the
docum entary evidence which showed they could not have been in
A fghanistan in 2000 after all. W ithin a few days, they were being
held in the ordinary cages again, and being given special privileges,
including w eekly movies in a building know n as the 'love shack' and
ham burgers from the Gitm o M cD onald's; a few weeks after that, the
Am erican governm ent began to talk to their British counterparts about
the m en's release. 'In the end, we could prove our alibis,' Rasul said.
'But w hat about other people, especially from countries where travel
records may not be available? W hat if they confess to som ething they
didn't do and then can't prove it w asn't true?'
The fate of M oazzem Begg, one of the British detainees who was
not released in March 2004, may well be an exam ple. Intelligence
officials have briefed the Am erican media that after being interrogated
at Bagram in Afghanistan for a year, he confessed to planning to
drop anthrax spores on the House of Com m ons from a 'drone',
an unm anned aerial vehicle. Accurate UAVs are part of the latest
generation of Am erican weaponry, and cost millions of dollars each.
Yet in previous reporting of his case, the absurdity of Begg's alleged
confession - he supposedly claim ed to be plotting to launch his
deadly flight from Suffolk - has not been addressed. It does not seem
im plausible to suggest that his 'UAV plot' may have been a fantasy,
induced by sheer desperation.
However, according to my interview s with senior US intelligence
officials, the m anifestly unreliable nature of 'confessions' such
as those m ade by Rasul and Begg is not the only shortcom ing of
the 'enorm ously valuable intelligence' supposedly gathered at
Guantanam o. Quite separately, four such officials have told me that
such claim s are m isleading and exaggerated, and that the value of
inform ation obtained by coercion at Gitm o is relatively low.
The first problem , they have said, is that m any of the Guantanam o
prisoners should not have been there at all, and knew nothing
im portant about terrorism in the first place. According to Lieutenant-
Colonel Christino, who spent much of 2003 as the senior watch
officer in the Pentagon's Joint Intelligence Task Force on Terrorism,
and previously provided 'intelligence support' for the operation
w hich moved prisoners to Guantanam o, the detainees w’ere victims
o f a woefully inadequate m ilitary intelligence screening process in
Afghnanistan. This was 'flaw ed from the get-go', and conducted
m ainly by very young and inexperienced reservist soldiers who had
58
American interrogation methods in the war on te rror
undergone only a single 16-week course before being thrust into the
intelligence frontline in the war on terror. In large measure, this was
the result of the historic over-concentration on electronic intelligence
discussed above. But the consequences were serious:
Worse, they were dealing w ith prisoners from the far side of a deep
cultural gulf, and were alm ost entirely reliant on interpreters, most
of them contracted by private corporations. Their quality was often
abysm al. A 2003 Pentagon report by Colonel Lawrence H. Saul,
director of a m ilitary evaluation unit called the Center for Army
Lessons Learned, supports that view. In both Iraq and Afghanistan,
it states:
59
Investigative Interviewing
who perceived they m ight get some benefit from doing so. In a
generic sense, they were selling their captives to the US Army,
and they came with w hat amounted to a sales pitch - a story to
convince our troops that these people were valuable. Their story
could be true; it could be fiction, or a com bination of the two.
The problem was that with inadequate training, little experience
and poor translation, our Ml soldiers were largely incapable of
discerning the difference.
60
American interrogation methods in the war on te rror
had access to that type of inform ation; if some claim that they
did, they probably did so to either earn the incentives or avoid
the m altreatm ent that General M iller instituted.
61
Investigative Interviewing
N o te s
R e fe re n ce s
Lewis, N.A. (2004) 'Broad use of harsh tactics is described at Cuba base', The
New York Times, 17 October.
National Com m ission on Terrorist Attacks upon the United States (2004) The
9/11 Commission Report. New York, NY: W.W. Norton.
Rose, D. (2004) Guantanamo: A m erica’s War on Human Rights. London: Faber
& Faber.
Spee, F. von Lagenfield (trans. Hellyer, M.) (2003) Cautio Criminalis: A Book on
Witch Trials. Charlottesville, VA: University of Virginia Press.
63
C h a p te r 4
John J. Pearse
In tro d u c tio n
64
The interrogation of terrorist suspects: the banality of torture
65
Investigative Interviewing
66
T h e interrogation o f te rro ris t suspects: the banality o f to rture
A d o p tin g th e p e rs p e ctive o f th e ‘o th e r ’
67
Investigative Interviewing
68
The interrogation of terrorist suspects: the banality of torture
the terrorist groups' need for supplies, finance, technical equipm ent,
and training and organizational details. It is in effect a sterile (emotion
free) tem plate that can be used to im prove understanding and, for
interrogators, to direct questioning in a m anner that reduces the
likelihood that the interaction w ill degenerate into a highly charged,
poorly focused and possibly violent exchange.
C u lt u r a l c o n s id e ra tio n s
69
Investigative Interviewing
In t e r r o g a to r bias
70
The interrogation of terrorist suspects: the banality of torture
71
Investigative Interviewing
The power of the situation and the ability to create their own
social forces were clearly evident in relation to the behaviour of
the subjects in the Stamford experiment, and the psychological
literature also provides some insight into the debilitating impact of
oppressive prison regimes such as those believed to be practised
at Gitmo and Abu Ghraib. Gudjonsson (1992, 2003), for example,
discusses the ways in which the physical environment can affect the
psychological state and well-being of detainees. It is recognized that
all forms of sensory deprivation, fatigue, social isolation, hunger,
sleep deprivation and physical and emotional pain or the threat
of such pain can exert a very powerful influence on the decision
making of detainees. According to Forrest (1999), confessions can be
extracted very effectively, without any special equipment, by using
sleep deprivation, prolonged wall standing, solitary confinement in
cold or cramped conditions, and such practices tend to leave no
physical after-effects (see also Hinkle 1961; Shallice 1974). It is also
known that there is considerable evidence that lack of sleep impairs
72
The interrogation of terrorist suspects: the banality of torture
C o m b in in g th e ro le s o f g u a rd and in te r ro g a t o r
73
Investigative Interviewing
74
The interrogation of terrorist suspects: the banality of torture
Delivery
75
Investigative Interviewing
M axim ization
76
The interrogation of terrorist suspects: the banality of torture
Deprivation
M anipulation
77
Investigative Interviewing
78
The interrogation of terrorist suspects: the banality of torture
Degradation
79
Investigative Interviewing
T h e b a n a lity o f t o rt u re
80
The interrogation of terrorist suspects: the banality of torture
C o n c lu s io n
R e fe re n ce s
81
Investigative Interviewing
82
T h e interrogation of te rro ris t suspects: the banality o f to rtu re
83
Part 2
Michel St-Yves
In tro d u c tio n
87
Investigative Interviewing
From the first contact to the last, we exam ine the im portance of
com m unication skills and em phasize five basic rules derived from
our research which we consider to be essential to conducting an
investigative interview successfully:
Com m unication skills are rarely taught in police academ ies and
in some it is still a taboo topic, which is unfortunate given that it
has such a central place in im proving investigative interview ing
practices.
To enter a relationship one has to m ake contact and exchange
inform ation in a way designed to create m utual rapport. It is
im portant to be able to keep an open mind, to pay attention and to
have an em pathetic attitude that prom otes good com m unication. For
rapport to develop, and especially when building up a relationship
with a witness, victim or suspect, the ideal is for the relationship
to be genuine. We believe that a genuine relationship is more likely
to lead to creating trust and confidence in the interview er and to
truthful accounts being provided. Rapport is defined as 'developing
an understanding relationship or com m unication betw een people'
(Oxford Reference Dictionary). It is the invisible w ave along which
inform ation can flow from the one to the other. If there is a problem
with rapport the inform ation received may be distorted or not
received at all.
K e e p in g an o p e n m in d and re m a in in g o b je c tiv e
We now consider the five basic rules that we have identified that
contribute to good rapport in the context of the psychological
research from which they have been developed. The first contact
with the person being interview ed is often decisive. It is on this first
contact that the two parties will form their opinion of one another.
This perception will then guide their behaviour. First impressions
will be created on the initial inform ation received by the investigator
regarding the person to be interview ed (e.g. a w ritten statem ent, a
testim ony from a neighbour, a forensic report, a judicial file or even
The psychology of rapport: five basic rules
Impression formation
89
Investigative Interviewing
deal more than the others. Rosenthal and Jacobson (1968) explain this
phenomenon in the following way: the prediction of an event (or the
belief in its arrival) by individual A regarding individual B wall be
fulfilled whether it is only in the mind of A or - by a subtle and
unforeseen process - by a modification of the real behaviour of B
under pressure from the expectations of A.
The perceived biases induced by these preconceived notions
therefore condition our behaviour and can have such influence on
others that the answers observed are none other than the results
of this perceived bias. In the investigative interviewing field, our
preconceived notions can also influence our perception and therefore
influence the result. In their study of interrogation of suspects,
Moston and Stephenson (1993) have observed that investigators
were less inclined to give suspects the benefit of the doubt if they
had criminal records. Investigators were more inclined to put the
emphasis on getting a confession rather than getting to the truth,
and may have been acting in a prejudiced and stereotypical way as
predicted by Rosenthal. Mortimer (1994) states that investigators who
take for granted the fact that their suspect is guilty, even before their
meeting, have a tendency to focus more on obtaining a confession
and thus use an accusatory interviewing style.
From the outset, the attitude of the investigator towards the suspect
will have a major impact on the unfolding of the interrogation. The
interviewer can enter into a relationship with the suspect by presuming
his or her guilt, his or her innocence or by keeping a neutral position.
All these attitudes have advantages and disadvantages.
According to Inbau et al. (2001: 68-70), the presumption of guilt
has the advantage of provoking a reaction of resentment from the
innocent person, whereas a guilty person has a tendency not to
demonstrate any resentment and to show certain non-verbal reactions.
Nevertheless, when there is little evidence, a guilty suspect who does
not confess swiftly could form the impression that the interviewer
is bluffing and therefore becomes psychologically stronger whilst
continuing to resist and deceive. If the interviewer is convinced the
suspect is guilty, he or she may be biased and may steer his or her
questions and interpret the suspect's answers in a biased way, as in
the Rosenthal effect. A problem with this approach is that an innocent
suspect can also become destabilized, disturbed and confused. There
is a risk that the interpretation of these suspicious behaviours may
90
The psychology of rapport: five basic rules
B u ild in g up ra p p o rt
91
Investigative Interviewing
92
The psychology of rapport: five basic rules
93
Investigative Interviewing
94
The psychology of rapport: five basic rules
are pejorative and imply defeat. Open questions reduce the risk of
perceptual biases.
• The 7': using T shows you are concerned about the transm itter's
remarks. It humanizes rapport.
• Silence: if you cannot find the words to stimulate or reassure the
transmitter, it is preferable to say nothing. Silences have their place
and have often proved to be beneficial. Silences also allow time to
think.
95
Investigative Interviewing
96
The psychology of rapport: five basic rules
97
Investigative Interviewing
98
The psychology of rapport: five basic rules
prisoners. After only a few days, the w ardens began to behave like
sadists and the prisoners becam e stressed and depressed. Zim bardo
prem aturely ended this experim ent after six days. Zim bardo came to
two conclusions. First, when a normal person is subm itted to extreme
conditions of hum iliation and violence, he or she can crumble
psychologically, can lose all self-esteem and can becom e depressed.
On the other hand, when an individual is given power, he or she may
reveal him or herself as a monster. According to Zim bardo, violence
is not som ething innate but, rather, is closely related to power. The
recent allegations of hum iliating and sadist behaviour from Am erican
soldiers towards Iraqi prisoners detained at Abu Ghraib prison is
perhaps a good exam ple of the type of behaviour the Milgram and
Zim bardo experim ents w^ould have predicted if guards and prisoners
are not properly managed.
Because of their duties, police officers have the pow er to arrest or
to detain people. During an interview with a suspect, police officers,
in spite of them selves, will exercise the pow er of their authority.
They are in a situation of authority and, therefore, m ust be conscious
of the im pact this power has on others and m ust not abuse it. This
should prevent serious injustices occurring. The expression of anger
or hostility during an investigative interview can lead to serious
consequences. For exam ple, an angry or hostile investigator may
react im pulsively or even aggressively when faced with a stubborn
suspect. This may be m anifested in im patience or unkind com ments.
The interview may then becom e oppressive w hich, as we have seen,
will destroy rapport and may jeopardize the adm ission of guilt in
court.
Cialdini (1993) suggests that authority can also be defined by
competency. Research has show n that people take it for granted that
experts know what they're talking about. People have a tendency to
accept inform ation on the grounds of the person who proffered it
rather than on the grounds of its content (M addux and Rogers 1983).
This is the aura (or halo) effect. For an investigator to be considered
a specialist or an authority figure in a suspect's eyes, the investigator
must be perceived as com petent.
Com petency is associated with self-esteem and, by extension, with
self-confidence. Interview ers, however, often lack self-confidence
(Baldwin 1992). To perform well during an interrogation, investigators
m ust appear self-confident. A lack of confidence can be expressed
not only by nervousness but also by hesitations and other verbal
hints (tone of voice, the excessive use of euphem ism s, etc.). If the
investigator is undecided, the evidence may also look uncertain
99
Investigative Interviewing
100
The psychology of rapport: five basic rules
asking open questions that allow the other person to say w hat he
or she wants and to express his or her em otions. To understand the
individual and to decode all the m essages he or she is sending out,
the negotiator has to be attentive to a m ultitude of details: tone of
voice, em otions and changes in attitude (Wargo 1990; Divasto 1996).
This is another exam ple of having to be on the same w avelength, and
it is exactly the same thing with investigative interviews. However,
the interview er is in a better position than the negotiator because the
interview er can assess the suspect's non-verbal behaviour.
The negotiator will build rapport w hich, consequently, will serve
as a bridge to allow the person with whom the negotiation is being
conducted to resolve the crisis. Then, according to the pace at which
the crisis unfolds (and according to the model developed at the Surete
du Q uebec and known as SIN CRO 3), the negotiator will accom pany
the person throughout the various phases of the crisis until he or she
surrenders, preferably voluntarily. The negotiator will do everything
in his or her power to perm it the person to preserve or to restore
his or her dignity, often flattering him or her and then m eeting the
person again once he or she has given him or herself up to the
police. Som e negotiators will even accom pany the person to hospital
to dem onstrate the authenticity of the rapport established betw een
them. During an investigative interview, the interview er who acts in
this m anner leaves not only a lasting, favourable im pression of the
rapport in the interview ee's m em ory but also actively contributes to
the reintegrative possibilities for that person through the process of
restorative justice (Braithw aite 1998).
A re a s fo r f u r t h e r re se a rc h
This chapter has exam ined the im portance of com m unication skills
for effective interview ing, and it is the author's opinion that this
is an area that has been under-researched. It has been argued that
com m unication skills and rapport can be taught. This assertion,
however, raises some interesting questions. For exam ple, can everyone
be taught com m unication skills? It seem s som e people have better
com m unication skills than others. Is this difference the result of
som ething that has been learnt or are there innate differences in skills
betw een individuals? Perhaps w hat is needed is a rigorous study that
draws on the m ethodologies of the subdiscipline of the psychology of
personality to ascertain w hether such individual differences do indeed
exist. A battery of selected psychom etric tests, or subsets of these
101
Investigative Interviewing
C o n c lu s io n
This chapter has described different interview ing styles and com pared
their effectiveness. Psychological research suggests that a style which
contributes to creating rapport is to be preferred, and from this five
basic com m unication rules considered essential for conducting an
effective investigative interview can be proposed:
1. Keep an open mind and remain objective. There are m any traps in
the field of interview ing. For exam ple, a lack of objectivity can
skew the investigation and can lead investigators to a fabricated
outcom e. This is called tunnel vision. On the other hand, objectivity
aims to find out the truth and does not focus solely on convicting
a suspect.
2. Build rapport. First and forem ost, welcom e the person w arm ly and
then create an atm osphere that will encourage him or her to talk
freely. This will find the balance betw een what the interview er
desires to know and w hat the suspect will agree to disclose. It is
necessary to observe and to understand if good rapport is to grow.
102
The psychology of rapport: five basic rules
N o te s
R e fe re n ce s
103
Investigative Interviewing
Bull, P. (2002) Communication under the Microscope. The Theory and Practice o f
Microanalysis. Hove: Routledge.
Bull, R. and Cherryman, J. (1995) Helping to Identify Skills Caps in Specialist
Investigative Interviewing: Enhancement o f Professional Skills. (Report to the
Home Office Police Research Group.)
Cassell, P.G. and Hayman, B.S. (1998) 'Police interrogation in the 1990s : an
empirical study on the effect of Miranda', in R.A. Leo and G.C. Thomas
III (eds) The Miranda Debate, Justice and Policing. Boston, MA: Northeastern
University Press.
Cherryman, J. and Bull, R. (2001) 'Police officers' perception of specialist
investigative interviewing skills', International Journal o f Police Science and
Management, 3: 199-212.
Cialdini, R.B. (1993) Influence: Science and Practice (3rd edn). Glenview, IL:
Scott, Foresman.
Clare, I.C.H. and Gudjonsson, G.H. (1993) 'Interrogative suggestibility,
confabulations and acquiescence in people with mild learning disabilities
(mental handicap): implications for reliability during police interrogations',
British Journal o f Clinical Psychology, 32: 295-301.
Clare, I.C.H. and Gudjonsson, G.H. (1995) 'The vulnerability of suspects
with intellectual disabilities during police interviews: a review and
experimental study of decision-making', Mental Handicap Research, 8:
110-28.
Collins, N.L. and Miller, L.C. (1994) Self-disclosure and liking: A meta-analytic
revieiu. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 457-475.
Crepault, D. and Boisvenue, J. (2003) 'Attentes et niveau de satisfaction des
victimes de crimes majeurs face S l'intervention des enqueteurs. Direction
conseil et developpement en enquetes criminelies', unpublished study,
Surete du Quebec.
Divasto, PV. (1996) Negotiating with Foreign Language-Speaking Subjects. FBI
Law Enforcement Bulletin, June 1992, 11-15.
Ekman, P. (1992) Telling Lies. Clues to deceit in the Market Place, Politics, and
Marriage. New York, NY: Norton.
Fisher, R.P. and Geiselman, R.E. (1992) Memory-enhancing Techniques for
Investigative Interviewing. Springfield, IL: Charles Thomas.
Freud, S. (1910) 'La technique psychanalytique', in Perspective d'avenir de la
psychologie analytique (1953). Paris: Presse Universitaire de France.
Freud, S. (1912) 'La dynamique du transfert', in Perspective d'avenir de la
psychologie analytique (1953). Paris: Presse Universitaire de France.
Getty, V.S. and Elam, J.D. (1988) "Identifying characteristics of hostage
negotiators, and using personality data to develop a selection model".
In J. Reese and J. Horn (eds.), Police Psychology: Operational Assistance,
pp. 159-171. Washington, DC: US Government Press.
Gudjonnsson, G.H. (1992) The Psychology o f Interrogations, Confessions and
Testimony. Chichester: Wiley.
104
T h e psychology o f rapport: five basic rules
Gudjonnsson, G.H. (1995) '"I'll help you boys as I can" - how eagerness to
please can result in a false confession', Journal o f Forensic Psychiatry, 6:
333-42.
Gudjonnsson, G.H. (2003) The Psychology o f Interrogations and Confessions. A
Handbook. Chichester: Wiley.
Gudjonnsson, G.H. and MacKeith, J.A.C. (1994) 'Learning disability and the
Police and Criminal Evidence Act 1984. Protection during investigative
interviewing: a video-recorded false confession to double murder', Journal
o f Forensic Psychiatry, 5: 35-49.
Holmberg, U. (2004) 'Police Interviews with victims and suspects of violent
and sexual crimes; interviewees' experiences and interview outcomes.'
Unpublished PhD thesis, Stockholm University.
Inbau, F.E., Reid, J.E., Buckley, J.P. and Jayne, B.C. (2001) Criminal Interrogation
and Confessions (4th edn). Gaithersburg, MA: Aspen.
Kassin, S.M. (1997) 'The psychology of confession evidence', American
Psychologist, 52: 221-33.
Lieberman, P. (2000) Human Language and our reptilian brain: The subcortical
bases o f speech, syntax, and thought. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University
Press.
Maddux, J.E. and Rogers, R.W. (1983) 'Protection motivation and self-
efficacy: a revised theory of fear appeals and attitude change', Journal o f
Experimental Social Psychology, 19: 469-79.
Milgram, S. (1974) Obedience to Authority. New York, NY: Harper & Row.
Mortimer, A. (1994) 'Asking the right questions', Policing, 10: 111-24.
Moston, S. and Engelberg, T. (1993) 'Police questioning techniques in tape-
recorded interviews with criminal suspects', Policing and Society, 6: 61-75.
Moston, S. and Stephenson, G. (1993) 'The changing face of police
interrogation', Journal o f Community and Social Psychology, 3: 101-15.
Moston, S., Stephenson, G.M. and Williamson, T.M. (1992) 'The effects of case
characteristics on suspect behaviour during police questioning', British
Journal o f Criminology, 32: 23-40.
Rogers, C.R. (1942) Counselling and Psychotherapy: Newer Concepts in Practice.
Boston, MA: Houghton-Mifflin.
Rosenthal, R. and Jacobson, L. (1968) Pygmalion in the Classroom. New York,
NY: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
Schafer, J.R. and Navarro, J. (2003) Advanced Interviewing Techniques.
Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas.
Sear, L. and Stephenson, G.M. (1997) 'Interviewing skills and individual
characteristics of police interrogators', in G.M. Stephenson and N.K.
Clark (ed.) Procedures in Criminal Justice: Contemporary Psychological Issues.
Leicester: British Psychological Society.
Shaw, G. (1998) 'Developing interview skills', in P. Southgate (ed.) New
Directions in Police Training. London: HMSO.
105
Investigative Interviewing
106
C h a p te r 6
Michel St-Yves
In tro d u c tio n
107
Investigative Interviewing
108
Confessions by sex offenders
Pearse et al. 1998; Phillips and Brown 1998; Clarke and M ilne 2001;
St-Yves and Lavallee 2002).
Nevertheless, in line w ith the Gudjonnsson and Sigurdsson study
(2000), we found that child m olesters, particularly hom osexual
paedophiles, collaborated more frequently with the police than
rapists. Child m olesters, who may have an underlying sense of guilt
and an introverted personality profile, had a confession rate of 71.8
per cent which com es close to percentages obtained by Gudjonnsson
and Sigurdsson (2000).
The low rates of confession obtained in the St-Yves (2002) study
may be explained in several ways. First and forem ost, the social
significance of the crime may be sufficient to inhibit a confession:
the subjects in the study had com m itted a crim e serious enough
to be sentenced to a term of im prisonm ent. The seriousness of the
offence may also be a significant factor. In general, sexual crim es are
judged more severely by the public than other types of crim es, even
when the latter are, objectively, more serious. Finally, proof of such a
crime is often difficult to establish, especially crimes involving young
children. These factors could explain w hy the num ber of confessions
increases dram atically once an offender is sentenced. Indeed, the
confession rate for sentenced offenders varies betw een 70 per cent
(for rapists) and 98.7 per cent (for child m olesters) (St-Yves 2002).
A factor in a study carried out by H olmberg and Christianson (2002)
could explain the reason why confession rates by sex offenders are
som etim es lower than all other types of crim inal, including those who
have com m itted more serious crimes. In an exploratory study aimed
at com paring the perception of 43 murderers and 40 sex offenders
regarding their interrogation by the police and their tendency to
confess or deny their crim es, Holm berg and Christianson (2002)
observed that the sex offenders were less likely than the murderers
to confess their crimes to the police. The percentages obtained were
28 per cent for the sex offenders and 49 per cent for the murderers.
To explain these results, the authors suggest that murderers often
regard their treatm ent by the police as being hum ane, whereas sex
offenders find this a hum iliating experience that inhibits them from
confessing.
W hilst it m ay be im possible to explain w hy these results are so
different, som e factors may account for the disparities in the results.
For exam ple, the M itchell study was undertaken more than 20 years
ago when the judicial and cultural framew'ork was different from the
four recent studies and the m ethodology em ployed also differed.
109
Investigative Interviewing
Certain characteristics of the suspect, the type of crime and the context
of the interview should allow us to predict w hether the suspect will
confess or not (Moston et al. 1992). To evaluate the im pact of the
chracteristics, this section exam ines such factors as m odus operandi,
the sex of the victim , the link betw een the offender and victim , the
offender's m arital status, his or her sense of guilt and IQ, and the
investigator's attitude towards the offender.
Sex offenders who used physical violence are less likely to confess
than those who did not use violence (St-Yves 2002). This finding
agrees with the work of N eubauer (1974) and M itchell (1983),
who established that suspects interrogated for violent crimes were
less likely to confess than those interrogated for crim es against
property. The significance of the crim e, therefore, seem s to influence
considerably the decision w hether or not to confess. It seems
reasonable to conclude that, the more violent the crim e, the more
suspects are afraid of the harshness of the sentence and, therefore,
the more they are likely to deny their guilt. Studies by M oston and
his colleagues, for exam ple, have show n that, the more serious the
crim e, the more suspects tend to use their right to remain silent and
to dem and the help of an attorney (M oston et al. 1992; Stephenson
and M oston 1994). Eysenck and Gudjonnsson (1989) also suggest
that, the more serious the crim e, the more harsh will be the sentence
and, therefore, the probability of obtaining a confession is slim (an
exception to this finding is the study by Holmberg and Christianson
2002). H olm berg and Christianson found that the confession rate of
murderers was twice that of sex offenders. This shows, therefore, that
the severity of the crim e does not solely account for differences in
confession rates.
In a recent study, Beauregard et al. (2005) observed that angry
sexual murderers confess more frequently to the police than sadistic
sexual m urderers (61.6 per cent as against 33.3 per cent, respectively).
Once sentenced, at the time of their induction to prison, all anger
murderers (100 per cent) had admitted their crim e, com pared with
81.3 per cent of sadistic murderers. Beauregard and his colleagues
believe these differences are due in part to the offenders' personalities
and to the severity of the crime. Having realized the seriousness of
their action, angry m urderers m ight be burdened by rem orse and
would therefore be more likely to confess their crimes to the police.
Confessions by sex offenders
This was not the case with sadistic m urderers. Furtherm ore, subjects
motivated by anger m ight have acted impulsively, often under the
influence of intoxicating substances (m inim izing factors), whereas
the sadistic m urderers often prem editate their crimes (aggravating
factors).
M od us opera ndi
The vast majority of victim s of sexual crim es are fem ale (Hanson
1991; Hilton 1993; St-Yves and Pellerin 2002) - about three out of
four victim s, according to Correctional Services of Canada statistics
(St-Yves et al. 1999). In the study we conducted on the interrogation
of sex offenders, offenders against m ale victim s confessed more often
to police officers than offenders against female victims (St-Yves 2002).
This could be explained by the fact that heterosexual offenders use
coercive violent m eans more often than hom osexual paedophiles
(Proulx et al. 1999) and that, as m entioned before, the more violent
the crim e, the less likely the suspect will confess. This link betw een
the sex of the victim and the confession rate could also be explained
by the victim 's age. In this study, all the male victim s were minors.
As many have observed, child m olesters confess more readily to
the police than rapists (N ugent and Kroner 1996; Gudjonnsson and
Sigurdsson 2000; St-Yves 2002).
C h a ra c te ris tic s o f th e su sp e ct
Age
M ost authors agree that the older the offender is, the less likely he
is to confess his crime to the police (Leiken 1970; N eubauer 1974;
Baldwin and M cConville 1980; Softley 1980; M itchell 1983; Richardson
et al. 1995; Pearse et al. 1998; Phillips and Brown 1998). However,
we did not observe this with sex offenders (St-Yves 2002). It should
be added, however, that the subjects in our study were on average
40 years old, which is much older than the prison population as a
whole (average age 32 years old) (St-Yves et al. 1999; Correctional
Service of Canada 2001).
Ethnic group
M a rita l status
St-Yves (2002) found that single men are more likely to confess than
married m en (38 per cent com pared with 24 per cent, respectively). A
partner seem s to be an inhibiting factor, possibly because crim es of a
sexual nature can jeopardize a relationship. As noted by G udjonnsson
(1992), am ongst the factors that inhibit a confession are concerns
regarding hurting the fam ily and relatives or the fear of being
rejected. Single men would obviously be less concerned by this.
U nlike many authors, St-Yves (2002) did not observe any link betw een
previous offending and confession rates. This was also observed by
Leiken (1970), Zander (1979) and Phillips and Brown (1998), who
found that previous offending had no influence on the incidence of
confession, even when a distinction is m ade betw een sex crim es and
non-sex crimes.
Intelligence
St-Yves et al. (1999) found that sex offenders tended not to be very
well educated and that, in 55.7 per cent of cases, the offenders were
considered to be of below -average intelligence. Using the same
sample of sex offenders, Guay (2001) observed that the intellectual
level of sex offenders sentenced to a term of im prisonm ent is largely
below that of the general population. In a more recent study, St-Yves
(2002) found that, the higher the IQ of sex offenders, the more likely
they are to confess during a police interrogation. The global average
IQ 2 for people who confessed is 85.2 per cent compared with 80.6 per
cent (the difference is m arginally significant) for those who did not
confess. This difference becom es significant when we com pare the
two groups for their verbal IQ (86.4 per cent com pared with 79.6 per
cent, respectively). This difference m ay be explained by the fact that,
the more intelligent or educated the subject, the more he understands
that collaboration can help to explain or justify his actions. They
perhaps also know that a co-operative attitude m ay be beneficial
when sentenced (Cusson 1998). Subjects who have a lower IQ may
prefer to remain quiet and to be represented by an attorney.
Sense o f guilt
T h e a ttitu d e s o f th e in v e s tig a to r
114
Confessions by sex offenders
W ithin this group m ost sex offenders, for the m ajor part (71.8 per
cent) (complete disclosure in 39.7 per cent of cases), confessed during
police interrogations. Only one subject out of five (20.5 per cent)
refused to talk a n d /o r to ask to speak to an attorney. These subjects
sexually assaulted a m inor (93.6 per cent) and dem onstrated a sense
of guilt after com m itting their crimes (78.2 per cent). Subm issive
collaborators have a dependency personality profile, as measured
by the M illon Clinical M ultiaxial Inventory (M CMI). They also
rated highly on this personality scale as being dependent, avoidant,
schizoid and schizotypic. Once convicted, alm ost all (98.7 per cent)
subm issive collaborators admitted having com m itted the crimes for
which they were found guilty. The majority of these subjects (61.5
per cent) made com plete confessions. They are also the ones who
recognize m ost often the negative consequences of their crimes for
the victim (60.3 per cent) and their responsibility for their crimes
(94.9 per cent). Two thirds (64.1 per cent) of subm issive collaborators
recognized that they had a sexual disorder.
These individuals alm ost never co-operated with the police authorities
(17.8 per cent). M ost refused to talk a n d /o r to ask to speak to an
attorney (47.8 per cent) or they denied everything (27.8 per cent).
W hen these subjects co-operated, only 6.7 per cent made a com plete
confession. These reluctant collaborators are alm ost all rapists (91.1
per cent) who rarely dem onstrated a sense of guilt (30 per cent).
Compared with the subm issive collaborators, they tend to have a
115
Investigative Interviewing
These offenders rarely confess on their arrest (13.3 per cent). Only
10 per cent of those who co-operate made a com plete confession.
M ost refused to talk (70 per cent) or denied everything (15 per cent).
Dorm ant collaborators have offended against a m inor (100 per cent)
but, unlike the subm issive collaborators, few dem onstrated a sense of
guilt for their crimes (20 per cent). Their personality profile is sim ilar
to that of reluctant collaborators. It was in this group that we found
the m ost narcissistic personalities, according to M CM I. They are also
the subjects who scored m ost highly as being histrionic and anti-social.
O nce convicted, dorm ant collaborators admitted their crim es in a
proportion com parable (93.3 per cent) w ith subm issive collaborators.
M ost recognized their responsibility for their crim es (88.3 per cent),
one out of two (48.3 per cent) recognized the negative consequences
of their actions for their victim s and a little more than a half (56.7
per cent) admitted having a sexual disorder. These percentages lie in-
betw een the ones we found for subm issive collaborators and reluctant
collaborators.
The grouping analysis (cluster X-m eans) we conducted in this
study allowed us to identify three sex-offender profiles, each reacting
differently during police interview s. The two groups of non-co
operative subjects (reluctant collaborators and dorm ant collaborators)
differed m ostly by the age of their victim s. One w as a paedophile, the
other a rapist. These non-co-operative subjects scored the m ost highly
on personality scales as being extroverted, narcissistic, histrionic
and anti-social. This supports the observations of G udjonnsson and
Petursson (1991), who established that extroverted individuals are
stronger during police interrogations. The co-operative subjects (the
subm issive collaborators) differed from the non-co-operative subjects
by their sense of guilt and by their personality profile, which is
m ore inclined to be introverted, dependent, avoidant, schizoid and
schizotypic. Once convicted, dorm ant collaborators confessed their
crimes in proportions com parable with subm issive collaborators,
Confessions by sex offenders
P ro m o tin g confessions
117
Investigative Interviewing
most. It is also the rapists who are the most resistant, and it is the
rapists who are most often diagnosed as being psychopaths, above
all if they are polymorphic (paedophiles and rapists) sex offenders
(Porter et al. 2000).
Since extroverted sex offenders experience few emotions concerning
their crimes, they have a greater tendency to give in to external
pressures (persuasive police interrogation techniques, the attitudes
of the investigator, fear of confinement). Since these individuals do
not usually live with remorse and do not feel the need to confess,
the investigator has to rely on the quality of the evidence and
on interrogation techniques to obtain a confession. According to
Gudjonnsson and Sigurdsson (2000), rapists are more sensitive to the
strength of the evidence than child molesters.
In short, the major obstacle to a confession is the offender's
personality profile. For sex offenders who have an introverted profile,
it seems that loss of self-esteem, shame and humiliation are the most
pronounced inhibiting factors, whereas for extroverted offenders it is
the personal consequences regarding their image and reputation. For
introverts, confession is a private matter whereas, for extroverts, it is
more of a public affair.
C o n c lu s io n
N o te s
R e fe re n c e s
120
Confessions by sex offenders
121
Investigative Interviewing
122
C h a p te r 7
Gisli H. Gudjonsson
In tro d u c tio n
In te rro g a tio n
123
Investigative Interviewing
124
The psychology of interrogations and confessions
125
Investigative Interviewing
• Suggest there was a non-criminal intent behind the act (e.g. the
criminal act may have been accidental or committed in self-defence
rather than intentionally).
126
The psychology of interrogations and confessions
for his or her innocence. It is argued that once the suspect feels
that the objections are not getting him or her anyw here he or she
begins to show signs of w ithdraw al from active participation in the
interrogation. The suspect is now thought to be at his or her lowest
point and the interrogator needs to act quickly in order not to lose
the psychological advantage he or she has gained.
127
Investigative Interviewing
128
The psychology of interrogations and confessions
129
Investigative Interviewing
C on fe ssion s
130
The psychology of interrogations and confessions
131
Investigative Interviewing
132
The psychology of interrogations and confessions
133
Investigative Interviewing
to these various options. For example, if they confess are they likely
to be charged with the offence? If they insist on their innocence is the
interrogation likely to continue and their detention prolonged?
The decision-making of the suspect is determined by the subjective
probabilities of occurrence of the perceived consequences. In other
words, it is what the suspect believes at the time to be the likely
consequences that influence his or her behaviour rather than the
objective reality of the situation. For example, an innocent suspect
may confess under the misguided belief that since he or she is
innocent no court will bring in a guilty verdict and that the truth
will eventually come out (Gudjonnsson 2003a).
The suspect has to balance the potential consequences against
the perceived value ('utilities') of choosing a particular course of
action. For example, would a confession inevitably lead to cessation
of interrogation and would the suspect be allowed to go home?
Hilgendorf and Irving argue that threats and inducements can
markedly influence the decision of the suspect to confess because
of the perceived power the police have over the situation and the
apparent credibility of their words.
Hilgendorf and Irving postulate that there are a number of social,
psychological and environmental factors that can influence the suspect's
decision-making during police interrogation. On occasions these
factors can undermine the reliability of the suspect's confession when
police interrogators:
134
The psychology of interrogations and confessions
art, language and other social activities, including crime. Reik relies
heavily on Freud's concepts of the id, ego and superego where a
confession is seen as 'an attempt at reconciliation that the superego
undertakes in order to settle the quarrel between the ego and the
id' (p. 216). The superego is seen to play an important role in the
need of the individual to confess. A punitive superego may result
in the development of a strong feeling of guilt and need for self
punishment. This may result in a 'compulsion' to confess, and on
occasion false confession.
Within this model, the development of the feeling of guilt after
transgression and the unconscious need for self-punishment are seen
as universal. It is only after the person has confessed that the ego
begins to accept the emotional significance of the deed. A confession
serves the function of relieving the person from the feeling of guilt
and is therefore inherently therapeutic. A similar argument is put
forward by Berggren (1975) who argues that a confession produces a
sense of relief with cathartic effects. For a satisfactory cathartic effect
to occur the confession has to be to a person in authority, such as a
priest or police officer.
Rogge (1975), like Reik and Berggren, argues that confessions are
based on feelings of guilt. He suggests that feelings of guilt are made
up of two components, fear of losing love and fear of retaliation.
135
Investigative Interviewing
• There are tw7o main types of consequence, 'im m ediate' (or 'short
term ') and 'long-term ' consequences. The im m ediate or short
term consequences occur within m inutes or hours of the suspect
confessing to the alleged crime. The long-term consequences take
place w ithin days, weeks or years of the suspect confessing. The
types of consequence, w hether im m ediate or delayed, depend on
the nature and circum stances of the case and the psychological
characteristics of the individual concerned.
136
The psychology of interrogations and confessions
Based on their work into disputed and false confessions, Ofshe and
Leo (1997a, 1997b) developed a classification of confessions, which
they believe apply equally to true and false confessions. There are five
levels of confession (voluntary, stress-compliant, coerced-compliant,
non-coerced-persuaded and coerced-persuaded), categorized into
two groups (true or false). Each type of confession can be either
true or false, depending on the circumstances of the individual case,
which means that there are 10 possible outcome scenarios. The focus
of this model is very much on the interrogation process itself (i.e.
what the police say and do) and a psychological description of the
type of confession elicited. The model postulates that confessions
are chiefly elicited due to police pressure or legally defined coercion
and individual differences and interactive processes are of relatively
minor importance. This model appears to be particularly relevant to
coerced confessions (i.e. where the police have made threats or offered
inducements) irrespective of the guilt or innocence of the suspect.
There are some overlaps between the different models, although each
makes different assumptions about why suspects confess to the police
during questioning. For example:
137
Investigative Interviewing
• The nature of the relationship betw een the suspect, the environm ent
and significant others within that environm ent.
• Interrogative pressure and coercion.
A m o d e l o f th e in te rro g a tiv e p ro ce ss
Police factors
Custodial pressures
Interrogative pressures
• Personality of interviewers
Vulnerabilities
• Physical health
• Mental health
• Psychological vulnerabilities
Support
• Solicitor
• Appropriate adult
138
The psychology of interrogations and confessions
139
Investigative Interviewing
of these three factors, but the single m ost im portant reason is the
suspect's perception of the evidence against him or her (Gudjonnsson
2003a).
There is evidence that the duration of detention, nature of the
interrogation techniques used and dynam ics in the police interview
are related to the severity of the crime being investigated and it is here
that custodial and interrogative factors tap more into psychological
vulnerabilities (Gudjonnsson 2003a). Pearse and G udjonnsson (1999,
2003) used a special coding fram e, the 'Police Interview ing Analysis
Fram ew ork' (PIAF), to analyze the social interaction between
the interview er and suspect from the tape recordings of real-life
interrogation and to identify the techniques that were associated
w ith moving suspects from a position of denial to a confession. Each
five-minute segm ent of interrogation was carefully scrutinized and
the results were subjected to a factor analysis. The three m ost salient
factors that were associated with the breaking down of resistance
were all 'overbearing' in character and were labelled as 'intim idation'
(e.g. m axim izing the seriousness of the offence and the anxiety of the
suspect w hen denying the offence), 'robust challenge' (i.e. repeatedly
challenging lies and inconsistencies) and 'm anipulation' (i.e. minim izing
seriousness of the offence, inducem ents, theme developm ent). These
techniques, which are sim ilar to those recom m ended by Inbau et al.
(2001), were effective in breaking down resistance and securing a
confession. However, this was achieved at a high level of risk of the
confession being rendered inadm issible by a court due to coercion
and the defendant consequently being acquitted. In contrast, there
were two further more 'sensitive' styles em ployed, albeit to a lesser
degree, referred to as 'appeal' and 'soft challenge', which proved
particularly effective with sex offenders and did not underm ine the
adm issibility of the confession as they were not construed as being
coercive.
The PIAF has not only succeeded in analyzing, m easuring and
displaying the nature and type of tactics em ployed, but it has
also discrim inated betw een overbearing and sensitive tactics, as
determ ined by legal judgm ents (Pearse and G udjonnsson 2003).
In a unique real-life observational study of run-of-the-m ill cases at
two English police stations, over 170 suspects were psychologically
assessed by clinical psychologists prior to their being interviewed
by the police (Gudjonnsson et al. 1993). Subsequently, all tapes of
interview s with the suspects were analyzed to find out what factors
were associated with denial and confessions (Pearse et al. 1998).
The great majority of the interview s were very short (i.e. 80 per
140
The psychology of interrogations and confessions
cent lasted less than 30 minutes and 95 per cent were completed
within one hour), the confession rate was 58 per cent, there was
little interrogative pressure in the tactics used and very few suspects
moved from a denial to a confession (see Gudjonnsson 2003a for
a detailed review of the findings). Logistic regression analysis was
performed on the data. The dependent (outcome) variable was
confession versus denial. The independent variables included the
suspect's age, ethnicity, mental state, intelligence, suggestibility, illicit
drug taking, criminal history, police interview tactics and presence or
absence of a legal adviser. The strength of the evidence against the
suspect was not measured in this study.
Two factors were highly predictive of a denial (i.e. the presence of
a legal adviser and a previous history of imprisonment), whereas only
one variable predicted a confession (i.e. whether the suspect had told
the researcher that he or she had taken illicit drugs within 24 hours
of arrest). The main implications of the findings are that in the run-
of-the-mill English cases where there is little interrogative pressure
or persuasion taking place and the great majority of suspects who
confess do so right at the beginning of the interview. Psychological
vulnerabilities, apart from illicit drug taking, which probably makes
suspects eager to be released from custody as quickly as possible and
therefore more willing to confess, are of little relevance, and having
a legal representative and previous experience of imprisonment are
strongly associated with a denial. However, in the more serious cases,
psychological vulnerabilities and police pressure become much more
important (Gudjonnsson 2003a).
The second important set of factors are the specific vulnerabilities
of the detainee, which are associated with his or her physical and
mental health, as well as more specific psychological vulnerabilities,
such as suggestibility, compliance, acquiescence and anti-social
personality traits (Gudjonnsson 2003a). In addition, children and
juveniles (Drizin and Colgan 2004; Redlich et al. 2004) and persons
with learning disabilities (Fulero and Everington 2004) are particularly
at risk of giving unreliable accounts of events when not carefully
interviewed. Sleep deprivation increases the person's susceptibility
to suggestions (Blagrove 1996).
Thirdly, the impact of the presence or absence of a solicitor and
appropriate adult during the interrogation needs to be evaluated
as a part of the overall custodial environment. The impact of the
presence of a solicitor on suspects' behaviour during interrogation
is well established (Gudjonnsson 2003a). There is evidence emerging
from our current work at police stations that the mere presence of
141
Investigative Interviewing
142
The psychology of interrogations and confessions
C o n clu s io n s
N o te
1. T hese in clud e W righ tsm an and K assin (1 9 9 3 ); O fshe and Leo (1997a,
1997b ); K assin (1 9 9 8 ); Leo and O fshe (1 9 9 8 ); M cC an n (1998); Leo (1998,
2 0 0 1 a ); an d , m ore recently, K assin and G u djonnsson (2004).
R eferences
143
Investigative Interviewing
Drizin, S.A. and Colgan, B.A. (2004) Tales from the juvenile confessions
front', in G.D. Lassiter (ed.) Interrogations, Confessions, and Entrapment.
New York, NY: Kluwer Academic.
Drizin, S.A. and Leo, R.A. (2004) 'The problem of false confessions in the
post-DNA world', North Carolina Law Review, 82: 891-1007.
Fisher, R.P and Geiselman, R.E. (1992) Memory Enhancing Techniques fo r
Investigative Interviewing: The Cognitive Interview. Springfield, IL: Charles
C. Thomas.
Fulero, S.M. and Everington, C. (2004) 'Mental retardation, competency
to waive Miranda rights, and false confessions', in G.D. Lassiter (ed.)
Interrogations, Confessions, and Entrapment. New York, NY: Kluwer
Academic.
Gudjonnsson, G.H. (1989) 'The psychology of false confessions', Medico-Legal
Journal, 57: 93-110.
Gudjonnsson, G.H. (1992) The Psychology o f Interrogations, Confessions and
Testimony. London: Wiley.
Gudjonnsson, G. (2002) 'W ho makes a good interviewer? Police interviewing
and confessions', in M. Bockstaele (ed.) Politieverhoor en Personality-Profiling.
Brussels: Uitgeverij Politeia nv.
Gudjonnsson, G.H. (2003a) The Psychology o f Interrogations and Confessions. A
Handbook. Chichester: Wiley.
Gudjonnsson, G.H. (2003b) 'Psychology brings justice: the science of forensic
psychology', Criminal Behaviour and Mental Health, 13: 159-67.
Gudjonnsson, G.H., Clare, I.C.H., Rutter, S. and Pearse, J. (1993) Persons at
Risk During Interviews in Police Custody: The Identification o f Vulnerabilities.
Royal Commission on Criminal Justice. London: HMSO.
Gudjonnsson, G.H. and MacKeith, J.A.C. (1988) 'Retracted confessions: legal,
psychological and psychiatric aspects', Medicine, Science and the Law, 28:
187-94.
Gudjonnsson, G.H. and MacKeith, J. (1997) Disputed Confessions and the
Criminal Justice System. Maudsley Discussion Paper 2. London: Institute of
Psychiatry.
Gudjonnsson, G.H. and Sigurdsson, J.F. (1999) 'The Gudjonnsson Confession
Questionnaire-Revised (GCQ-R): factor structure and its relationship with
personality', Personality and Individual Differences, 27: 953-68.
Hilgendorf, E.L. and Irving, M. (1981) 'A decision-making model of
confessions', in M. Lloyd-Bostock (ed.) Psychology in Legal Contexts:
Applications and Limitations. London: Macmillan.
Holmberg, U. and Christianson, S.A. (2002) 'Murderers' and sexual offenders'
experiences of police interviews and their inclination to admit and deny
crimes', Behavioral Sciences and the Law, 20: 31-45.
Home Office (1985) Police and Criminal Evidence Act 1984. London: HMSO.
Home Office (2004) Police and Criminal Evidence Act 1984 (s. 66), Codes o f
Practice A -F. London: HMSO.
Inbau, F.E., Reid, J.E. and Buckley, J.P. (1986) Criminal Interrogation and
Confessions (3rd edn). Baltimore, MD: Williams & Wilkins.
144
T h e psychology o f interrogations and confessions
Inbau, F.E., Reid, J.E., Buckley, J.R and Jayne, B.C. (2001) Criminal Interrogation
and Confessions (4th edn). Gaithersberg, MD: Aspen.
Irving, B. (1980) Police Interrogation. A Case Study o f Current Practice. Research
Studies 2. London: HMSO.
Jayne, B.C. (1986) 'The psychological principles of criminal interrogation',
in F. Inbau et al. (eds) Criminal Interrogation and Confessions (3rd edn).
Baltimore, MD: Williams & Wilkins.
Kassin, S.M. (1998) 'More on the psychology of false confessions', American
Psychologist, 53: 320-1.
Kassin, S.M. and Gudjonnsson, G.H. (2004) 'The psychology of confessions:
a review of the literature and issues', Psychological Science in the Public
Interest, 5, 33-67.
Kassin, S.M. and McNall, K. (1991) 'Police interrogations and confessions:
communicating promises and threats by pragmatic implication', Law and
Human Behavior, 15: 233-51.
Leo, R.A. (1992) 'From coercion to deception: the changing nature of police
interrogation in America', Crime, Law and Social Change: An International
Journal, 18: 35-59.
Leo, R.A. (1994) 'Police interrogation in America: a study of violence, civility
and social change.' Unpublished PhD thesis, University of California at
Berkeley.
Leo, R.A. (1996) 'Inside the interrogation room', Journal o f Criminal Law and
Criminology, 86: 266-303.
Leo, R.A. (1998) 'Miranda and the problem of false confessions', in
R.A. Leo and G.C. Thomas III (eds) The Miranda Debate, Justice and Policing.
Boston, MA: Northeastern University Press.
Leo, R.A. (2001a) 'False confessions. Causes, consequences and solutions', in
S.A. Westervelt and J.A. Humphrey (eds) Wrongly Convicted. Perspectives
on Failed Justice. London: Rutgers University Press.
Leo, R.A. (2001) 'Questioning the relevance of Miranda in the twenty-first
century', Michigan Lazv Review, 99: 1000-28.
Leo, R.A. and Ofshe, R.J. (1998) 'The consequences of false confessions:
deprivations of liberty and miscarriages of justice in the age of psychological
interrogation', Journal o f Criminal Law and Criminology, 88: 429-96.
Mackey, C. and Miller, G. (2004) The Interrogator's War. Inside the Secret War
against Al Qaeda. London: John Murray.
McCann, J.T. (1998) 'A conceptual framework for identifying various types of
confessions', Behavioral Sciences and the Law, 16: 441-53.
Medford, S., Gudjonnsson, G.H. and Pearse, J. (2003) 'The efficacy of the
appropriate adult safeguard during police interviewing', Legal and
Criminological Psychology, 8: 253-66.
Moston, S., Stephenson, G.M. and Williamson, T.M. (1992) 'The effects of case
characteristics on suspect behaviour during questioning', British Journal o f
Criminology, 32: 23-40.
Ofshe, R.J. and Leo, R.A. (1997a) 'The decision to confess falsely: rational
choice and irrational action', Denver University Lazv Reviezv, 74: 979-1122.
Investigative Interviewing
Ofshe, R.J. and Leo, R.A. (1997b) T h e social psychology of police interrogation:
the theory and classification of true and false confessions', Studies in Law,
Politics and Society, 16: 189-251.
Pearse, J. and Gudjonnsson, G.H. (1996) 'Police interviewing techniques at
two south London Police Stations', Psychology, Crime and Law, 3: 63-74.
Pearse, J. and Gudjonnsson, G.H. (1999) 'M easuring influential police
interviewing tactics: a factor analytic approach', Legal and Criminological
Psychology, 4: 221-38.
Pearse, J. and Gudjonnsson, G.H. (2003) 'The identification and measurement
of "oppressive" police interviewing tactics in Britain', in G.H. Gudjonnsson
(ed.) The Psychology o f Interrogations and Confessions. A Handbook. Chichester:
Wiley.
Pearse, J., Gudjonnsson, G.H., Clare, l.C.H. and Rutter, S. (1998) 'Police
interviewing and psychological vulnerabilities: predicting the likelihood of
a confession', Journal o f Community and Applied Social Psychology, 8: 1-21.
Redlich, A.D., Silverman, M., Chen, J. and Steiner, H. (2004) 'The police
interrogation of children and adolescents', in G.D. Lassiter (ed.)
Interrogations, Confessions, and Entrapment. New York, NY: Kluwer
Academic.
Reik, T. (1959) The Compulsion to Confess: On the Psychoanalysis o f Crime and
Punishment. New York, NY: Farrar, Straus & Cudahy.
Rogge, O.J. (1975) Why Men Confess. New York, NY: Da Capo Press.
Rose, D. (2004) Guantanamo. America's War on Human Rights. London: Faber
& Faber.
Vrij, A. (2004a) 'Guidelines to catch a liar', in P.A. Granhag and
L.A. Stromwall (eds) The Detection o f Deception in Forensic Contexts.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Vrij, A. (2004b) 'Why professionals fail to catch liars and how they can
improve', Legal and Criminal Psychology, 9: 159-81.
Williamson, T.M. (1993) 'From interrogation to investigative interviewing.
Strategic trends in the police questioning', Journal o f Community and
Applied Social Psychology, 3: 89-99.
Williamson, T.M. (1994) 'Reflections on current police practice', in D. Morgan
and G. Stephenson (eds) Suspicion and Silence. The Rights o f Silence in
Criminal Investigations. London: Blackstone Press.
Wrightsman, L.S. and Kassin, S.M. (1993) Confessions in the Courtroom.
Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Zimbardo, P.G. (1967) 'The psychology of police confessions', Psychology
Today, 1: 17-20, 25-7.
146
C h a p te r 8
Tom Williamson
In tro d u c tio n
147
Investigative Interviewing
148
Towards greater professionalism: minimizing miscarriages of justice
Another survey found that the public were more critical of the police
where they have a high degree of contact with the police and are
subject to a high level of victim ization (Jones et al. 1986).
One of the reasons for these concerns is the im portance that confession
evidence plays in court proceedings. Bottom ley and Colem an (1980)
found that only 10 per cent of cases were detected as a result of
intelligence or forensic evidence. The interrogation of suspects was the
m ost im portant means of detecting offences. In 1977 approxim ately 25
per cent of all detections were offences 'taken into consideration' by
the courts for sentencing purposes that resulted from the questioning
of som eone arrested for another offence.3
In an observational study for the royal com m ission of how police
interrogations were conducted in four police stations Softley found
that about 60 per cent of suspects made a full confession or a
dam aging adm ission (Softley 1981). In a sim ilar study Irving (1981)
found that obtaining a confession was the m ain purpose of a police
interrogation. An exam ination of cases heard in the Crown Court
(M cConville et al. 1991; Baldwin 1992) found that 13 per cent would
ISO
Towards greater professionalism: minimizing miscarriages of justice
the com m on law system has never made the search for the truth,
as such, its highest aim. It is not that there is any objection to
the truth emerging. But centuries ago it was appreciated that the
truth is m any-sided, com plex, and difficult to ascertain. Even
when all the relevant evidence is adm issible, we com m only
do not know for sure w hether the defendant was, or, was
not, innocent or guilty The com m on law system does not ask
whether the defendant is guilty or innocent but rather the more
m anageable question - can it be proved beyond a reasonable
doubt that he is guilty?
151
Investigative Interviewing
When the Police and Criminal Evidence Act 1984 was implemented
it provided a legislative framework for the regulation of custodial
questioning. Under s. 76 of PACE it is no longer up to the defence
to show that something had happened in the interrogation that
would render the statement unreliable, it is up to the prosecution to
show that nothing had happened to make the statement unreliable.
Furthermore, under s. 78 of the Act the trial judge can exclude
anything that is considered to be 'unfair' such as deception by
the interviewing officers or providing misleading information. In
R. v. Heron the judge acquitted the defendant when, inter alia, the
interviewing officers misled the accused regarding identification
evidence in a homicide case.4
Section C of the codes of practice cover the detention, treatment and
questioning of persons by police officers. The Act entitled suspects to
free legal advice and also provided for the tape recording of interviews
with suspects. It is now almost unheard of that an interview will be
conducted which is not either audio or videotape recorded and this
has made a very important contribution to the regulation of custodial
questions and also in identifying skills deficits.
Early research into the quality of the police interviews, which was
possible through the analysis of video and audio tape recordings,
revealed that interviewing skills were generally poor. The interviewers
appeared inept, nervous, ill at ease and lacking in confidence.
Questioning was conducted on the basis of assumption of the suspect's
152
Towards greater professionalism: minimizing miscarriages of justice
guilt. Suspects were given very little opportunity to speak and when
they did so the interviewing officer(s) constantly interrupted them.
The officers had a fragile grasp of the legal points needed to prove
the offence. The interviewing style was harrying and aggressive.
There were examples of unfair inducements (Baldwin 1992). This
reflected a continuing over-reliance on confession evidence under the
new legislation. The reliance on confession evidence also meant that
witnesses and victims were frequently not interviewed thoroughly
and so were unable to provide all the information they were capable
of giving as evidence. The role of the police in the investigation of
offences was still one of persuading suspects to confess rather than
engaging in a process of inquiry, which was a search for the truth.
In an analysis of over 1,000 tape recorded interviews in London,
Williamson (1990) found that there was a very strong statistical
correlation between the strength of the evidence and the outcome of
the interview. Where the evidence was weak, 77 per cent of suspects
denied the allegation and where the evidence was strong, 67 per cent
of suspects made admissions (see Table 8.1).
153
Investigative Interviewing
154
Towards greater professionalism: minimizing miscarriages of justice
155
Investigative Interviewing
156
Towards greater professionalism: minimizing miscarriages of justice
C rim in a l C a se s R e vie w C o m m is s io n
The Crim inal Appeal Act 1995 created the Crim inal Cases Review
Com m ission whose function is to review all allegations of m iscarriages
of justice. So far over 100 cases have been referred to the Court of
Appeal. The com m ission has reviewed over 4,000 cases and currently
has 450 cases under review .1 A sim ilar review com m ission has been
established in Scotland but with wider term s of reference m aking
intervention easier w henever they suspect a miscarriage of justice.
A Hom e Affairs Select Com m ittee of the House of Com m ons has
investigated cases of historical child abuse and has drawn attention
to w hat it believes to be w idescale m iscarriages of justice (HAC
2002). Cases of this type represent a significant proportion of the
applications to the Crim inal Cases Review Commission.
157
Investigative Interviewing
158
Towards greater professionalism: minimizing miscarriages of justice
159
Investigative Interviewing
160
Towards greater professionalism: minimizing miscarriages of justice
161
Investigative Interviewing
C o n c lu s io n
Recent research has led to serious questions being raised about the
reliability of evidence obtained by investigators through questioning.
This can be affected by the way investigators adapt to the requirements
of the system of justice in which they operate. There would appear
to be a number of steps that societies can take in order to minimize
miscarriages of justice that result from 'tunnel vision' and over
reliance on confession evidence.
Good pre-trial investigation and custodial questioning processes
will provide better evidence and reduce the over-reliance on
confession evidence and encourage a search for the truth. Making
better use of forensic evidence and more thorough questioning of
victims and witnesses to enable them to give their best evidence will
be more likely to enable courts to reach the truth. Greater sensitivity
in interviewing might help the guilty to provide a truthful account
and for those who are vulnerable it would prevent many future
miscarriages of justice. Quality legal representation for accused at
public expense and disclosure of prosecution evidence are important
safeguards. Formal systems for reviewing alleged miscarriages of
justice and robust civilian oversight arrangements are an important
investment. Independent status for forensic science laboratories
should prevent some of the 'junk' science that has been a feature in
miscarriage of justice cases.
There needs to be greater recognition that truth and justice suffer
when criminal justice systems become too adversarial. As Stephenson
(1992) identified, many of the current systems are fundamentally
flawed. New approaches to professionalizing investigative competence
are being developed which should lead to twenty-first century
162
Towards greater professionalism: minimizing miscarriages of justice
Im proving trust and confidence in the crim inal justice system will
occur when there is a greater level of professionalism in the core
com petency of investigative interviewing.
N o te s
163
Investigative Interview ing
R e fe r e n c e s
164
Towards greater professionalism: minimizing miscarriages o f justice
165
Investigative Interviewing
166
C h a p te r 9
In tro d u c tio n
167
Investigative Interviewing
168
W ill it all end in tiers?
A r e c e n t h is to r y o f p o lic e in t e r v ie w s w it h s u s p e c ts
169
Investigative Interviewing
170
W ill it all end in tiers?
T i e r 3: a d va n ce d s u s p e ct in te r v ie w tra in in g
171
Investigative Interviewing
• Planning and preparation deals with both the legal and logistical
issues of interview preparation. Under 'legal' an officer would
prepare an interview plan encom passing the points to prove and
defences to an offence plus the subject areas to be covered in
the interview. Logistical considerations would include preparing
the interview room, assem bling equipm ent and arranging the
attendance of other professionals.
• Closure deals with the end phase of the interview. There are legal
requirem ents in the closing of an interview as there are at the start.
It also includes explanations to the suspect of w hat may happen
after the interview.
174
W ill it all end in tiers?
175
Investigative Interviewing
Figure 9.1 show s the com parative results across the sam ple of 15
detective officers in terms of their overall perform ance in four assessed
interview's (60 interview s). Interview' A is the role-play interview that
officers conduct to gain access to the course. The average perform ance
of the sam ple in this interview' is assessed as 2.25. The m arking
guide for the scale suggests '3 ' as PEACE standard and so this result
indicates a poor overall standard. This is especially true when one
considers that the figure represents only successful applicants for
the course. This result is significant because officers have time to
prepare for the assessm ent and attend as volunteers seeking access
to the course. Interview B, the final assessed interview undertaken
at the conclusion of the training course, shows there is a significant
im provem ent in the overall standard of interview s conducted across
this group. The mean score of the group is 4.1. This is classified
as 'skilful' within the m arking guide. This score is achieved under
sim ilar test pressure as the first interview in that the officers have
5.0 "
4 .5 1
A B C D
Interview
176
W ill it all end in tiers?
Interview
F ig u re 9 .2 Assessment of the delivery of the caution
177
Investigative Interviewing
178
W ill it all end in tiers?
5.0
4 .5
4.0
J 3.5
3
(D
c 2.5
CO
2.0
1.5
1.0
A B C D
Interview
179
Investigative Interviewing
5.0
A B C D
Interview
Figure 9.4 A ssessm en t of th e stru ctu re of topics in sequ ence
is poor on average, the mean score being 2.1. Very few officers
scored higher than 3. Interview B concerns a serious crime with a
significant amount of information to question the suspect about.
Despite this the sample display a skilled performance in this area,
the mean score being 4.4, which shows a significant improvement
over interview A. In the discussion regarding Figure 9.3 the point
was made about officers practising their introductions in order to
improve their scores. This is not possible with the criterion subject
of Figure 9.4 because the information available for every interview is
unique and presents previously unseen issues. The skills loss found
in the workplace interviews (C and D) concerning this criterion is
significant. In particular, interview D shows a significant decrease
since training. This illustrates the difference between maintaining
improvement within simple or complex criteria. As a final point it
should be noted that even with the skills loss officers show a higher
level of skill in more complex interviews one year after training than
before training.
The ability to question a suspect using appropriate question
types is fundamental to advanced interviewing. The course spends
180
W ill it all end in tiers?
5.0
A B C D
Interview
181
Investigative Interviewing
182
W ill it all end in tiers?
183
Investigative Interviewing
Time
184
W ill it all end in tiers?
Closed
----------------------------------------------------------------------------- 0
Leading
Multiple
Forced
Choice
Opinion
Stmt
185
Investigative Interviewing
Open
Probing
186
W ill it all end in tiers?
C o n clu s io n s
187
Investigative Interviewing
T ier fo u r is so lely co n cern ed w ith the m o n ito rin g and ev alu ation of
in terv iew s in ord er to p ro v id e reg u lar ob jectiv e feed b ack . T his area
sh ou ld be the su b ject o f fu rth e r research to ev a lu a te its effect. T h ese
p relim in ary find ings d em o n stra te that, w ith the a p p ro p riate training,
officers can im p ro ve th eir sk ills and co n d u ct effectiv e and p ro fessio n a l
in terv iew s. T h is is essen tial in secu rin g safe co n v ictio n s in the m ost
serio u s o f cases if the co n fid en ce o f the p u b lic is to be m ain tain ed .
N o te
1. The term 'Britain' or 'British' has been used throughout the chapter for
ease of reading. However, it should be noted that England and Wales
have different laws from Scotland and Northern Ireland. Comments about
historical cases relate to England and Wales only. However, police forces
within Scotland and Northern Ireland are adopting the five-tier strategy
and so comments regarding the development of interviewing are relevant
to all four countries.
R e fe re n c e s
188
W ill it all end in tiers?
189
C h a p te r 10
Joseph P. Buckley
In tro d u c tio n
190
The Reid Technique of interviewing and interrogation
Characteristics o f an interview
An interview is non-accusatory
This should be the case even when the investigator has clear reason
to believe that the suspect is involved in the offence or has lied to
him or her. By maintaining a non-accusatory tone, the investigator
is able to establish a much better rapport with the suspect which
will assist in any interrogation that might follow the interview. A
guilty subject is more likely to volunteer useful information about his
or her access, opportunity, propensity and motives if the questions
are asked in a non-accusatory fashion. In addition, the suspect's
behavioural responses to interview questions can be more reliably
interpreted when the questions are asked in a conversational, rather
than challenging manner. The investigator should remain neutral and
objective during the interview process.
191
Investigative Interviewing
Characteristics o f an interrogation
An interrogation is accusatory
A deceptive suspect is not likely to offer admissions against self-
interest unless he or she is convinced that the investigator is certain
192
The Reid Technique of interviewing and interrogation
193
Investigative Interviewing
The investigator should not take any notes until after the suspect has told
the truth and is fully committed to that position
Premature note-taking during an interrogation serves as a reminder
to the suspect of the incriminating nature of his or her statements
and can therefore inhibit further admissions against self-interest. Only
after the suspect has fully confessed, and perhaps after the confession
has been witnessed by another investigator, should written notes be
made documenting the details of the confession.
194
The Reid Technique of interviewing and interrogation
Sum mary
195
Investigative Interviewing
The room should be quiet, with none of the usual 'police' surroundings
and with no distractions within the suspect's view. (If existing
facilities perm it, a special room or rooms should be set aside for this
purpose.) The room should be as free as possible from outside noises
and should also be a room into which no one will have occasion
to enter or pass through during an interview. This will not only
instil a sense of privacy, but also the less the surroundings suggest
a police detention facility, the less difficult it will be for the suspect
or arrestee who is really guilty to im plicate him or herself. The
sam e surroundings will also be reassuring to the innocent suspect.
Therefore, there should be no bars on the window s. (There should be
an alternative m eans of protection against any attem pts to escape.) In
a window less room that has no air-conditioning system , a m echanical
blow er or exhaust system may be installed w ithout m uch difficulty
to improve ventilation and to elim inate, or at least m inim ize, noises.
(The room should have its own thermostatic controls.)
196
The Reid Technique of interviewing and interrogation
To be clear, the word 'guilt' as used in this text only signifies the
investigator's opinion. In no way does it connote legal guilt based
upon proof beyond a reasonable doubt. Accordingly, it is in that
context this part of the text presents the tactics and techniques
for the interrogation of suspects whose guilt, in the opinion of the
investigator, seems definite or reasonably certain. Amongst them are
the nine steps of interrogation.
197
Investigative Interviewing
198
The Reid Technique of interviewing and interrogation
199
Investigative Interviewing
Finally, step 9 relates to the confession itself. This step involves the
recommended procedure for converting an oral confession into a
written one.
200
The Reid Technique of interviewing and interrogation
20 1
Investigative Interviewing
Threats/promises
Them e development
202
The Reid Technique of interviewing and interrogation
Alternative questions
203
Investigative Interviewing
buddies cam e up with the idea you w on't have any problem s'. This
theory is called 'pragm atic im plication' and was developed from a
research study in which college students read various transcripts
of interrogations and then speculated on the type of punishm ent
the suspects would receive based on the interrogation process
used. Specifically, the students theorized that when the interrogator
suggested in a m urder case interrogation that the victim may have
done or said som ething to have provoked the suspect, that he or
she would receive the same punishm ent as in those interrogations in
w hich the suspect was directly offered a prom ise of leniency that if
he or she confessed he or she would receive less punishm ent.
The courts have rejected the idea that a confession is inadm issible
if a suspect confesses because he or she harbours some internal hope
that his or her confession may lead to a lesser sentence:
Confession corroboration
204
The Reid Technique of interviewing and interrogation
everything that they did and they do not alw ays rem em ber all the
details themselves:
M any crimes are com mitted when the suspect is experiencing intense
em otions (fear, anger, frustration). Just as victim s tend to focus on
the robber's weapon during a robbery, the em otions a guilty suspect
experiences can bias attention and m em ory retrieval of specific
details. As cognitive psychologist Daniel Schacter writes (1996: 473):
'W hen a person has actually experienced traum a, the central core of
the experience is almost always well rem em bered; if distortion does
occur, it is m ost likely to involve specific details.'
N evertheless, when significant and substantial contradictions exist
betw een the known facts about the crime and w hat the suspect
describes in his or her confession, extrem e care m ust be exercised in
the assessm ent of the confession's validity.
Factors to consider
With the above discussion in m ind, the follow ing represents some
factors to consider in the assessm ent of the credibility of a suspect's
confession. These issues are certainly not all inclusive, and each case
m ust be evaluated on the 'totality of circum stances' surrounding the
205
Investigative Interviewing
N o te s
R eferences
G rano, J. (1984) 'Selling th e idea to tell the truth: the professional in terrog ator
and m od ern confession la w ', Michigan Lazv Review, 8 4: 662.
Inbau, F.E., R eid, J.E ., Buckley, J. and Jayn e, B. (2 001) Criminal Interrogation
and Confessions (4th edn ). G aithersburg, M D: A spen.
Jayn e, B. and Buckley, J. (2 000) The Investigator Anthology. C h icag o, IL: John
E. Reid & A ssociates.
Schacter, D. (1996) Searching for Memory: The Brain, the Mind and the Past.
N ew York, NY: H arperC ollins.
206
C h a p te r I I
Saul M. Kassin
In tro d u c tio n
Let me begin with a story that already has historic value in the
annals of wrongful convictions. This was an infam ous case that took
place in 1989 in New York City. Known as the 'Central Park jogger
case', it involved a young wom an, an investm ent banker, who was
beaten senseless, raped and left for dead. It was a heinous crime that
horrified the city. The victim 's skull had m ultiple fractures, her eye
socket was crushed and she lost three quarters of her blood. Defying
the odds, she survived; but to this day, she is com pletely am nesic
for the incident. Soon thereafter, solely on the basis of police-induced
confessions taken within 72 hours of the crim e, five A frican- and
Hispanic-Am erican boys, 14-16 years old, were convicted of the
attack and sentenced to prison. There were no physical traces of the
defendants at the crim e scene and no traces of the scene on them.
At the time, however, it was easy to understand why detectives
aggressively interrogated the boys, some of w hom were 'w ilding' in
the park that night.
Four of the five jogger confessions were videotaped and presented
to the juries at trial. The tapes (which showed only the confessions,
not the precipitating 14V$-30 hours of interrogation) were com pelling,
as the boys described in vivid detail how the jogger was attacked,
when, where and by w hom , and the role that they played in the
process. One boy physically re-enacted the way he allegedly pulled
off the jo g g er's running pants. A second boy said he felt peer-
pressured to join in his 'first rape' and he expressed remorse. These
207
Investigative Interviewing
208
A critical appraisal of modern police interrogations
The first problem is that innocent people are often targeted for
interrogation, despite the absence of any evidence of their involvement,
based solely on an investigator's hunch. Consider, for example, the
209
Investigative Interviewing
210
A critical appraisal of modern police interrogations
212
A critical appraisal of modern police interrogations
213
Investigative Interviewing
214
A critical appraisal of modern police interrogations
one would hope that police would remain open-minded and re
evaluate their beliefs over the course of the interrogation. But the
two-step approach makes this an unreasonable expectation. Over
the years, research has shown that once people form a belief, they
selectively seek and interpret new information in ways that verify
that belief even in the face of contradictory evidence. This problem
contributes to the errors committed by forensic examiners, whose
assessments of handwriting samples, ballistics, and other 'scientific'
evidence are often corrupted by prior beliefs, a problem uncovered
in many DNA exoneration cases (Risinger et al. 2002). To complicate
matters further, people unwittingly create behavioural support for
their beliefs, producing a self-fulfilling prophecy. This effect was first
demonstrated by Rosenthal and Jacobson (1968) in their classic report
on the effects of teachers' expectancies on students' performance.
Similar results have been obtained in military, business and other
organizational settings (McNatt 2000).
In a story that illustrates how investigators can be blinded by
the guilt-presumptive lens they wear, a man confessed to his wife's
murder after 19 hours of interrogation when police 'bluffed' him into
thinking they had DNA evidence to be tested (Missouri v. Johnson
2001). During interrogation, it is common for police to bluff in this
manner about having independent evidence on the assumption that
the suspect, whom they presume guilty, will realize the futility of
denial and capitulate. W hat they cannot see, however, is that to an
innocent but beleaguered person, who is naive about the use of this
tactic, the 'threat' of DNA may be construed as a promise of future
exoneration - ironically making it easier to confess. In this case,
the defendant - who was instantly acquitted by a jury - explained
afterwards that he confessed because he was exhausted and knew
that the test results would show his innocence.
The process of interrogation is not only guilt presumptive but
powerful in its impact. Inbau et al. (2001) advise interrogators to
remove the suspect from familiar surroundings and place him or
her in a small, barely furnished, soundproof room housed within the
police station. Against this physical backdrop, a nine-step process
begins with the positive confrontation and the development of
alternative themes - and ends with a full written or oral confession.
Conceptually, this approach is designed to get suspects to incriminate
themselves by increasing the anxiety associated with denial,
plunging them into a state of despair and minimizing the perceived
consequences of confession. Glossing over the specifics, interrogation
is reducible to an interplay of three processes: isolation for some
215
Investigative Interviewing
216
A critical appraisal of modern police interrogations
(e.g. M eyer and Youngjohn 1991; Kassin and Kiechel 1996). Especially
disconcerting in this regard is the role that the polygraph has played.
The polygraph is best know n for its use as a lie-detector test but,
because it is not adm issible in m ost courts, police use it prim arily to
induce suspects to confess. Far too often, however, false confessions
have been extracted by police exam iners who told suspects they had
failed a lie-detector test. This tactic is so com m on that Lykken (1998:
235) coined the term 'fourth degree' to describe it. This problem
recently led the N ational Research Council Com m ittee to Review the
Scientific Evidence on the Polygraph to warn of the risk of polygraph-
induced false confessions (N ational Research Council 2003).
A third potential problem concerns the use of m inim ization, the
process by which the police suggest to a suspect that the crime in
question was provoked, an accident or otherw ise m orally justified.
By design, m inim ization tactics lead people to infer that they will be
treated with leniency if they confess - even when no explicit promises
are made (Kassin and M cNall 1991). In the laboratory, this tactic led
18 per cent of innocent college students to confess that they cheated
on a problem that they were supposed to solve w ithout assistance
(Russano et al. 2005). Although more work is needed to com pare
the different alternative them es and the conditions under which this
tactic puts innocent people at risk, it appears that m inim ization - by
com m unicating leniency 'under the rad ar' - may at times induce
confessions in suspects who are beleaguered and feeling trapped,
even if innocent.
Taking stock of what psychological science has, and has not,
achieved w hen it com es to police interrogations, it is clear that
researchers have thus far sought to identify the risks, with an eye
towards reducing the num ber of false confessions and wrongful
convictions. To develop fully a science of interrogation, however,
researchers m ust also help the police to build a better m ousetrap. The
surgical objective is sim ple: develop interrogation techniques that are
'diagnostic' to the extent that they increase the observed ratio of true
to false confessions.
This objective brings w ith it som e im portant im plications. First,
because the decision to confess is largely influenced by a person's
expectations of the consequences, both guilty and innocent people
are m ost likely to capitulate when they believe that there is strong
evidence against them (M oston et al. 1992). As the police are more
likely in nature to have direct and circum stantial proof of guilt against
perpetrators than against innocent suspects who are falsely accused,
the practice of confronting suspects w ith real evidence should increase
217
Investigative Interviewing
218
A critical appraisal of modern police interrogations
219
Investigative Interviewing
and half were false. This manipulation did reduce the overall num ber
of 'true' judgm ents am ongst investigators, but they were still not more
accurate than students or chance perform ance, only more confident.
W hen it com es to the assum ption that 'I'd know a false confession
if I saw one', there is a third reason for concern: real-life false
confessions, when elicited through a process of interrogation,
contain content cues that people associate with truth-telling. In m ost
docum ented false confessions, the statem ents ultim ately presented in
court are com pelling, as they often contain vivid and accurate details
about the crim e, the scene and the victim - details that can becom e
know n to an innocent suspect through the assistance of leading
interview questions, overheard conversations, photographs, visits
to the crime scene and other second-hand sources of inform ation
invisible to the naive observer. To further obfuscate m atters, many
confessions are textured with w hat I call 'elective' statem ents. Often
innocent suspects describe not just w hat they allegedly did, and
how they did it, but why - as they self-report on revenge, jealousy,
desperation, capitulation to peer pressure and other prototypical
m otives for crime. Som etim es they add apologies and expressions of
remorse. In some cases, innocent suspects will correct m inor errors
that appear in the written statem ents that are derived from them,
suggesting that they read, understood and verified the contents. To
the naive spectator, such statem ents appear to be voluntary, textured
with detail and the product of personal experience. Uninform ed,
however, this spectator m istakes illusion for reality, not realizing that
the taped confession is much like a Hollywood drama - scripted by
the police theory of the case, rehearsed during hours of unrecorded
questioning, directed by the questioner and ultim ately enacted on
paper, tape or cam era by the suspect.
The Reid Technique offers advice on how to create these illusions
of credibility. Inbau et al. (2001) recomm end that interrogators
insert m inor errors (such as a wrong nam e, date or street address)
into written confessions so that the suspect will spot them, correct
them and initial the changes. The goal is to increase the perceived
credibility of the statem ent and m ake it difficult for the defendant
later to distance him or herself from it. Because only perpetrators
should be in a position to spot these errors, this technique appears
to have great potential. However, Inbau et al. advise that, to play
it safe, 'the investigator should keep the errors in mind and raise
a question about them in the event the suspect neglects to do so'
(p. 384). Similarly, they advise detectives to insert into written
confessions irrelevant personal history item s know n only to the
220
A critical appraisal of modern police interrogations
'offend er'. 'For instance, the suspect may be asked to give the name
of the grade school he attended, the place or hospital in which he
w as born, or other sim ilar inform ation' (p. 383). O f course, for the
suspect who is not the offender but an innocent person, the insertion
of neutral, crim e-irrelevant biographical details from his or her own
life has no diagnostic value. Like the error correction trick, how ever
it merely creates a false illusion of credibility.
221
Investigative Interviewing
222
A critical appraisal of modern police interrogations
half empty. In the light of all that is known about the problems with
eyewitness memory, it is not reasonable to expect perfection in the
accounts of crime suspects. This assertion, however, invites a third
analytical step, an attribution as to the source of the accurate details.
A confession can prove guilt if it contains details knowable only to
the perpetrator, details not derivable by second-hand sources. Yet
in the jogger case, after dozens of collective hours of unrecorded
questioning, and amidst disputes as to what transpired, there is no
way to know whether crime facts were furnished to the defendants,
wittingly or unwittingly, through the process. Indeed, one need not
stray from the videotaped confessions to hear the prosecutor ask
leading questions that functioned not only to elicit information from
the suspects but to communicate information to the suspects. Without
apparent regard for the ownership of the facts being extracted, she
steered one boy's story through a broken but persistent sequence of
leading questions: 'M edical evidence says something other than a
hand was u se d ... what?' and 'D on't you remember someone using a
brick or a stone?' In a move that grossly undermined all opportunity
to get a confession indicative of guilty knowledge, the detectives
inexplicably took one suspect on a supervised visit to the crime
scene before taking his videotaped confession. The district attorney
then showed him graphic photographs of the victim. For diagnostic
purposes, it makes no sense to contaminate a suspect's confession
by spoon feeding him information in these ways, rendering the
source of his subsequent knowledge ambiguous. Whether he was
there or not, the visit and photographs endowed him with key
visual facts about the victim, crime and place - facts fit for a full
confession. Importantly, Inbau et al. (2001) advise police to withhold
key crime details so that they can ask suspects to corroborate their
admissions.
Crime perpetrators have the unique capacity to reveal information
about their actions that the police did not already know and produce
evidence that police did not already have. Yet the statements of the
Central Park jogger defendants - individually and collectively - were
not generative in these ways. Lacking such corroboration, the case
against the five defendants was like a house of cards, with each
boy's confession built squarely and solely upon the foundation of the
others' confessions. In December 2002, this house of cards collapsed
under the weight of an imprisoned serial rapist who voluntarily
confessed to the attack, who furnished the police with crime facts
that proved accurate and not previously known, and whose semen
was present on the jogger.
223
Investigative Interviewing
T o w a rd s th e v id e o ta p in g o f in te rro g a tio n s
224
A critical appraisal of modern police interrogations
N o te
R e fe re n ce s
225
Investigative Interviewing
226
A critical appraisal o f m odern police interrogations
Leo, R.A. (1996) 'Inside the interrogation room', The Journal o f Criminal Lazv
and Criminology, 86: 266-303.
Leo, R.A. (2004) T h e third degree', in G.D. Lassiter (ed.) Interrogations,
Confessions, and Entrapment. New York, NY: Kluwer Academic.
Lykken, D.T. (1998) A Tremor in the Blood: Uses and Abuses o f the Lie Detector.
Reading, MA: Perseus Books.
McNatt, D.B. (2000) 'Ancient Pygmalion joins contemporary management: a
meta-analysis of the result', Journal o f Applied Psychology, 85: 314-322.
Meissner, C.A. and Kassin, S.M. (2002) '"H e's guilty!" Investigator bias in
judgments of truth and deception', Lazv and Human Behavior, 26: 469-80.
Meissner, C.A. and Kassin, S.M. (2004) '"You're guilty, so just confess!"
Cognitive and behavioral confirmation biases in the interrogation room',
in D. Lassiter (ed.) Interrogations, Confessions, and Entrapment. New York,
NY: Kluwer Academic/Plenum .
Memon, A., Vrij, A. and Bull, R. (2003) Psychology and Lazv: Truthfulness,
Accuracy and Credibility. London: Jossey-Bass.
Meyer, R.G. and Youngjohn, J.R. (1991) 'Effects of feedback and validity
expectancy on response in a lie detector interview', Forensic Reports, 4:
235-44.
Milne, R. and Bull, R. (1999) Investigative Interviewing: Psychology and Practice.
New York, NY: John Wiley.
Moston, S., Stephenson, G.M. and Williamson, T.M. (1992) 'The effects of case
characteristics on suspect behaviour during questioning', British Journal o f
Criminology, 32: 23-40.
National Research Council, Committee to Review the Scientific Evidence on
the Polygraph, Division of Behavioral and Social Sciences and Education
(2003) The Polygraph and Lie Detection. Washington, DC: National Academies
Press.
Newman, M.L., Pennebaker, J.W., Berry, D.S. and Richards, J.M. (2003) 'Lying
words: predicting deception from linguistic styles', Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin, 29: 665-75.
Nickerson, R.S. (1998) 'Confirmation bias: a ubiquitous phenomenon in many
guises', Reviezv o f General Psychology, 2: 175-220.
Ofshe, R.J. and Leo, R.A. (1997) 'The decision to confess falsely: rational
choice and irrational action', Denver University Lazo Reviezv, 74: 979-1122.
Risinger, D.M., Saks, M.J., Thompson, W.C. and Rosenthal, R. (2002)
'The Daubert/Kumho implications of observer effects in forensic science:
hidden problems of expectation and suggestion', California Lazv Reviezv,
90: 1-56.
Rosenthal, R. and Jacobson, L. (1968) Pygmalion in the Classroom: Teacher
Expectation and Pupils' Intellectual Development. New York, NY: Holt,
Rinehart & Winston.
Russano, M.B., Meissner, C.A., Narchet, F.M. and Kassin, S.M. (2005)
'Investigating true and false confessions within a novel experimental
paradigm', Psychological Science, 16: 481-86.
227
Investigative Interviewing
Scheck, B., Neufeld, P. and Dwyer, J. (2000) Actual Innocence. Garden City,
NY: Doubleday.
Simon, D. (1991) Homicide: A Year on the Killing Streets. New York, NY: Ivy
Books.
Sullivan, T.P. (2004) Police Experiences with Recording Custodial Interrogations.
Chicago, IL: Northwestern University School of Law, Center on Wrongful
Convictions.
Vrij, A. (2000) Detecting Lies and Deceit: The Psycholog}/ o f Lying and the
Implications fo r Professional Practice. London: Wiley.
Vrij, A. (2004) 'Why professionals fail to catch liars and how they can
improve', Legal and Criminal Psychology, 9: 159-81.
Vrij, A., Edward, K. and Bull, R. (2001) 'Police officers' ability to detect
deceit: the benefit of indirect deception detection measures', Legal and
Criminological Psychology, 6: 185-97.
Walczyk, J.J., Roper, K.S., Seemann, E. and Humphrey, A.M. (2003) 'Cognitive
mechanisms underlying lying to questions: response time as a cue to
deception', Applied Cognitive Psychology, 17: 755-74.
Zuckerman, M., DePaulo, B.M. and Rosenthal, R. (1981) 'Verbal and nonverbal
communication of deception', Advances in Experimental Social Psychology,
14: 1-59.
228
C h ap ter 12
T h e Im p ro v in g In te rp e rs o n a l E v a lu a tio n s fo r L aw E n fo rc e m e n t
and N a tio n a l S e c u rity te c h n iq u e
229
Investigative Interviewing
230
Investigative interviewing and the detection of deception
231
Investigative Interviewing
• Aw areness
• Baseline
• Changes
• Discrepancies
• Engagem ent
• Follow-up.
Awareness
232
Investigative interviewing and the detection of deception
an area that the investigator m ust becom e aware of are the cross-
cultural dynamics that develop in an interview. For exam ple, many
Asian cultures (such as Chinese) dem onstrate respect by not looking
directly into the eyes of the other. This is in contrast to many western
cultures, in which one is expected to look into the eyes of the person
one is talking to. Moreover, many of the physical traits described
earlier will interact with these cultural dynam ics - for example,
many cultures apply very strict age, gender a n d /o r generational
status rules. Regardless, an interview er who is aware of these issues
will be m uch less likely to m isinterpret the behaviours and accounts
presented to him or her.
Similarly, one m ust be aware of personality traits and particular
biases and how they may w ork against obtaining accurate
inform ation. An investigator who is aggressive and high energy may
interrupt his or her interview ee too m uch and thus disrupt his or
her account. If an investigator is aware of this trait, he or she can
develop techniques to nullify it. For exam ple, investigators may
teach them selves to allow for the silence, or to expect it, or they may
learn to listen w hilst positioning their hand over their m ouths and
gently grab their lip as a constant rem inder not to interrupt. These
sorts of skills were observed in the anecdotal study of effective
police officers, and laboratory research confirm s that more socially
com petent and sensitive people are more likely not to interrupt
others, particularly distressed individuals offering accounts of their
problem s (Christensen et al. 1980). Likewise, an interview er who is
aware of a bias against certain types of people - red-haired people,
snobby people or w hatever - would be able to take com pensatory
steps to insure he or she does not treat these individuals in way that
m ay put off the subject and serve as an obstacle to com m unication.
Baseline
233
Investigative Interviewing
Changes
Ekman (Ekman and Friesen 1969; Ekman 1985/2001) first noted that
detecting deceptive behaviours is most effectively done within the
subject - i.e. one must compare the subject to the subject (Ekman and
Friesen 1972, 1974; Ekman et al. 1976). The IIE approach adopted this
idea and built in training techniques to help investigators improve
their abilities to note not only the baseline behaviours, but also when
behaviours deviate from this baseline. These changes are essential
to understanding when an individual is convinced of the accuracy
of a statement he or she just uttered, uncertain of that statement
or even deliberately misleading with that statement. Moreover, the
IIE approach instructs investigators to label these changes from the
baseline behaviour as 'hotspots', rather than lies, although research
has shown that these hotspots can betray deception or concealed
emotion at rates greater than chance (Ekman and Frank 1993; Frank
and Ekman 1997; Ekman 1985/2001). At its core, a hotspot means
that the topic under discussion, or some segment of it, has caused the
subject to experience an emotion, or has caused the subject to have to
think hard on his or her feet. The IIE approach recognizes that there
are a number of reasons why a person would feel an emotion, or
be forced to think on his or her feet, besides lying. This is why IIE
stresses the phrase 'hotspot' when these behaviours are noted, rather
than lie - to force the interviewer to recognize this topic has caused
a reaction and, later, when the moment is right, to address that topic
to ascertain why the subject showed such a reaction to it. It could
be because the subject is lying, but it could be due to the topic
generating an emotion for a different reason. Ekman (1985/2001)
describes how a high US government official who was under
investigation showed a marked demeanour change when questioned
about an important lunch meeting with another official who had
violated US law. He pointed out that this change in demeanour could
be due to lying, but it could also be due to an argument he had with
his wife about lunch-related issues. Thus, the most accurate inference
when recognizing a hotspot is that it is strong evidence that the
subject is thinking on his or her feet, or is experiencing an emotional
reaction to the topic (Ekman 1985/2001). But it is still enormously
useful to the investigator who spots it because he or she can then
ask questions to ascertain the source of the hotspot, which in turn
may reveal an important area of inquiry that the interviewee may be
trying to avoid or conceal. The scientific basis for this is discussed
shortly.
234
Investigative interviewing and the detection of deception
Discrepancies
Discrepancies refer to the discrepancy not only within the verbal and
non-verbal communication channels, but also between the verbal and
non-verbal communication channels. For example, an investigator
observes a person issuing a statement about his or her kidnapped
child, stating he or she witnessed the kidnapping whilst exhibiting
the emotion of sadness. The IIE-trained interviewer would examine
this person's expressive behaviour looking for clear evidence of
discrepancies in the person's verbal and non-verbal behaviour, or
within the non-verbal elements of sadness such as looking for sadness
in the face, voice, and body. In a falsified sadness, not all these
channels will be consistent with the emotion of sadness. Likewise
across these communicative channels, a subject who shows a shrug
when he or she states 'I saw the whole thing', is discrepantly non
verbally communicating 'I am uncertain'. Or a subject who says 'no'
in response to a question, yet nods his or her head indicating 'yes',
is also showing discrepant behaviour. When these discrepancies are
noted, they too are classified as hotspots (Ekman 1985/2001). The
scientific basis for discrepancies being informative to the investigator
will be discussed shortly.
Engagem ent
235
Investigative Interviewing
the same course of study in school, have follow ed the sam e sports or
teams and so forth. Thus, in the process of talking w ith the subject an
interview er would identify areas of com m onality as m eans by which
he or she can establish this personal connection with the subject. This
then functions as a means to foster conversation betw een the two. It
is astounding how hum an beings will organize them selves into in
and out groups based on even the m ost trivial characteristics, and
that once som eone feels he or she has this in-group bond he or she
views the other quite differently (e.g. Tajfel 1978). Im agine meeting
som eone from your hom e town whilst on holiday in a very remote
location. One would typically feel som e special bond to this person
alm ost im m ediately (however, we would not feel this special bond
if we m et him or her w alking dow n the street of our hom e town
because this sim ilarity would not be salient). Regardless, one can
be made to feel some connection to total strangers by finding any
thread of commonality. Thus, an investigator who does this creates
a more com fortable environm ent for conversation - and particularly
for a truthful person who, odds are, will be nervous if he or she is
falsely suspected of a crime.
A nother technique to facilitate rapport is m irroring. Mirroring
refers to the active effort of the interview er to match the behaviours
of the interview ee. This is based upon the finding that two people
in rapport tend to exhibit postural and speech congruence (Charney
1966; LaFrance and Broadbent 1976; Trout and Rosenfeld 1980; Capella
1981; LaFrance 1979, 1985). Som e behaviours that an interview er may
m irror include seating posture, resting a hand on the chin or even
using the sam e level of vocabulary. Research show s that when people
are mirrored they feel much more com fortable with their interaction
partner and like him or her more than when they are not mirrored
(Chartrand and Bargh 1999). Moreover, not only do those in rapport
show congruent behaviours, but when they are com fortable with the
conversation they tend to synchronize their behaviours (Bernieri and
Rosenthal 1991). In fact, IIE suggests periodically testing rapport by
having the interview er adjust his or her position deliberately, to see if
the subject follows. IIE also warns against being too obvious in doing
this, for if a subject thinks an interview er is sim ply im itating him or
her, he or she m ay feel m ocked, and that will destroy rapport.
Another technique to building rapport - although limited by
jurisdiction and local custom - is to touch the subject. This means
an occasional gentle touch, in socially appropriate areas like the
forearm or upper arm, can facilitate rapport by dem onstrating that
the interview er feels em pathy and even likes the subject (or at least
236
Investigative interviewing and the detection of deception
237
Investigative Interviewing
the subject for more detail on specific issues. Words that connote
detail encourage the subject to sharpen the topic further, and include
words such as 'specifically' (as in 'tell me specifically about') or
'describe in detail your relationship'. The IIE approach also teaches
techniques for review ing the inform ation with the subject, obtaining
sequences of events and so forth, including techniques derived from
the cognitive interview (Geiselman et al. 1986) which have show n to
be effective in generating accurate recall.
Follow-up
238
Investigative interviewing and the detection of deception
Cognitive clues
For exam ple, in order to mislead som eone deliberately, a liar must
fabricate, distort or conceal facts, describe events that did not happen
or that he or she did not witness or suppress critical information.
However, the process of thinking about or creating this misinform ation
can leave behavioural signs. These signs range from a hesitation in
the speech, to a m isplaced word or a contradictory statem ent, to
very vague accounts with less logical structure (see DePaulo et al.
2003 for a review). These types of clues are particularly evident in
situations in which the liar should know exactly w hat he or she has
done w ithout having to think too m uch about it. A witness who
claim s to have been present at a crim e scene should be able to tell
the court, w ithout too much thought, where he or she was standing
w hen he or she witnessed the event. If the witness was not present
at that scene, he or she would have to create the details necessary to
convince som eone that he or she was there. This on-the-spot thinking,
research has show n, often m anifests itself in m any speech hesitations,
speech disfluencies and errors, often with few er of the hand or facial
gestures that typically illustrate speech (Ekman and Friesen 1972;
Ekm an et al. 1976; DePaulo et al. 1985).
The choice of words can also betray this on-the-spot thinking (e.g.
Stiff and M iller 1986). In sim ple terms, liars are less im m ediate, use
more general, sim ple words to recall and generate thoughts and use
less concrete words (reviewed by DePaulo et al. 2003). On top of this,
the deceptive witness would have to be very careful not to contradict
his or her statem ents m ade during his or her initial interview s. In
more precise terms, work by Yuille and colleagues on credibility
assessm ent (e.g. Porter and Yuille 1995, 1996; Yuille and Porter 2000)
identified a num ber of characteristics - up to 24 - that distinguished
an actual m em ory of an event from a fabricated m em ory of an
event. One exam ple of a characteristic includes w hether a subject
spontaneously reproduces dialogue in his or her account - it appears
real m em ories are more likely to feature such elements.
Em otional clues
239
Investigative Interviewing
can enter into the lie process in one of two ways - first, the person
testifying could be lying about his or her feelings or emotions, or,
secondly, the act of lying may produce feelings or emotions within
the liar. The same principles apply regardless of how the emotions
are initiated. Research has shown that when emotions are aroused,
changes are unbidden and occur automatically (reviewed by Ekman
2003). Subjectively people report that they do not choose which emotion
to experience, but instead report that emotions happen to them in
specific situations. We know this through our own experience. For
example, during times when we may feel blue, we do things that we
hope will make us feel better - we go for a walk, eat some calorie
laden food, rent a comedy video - but whether we actually feel
happy afterwards is never guaranteed. Likewise, if we have to give
an important presentation to the boss, and find ourselves nervous, it
is very hard simply to stop being nervous (despite the suggestions of
our well meaning friends). These changes occur within a split second
and are considered fundamental features of an emotional response
(Frijda 1986; Ekman 2003).
Research has shown that anger, disgust, fear, happiness, sadness,
and surprise each have unique physiological profiles (Ekman et al.
1983); moreover, when subjects are asked to pose facial expressions
of anger, disgust, fear, happiness, sadness and surprise, the same
patterns of emotion-specific physiology have been found (Levenson
et al. 1990). This same link between facial expressions and emotion-
specific physiology has also been found in different subject groups,
such as the elderly (Levenson et al. 1991), and in a matrilineal,
Muslim, non-western culture (Levenson et al. 1992). Research has also
found that specific emotions have specific central nervous system
(CNS) patterns of hemispheric brain activation, as measured by the
electroencephalogram (EEG).
However, part of this emotional response, besides changes in heart
rate, blood pressure, and so forth (Ekman et al. 1983; Levenson et
al. 1992), is a change in voice tone (Scherer 1984) as well as a facial
expression of that emotion (Izard 1994; Ekman 1994, 2003). In terms
of the voice, researchers note particular patterns in fundamental
frequency and amplitude that distinguish anger from fear and these
emotions from others, and there is some limited evidence that these
vocal profiles for emotions are universal across cultures (Scherer and
Walbott 1994). For example, in anger the fundamental frequency gets
lower (lower pitch), and the amplitude higher (i.e. louder), whereas
in fear the fundamental frequency gets higher (higher pitch) and the
amplitude softer (i.e. quieter).
240
Investigative interviewing and the detection of deception
241
Investigative Interviewing
242
Investigative interviewing and the detection of deception
243
Investigative Interviewing
im aging (fMRI) has show n that there are specific areas of the brain
that respond just to these em otional expressions. W hen researchers
show a photo of a facial expression of em otion (typically the same
photos used in the universality studies described earlier), they have
found that anger seems to activate the right orbitofrontal cortex and
anterior cingulate cortex (Blair et al. 1999). D isgust seem s to activate
the anterior insula and limbic cortico-striatal-thalam ic area (Phillips et
al. 1997). Fear seems to activate the left am ygdala (M orris et al. 1996;
W halen et al. 1998). Flappiness seem s to activate the left side of the
lateral frontal, mid-frontal, anterior tem poral and central anterior scalp
regions (Davidson et al. 1990; Ekman and Davidson 1993). Sadness
seem s to activate the left amygdala and right temporal lobe (Blair et
al. 1999). W hat this m eans is that hum ans appear to be biologically
wired to perceive these specific em otion facial expressions.
244
Investigative interviewing and the detection of deception
truth tellers at over 76 per cent accuracy (Frank and Ekm an 1997);
when voice m easures are added, this accuracy rises to 86.5 per cent
(Ekm an et al. 1991). Even more recent w ork by Frank and Ekman
(2005), using a counterterrorism paradigm , has pushed this up to 90
per cent accuracy. These behavioural signs are often obvious but, at
other times, occur so fleeting as to be micromomentary. Ekm an and
Friesen (1969) called these m icrom om entary expressions of emotion
'm icroexpressions', and the evidence suggests that they can be as
brief as Vi of a second. It also appears that only expert lie catchers
detect these m icroexpressions with any accuracy, w hereas average to
poor lie catchers m iss them (Ekm an and O 'Sullivan 1991; Ekman et
al. 1999; Frank and Ekman 1997; Frank 2005). These expressions are
often micro because in a situation where som eone is m otivated to
lie, they will attem pt to manage their facial behaviour so as to not
look 'guilty'. Thus, a 'tug of w ar' over control of the face ensues
betw een the subcortical, em otional regions and the cortical motor
strip. This attem pt to squelch the expressions m inim izes their scale
and duration. These m icroexpressions were first discovered using
Ekm an and Friesen's Facial Action Coding System - a laborious
technique for reliably coding all visible m uscle m ovem ent, not just
that presum ed to be related to em otion (Ekman and Friesen 1978).
It involves careful, back and forth view ing of behaviours that can
take up to 3 hours of coding time to code one m inute of behaviour.
However, this hard w ork did identify these em otional displays that
people with the naked eye did not see. Moreover, with as little as
30 m inutes of training, people can be taught to improve significantly
their abilities to spot m icroexpressions in real time (Ekman 2005;
Frank and Ekm an, 2005).
There are situations in w hich lies are betrayed by people who falsely
portray an em otion. One of the m ost argued-about aspects of the
M enendez case in California - where two brothers killed their well-
to-do parents but claim ed to have been driven to that act by their
parents' relentless physical and sexual abuse - involved whether the
displays of sadness by the two brothers when they described their
actions were real or feigned; that is, w hether their sadness w as a
genuine display of remorse over being forced, due to the years of
abuse, to kill these evil parents before their parents killed them;
or w hether it was the sim ulated sadness of two culpable siblings
designed to gain sym pathy from the judge and jury so they could
collect their sizeable inheritance.
245
Investigative Interviewing
246
Investigative interviewing and the detection of deception
besides the fear of being caught, a lying witness may show fear of
punishment - that is, punishment for the act upon which the lie was
designed to conceal. In other words, the stronger the punishments
for the crime, for perjury or getting caught in general, the more fear
a deceptive witness is likely to show (e.g. Frank and Ekman 1997).
Deception guilt
Deception guilt refers to a feeling about lying, and not the legal issue
of whether someone is guilty or innocent. Deception guilt refers to
the guilt felt when lying; for example, a witness may feel happiness
at helping out a friend by claiming that the defendant threw the first
punch, but later may feel guilty about lying. This situation can be
reversed as well - he or she may feel guilt about helping out the
friend, but feel no guilt about lying about it (or some can feel guilt
for both, and some for neither). What is important is that it is not
necessary to feel guilty about the content of a lie in order to feel
guilty about lying. Like fear of being caught, deception guilt can vary
in strength. For example, severe guilt can be a tortuous experience,
undermining the sufferer's most fundamental feelings of self-worth
(Ekman 1985/2001).
There are a number of factors which function to increase the amount
of guilt a liar might feel. First, it seems that there are people who
are particularly prone to guilt - for example, those who suffer from
generalized anxiety disorders. These individuals often have very strict
upbringings and have been severely punished for lying, or have been
led to believe that lying is one of the most severe sins. Conversely,
psychopaths - who have been reported to show no remorse or shame
and an incapacity for love - may be much harder to detect than the
average person due to their limited capacity to feel guilt (Hare 1970).
Secondly, a close relationship between the liar and the target of the
lie, such that they share values, respect each other and so forth, also
functions to augment guilt feelings. Conversely, a liar who does not
share values with the target would feel less guilt; and a witness who
despises the legal system may not feel guilty about lying in court,
much the same way in which a spy or terrorist feels no guilt about
lying to a representative of an enemy government. Finally, if the
target of the lie is impersonal or anonymous, then less deception guilt
is generally felt. A witness who lies to a videotape camera or in an
affidavit may feel less guilt than if he or she lied to an actual person.
However, guilt often causes people to rationalize their deceits, so the
witness may convince him or herself that the defendant had always
been a troublemaker and deserved to be arrested, even though the
defendant was not responsible for initiating the current altercation.
247
Investigative Interview ing
D u p in g delight
Lying can produce positive as well as negative emotions. The lie
may be viewed as a proud accom plishm ent. Peter Sutcliffe, the man
convicted of being the Yorkshire Ripper, expressed his delight whilst
he was twice interrogated and then dism issed by the police before
ultim ately being caught. There are a num ber of factors that may
cause an increase in duping delight - if the target is hard to fool, or
if there is an audience who is aware of the deception and enjoying
the perform ance. Thus, a lying witness may enjoy the fact that he or
she is sitting in centre stage of the courtroom , in front of his or her
friends, whilst he or she regales them with his or her bogus account
of an assault.
It should be noted that these em otions can occur sim ultaneously
or in any com bination. For exam ple, w itnesses may feel guilt over
producing a bogus account of the assault, or fear being caught
perjuring them selves, as well as a certain delight in being able
to pull off the lie in front of all these supposedly im portant legal
professionals.
Cautions
It m ust be noted that these thinking and feeling clues are just that
- clues that witnesses, defendants and victim s are thinking, or clues
that they are feeling or concealing som e emotion. To date no one has
been able to identify a hum an equivalent of a 'Pinocchio response' -
that is, there is no one behavioural sign or constellation of signs that,
across every person, in all situations, indicates that a person is lying.
Thus, a lie catcher who identifies the behavioural clues described
above m ust alw ays infer why a witness would show guilt, or fear
or delight, or why a witness would m ull som ething over. This is
why the concept of calling these incidents of em otional or cognitive
leakage 'hotspots' rather than lies is the preferred notation in IIE.
Som eone who judges leaked em otions or cognitive efforts as a lie
may be right, but may be wrong, as there can be other reasons why
som eone would feel fear or enjoym ent besides lying. Som eone who
judges these sam e behaviours as hotspots will be much less likely to
be wrong, and can use this evidence to gather even more evidence.
Moreover, the interview er who judges hotspot, rather than liar will
be more likely to continue to ask questions and gather inform ation,
rather than succum b to the human tendency to assum e if he or
she caught the person lying that person m ust be guilty - and this
prem ature conclusion can result in more haphazard and incom plete
inform ation collection from that point.
248
Investigative interviewing and the detection of deception
The IIE approach suggests the most effective way to detect deception
from behaviour is to look for changes in baseline - an expressive
person suddenly becomes much less expressive for a particular topic,
but then becomes expressive again for different topics - an d /o r to
note when the verbal and non-verbal information do not match -
subtle or micro happiness when talking about a murder, or fear when
talking about an innocuous topic like lunch, or even micro shrugging,
which indicates uncertainty, when talking about something he or she
claims he or she definitely did. The IIE approach trains investigators
to recognize these changes in baseline - or times when the verbal
and non-verbal behaviours are discrepant - and teaches investigators
to understand thoroughly why the person is showing these hotspots.
However, the IIE approach discourages investigators from making a
judgment of 'lie' when he or she sees these changes or discrepant
behaviours but, instead to make a judgment of hotspot. Given that
there are no universal clues to deceit, an investigator who renders a
judgment of 'lie' might be wrong; however, given the years of work
on emotion and cognition, an investigator who instead renders a
judgment of 'hot spot' will not be wrong. Moreover, a judgment of
hotspot encourages the investigator to keep gathering information,
whereas we have noted in the past that investigators who make a
judgment of liar tend to slack off in their information gathering,
because now they assume they have the right person - despite
evidence that shows most people, including trained law enforcement
officers, should exercise caution about their abilities to spot lies (Kraut
and Poe 1980; DePaulo and Pfeifer 1986; Ekman and O'Sullivan 1991;
Ekman et al. 1999).
The IIE approach recognizes that an oppressive, pressured push
towards a confession does not generate the best information or
make the strongest cases as they are put forward in a court of law.
It recognizes the importance of uninterrupted accounts. It recognizes
a careful consideration of the means of putting forth questions to
the subjects and also recognizes the use of behavioural indicators to
help generate areas of inquiry and to facilitate the vetting of such
information. However, as stated earlier, it views these behavioural
indicators and techniques to be a means towards helping the
investigator gather information, and not as evidence in and of
itself. The only way to know with 100 per cent certainty is to have
unimpeachable corroborating evidence. That sort of evidence is only
gained by a close examination of the physical evidence, and through
249
Investigative Interviewing
E p ilo g u e
In 2005 the scientists (deception experts, Ekm an, Frank and O 'Su lliv an,
and m em ory and verbal statem ent expert, Yuille) and m ost of the
law enforcem ent officers (Fretter, H arm s and Yarbrough) form ed a
separate organization called 'Im p roving Interpersonal Evaluations
for Law Enforcem ent and N ational Secu rity ', w ith the acronym IIE.
The prim ary focus continu es to be on interview ing and com bining
scientific know led ge w ith law enforcem ent experience. J.J. N ew berry
continu es to direct the Institute of A nalytic Interview ing, b u t w ithout
the participation of the individuals nam ed above.
R e fe re n c e s
250
Investigative interviewing and the detection o f deception
Collins, R., Lincoln, R. and Frank, M.G. (2002) 'The effect of rapport in
forensic interviewing', Psychiatry, Psychology, and Laiv, 9: 69-78.
Darwin, C. (1872/1998) The Expression o f the Emotions in Man and Animals.
New York, NY: Oxford University Press (3rd edn, with commentaries by
Paul Ekman).
Davidson, R.J. (1984) 'Affect, cognition and hemispheric specialization', in
C.E. Izard et al. (eds) Emotion, Cognition, and Behavior. New York, NY:
Cambridge University Press.
Davidson, R.J. (1992) 'Emotion and affective style: hemispheric substrates',
Psychological Science, 3: 39-43.
Davidson, R.J., Ekman, P., Saron, C., Senulius, J. and Friesen, W.V. (1990)
'Approach-withdrawal and cerebral asymmetry: emotional expression and
brain physiology I', journal o f Personality and Social Psychology, 58: 330-41.
Davidson, R.J. and Tomarken, A.J. (1989) 'Laterality and emotion: an
electrophysiological approach', in F. Boiler and J. Grafman (eds) Handbook
o f Neuropsychology. Amsterdam: Elsevier.
DeMyer, W. (1980) Technique o f the Neurological Examination. New York, NY:
McGraw-Hill.
DePaulo, B.M., Lindsay, J.J., Malone, B.E., Muhlenbruck, L., Charlton, K. and
Cooper, H. (2003) 'Cues to deception', Psychological Bulletin, 129: 74-112.
DePaulo, B.M. and Pfeifer, R.L. (1986) 'On-the-job experience and skill at
detecting deception', Journal o f Applied Social Psychology, 16: 249-67.
DePaulo, B.M., Stone, J. and Lassiter, D. (1985) 'Deceiving and detecting
deceit', in B.R. Schlenker (ed.) The S elf and Social life. New York, NY:
McGraw-Hill.
Dwyer, J., Neufeld, P. and Scheck, B. (2000) Actual Innocence: Five Days to
Execution and Other Dispatches from the Wrongly Convicted. New York, NY:
Doubleday.
Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1973) 'The expressive behavior of the deaf-and-blind born',
in M. von Cranach and I. Vine (eds) Social Communication and Movement.
San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Ekman, P. (1972) 'Universal and cultural differences in facial expressions of
emotion', in J.K. Cole (ed.) Nebraska symposium on Motivation 1971. Lincoln,
NE: University of Nebraska Press.
Ekman, P. (1977) 'Biological and cultural contributions to body and facial
movement', in J. Blacking (ed.) Anthropology o f the Body. London: Academic
Press.
Ekman, P. (1979) 'About brows: emotional and conversational signals', in
M. von Cranach et al. (eds) Human Ethology. Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.
Ekman, P. (1992) 'Facial expressions of emotion: new findings, new questions',
Psychological Science, 3: 34-8.
Ekman, P. (1994) 'Strong evidence for universals in facial expressions: a reply
to Russell's mistaken critique', Psychological Bulletin, 115: 268-87.
Ekman, P. (1985/2001) Telling Lies. New York, NY: Norton.
251
Investigative Interview ing
252
Investigative interviewing and the detection o f deception
253
Investigative Interviewing
Levenson, R.W., Ekman, P., Heider, K. and Friesen, W.V. (1992) 'Emotion and
autonomic nervous system activity in the Minangkabau of West Sumatra',
Journal o f Personality and Social Psychology, 62: 972-88.
Meihlke, A. (1973) Surgery o f the Facial Nerve. Philadelphia, PA: Saunders.
Moore, M.M. (1985) 'Nonverbal courtship patterns in women: content and
consequences', Ethology and Sociobiology, 6: 237-47.
Morris, J.S., Frith, C.D., Perrett, D.I., Rowland, D., Young, A.W., Calder,
A.J. and Dolan, R.J. (1996) 'A differential neural response in the
human amygdala to fearful and happy facial expressions', Nature, 383:
812-15.
Myers, R.E. (1976) 'Comparative neurology of vocalization and speech: proof
of a dichotomy', Annual Review o f the New York Academy o f Sciences, 280:
745-57.
Newberry, J.J. (1999) The Analytic Interview Manual. Unpublished manual.
O'Sullivan, M. (2005) 'Emotional intelligence and deception detection: why
most people can't "read" others, but a few can', in R.E. Riggio and
R.S. Feldman (eds) Applications o f Nonverbal Communication. Mahwah, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Phillips, M.L., Young, A.W., Senior, C., Brammer, M., Andrew, C., Calder,
A.J., Bullmore, E.T., Perrett, D.I., Rowland, D., Williams, S.C.R., Gray, J.A.
and David, A.S. (1997) 'A specific neural substrate for perceiving facial
expressions of disgust', Nature, 389: 495-8.
Plutchik, R. (1962) The Emotions: Facts, Theories, and a New Model. New York,
NY: Random House.
Porter, S. and Yuille, J.C. (1995) 'Credibility assessment of criminal suspects
through statement analysis', Psychology, Crime and Law, 1: 319-31.
Porter, S. and Yuille, J.C. (1996) 'The language of deceit: an investigation of
the verbal clues in the interrogation context', Law and Human Behavior, 20:
443-58.
Rinn, W.E. (1984) 'The neuropsychology of facial expression: a review of
the neurological and psychological mechanisms for producing facial
expressions', Psychological Bulletin, 95: 52-77.
Scherer, K. (1984) 'On the nature and function of emotions: a component
process approach', in K. Scherer and P. Ekman (eds) Approaches to Emotion.
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Scherer, K.R. and Walbott, H.G. (1994) 'Evidence for universality and cultural
variation of differential emotion response patterning', Journal o f Personality
and Social Psychology, 66: 310-28.
Stiff, J.B. and Miller, G.R. (1986) '"Com e to think of it ...": interrogative
probes, deceptive communication, and deception detection', Human
Communication Research, 12: 339-57.
Tajfel, H. (1978) Differentiation between Social Groups: Studies in the Social
Psychology o f Intergroup Relations. Oxford: Academic Press.
Tickle-Degnen, L. and Rosenthal, R. (1990) 'The nature of rapport and its
nonverbal correlates', Psychological Inquiry, 1: 285-93.
254
Investigative interviewing and the detection o f deception
Tomkins, S.S. (1962) Affect, Imagery, Consciousness (Vol. 1, The Positive Affects).
New York, NY: Springer.
Tomkins, S.S. (1963) Affect, Imagery, Consciousness (Vol. 2, The Negative Affects).
New York, NY: Springer.
Trout, D.L. and Rosenfeld, H.M. (1980) T h e effect of posture lean and body
congruence on the judgment of psychotherapeutic rapport', Journal o f
Nonverbal Behavior, 4: 176-90.
Tschiassny, K. (1953) 'Eight syndromes of facial paralysis and their significance
in locating the lesion', Annual Review o f Otology, Rhinology, and Laryngology,
62: 677-91.
Webb, N.C. (1974) T h e use of myoelectric feedback in teaching facial
expression to the blind', American Foundation fo r the BlindResearch Bulletin,
27: 231-62.
Whalen, P.J., Rauch, S.L., Etcoff, N.L., Mclnerney, S.C., Lee, M.B. and
Jenike, M.A. (1998) 'Masked presentations of emotional facial expressions
modulate amygdala activity without explicit knowledge', The Journal o f
Neuroscience, 18: 411-18.
Yuille, J.C. (1989) Credibility Assessment. New York, NY: Kluwer Academ ic/
Plenum Publishers.
Yuille, J.C. and Porter, S. (2000) 'Deception', in A.E. Kazdin (ed.) Encyclopedia
o f Psychology. Vol. 2. London: American Psychological Association, Oxford
University Press.
255
Part 3
Developments in Regulation
C h ap ter 13
Recovered memories
James Ost
T h e m e m o ry w a rs
259
Investigative Interviewing
individuals like Alice to sue, or bring crim inal charges against, their
parents (or other alleged abusers) where the only evidence was
previously 'repressed' or 'dissociated' m em ories of childhood abuse
that individual had allegedly 'recovered' in adulthood (Underw ager
and Wakefield 1998). One problem with allow ing such testim ony is
that there is, in fact, no reliable evidence that individuals 'repress' or
'dissociate' m em ories of traum atic events (although they may choose
not to report such events; see M cN ally 2003; cf. Brown et al. 1998). A
further problem is that, as w e shall see in this chapter, research has
show n that it is possible for people to com e to report com pelling and
vivid 'm em ories' of events that never occurred (Hym an et al. 1995;
Loftus and Pickrell 1995; Pezdek et al. 1997; Porter et al. 1999; Ost et
al. in press). This raises the serious possibility that at least some of
these 'recovered m em ories' might, in fact, be iatrogenic productions
of the therapeutic process itself (hence the term 'false m em ories').
Indeed, there have been a num ber of high-profile m alpractice
cases in the USA where patients have taken legal action against their
form er therapists, accusing them of im planting 'false' m em ories of
abuse, som etim es w inning considerable damage settlem ents (see
Loftus 1997). Furtherm ore, in one case an accused father was allowed,
as a third party, to bring m alpractice charges against his daughter's
form er therapist and was awarded $500,000 in dam ages (see Johnston
1997). In a recent case in the UK, the General Medical Council (GMC)
disciplined a general practitioner for using inappropriate questions
and suggestions to lead a 13-year-old patient to believe falsely
that she had been sexually assaulted (Catchpole 2003). These, and
other cases like them, highlight the im portance of raising awareness
am ongst practitioners and policy-m akers of the issues surrounding
such cases in order that potential m iscarriages of justice are avoided
and that genuine victims of abuse receive the support they need.
However, the issues are far from straightforward.
One concern is that claim s of childhood abuse are som etim es
made follow ing, or during, an individual's participation in so-called
'recovered m em ory therapy'. Recovered m emory therapy, although a
contentious term to some, is a blanket term covering any therapeutic
treatment in which the prim e goal is to uncover repressed, dissociated
or otherw ise unavailable 'm em ories' of traum a, in order to resolve
present-day psychological problem s (Lindsay and Read 1994, 2001).
Professional opinion, however, is sharply divided over the risks
associated with such therapy. On the one hand there are researchers
who claim that certain traum atic experiences are perm anently stored
in one form or another, that it is possible to revive 'm em ories' of
260
Recovered memories
these long-forgotten events and that such 'm em ories' are generally
accurate (see Brown et al. 1998; Cam eron 1996; Freyd 1998; Salter
1998). If this argum ent is wrong then fam ilies can be torn apart,
individuals falsely branded as paedophiles and, som etim es, wrongly
incarcerated (see Pendergrast 1996) on the basis of such 'recovered
m em ories'. On the other hand are researchers who claim that some
of these 'recovered m em ories' arose as a result of inappropriate and
highly suggestive therapeutic techniques (Brandon et al. 1997; Hyman
and Loftus 1997; Tsai et al. 2000; Lindsay and Read 1994, 2001; Lynn
et al. 2003; see also Hyman 2000). But if this claim is wrong the
results are equally as tragic, not only in individual cases, but also at
a w ider level. As Conw ay (1997) states, one serious concern is that
genuine victim s of childhood sexual assault will be less w illing to
com e forward if there is a risk that their testim ony may be dismissed
as a case of 'false m em ory'. Despite the polarized nature of the debate
there is some evidence that a 'm iddle ground' is em erging (Read
1999; Shobe and Schooler 2001; Ost 2003) with researchers on both
sides acknow ledging the possibility that som e long-delayed claims
of childhood abuse are genuine, w hilst some are not. W hilst it is
unethical to test the specific claim that participants can be led falsely
to report that they were victim s of abuse, a num ber of laboratory
m ethods have been developed to exam ine how, and under what
circum stances, people m ight com e falsely to report other, som etim es
traum atic, events from their past. The purpose of this chapter is to
review critically 1) laboratory studies that have exam ined how people
can com e to report 'm em ories' of childhood events that did not
occur; 2) the effect of certain 'm em ory recovery' techniques on the
accuracy of m emory reports; and 3) the possible effects of support
groups on rem em bering. We will see that, although m uch research is
still required, the available evidence clearly show s that it is possible,
under certain circum stances, for people to com e to report falsely that
they rem em ber entire events that did not occur, or that they did not
experience.
T h r e e m a in ‘false m e m o r y ’ m e th o d s
261
Investigative Interviewing
and w eaknesses with each method. For exam ple, whilst the parental
m isinform ation method appears to be directly m easuring the
likelihood that an individual will report an entire memory of an
event he or she did not experience, the DRM method exam ines the
likelihood that an individual w ill falsely recognize a lure as being
part of a previously presented list of sem antically related items. This
means that these m ethods do not speak equally to the question of
w hether an individual can com e to report that he or she remembers
an entirely false, autobiographical, em otionally charged childhood
event (see Sm eets et al. in press). There is also a further problem atic
distinction concerning w hether these m ethods are tapping into, or
changing, memories of past events, beliefs about past events, confidence
about w hether a past event occurred or not, or sim ply reports about
past events (Sm eets et al. in press; see also Ost 2003; Loftus and
Bernstein 2005). Furthermore, whilst it has been argued that a false
belief is an im portant (and necessary) precursor to developing a false
memory (Gudjonnson 2003), it does not follow that a false belief will
alw ays lead to a false m em ory (Ost 2003). Therefore some caution is
w arranted in interpreting these findings as a w hole. N evertheless, all
three m ethods are widely cited in the literature as providing evidence
that 'false m em ories' can occur. With these im portant caveats in mind,
each method will now be critically examined.
The D R M m ethod
262
Recovered memories
Winograd et al. (1998) found that participants who scored more highly
on m easures of dissociation (high scorers on this measure have a
tendency to experience problem s in the integration of thoughts and
feelings) and vividness of mental imagery (high scorers report having
more vivid im aginative abilities) were more likely to claim to rem em ber
the critical non-presented words. However, as Freyd and Gleaves
(1996) note, there are im portant differences betw een m isrem em bering
words that have not been presented in a list and m isrem em bering
an otherw ise happy childhood as being abusive. Similarly, Wilkinson
and Hyman (1998) dem onstrated that, in laboratory experim ents,
there are im portant differences betw een participants' perform ance on
w ord-list tasks and their perform ance on autobiographical memory
tasks. They found that self-reported dissociative tendencies were
related to errors on both the word list and autobiographical memory,
but that self-reported vividness of mental imagery was only related
to errors on the w ord-list (DRM) task. W ilkinson and Hym an (1998)
argue that this is because rem em bering words and rem em bering
autobiographical events rely on different underlying psychological
processes. They argue that it is therefore unwise to assum e that
participants who are susceptible to the DRM are also more vulnerable
to developing false autobiographical memories.
The second method that psychologists have used exam ines whether
subtle changes in the way in which questions are asked about an
event can change what participants subsequently claim to remember
about that event. The classic studies of the m isinform ation effect,
conducted by Loftus and colleagues, examined w hether m isleading
post-event inform ation could alter eyew itness' m em ories of events
they had recently w itnessed (Loftus 1979). In two studies by Loftus
and Palm er (1974), participants were show n a film of an accident
involving two cars. Participants were then questioned to find out
how m uch they could rem em ber about the event. Loftus and Palm er
found that the question 'A bout how fast were the cars going when
they sm ashed into each other?' elicited higher estim ates of speed than
questions in which the verbs collided, bumped, contacted or hit were
used (see also Loftus et al. 1978). This is a robust and easily replicated
effect although there are still disagreem ents as to the m echanism s
that cause the m isinform ation effect (e.g. Bekerian and Bow ers 1983;
M cCloskey and Zaragoza 1985; Zaragoza et al. 1987; W eingardt et al.
1995). N evertheless it seem s that subtle changes in wording can lead
263
Investigative Interviewing
264
Recovered memories
The studies m entioned above show that some individuals will, when
misled by subtle changes in wording, or by a confederate, com e to
report that they rem em ber events (or details of events) that they
did not witness. However, they are limited in generalizability when
applied to cases of allegedly false or recovered m em ory of childhood
events. The events in the Crom bag et al., O st et al. and Granhag et
al. studies were all relatively recent, and had occurred whilst the
participants in their studies were adults. In contrast m ost delayed
claim s of childhood abuse concern events that, by definition, occurred
many years beforehand (see Pendergrast 1996). Can individuals be
misled to report false events from their childhood?
Loftus and Coan (cited in Loftus and Pickrell 1995) describe a
study in which a 14-year-old boy (Chris) was asked to recall details
over five days regarding four events involving fam ily members.
One of the events was false and three of them were true (as verified
by the family). Chris was interview ed in the presence of a sibling
(who was a confederate of the investigators) about these events. The
sibling provided verbal corroboration that all the events (including
the false event) had taken place. Over time Chris began to report
more about the four events, even rating the false event (becoming
lost in a shopping mall as a child) as more likely to have occurred
than all but one of the three true events.
Loftus and Pickrell (1995) replicated this effect with a larger sample
of undergraduate students. In this study participants were asked to
com plete a booklet concerning four events, the third of which was
false (becom ing lost in a shopping mall). Parents, w'ho confirmed that
their child had never becom e lost in a shopping m all as a child, also
provided details of the real events. Participants were interview ed
three times over three weeks and also asked, betw een interview s,
to write down in their booklets anything that came to mind about
the events. Loftus and Pickrell (1995) found that after three weeks,
6 out of 24 participants (25 per cent) erroneously believed part or all
of the false event. However, as Pezdek et al. (1997) argue, becom ing
lost in a shopping mall is a fairly com m on event that many people
w ould have a 'script' (or 'schem a') for such an event around which
265
Investigative Interviewing
266
Recovered memories
267
Investigative Interviewing
There are, however, several criticisms of the above studies that limit
their generalizability to cases of delayed reports of childhood trauma.
As already noted, Pezdek et al. (1997) argue that most of the false
events that participants are asked to recall are events for which
participants are likely to have a 'script'. An event that is lower in
plausibility, and for which participants are less likely to have 'script
relevant' knowledge (such as childhood sexual abuse), is less likely
to be falsely reported.
Pezdek et al. (1997) tested this hypothesis by suggesting to Jewish
and Catholic participants that they had taken part in both a Catholic
ritual (receiving Communion) and a Jewish ritual (Shabbot), neither
of which had actually occurred. Pezdek et al. (1997) argue that the
plausibility of having taken part in a Catholic ritual would be low
for Jewish participants, that they would have less script-relevant
knowledge to draw upon and vice versa. They found, in line with
their predictions, that seven of the Catholic participants but none of
the Jewish participants reported the false Catholic event and three
Jewish participants and one Catholic participant reported the Jewish
false event. This shows that participants were more likely to remember
the plausible false event than the implausible false event (i.e. Jewish
participants were more likely to remember the false Jewish ritual than
the false Catholic ritual). In a second experiment, Pezdek et al. (1997)
replicated the study by Loftus and Pickrell (1995) and extended it by
suggesting to participants that they had 1) been lost in a shopping
mall (a plausible event); and 2) that they had received an enema as
a child (an implausible event). Again, all events were suggested as
having occurred by a close relative or sibling who confirmed that no
events of this kind had ever occurred to the participants. Pezdek et
al. (1997) found that whilst 3 out of 20 participants falsely reported
becoming lost in a shopping mall, none of the participants falsely
reported receiving an enema. These experiments therefore suggest that
the probability that participants can be misled to report false events
from their childhood is likely to be a function of the plausibility of,
or familiarity with, the event concerned (although see Scoboria et
al. 2004 for a discussion of the differences between plausibility and
script-consistency).
268
Recovered memories
269
Investigative Interviewing
their false beliefs that they initiated legal proceedings against their
alleged abusers (de Rivera 1998; Ost et al. 2001, 2002a). Worryingly,
retractors reported that they experienced levels of social pressure, and
inappropriate questioning techniques, not dissimilar to those that can
lead to false confessions in police interrogations (Wrightsman and
Kassin 1993; Kassin 1997, 2005; Ost, et al. 2001; Gudjonnsson 2003).
A fourth limitation with parental misinformation studies is that it
is not clear what degree of social pressure is required in order to
lead participants to make false claims about the past (Ost et al. in
press). For example, Loftus and Pickrell (1995: 722) note that their
interviewer 'maintained a pleasant and friendly manner, whilst
pressing for details' yet do not provide details of how participants
were 'pressed for details'. Porter et al. (1999) state that they employed
a degree of verbal encouragement ('most people can remember details
if they try really hard'; 522) but the specific effects of this part of
their methodology are not clear. Malinoski and Lynn (1999) found
that positive verbal encouragement led participants to report earlier
(and more implausible) memories. Ost et al. (in press) found that,
even when social pressure was kept to a minimum (by appropriately
training interviewers not to pressure participants), a number of
participants still came to report false events from their childhood (see
also Erdmann et al. 2004 for similar findings with child witnesses).
The role of social pressure in the genesis and development of false
reports of childhood events is an important avenue for future research
(Ost et al. 2001 in press).
A final limitation with the parental misinformation studies concerns
the complexity of the experimental set-up that is required:
W hilst the limits to generalizability are well noted there are also a
number of similarities between the experimental and therapeutic
contexts that mean some conclusions can be drawn (Hyman and
Loftus 1997; Bekerian and O'Neill 2001; see also Ost 2003). For
270
Recovered memories
exam ple, in both cases there are strong social dem ands to remember,
either from a participant's m otivation to be a good experim ental
participant (see Ost et al. 2002a), or from a client's m otivation to get
better from whatever current problem s he or she is suffering.
In addition to the social dem ands to rem em ber in both the
experim ental and therapeutic contexts, therapists som etim es suggest
that certain techniques m ight help individuals to 'recover' m emories
of childhood sexual abuse (referred to by Lindsay and Read 1994 as
m em ory recovery therapy, or MRT). Exam ples of such techniques
are: hypnosis; guided im agery; the use of fam ily photographs;
instructions to give your im agination free rein; dream interpretation;
interpreting physical sym ptom s; w ritin g/artw ork; and participating
in support groups w ith other individuals who have 'recovered'
m em ories of abuse (see also Poole et al. 1995; Andrew s 2001). Poole
et al. (1995) surveyed British and North Am erican practitioners and
found that over two thirds of them used such techniques in order
to assist clients recover suspected m em ories of childhood sexual
abuse. A sim ilar survey by Andrew s (2001) found that 67 per cent
of a British sam ple of practitioners reported using such techniques
either before, or after, the client reported his or her first m em ory of
abuse although, as Andrew s (2001) notes, in only 21.5 per cent of the
cases reported in this survey were the techniques used before a client
reported any m em ory of abuse. As Hyman and Loftus (1997) note, in
some cases such techniques m ight act as appropriate m em ory cues in
order to help clients recover m em ories they had not thought about
for a long time. However, they also strongly caution that many of
the techniques could lead to false m em ories of abuse by, for exam ple,
encouraging clients to im agine abuse occurring. The problem with
this, as outlined below, is that it m ay encourage clients to evaluate
their recovered m emories using very lax source-m onitoring criteria.
The following section exam ines research that has investigated the
effects, that various m em ory recovery techniques have on reports of
past events (for another recent review, see Lynn et al. 2003).
M e m o r y re c o v e ry te c h n iq u e s
H ypnosis
271
Investigative Interviewing
272
Recovered memories
Guided imagery
Take an event in your family history that you can never actually
find out about. It could be your father's childhood or the
circumstances in your m other's life that kept her from protecting
you. Using all the details you do know, create your own story.
Ground the experience or event in as much knowledge as you
have and then let yourself imagine what actually might have
happened.
273
Investigative Interviewing
asked to think about events they could not remember. Hyman and
Pentland (1996) found that 40 per cent of participants who were
asked to imagine the event created a false memory, compared with
15 per cent of participants who were simply asked to think about
the event. One possible reason for this, as Hyman and Loftus (1997)
argue, is that people generally do not monitor the source of an image
that comes to mind. In other words, people may confuse something
they imagined for something that actually happened in the past
(Johnson 1988). Furthermore, as Hyman and Loftus (1997) note, we
usually associate vivid mental images as being indicators of accurate
memories (Johnson 1988; Johnson et al. 1988). Indeed Hyman and
Billings (1998) found that a self-reported tendency to experience vivid
mental imagery (as well as a self-reported tendency to dissociate)
was related to the creation of false reports of childhood events.
In fact, people can report very vivid 'm emories' that are totally
inaccurate even without instructions to imagine. For example, a
number of studies have been conducted on so-called 'flashbulb
memories', in which individuals claim to have vivid recollections of
highly charged events (usually surrounding the death of a famous
person such as John F. Kennedy or Princess Diana; see Conway
1995). The results of these studies have shown that, despite claiming
to remember them vividly, people sometimes misremember these
events. For example, Neisser and Harsch (1992) studied undergraduate
students' recollections of how they heard the news about the explosion
of the Challenger space shuttle in 1986. Over a hundred students
completed a questionnaire on the morning after the explosion, which
asked them to provide details such as 'where were you', 'who told
you' and 'what time was it'. Two and a half years later, 44 of these
undergraduates were recontacted and asked to complete the same
questionnaire for a second time. Neisser and Harsch (1992) compared
participants' responses to these two questionnaires and found that, in
fact, only three participants gave the same information on the second
questionnaire as they had on the original questionnaire completed on
the morning after the explosion. Eleven of the 44 participants were
wrong about every single aspect of the information they had provided
in their original questionnaire. Neisser and Harsch (1992) then
invited 40 of these participants to attend an interview during which
their attention was drawn to discrepancies between their responses
to the two questionnaires. Many participants were surprised when
confronted with their own original reports and the responses they
had given to the second questionnaire two years later. One participant
was so surprised by the discrepancy between the two accounts that
274
Recovered memories
Im agination inflation
275
Investigative Interviewing
D ream interpretation
276
Recovered memories
277
Investigative Interviewing
Wade et al. (2002) had created this false photograph using com puter
software. After three interview s 50 per cent of participants reported
'partial' or 'clear' false 'm em ories' of having been taken for a ride in
a hot-air balloon.
Although this study dem onstrates that photographs are powerful
cues that can lead individuals to rem em ber events that did not occur
the obvious limitation is that individuals are unlikely to encounter
doctored photographs of them selves (especially in a therapeutic
context; Lindsay et al. 2004). However, even real, undoctored
photographs are pow erful cues that can lead individuals to recall
events that did not occur. Lindsay et al. (2004) asked participants
about a num ber of childhood events - one of which was false
(putting 'slim e' in a teacher's desk). Lindsay et al. (2004) show ed half
their participants a school class photograph (which was taken in the
sam e year as the suggested false event) in order to help them 'jog
their m em ory' for the events. In line with previous research, 27 per
cent of participants in the 'no-photo' group were judged to have a
false m emory for the 'slim e' event (e.g. H yman et al. 1995; Loftus and
Pickrell 1995; H ym an and Billings; 1998; Porter et al. 1999). However,
in the 'photo' group, 65 per cent of participants were judged to have
developed a false m em ory for the suggested event (Lindsay et al.
2004). This dem onstrates that even real, undoctored photographs
are, if used in conjunction with m isleading suggestions, extrem ely
powerful cues that can lead to the developm ent of false reports of
childhood events.
Support groups
Som e therapists recom m end that one way to help clients recover
mem ories of abuse is to participate in support groups with other
people who have also recovered such m em ories. For exam ple,
Herm an (1992: 224) states that:
The group provides a pow erful stim ulus for the recovery of
traumatic memories. As each group m em ber reconstructs her
own narrative, the details of her story alm ost inevitably evoke
new recollections in each of the listeners. In the incest survivor
groups, virtually every m em ber who has defined a goal of
recovering m em ories has been able to do so.
278
Recovered memories
that the social demands of such groups may also act to encourage
individuals to report falsely events that did not occur. Consider the
example below, provided by a retractor (an individual who came to
believe she had been abused, only later to repudiate that belief). She
claims that members of her support group, as well as her therapist:
279
Investigative Interviewing
280
Recovered memories
C o n c lu s io n
281
Investigative Interviewing
false beliefs and false reports about the past. However, we do not
yet know if everyone is equally susceptible to the m isleading and
suggestive nature of such techniques. Finally, a review of recent
evidence suggests that participating in support groups may place
particularly strong social dem ands on an individual to alter his or
her m em ory reports in line with the social influence exerted by other
people, although aspects of this hypothesis have yet to be directly
tested and remain an im portant avenue for future research. Overall
this suggests that using certain techniques to help individuals
rem em ber events from their distant, or even recent, past is best
approached with extrem e caution. At best the resulting report is
likely to be a distorted version of what did happen and, at worst,
it may w ell be a com pelling and vivid report of an event that never
actually occurred. Practitioners and policy makers therefore need
to be sensitive to these issues if we are to deal appropriately with
cases like A lice's, to prevent future m iscarriages of justice, to ensure
that genuine victim s of abuse receive the support they require and
ultimately, to reduce the num ber of casualties on both sides of the
'm em ory w ars'.
N o te s
282
Recovered memories
R e fe re n c e s
Almerigogna, J., Bull, R., Ost, J. and Akehurst, L. (2004) 'The effects of
state/trait anxiety and interviewer manner on the suggestibility of
child witnesses.' Paper presented at the 13th European Association of
Psychology and Law conference, 7-10 July, Krakow, Poland.
Andrews, B. (2001) 'Recovered memories in therapy: clinicians' beliefs and
practices', in G.M. Davies and T. Dalgleish (eds) Recovered Memories:
Seeking the Middle Ground. Chichester: Wiley.
Arndt, J. and Greenberg, J. (1996) 'Fantastic accounts can take many forms:
false memory construction? Yes. Escape from self? We don't think so',
Psychological Inquiry, 7: 127-32.
Asch, S.E. (1951) 'Effects of group pressure upon the modification and
distortion of judgments', in H. Guetzkow (ed.) Groups, Leadership, and
Men. Pittsburgh, PA: Carnegie Press.
Bain, S.R. and Baxter, J.S. (2000) 'Interrogative suggestibility: the role of
interviewer behaviour', Legal and Criminological Psychology, 5: 123-33.
Bekerian, D.A. and Bowers, J.M. (1983) 'Eyewitness testimony: were we
misled?', Journal o f Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition,
9: 139-45.
Bekerian, D.A. and O'Neill, M.H. (2001) 'Therapeutic techniques, therapeutic
contexts and memory', in G.M. Davies and T. Dalgleish (eds) Recovered
Memories: Seeking the Middle Ground. Chichester: Wiley.
Betz, A.L., Skowronski, J.J. and Ostrom, T.M. (1996) 'Shared realities: Social
influence and stimulus memory', Social Cognition, 14: 113-40.
Bjorklund, D. (2000) False Memory Creation in Children and Adults: Theory,
Research, and Implications. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Elbaum Associates.
Bless, H., Strack, F. and Walther, E. (2001) 'Memory as a target of social
influence? Memory distortions as a function of social influence and
metacognitive knowledge', in J.P. Forgas and K.D. Williams (eds) Social
Influence: Direct and Indirect Processes. Philadelphia, PA: Psychology Press.
Brainerd, C.J. and Reyna, V.F. (1998) 'When things that were never experienced
are easier to "rem em ber" than things that were', Psychological Science, 9:
484-9.
Brandon, S., Boakes, J., Glaser, D., Green, R., MacKeith, J. and Whewell, P.
(1997) 'Reported recovered memories of child sexual abuse:
recommendations for good practice and implications for training,
continuing professional development and research', Psychiatric Bulletin,
21: 663-5.
Brown, D., Scheflin, A.W. and Hammond, D.C. (1998) Memory, Trauma
Treatment, and the Law. New York, NY: W.W. Norton.
Brown, R., Goldstein, E. and Bjorklund, D.F. (2000) 'The history and Zeitgeist of
the repressed-false-memory debate: scientific and sociological perspectives
on suggestibility', in D.F. Bjorklund (ed.) False Memory Creation in Children
and Adults. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
283
Investigative Interview ing
Bull, R. and Corran, E. (2003) 'Interviewing child witnesses: past and future',
International Journal o f Police Science and M anagement, 4: 315-22.
Cam eron, C. (1996) 'Com paring amnesic and nonam nesic survivors of child
hood sexual abuse: a longitudinal study', in K. Pezdek and W.P. Banks
(eds) The Recovered M em ory/False M emory Debate. San Diego, CA: Academic
Press.
Catchpole, Z. (2003) 'D octor led his patient to make sex claim s', Daily M ail,
9 September.
Cialdini, R.B. (2001) Influence: Science and Practice (4th edn). Boston: Allyn &
Bacon.
Conte, J.R. (1999) 'Memory, research, and the law: future directions', in L.M.
Williams and V.L. Banyard (eds) Trauma and M emory. Thousand Oaks, CA:
Sage.
Conway, M.A. (1995) Flashbulb M emories. Hove: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Conway, M.A. (1997) Recovered M em ories and False M emories. Oxford: Oxford
University Press.
Crombag, H.F.M., Wagenaar, W.A. and van Koppen, PJ. (1996) 'Crashing
memories and the problem of "source m onitoring"', Applied Cognitive
Psychology, 10: 95-104.
D avies, G.M. (2001) 'Is it possible to discriminate betw een true and false
m em ories?', in G.M. Davies and T. Dalgleish (eds) Recovered M emories:
Seeking the M iddle Ground. Chichester: Wiley.
Davies, G.M. and Dalgleish, T. (2001) Recovered M emories: Seeking the M iddle
Ground. Chichester: Wiley.
Deese, J. (1959) 'On the prediction of occurrence of particular verbal intrusions
in immediate recall', Journal o f Experimental Psychology, 58: 17-22.
de Rivera, J. (1998) 'Relinquishing believed-in imaginings: narratives of
people who have repudiated false accusations', in J. de Rivera and
T.R. Sarbin (eds) Believed-in Imaginings: The Narrative Reconstruction o f
Reality. W ashington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Deutsch, M. and Gerard, H.B. (1955) 'A study of norm ative and informational
social influences upon individual judgm ent', Journal o f Abnormal and Social
Psychology, 51: 629-36.
Erdmann, K., Volbert, R. and Bohm, C. (2004) 'Children report suggested
events even when interviewed in a non-suggestive manner: what are its
im plications for credibility assessm ent?', Applied Cognitive Psychology, 18:
589-611.
Freyd, J.J. (1998) 'Science in the memory debate', Ethics and Behavior, 8:
101-13.
Freyd, J.J. and Gleaves, D.H. (1996) '"R em em berin g" words not presented in
lists: relevance to the current recovered/false m emory controversy', Journal
o f Experimental Psychology: Learning, M emory, and Cognition, 22: 811-13.
Gabbert, F., M emon, A. and Allan, K. (2003) 'M em ory conform ity: can
eyew itnesses influence each other's memories for an event?', Applied
Cognitive Psychology, 17: 533-43.
284
Recovered memories
Gabbert, F., Memon, A., Allan, K. and Wright, D.B. (2004) Say it to my face:
Examining the effects of socially encountered misinformation. Legal and
Criminological Psychology, 9, 215-27.
Gallo, D.A., Roberts, M.J. and Seamon, J.G. (1997) 'Remembering words not
presented in lists: can we avoid creating false memories?', Psychonomic
Bulletin and Review, 4: 271-6.
Garry, M., Manning, C.G., Loftus, E.F. and Sherman, S.J. (1996) 'Imagination
inflation: imagining a childhood event inflates confidence that it occurred',
Psychological Bulletin and Review, 3: 208-14.
Garry, M., Sharman, S.J., Wade, K.A., Hunt, M.J. and Smith, P.J. (2001)
'Imagination inflation is a fact, not an artifact', Memory and Cognition, 29:
719-29.
Goff, L.M. and Roediger, H.L. (1998) 'Imagination inflation for action events:
repeated imaginings lead to illusory recollections', Memory and Cognition,
26: 20-33.
Granhag, P.-A., Stromwall, L. and Billings, F.J. (2003) '" I'll never forget the
sinking ferry": how social influence makes false memories surface', in
M. Vanderhallen et al. (eds) Much Ado About Crime: Chapters on Psychology
and Laxv. Belgium: Uitgeverij Politeia.
Greenberg, D.L. (2004) 'President Bush's false "flashbulb" memory of
9 /1 1 /0 1 ', Applied Cognitive Psychology, 18: 363-70.
Gudjonnsson, G.H. (2003) The Psychology o f Interrogations and Confessions.
Chichester: Wiley.
Halverson, C.F. (1988) 'Remembering your parents: reflections on the
retrospective method', Journal o f Personality, 56: 435-43.
Harvey, M.R. (1999) 'Memory research and clinical practice: a critique of three
paradigms and a framework for psychotherapy with trauma survivors',
in L.M. Williams and V.L. Banyard (eds) Trauma and Memory. Thousand
Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
Heaps, C.M. and Nash, M. (1999) 'Individual differences in imagination
inflation', Psychonomic Bulletin and Reviezv, 6: 313-18.
Heaps, C.M. and Nash, M. (2001) 'Comparing recollective experience in true
and false autobiographical memories', Journal o f Experimental Psychology:
Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27: 920-30.
Herman, J.L. (1992) Trauma and Recovery: Prom Domestic Abuse to Political
Terror. New York, NY: Basic Books.
Herman, J.L. and Schatzow, E. (1987) 'Recovery and verification of memories
of childhood sexual trauma', Psychoanalytic Psychology, 4: 1-14.
Hoffman, H.G., Granhag, P.A., Kwong See, S.T. and Loftus E.F. (2001) 'Social
influences on reality monitoring decisions', Memory and Cognition, 29:
394-404.
Hyman, I.E. (2000) 'The memory wars', in U. Neisser and I.E. Hyman (eds)
Memory Observed: Remembering in Natural Contexts (2nd edn). New York,
NY: Worth Publishers.
Hyman, I.E. and Billings, F.J. (1998) 'Individual differences and the creation
of false childhood memories', Memory, 6: 1-20.
285
Investigative Interviewing
Hyman, I.E., Husband, T.H. and Billings, F.J. (1995) 'False memories of
childhood experiences', Applied Cognitive Psychology, 9: 181-97.
Hyman, I.E. and Loftus, E.F. (1997) 'Some people recover memories of
childhood trauma that never really happened', in P.S. Applebaum et al.
(eds) Trauma and Memory: Clinical and Legal Controversies. New York, NY:
Oxford University Press.
Hyman, I.E. and Pentland, J. (1996) 'Guided imagery and the creation of
false childhood memories', Journal o f Memory and Language, 35: 101-17.
Jelicic, M., Smeets, T., Peters, M.J.V., Candel, I., Horselenberg, R. and
Merckelbach, H. (2005) Assassination of a controversial politician:
Remembering details from another non-existent film. Paper presented at the
SARMAC VI conference, 5 -8 January, Wellington, New Zealand.
Johnson, M.K. (1988) 'Reality monitoring: an experimental phenomenological
approach', Journal o f Experimental Psychology: General, 117: 390-4.
Johnson, M.K., Foley, M.A., Suengas, A.G. and Raye, C.L. (1988) 'Phenomenal
characteristics of memories for perceived and imagined autobiographical
events', Journal o f Experimental Psychology: General, 117: 371-6.
Johnson, M.K., Hashtroudi, S. and Lindsay, D.S. (1993) Source monitoring.
Psychological Bulletin, 114, 3-28.
Johnston, M. (1997) Spectral Evidence. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.
Kassin, S.M. (1997) 'False memories turned against the self', Psychological
Inquiry, 8: 300-2.
Kassin, S.M. (2005) On the psychology of confessions: Does innocence put
innocents at risk? American Psychologist, 60, 215-228.
Lampinen, J.M. and Smith, V.L. (1995) 'The incredible (and sometimes
incredulous) child witness: Child eyewitnesses' sensitivity to source
credible cues', Journal o f Applied Psychology, 80: 621-7.
Lindsay, D.S., Hagen, L., Read, J.D., Wade, K.A. and Garry, M. (2004) 'True
photographs and false memories', Psychological Science, 15: 149-54.
Lindsay, D.S. and Read, J.D. (1994) 'Psychotherapy and memories of childhood
sexual abuse: a cognitive perspective', Applied Cognitive Psychology, 8:
281-338.
Lindsay, D.S. and Read, J.D. (2001) 'The recovered memories controversy:
Where do we go from here?', in G.M. Davies and T. Dalgleish (eds)
Recovered Memories: Seeking the Middle Ground. Chichester: Wiley.
Loftus, E.F. (1979) Eyewitness Testimony. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University
Press.
Loftus, E.F. (1997) 'Dispatch from the (un)civil memory wars', in J.D. Read
and D.S. Lindsay (eds) Recollections o f Trauma: Scientific Evidence and
Clinical Practice. New York, NY: Plenum.
Loftus, E.F. and Bernstein, D.M. (2005) 'Rich false memories: the royal road
to success', in A.F. Healy (ed.) Experimental Cognitive Psychology and
its Applications. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association
Press.
286
Recovered memories
Loftus, E.F. and Ketcham, K. (1994) The Myth o f Repressed Memory: False
Memories and Allegations o f Sexual Abuse. New York, NY: St Martin's
Press.
Loftus, E.F., Miller, D.G. and Burns, H.J. (1978) 'Semantic integration of verbal
information into a visual memory', Journal o f Experimental Psychology:
Human Learning and Memory, 4: 19-31.
Loftus, E.F. and Palmer, J.C. (1974) 'Reconstruction of automobile destruction:
an example of the interaction between language and memory', Journal o f
Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 13: 585-9.
Loftus, E.F. and Pickrell, J.E. (1995) 'The formation of false memories',
Psychiatric Annals, 25: 720-5.
Lynn, S.J. and Kirsch, 1.1. (1996) 'Alleged alien abductions: False memories,
hypnosis, and fantasy proneness', Psychological Inquiry, 7: 151-5.
Lynn, S.J., Lock, T.G., Loftus, E.F., Krackow, E. and Lilienfeld, S.O. (2003) 'The
remembrance of things past: problematic memory recovery techniques in
psychotherapy', in S.O. Lilienfeld, S.J. Lynn and J.M. Lohr (eds) Science
and Pseudoscience in Clinical Psychology. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Lynn, S.J., Lock, T.G., Myers, B. and Payne, D.G. (1997a) 'Recalling
the unrecallable: should hypnosis be used to recover memories in
psychotherapy?', Current Directions in Psychological Science, 6: 79-83.
Lynn, S.J., Myers, B. and Malinoski, P. (1997b) 'Hypnosis, pseudomemories,
and clinical guidelines: a sociocognitive perspective', in J.D. Read and
D.S. Lindsay (eds) Recollections o f Trauma: Scientific Evidence and Clinical
Practice. New York, NY: Plenum Press.
Malinoski, P.T. and Lynn, S.J. (1999) 'The plasticity of early memory
reports: social pressure, hypnotizability, compliance, and interrogative
suggestibility', The International Journal o f Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis,
47: 320-45.
Mazzoni, G.A.L. and Loftus, E.F. (1998) 'Dreaming, believing, and
remembering', in J. de Rivera and T.R. Sarbin (eds) Believed-in Imaginings:
The Narrative Reconstruction o f Reality. Washington, DC: American
Psychological Association.
Mazzoni, G.A.L., Loftus, E.F., Seitz, A. and Lynn, S.J. (1999) 'Changing beliefs
and memories through dream interpretation', Applied Cognitive Psychology,
13: 125-44.
McCloskey, M. and Zaragoza, M. (1985) 'Misleading postevent information
and memory for events: arguments and evidence against memory
impairment hypotheses', Journal o f Experimental Psychology: General, 114:
1-16.
McConkey, K.M., Barnier, A.J. and Sheehan, P.W. (1998) 'Hypnosis and
pseudomemory: understanding the findings and their implications', in
S.J. Lynn and K.M. McConkey (eds) Truth in Memory. New York, NY:
Guilford Press.
McNally, R.J. (2003) Remembering Trauma. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University
Press.
287
Investigative Interviewing
288
Recovered memories
289
Investigative Interviewing
Shobe, K.K. and Schooler, J.W. (2001) 'Discovering fact and fiction: case-based
analyses of authentic and fabricated discovered memories of abuse', in
G.M. Davies and T. Dalgleish (eds) Recovered Memories: Seeking the Middle
Ground. Chichester: Wiley.
Smeets, T., Merckelbach, H., Horselenberg, R. and Jelicic, M. (in press)
'Trying to recollect past events: confidence, beliefs, and memories', Clinical
Psychology Reviezv.
Smith, R.E. and Hunt, R.R. (1998) 'Presentation modality affects false
memory', Psychonomic Bulletin and Reviezv, 5: 710-15.
Smith, V.L. and Ellsworth, P.C. (1987) 'The social psychology of eyewitness
accuracy: misleading questions and communicator expertise', Journal o f
Applied Psychology, 72: 294-300.
Snyder, M. (1974) 'The self-monitoring of expressive behavior', Journal o f
Personality and Social Psychology, 30: 526-37.
Spanos, N.P. (1996) Multiple Identities and False Memories: A Sociocognitive
Perspective. New York, NY: American Psychological Association.
Spanos, N .P, Cross, P.A., Dickson, K. and DuBreuil, S.C. (1993) 'Close
encounters: an examination of UFO experiences', Journal o f Abnormal
Psychology, 95, 21-3.
Spanos, N.P., Menary, E., Gabora, N.J., DuBreuil, S.C. and Dewhirst, B. (1991)
'Secondary identity enactments during hypnotic past-life regression: a
sociocognitive perspective', Journal o f Personality and Social Psychology, 61:
308-20.
Templeton, V. and Hunt, V. (1997) 'The effects of misleading information and
level of authority of interviewer on children's witness memory.' Poster
presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child
Development, Washington, DC.
Thomas, A.K. and Loftus, E.F. (2002) 'Creating bizarre false memories through
imagination', Memory and Cognition, 30: 423-31.
Tobey, A.E. and Goodman, G.S. (1992) 'Children's eyewitness testimony:
effects of participation and forensic context', Child Abuse and Neglect, 16:
779-96.
Tsai, A., Loftus, E.F. and Polage, D. (2000) 'Current directions in false memory
research', in D.F. Bjorklund (ed.) False Memory Creation in Children and
Adults: Theory, Research and Implications. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates.
Underwager, R. and Wakefield, H. (1998) 'Recovered memories in the
courtroom', in S.J. Lynn and K.M. McConkey (eds) Truth in Memory. New
York, NY: Guilford Press.
Vrij, A., Pannell, H. and Ost, J. (2005) 'The influence of social pressure and
black clothing on crime judgements', Psychology, Crime and Laiv.
Wade, K.A., Garry, M., Read, J.D. and Lindsay, D.S. (2002) 'A picture is
worth a thousand lies: using false photographs to create false childhood
memories', Psychonomic Bulletin and Reviezv, 9: 597-603.
290
Recovered memories
291
C h a p t e r 14
Robert Roy
292
Investigative interviewing: suspects’ and victims’ rights in balance
M o ra l ju d g m e n t in genera l
293
Investigative Interviewing
294
Investigative interviewing: suspects’ and victims’ rights in balance
295
Investigative Interviewing
296
Investigative interviewing: suspects’ and victims’ rights in balance
297
Investigative Interviewing
F a c to rs th a t g e n e ra te e th ic a l p re s su re s on in te rv ie w in g
298
Investigative interviewing: suspects’ and victims’ rights in balance
299
Investigative Interviewing
300
Investigative interviewing: suspects’ and victims’ rights in balance
301
Investigative Interviewing
302
Investigative interviewing: suspects’ and victims’ rights in balance
oath is surely more sym bolic today, however, and a suspect who
is facing a m ajor sentence will often be willing to risk a perjury
conviction to increase his or her chances of an acquittal. Scientific
research has also highlighted the great differences that exist in the
accuracy and reliability of visual and auditory recollections, along with
the existence of selective and even creative m em ory (Laurence 2004)
and people's propensity to give the existence of false confessions even
from norm al individuals (Scheck et al. 2000; St-Yves 2004a). Finally,
alleged abuses com m itted w hilst interview ing have also underm ined
people's trust in the reliability of the confessions and testim onies the
police have obtained. Video recording is proof of this lost credibility
but, at the sam e time, is a way to restore it.
Confessions, however, are still very important. First, many
investigations rest on circum stantial evidence or on contradictory
versions put forward by the victim and suspect. Secondly, confessions
often lead to the discovery of new evidence: a body, a weapon, a
disguise, etc. Finally, because the suspect's rights are protected by
the law, this secondary evidence is often the only way to establish
proof beyond reasonable doubt. In these circum stances a confession
is a key elem ent for conviction, and the ethical pressures to obtain a
confession during an interview continues.
V a lu e s to ba lan ce w h ils t in te rv ie w in g a su sp e ct
303
Investigative Interviewing
304
Investigative interviewing: suspects’ and victims’ rights in balance
305
Investigative Interviewing
306
Investigative interviewing: suspects’ and victims’ rights in balance
3. The suspect has nothing to do with the crim e but will not reveal
inform ation because he or she has been threatened by someone.
4. The suspect is innocent but will not reveal inform ation in order to
protect a friend or relative.
307
Investigative Interviewing
6. The suspect is guilty but cannot recall the events because of shock,
alcohol or drug abuse.
In the first and second scenarios, the suspect is com pletely innocent.
In the third, he or she is innocent and is him or herself the victim
of a crime. In the fourth, he or she is not guilty but is ham pering
progress in achieving justice. In the fifth, the suspect is innocent of
the crime he or she is suspected of, but guilty of a different one.
In the sixth and seventh, he or she is guilty but will only divulge
inform ation as a last resort.
The first question these scenarios raise is: 'Is it legitim ate to submit
a suspect, who may be totally innocent, to some pressure because we
have reasonable grounds to believe he or she is involved in a crim e?'
To assure the protection of those who are innocent, w e should answer
'no'. But, in certain circum stances, such an answ er could lead to a
com plete denial of the victim 's rights. For exam ple, the police receive
a phone call that bom bs have been set along a railway line. The police
succeed in tracing the call and arrest the caller because he claims
to be the person who placed the bom bs. M aterial and instructions
regarding how to build such bom bs are found in the house where
the arrest was made. The suspect refuses, however, to give details of
where bom bs are placed. In theory, individual rights are not in conflict
but, in practice, they are. Protecting the suspect's rights in such a case
places at risk the right to life of m any other people. Article 7 of the
Canadian Charter of Rights and Freedoms stipulates that 'Everyone
has the right to life, liberty and security of the person and the right
not to be deprived thereof except in accordance with the principles
of fundam ental ju stice'. In reality, com plex ethical dilem m as do not
oblige us to choose betw een right and wrong. The choice we have, in
such situations, is often to decide which of two positions we would
prefer to avoid, and hence creating the less evil. If we adm it less evil
choice situations exist, the question we now face is: 'W hen, and up to
which point, can we legitim ately put pressure on a suspect we have
reasonable grounds to believe is involved in a crim e? A ccording to
Michel St-Yves: 'N o m atter what the model is we are referring to, one
factor is a must: pressure. Should it com e from the suspect him self
(internal) or from another source (external), this pressure show s
that a confession, even if it is som etim es provided by a suspect, is
never really free and voluntary' (2004b: 49, my translation). Internal
308
Investigative interviewing: suspects’ and victims’ rights in balance
309
Investigative Interviewing
Oppression. The court referred here to the general context in which the
interview is conducted. Was the suspect questioned for too long or
too aggressively? Was the suspect deprived of clothing, food, drink,
sleep, medication? Was he allowed to talk to an attorney?
310
Investigative interviewing: suspects’ and victims’ rights in balance
Other police trickery. The court referred here to all the tactics
investigators may use to obtain inform ation from som eone (for
exam ple, planting an undercover agent in the same cell or, as in R.
c. Corak ([1994], 29 C.R. (4th) 388 C.A. C.-B.), where the investigators
left a hat found at the scene of the crime well within the suspect's
view, hoping he would claim it as his own). The court is clear about
this last group of factors: 'U nlike the previous three headings, this
doctrine is a distinct inquiry. W hile it is still related to voluntariness,
its more specific objective is m aintaining the integrity of the criminal
justice system .' The court is not prim arily interested in the validity
of the confession but in the role the state's agent played to obtain it.
It is in this context that the court quoted the often referenced Judge
Lam er decision in Rothman c. Reine ([1981] 1 R.C.S. 640, 697):
Lies are considered by the court as one form of police trickery that
cannot be totally prohibited. In another decision (R. c. Cook, [1982],
2 R.C.S.), the court recognized that 'policem en m ust som etim es lie.
In m any situations, it is not only appropriate but necessary and
obviously acceptable' (my translation).
We m ust alw ays bear in mind that, from a legal point of view, the
stress is on voluntariness. Som etim es one factor will be sufficient to
vitiate it w hilst in another situation, m any factors m ay contribute to
the same result. In yet other situations, many small factors will harm
voluntariness w ithout being sufficient to vitiate it.
We are now in a position to com pare the law with the ethical
position as I see it. To do this, I offer eight guidelines. First of all,
since we are interview ing a hum an being, hum an dignity requires
the prohibition of som e practices. Because, even in w artim e, the
international com m unity has agreed to prohibit torture as an
311
Investigative Interviewing
312
Investigative interviewing: suspects’ and victims’ rights in balance
Here again, we can say this guideline is a concrete exam ple of the
general preoccupation of what Oickle presented as the 'tw in goals' of
the rule of confessions.
The sixth guideline is to be aware of psychological distress. It
is relatively easy to avoid physical torture because the relationship
betw een physical intervention and physical pain can be anticipated.
But, w'hen trying to avoid psychological cruelty, things are not so
sim ple: the sam e treatm ent w'ill have different effects on different
suspects, and even on the same suspect at different times. The
im portant thing is not to avoid a specific treatm ent but to observe
the suspect's reactions. The ethical and legal im plications for each
case must be judged on an individual basis.
The seventh guideline is to be wary of lies. If one looks at the
literature some im portant Canadian police organizations follow, there
seems to be a tendency to rely on lies. Stella Gabino has made an
inventory of the interview ing tricks that seem to be accepted by
Canadian courts. Am ongst these, we find that police officers have
lied about the evidence they have, about w itnesses being willing to
testify against suspects and about confessions made by accom plices
(Gabino 1997). The Montreal Police have cited Gabino and the
Canadian Suprem e Court (in R. c. Cook, [1982], 2 R.C.S.) in literature
widely distributed to police investigators as a vindication for the use
of such measures. UK law s and PEACE supporters, however, are
com pletely opposed to the use of lies during interview's. Indeed,
some research has show n a link betw een the police's use of lies and
false confessions (Scheck et al. 2000). Because lying is often the only
way to break the 'code of silence' that may exist am ongst organized
crim inals, to abandon lying com pletely may display a lack of realism.
Nevertheless, it should only be used as a calculated risk and not
as part of norm al practice. A close analysis of the Oickle decision
shows that this ethical position is also the one proposed by Canada's
Suprem e Court.
The eighth guideline is to assess w hether there are limits to the
presum ption of innocence and w hether these are appropriate. It is
this guideline that probably raises the m ost controversy because of
the neat difference betw een the ethical and legal positions. According
to the Canadian Suprem e Court, the presum ption of innocence is
guaranteed to a suspect if, and only if:
313
Investigative Interviewing
• proof, or part of the proof, obliged the suspect to prove his or her
innocence (R. c. Downey, [1992] 2 R.C.S. 10); a n d /o r
• criminal prosecution was unfair or partial (R. c. S. (R.D.), [1997]
3 R.C.S. 484).
C o n c lu s io n
314
Investigative interviewing: suspects’ and victims’ rights in balance
dignity and the presum ption of innocence. This balance is alw ays in
question in dem ocracies because a suspect's status, at the time of an
interview, is alw ays marked by uncertainty. The contem porary nature
of social, econom ic and political trends is building two contradictory
stances. The first favours increasing the suspect's protection w hereas
the other favours the opposite. The legitim acy of interview ing
m ethods cannot be enforced by instituting uniform procedures for
all interview s in all dem ocratic countries. Each country has its own
institutions, each investigation is different and each suspect is a
unique individual with his or her own psychological profile and past.
All these differences m ust form part of the interview 's legitimacy.
Such com plexity dem ands that we have a better chance of m aking
the correct decision if w?e rely on guidelines to analyze a specific
situation instead of fixed rules.
This chapter has developed the general idea that interview s must
not have confession as a goal, and that they m ust not rely on tricks,
lies or psychological pressure. They should be directed at obtaining
inform ation from non-custodial victim s, w itnesses and suspects who
are under investigation on an intuitive basis only. A n interrogation,
on the other hand, may have a confession as its goal, and may use
m eans prohibited in interviews. Interrogation m ethods are aimed at
getting inform ation from suspects we have reasonable evidence to
believe are related to the crime under investigation and, thus, can
be held in detention or arrested. But, for the sake of hum an dignity,
interrogations should never rely on physical or psychological torture
or on the threat of such, and should only be undertaken if the
suspect is in a conscious state of mind. The chapter has also asked
for a re-exam ination of the limits to a suspect's presum ption of
innocence, especially when the burden of proof always rests on the
state's shoulders, no m atter w'hat the circum stances or the suspect's
criminal past or acquaintances. Such a re-exam ination would at least
restore the public's respect for the presum ption of innocence or, at
best, avoid the situation where notorious crim inals use suspects'
rights to escape justice.
R e fe re n ce s
315
Investigative Interview ing
Bellemare, J. (1996) Les pratiques eti matiere d'enquetes criminelles au sein des
corps de police du Quebec. Rapport fin al depose au ministre de la Securite
publique M onsieur Robert Perreault. Quebec: Publications du Quebec.
Cournoyer, G. and Ouimet, G. (2002) Code crimine! annote 2002. Cowansville:
Editions Yvon Blais.
Division des affaires juridiques de la Police de la Com m unaute urbaine de
M ontreal (1995) 'Le droit a l'avocat', Le conseiller juridique, 3.
Division des affaires juridiques de la Police de la Com m unaute urbaine de
M ontreal (1996a) 'Le droit au silence', Le conseiller juridique, 6.
Division des affaires juridiques de la Police de la Com m unaute urbaine de
Montreal (1996b) 'Le policier doit offrir au detenu la possibility raisonnable
de com m uniquer avec l'avocat de son choix', Le conseiller juridique, 8.
Division des affaires juridiques de la Police de la Com m unaute urbaine de
M ontreal (1997) 'Le pouvoir de detention', Le conseiller juridique, 17.
Division des affaires juridiques de la Police de la Com m unaute urbaine de
Montreal (1998) 'Les motifs raisonnables', Le conseiller juridique, 24.
Gabino, S. (1997) Interrogatoire sur video, aspect legal. Nicolet: Institut de police
du Quebec.
H aberm as, J. (1997) Droit et democratic. Entre faits et normes. Paris:
Gallimard.
H aberm as, J. (1999) De Vethique de la discussion. Paris: Champs, Flammarion.
Kymlicka, W. (1999) Les theories de la justice. Quebec: Editions du Boreal.
Lacroix, A. and Letourneau, A. (eds) (2000) M ethodes et interventions en ethique
appliquee. Quebec: Fides.
Landry, J. and St-Yves, M. (2004) 'La pratique de l'interrogatoire de police',
in J. Landry et M. St-Yves (eds) Psychologie des entrevues d'enquite: de la
recherche a la pratique. Cowansville: Editions Yvon Blais.
Laurence, J.-R. (2004) 'H ypnose et memoire: un bref survol de la litterature
scientifique', in J. Landry et M. St-Yves (eds) Psychologie des entrevues
d'enquete: de la recherche a la pratique. Cowansville: Editions Yvon Blais.
Leblond, C. (ed.) (2002) Droit penal, procedure et preuve. Collection de droit
2001-2002. Volume 10. Cowansville: Editions Yvon Blais.
Legault, G.A. (1999) Professionnalisme et deliberation ethique. Quebec: Presses
de l'Universite du Quebec.
Lenoble, J. (1994) Droit et communication. La transformation du droit contemporain.
Paris: Les editions du Cerf.
Lenoble, J. and M aesschalck, M. (2003) Toivard a Theory o f Governance: The
Action o f Norms. The H a g u e/L o n d o n /N ew York, NY: Kluw er Law
International.
M odoux, F. (2003) 'Un policier allemand ordonne la torture pour extorquer
des aveux a un suspect', Le Temps.ch, online, 25 February: 2.
Pech, T. (2001) 'La dignite humaine. Du droit a l'ethique de la relation',
£thique publique, 3: 93-120.
Roy, R. (2004) 'Enjeux ethiques des entrevue d'enquete', in J. Landry et
M. St-Yves (eds) Psychologie des entrevues d'enquete: de la recherche a la
pratique. Cowansville: Editions Yvon Blais.
316
Investigative interviewing: suspects’ and victim s’ rights in balance
Scheck, B., Neufeld, P. and Dwyer, J. (2000) Actual Innocence: Five Days to
Execution and Other Dispatches from the Wrongly Convicted. New York, NY:
Random House.
St-Yves, M. (2004a) 'Les fausses confessions: comprendre et prevenir', in
J. Landry et M. St-Yves (eds) Psychologie des entrevues d'enquete: de la
recherche a la pratique. Cowansville: Editions Yvon Blais.
St-Yves, M. (2004b) 'La psychologie de l'aveu', in J. Landry et M. St-Yves (eds)
Psychologie des entrevues d'enquete: de la recherche cl la pratique. Cowansville:
Editions Yvon Blais.
317
C h a p te r I 5
David Dixon
In tro d u c in g re g u la tio n
318
Regulating police interrogation
319
Investigative Interviewing
regime has to fit with the aims and purposes of the activity to be
regulated.
Doing so may seem unnecessary: isn't interrogation obviously about
getting a suspect to tell the truth? When police officers are asked to
explain their purpose in interviewing suspects, they typically refer
to the process as 'a search for the truth'. This is attractive rhetoric.
However, it has two flaws. First, it is almost trite to say that any
account involves selection and construction. Accounts provided
in police interrogations are selected and constructed primarily not
because police routinely use ploys to trick suspects (McConville et
al. 1991), but because this is how memory, recall and account-giving
work:
Q Well let's start telling the truth George, I'm just asking you
a simple question, right, I believe Iknow the answer to it
and I'm asking you to tell the truth
A I am telling the truth ...
320
Regulating police interrogation
Q You can keep saying that over and over again but that
doesn't mean to me that you didn't do it and you know,
sat there, the tru th ... all the evidence is pointing straight in
your direction...
A I am not admitting to som et't [I didn't do]
Q We are not asking you to admit that you didn't do, we are
asking for the truth about the murder of Nikki Allen.2
321
Investigative Interviewing
- if that. We collect, and then we pass the pieces on, hoping that
someone above us can assemble them (Mackey 2004: xxv).
322
Regulating police interrogation
323
Investigative Interviewing
324
Regulating police interrogation
325
Investigative Interviewing
326
Regulating police interrogation
327
Investigative Interviewing
1997: chs. 4 and 5). First, between 1900 and the 1980s, judges stepped
back from active regulation of custodial interrogation. Secondly,
they often made clear their strong disapproval of defence lawyers
who criticized the police. This history was ignored by critics of the
statutory reforms of the 1980s and 1990s, who looked back through
rose-coloured glasses to common law control of policing.
What impact judicial regulation had on police practice and
effectiveness has been the subject of much controversy in the
USA. The most convincing interpretation of the extensive research
evidence is that heroic Supreme Court cases such as Miranda did not
significantly reduce police ability to investigate crime, with officers
soon finding ways to minimize their impact (Walker 1993: 44-6; Leo
1996: 287). Specifically, they hastened shifts already under way in
police interrogation, as techniques of persuasion and psychological
coercion were developed in training manuals by Inbau and Reid, and
others. However, they did have some beneficial effects: these cases led
police organizations to improve their training, supervisory practices
and general professionalism (Walker 1980: 229-32; Leo 1994: 114) and
encouraged a cultural shift towards the acceptance of accountability
and legality. Skolnick writes of a 'legal archipelago... sets of islands
of legal valu es... distributed throughout the broad experience of
policing' (1993: 196).
This experience suggests broader lessons about potential judicial
contributions to regulation of interrogation. To date, judicial control
has shared many of the undesirable characteristics of com m and/
control regulation - it is distanced, unwieldy, non-responsive. If
judges want their decisions to have more positive impact, they have
to know more about the world they seek to regulate and to express
them more clearly and more positively, and with more emphasis on
policy and less on the individual case. (Appeal courts, particularly at
more senior levels, are obviously more able to do this than are trial
judges.) They should be aware of the need to communicate decisions
and to participate in processes which review their impact. Attention
should be paid to indirect as well as direct effects.
Despite the convenient divisions within this chapter, regulatory
measures should not be considered in isolation from each other.
Judicial regulation will be affected by other pressures, particularly
the structure of rules provided for the detention and interrogation of
suspects which are considered in the next section. It is appropriate to
point out here that there was a significant shift in judges' approach
to the regulation of interrogation following the introduction of the
Police and Criminal Evidence Act 1984 (PACE) in England and
328
Regulating police interrogation
329
Investigative Interviewing
330
Regulating police interrogation
331
Investigative Interviewing
'If rules are prom ulgated perm itting torture in defined circum stances,
som e officials are bound to w ant to explore the outer bounds of the
rules. Having been regularized, the practice will becom e regular'
(Posner, quoted in Dershow itz 2003: 279). Posner was echoing claims
m ade in debates around the introduction of PACE: 'The problem
is that if the law is to “inhibit" police officers then it m ust always
diverge to some extent from the rules that officers would write for
them selves' (Baldwin and Kinsey 1985: 91). From this perspective if,
metaphorically, the police are given authority to drive at 30 mph,
they will drive at 35. 'To increase the lim it to 35 m ph will not close
the gap', but will sim ply lead to 40 mph driving (Baldw in 1985:
21). Such accounts can slip too easily into what Reiner calls 'a law
of inevitable increment: w hatever powers the police have they will
exceed by a given m argin' (Reiner 1992: 217).14 Reiner goes on to
provide a good critique of such formulations:
If there is too great a gap betw een what police are expected to
achieve and the m eans allow ed to them, then they may go beyond
the rules, and the rules m ay follow. Regulatory decline under the
weight of 'objectives' is vividly illustrated by a participant's account
of interrogation in Afghanistan and Iraq:
332
Regulating police interrogation
of lesser crimes' (Parry 2002: 763). They may well demand this, but
law making is not a slippery slope from which escape is impossible.
More positively, statutory regulation's provision to officers of
an authoritative statement of what is required can be significant
symbolically as well as instrumentally. Officers who want to do
'the right thing' are given a resource for use in arguments with
others about how suspects should be treated. Similarly, reducing the
potential for 'comeback' from a system often experienced as uncertain
and capricious is a significant benefit to officers.
The limits of regulation by rules have to be acknowledged. Those
favouring legal regulation are often over-optimistic about its likely
impact. A wide range of positions are included here, from the
simple legalistic-bureaucratic approach of the politician who thinks
that passing a law is the remedy for all ills (Dixon 1997: 1-2) to
the sophistication of Dershowitz's attempt to make liberal principle
connect with empirical reality.
We may not be able to set the limit at 20 mph, even if we want
to. So, when Dershowitz says that he hopes 'no torture warrant
would ever be issued, because the criteria for obtaining one would
be so limited and vigorous' (quoted in Parry 2002: 747) the realism
of his approach dissipates. Would no warrant mean no torture - or
just torture more carefully hidden in the gulag of Am erica's military
bases and the prisons of dependent states?15 His legalistic faith in
the power of law seems quaint: legality has never appeared to be a
particularly significant factor in the operations of the CIA. It is hard,
for example, to read the American government's list of approved
techniques of interrogation without concluding that it is largely for
presentational purposes (Department of Defence 2003).
The key lesson is that the impact of rules is contingent, not
determined. A review of the extensive literature on legal regulation
(Dixon 1997) leads to the conclusion that, whilst the potential of
rules should not be overstated, nor should it be underestimated: 'In
short, some things work' (Walker 1993: 150). Crucial factors include:
the nature of the activity; the degree of commitment to practice;
other cultural commitments and factors amongst practitioners; its
vulnerability to regulation; the nature of the rules and other regulatory
strategies deployed; the commitment of supervisors to implementation;
allocation of resources to supervision and accountability; countervailing
pressures from institutions on practitioners; and perceptions of the
likelihood and weight of punishment for breach. Such factors account
for the difference between stop/search and treatment of suspects in
police stations in England and Wales. The former involves a low-
333
Investigative Interviewing
334
Regulating police interrogation
T ra in in g as re g u la tio n
This was not a skill that all could acquire equally well: Innes found
that 'at the heart of police notions of "the good detective" was the
sense that certain individuals had a particular flair for the work. The
most valuable skills were held to be those developed through natural
instinct and experience' (2003: 9).
This approach to interviewing has come under increasing criticism.
In England, concerns about investigative weaknesses and miscarriages
of justice led to scrutiny of the quality of police interviewing. The
Royal Commission on Criminal Procedure recommended reform of
interview training (Philips 1981: 10.14), but this was 'the neglected
recommendation and little was done for nearly ten years to
implement it' (Williamson 1993: 92). In this vacuum, police officers
who were interested in improving their interviewing skills almost
inevitably looked to the USA. American interview training manuals
- notably Inbau and Reid's - were a significant influence in some
areas, encouraging officers to use persuasive interrogation techniques
(Williamson 1993: 92; Milne and Bull 1999: 156). American material
was used in training materials, police magazines, and strongly
influenced the first British police interview manual, W alkley's Police
Interrogation (1987). When such approaches to interrogation began to
be officially frowned upon, nothing was provided to replace them,
and so they continued to form a significant part of an informal
curriculum of material distributed amongst police officers.
Research on interrogation practice discovered significant
problems:
335
Investigative Interviewing
336
Regulating police interrogation
337
Investigative Interviewing
interviewee decently. Crucially, new role models for the young officer
become available: the tradition of arresting on hunches, interrogating,
and giving weak cases a run has been challenged by according status
to officers who investigate and collect evidence carefully, find ways
of working within the rules, interrogate skilfully and get convictions
which are not overturned on appeal (Dixon 1997: 163-4).19
From the regulatory perspective adopted here, PEACE is a form of
self-regulation induced by sustained external criticism. W hilst it was
encouraged by the Flome Office, PEACE was developed by the police,
working out a way of interrogating that would both withstand judicial
scrutiny and would produce results. Rather than imposing rules
backed by threat of sanction - the classic com m and/control mode -
PEACE provides a way of doing the job. The challenge was to provide
an alternative to established working practices which were connected
to cultural norms and values and beliefs - the imperative of obtaining
confessions, the working style of the detective, the expectation that
suspects will crack under coercive, persuasive questioning. The toe
hold that PEACE could exploit was the evidence that traditional
practices were inefficient, leading to the conviction of the innocent
and the evasion of justice by the really guilty. This provided an
opportunity for a new approach to interrogation, a partial application
of PEACE, to become the standard way of working, to make the
crucial transition from externally imposed standard to working norm
and self-regulation (Parker and Braithwaite 2003: 137). The need to
address existing norms was illustrated well at the Quebec conference
by Shaw 's and Griffith's insistence that investigative interviewing is
not a soft option and that it can incorporate searching and intensive
questioning of suspects.20
338
Regulating police interrogation
339
Investigative Interviewing
340
Regulating police interrogation
Clarke and M ilne's research and the work of key exponents of good
interviewing practice at force and national level (such as Gary Shaw,
initially in Northumbria Police, now at Centrex) have challenged the
individualistic model of criminal investigation by demonstrating the
value of active management and supervision of interrogation. This
approach is now expressed in a series of ACPO policies and national
training strategies which pass on the message that active supervision
can improve performance in suspect interviewing (Burbeck 2003:
52). Supervision is to be encouraged by the appointment of 'force
champions' for investigative interviewing, and 'interview advisers',
whose role is to give advice and assistance on a wide range of
matters to investigating officers in complex, volume and serious
crime and to improve the quality of interviewing (Centrex 2003:
4-5, 36-7). Crucially, these policies seek to spread best practice
which has been developed at operational level. This is an important
example of regulation working from the bottom up rather than the
top down. Notably, the role of 'interview adviser' was developed by
Northumbria Police and then adopted by several other forces before
being endorsed by ACPO. Quality is to be tested both by remote
monitoring of interrogations as they are being conducted (Home
Office 1995) and by emphasizing the importance of the retrospective
'evaluation' required by the PEACE model. The latter should involve
sampling of interview records.
At a basic level, the need for such supervision was demonstrated in
my research on audio-visual recording in New South Wales, Australia
(Dixon 2004b, forthcoming). A random sample of tapes showed that,
at some stations, the same technological problem recurred over
extended periods. In the worst case, six tapes recorded over a 12-
month period in one station suffered from what was apparently
the same malfunction, producing a badly blurred visual image. If
supervisors were not taking responsibility for simple processes of
tape quality control, then a fortiori they were not taking advantage
of the opportunity offered by audio-visual recording to monitor and
improve the interrogation practices of their officers.
As discussed above, police practice may bear little relation to
what policy or training requires: but there is now, at very least,
an authoritative endorsement of an approach to managerial and
supervisory regulation of interrogation which is very different
from what went before. The need for active supervision is bluntly
expressed:
341
Investigative Interviewing
O p e n n e ss and a u d io -visu a l re c o rd in g
Since K.C. Davis's Discretionary Justice (1971), the need for openness
has been a key theme in public law and regulation: opening official
practices to scrutiny, complaint and praise are a vital part of regulation
and accountability:
342
Regulating police interrogation
343
Investigative Interviewing
C o n clu s io n
344
Regulating police interrogation
345
Investigative Interviewing
N o te s
346
Regulating police interrogation
11. Jam es Boyd W hite, quoted Krygier (1990: 640). Self-evidently, this
depends on the breadth of those limits.
12. A problem for this argument is that the law, in the form of the Geneva
Convention against Torture, already deals with torture by im posing a
com plete ban. Dershowitz argues that the US adoption of the convention
w as subject to a reservation that the USA is bound 'only to the extent
that it is consistent with the Eighth A m endm ent' (2002: 136).
13. But for the record, I am with the latter. Note particularly Langbein's
historical dem onstration that 'efforts to accom modate the use of torture
within the Western legal tradition have a long history', which included
its abandonm ent because the resulting confessions were unreliable (2004:
93). It seem s that we are returning to the M iddle Ages in more ways
than one.
14. It is strange that Baldwin, who produces som e of the best work on
regulation, misread PACE so badly; see Dixon (1997: 169-70).
15. Note the limited im pact of Israel's attem pt to constrain torture by law
(Krem nitzer and Segev 2000).
16. PEACE = preparation and planning; engage and explain; account,
clarification and challenge; closure; evaluation.
17. op. cit., n. 2.
18. op. cit., n. 6; see also Sekar (1997).
19. O f course, some other less attractive role m odels are increasingly available
- for example, a police officer may feel that 'conversation m anagem ent'
is not enough when harsher m ethods are officially approved elsewhere
(e.g. Departm ent of Defence 2003). Hollywood also contributes - for
example, in the casting of Denzel Washington in Man on Fire, 'the first
authentic Am erican blockbuster in which the hero doesn't just go on a
killing rampage but on a torture spree as w ell' (Cox 2004). M eanwhile,
for the counter terrorism unit in the TV series 24, torture is as routine
and m orally unproblem atic as taking fingerprints.
20. Presentation given at International Conference on Police Interviewing,
Nicolet, Quebec, Canada, 9-11 February, 2004.
21. Governor Rod Blagojevich, quoted in '111. Law 1st to order taping
m urder confessions' (USA Today 18 July 2001: 3A). M aine has followed
Illinois, with more states to follow. Taping is required in Alaska and
Minnesota as a result of court rulings (Drizin and Leo 2004; Drizin and
Reich 2004).
22. See, e.g., the stress on the need for supervisors to identify what went
well and what could be improved (rather than negative criticism or
checking com pliance with PACE) in providing feedback on interviews
(National Crim e Faculty 1998b: 7 -8 ; cf. Audit Com m ission 1993:
27).
23. In criminal justice, there is, however, no indication of the drift towards
punitive regulation which Baldwin (2004) identifies in other fields.
347
Investigative Interviewing
R e fe re n c e s
348
Regulating police interrogation
349
Investigative Interviewing
Hood, C., Scott, C., James, O. and Travers, T. (1999) Regulation inside
Government. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Ignatieff, M. (2004) The Lesser Evil: Political Ethics in an Age o f Terror.
Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.
Inbau, F.E., Reid, J.E., Buckley, J.P. and Jayne, B.C. (2001) Criminal Interrogation
and Confessions (4th edn). Gaithersburg, MD: Aspen.
Innes, M. (2003) Investigating Murder. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Krantz, S., Gilman, B., Benda, C , Hallstrom, C.R. and Nadworny, E.J. (1979)
Police Policymaking: The Boston Experience. Lexington, MA: Lexington
Books.
Kremnitzer, M. and Segev, R. (2000) 'The legality of interrogational torture'
Israel Law Review 34: 509-59.
Krygier, M. (1990) 'Marxism and the rule of law: reflections after the fall of
communism', Lazv and Social Inquiry, 15: 633-63.
Lacey, N. (1992) 'The jurisprudence of discretion: escaping the legal paradigm',
in K. Hawkins (ed.) The Uses o f Discretion. Oxford: Clarendon Press.
Langbein, J.H. (2004) 'The legal history of torture', in S. Levinson (ed.)
Torture. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Lee, J.A. (1990) Report o f the Royal Commission o f Inquiry into the Arrest,
Charging and Withdrawal o f Charges Against Harold James Blackburn. Sydney:
Government Printer.
Leo, R. (1994) 'Police interrogation and social control', Social and Legal Studies,
3: 93-120.
Leo, R. (1996) 'Inside the interrogation room', Journal o f Criminal Lazv and
Criminology, 86: 266-303.
Leo, R. (2001) 'False confessions: causes, consequences and solutions', in
S.A. Westervelt and J.A. Humphrey (eds) Wrongly Convicted: Perspectives
on Failed Justice. New Bruswick: Rutgers University Press.
Levinson, S. (ed.) (2004) Torture. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Mackey, C. with Miller, G. (2004) The Interrogator's War. London: John
Murray.
Maguire, M. (2003) 'Criminal investigation and crime control', in T. Newburn
(ed.) Handbook o f Policing. Cullompton: Willan Publishing.
McConville, M. (1989) 'Weaknesses in the British judicial system', Times
Higher Education Supplement, 3 November.
McConville, M., Sanders, A. and Leng, R. (1991) The Case fo r the Prosecution.
London: Routledge.
McGurk, B.J., Carr, M.J. and McGurk, D. (1993) Investigative Interviewing
Courses fo r Police Officers: An Evaluation. Police Research Series Paper 4.
London: Home Office Police Department.
Milne, R. and Bull, R. (1999) Investigative Interviezving. Chichester: Wiley.
Moston, S. and Engelberg, T. (1993) 'Police questioning techniques in tape
recorded interviews with criminal suspects', Policing and Society, 3:
223-37.
National Crime Faculty (1998a) A Practical Guide to Investigative Interviezving.
Bramshill: NCF.
350
Regulating police interrogation
351
Conclu sion
Tom Williamson
353
Investigative Interviewing
354
Conclusion
355
Investigative Interviewing
R eferences
356
Index
357
Investigative Interviewing
358
Index
359
Investigative Interviewing
360
Index
361
Investigative Interviewing
362
Index
363
Investigative Interviewing
364
Index
365
Investigative Interviewing
366
Index
367
Investigative Interviewing
368
Index
369
Investigative Interview ing
370