Consciousness 1st Edition Paul Budra
Consciousness 1st Edition Paul Budra
Consciousness 1st Edition Paul Budra
com
https://textbookfull.com/product/shakespeare-and-
consciousness-1st-edition-paul-budra/
https://textbookfull.com/product/mary-wroth-and-shakespeare-routledge-
studies-in-shakespeare-1st-edition-paul-salzman-marion-wayne-davies/
textbookfull.com
https://textbookfull.com/product/shakespeare-and-protestant-poetics-
jason-gleckman/
textbookfull.com
https://textbookfull.com/product/full-stack-javascript-learn-backbone-
js-node-js-and-mongodb-mardan-azat/
textbookfull.com
Introducing Bootstrap 4: Create Powerful Web Applications
Using Bootstrap 4.5 - Second Edition Joerg Krause
https://textbookfull.com/product/introducing-bootstrap-4-create-
powerful-web-applications-using-bootstrap-4-5-second-edition-joerg-
krause/
textbookfull.com
https://textbookfull.com/product/micro-and-nanotechnology-in-vaccine-
development-1st-edition-mariusz-skwarczynski/
textbookfull.com
https://textbookfull.com/product/the-british-prime-minister-in-the-
core-executive-birgit-bujard/
textbookfull.com
https://textbookfull.com/product/this-is-not-dixie-racist-violence-in-
kansas-1861-1927-brent-m-s-campney/
textbookfull.com
Transforming the Landscape Rock Art and the Mississippean
Cosmos Carol Diaz-Granados
https://textbookfull.com/product/transforming-the-landscape-rock-art-
and-the-mississippean-cosmos-carol-diaz-granados/
textbookfull.com
Shakespeare and
Consciousness
Series Editors
Bruce McConachie
Department of Theatre Arts
University of Pittsburgh
Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania, USA
Blakey Vermeule
Department of English
Stanford University
Stanford, California, USA
This series offers cognitive approaches to understanding perception, emo-
tions, imagination, meaning-making, and the many other activities that
constitute both the production and reception of literary texts and embod-
ied performances.
Shakespeare
and Consciousness
Editors
Paul Budra Clifford Werier
Simon Fraser University Mount Royal University
Burnaby, British Columbia, Canada Calgary, Alberta, Canada
This work is subject to copyright. All rights are solely and exclusively licensed by the
Publisher, whether the whole or part of the material is concerned, specifically the rights of
translation, reprinting, reuse of illustrations, recitation, broadcasting, reproduction on
microfilms or in any other physical way, and transmission or information storage and retrieval,
electronic adaptation, computer software, or by similar or dissimilar methodology now
known or hereafter developed.
The use of general descriptive names, registered names, trademarks, service marks, etc. in this
publication does not imply, even in the absence of a specific statement, that such names are
exempt from the relevant protective laws and regulations and therefore free for general use.
The publisher, the authors and the editors are safe to assume that the advice and information
in this book are believed to be true and accurate at the date of publication. Neither the pub-
lisher nor the authors or the editors give a warranty, express or implied, with respect to the
material contained herein or for any errors or omissions that may have been made.
vii
viii SERIES EDITORS’ PREFACE
Blakey Vermeule
Bruce McConachie
NOTES
1. Susan Carey, The Origin of Concepts (Oxford: Oxford University Press,
2009), 4.
2. See Lisa Zunshine, “What is Cognitive Cultural Studies?” Introduction to
Cognitive Cultural Studies, ed. Lisa Zunshine (Baltimore: Johns Hopkins
University Press), 1.
Visit https://textbookfull.com
now to explore a rich
collection of eBooks, textbook
and enjoy exciting offers!
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
xi
CONTENTS
1 Introduction 1
Paul Budra and Clifford Werier
xiii
xiv CONTENTS
Index 297
CHAPTER 1
Introduction
Consciousness as a critical category has largely been absent from the pro-
liferation of literary and historical studies that have interrogated what has
come to be known as “the early modern subject.”1 Instead, scholars have
turned to the “body-mind,” “inwardness,” “emotions,” and “selves” as
ways of approaching the messy and often confusing matrices of expe-
rience that constitute awareness, almost as if “consciousness,” a term
which captures the very immanence of life, is too unstable.2 To be fair,
this resistance to consciousness has occurred not only in the study of early
modern literatures and cultures; until recently it has been shunted to the
side in many philosophic and scientific discourses because it was per-
ceived as intractable, “the last surviving mystery.”3 Thomas Nagel sum-
marized the problem: “Without consciousness the mind-body problem
would be much less interesting. With consciousness it seems hopeless.”4
P. Budra ( )
Simon Fraser University,
Burnaby, BC, Canada
C. Werier
Mount Royal University,
Calgary, AB, Canada
“that which is explicitly absent is me.”12 Deacon takes over 500 pages to
explain the mysteries of this cryptic conclusion and to explore the intrin-
sic incompleteness of self and identity before finally concluding that the
“core characteristics of conscious experience—are accurate reflections of
the fact that self is literarily sui generis, emerging each moment from
what is not there.”13
Clearly, debates about consciousness are now at the forefront of emerg-
ing disciplines that are necessarily hybrid (Deacon is a professor of biological
anthropology and neuroscience), as approaches to questions of conscious-
ness seem to promote the dissolution of traditional academic boundar-
ies. In literary studies, consciousness has been approached indirectly
through a number of cognitive biocritical lenses that examine particular
sensory, emotional, mnemonic, psychological, and evolutionary processes
and apply these discrete operations to cultural practices and problems of
interpretation.14 Although literary study’s reduction of consciousness into
its cognitive constituents was not part of a systematic critical strategy, it
makes sense that individuated theories of cognition would provide more
concentrated applications than the sometimes overwhelming ideas related
to mapping the totality of consciousness. So, for example, Distributed
Cognition, Cognitive Blends, and Theory of Mind provided focused criti-
cal models that could be easily applied to literary artifacts, practices, and
characters in Shakespeare, whereas the Global Workspace Theory and
other more generalized approaches to consciousness may be too esoteric
to yield tangible results.15
This book emerges as a response to recent scientific and cultural
preoccupations with consciousness and our curiosity about how such
a category shift could lead to new insights about Shakespeare and his
world. Because consciousness, neuroscience, the brain, mindfulness,
and other cognitive categories are ubiquitous in today’s print and elec-
tronic media—and cognitive science continues to capture the attention of
scholars across disciplines16—we hoped to harness some of this energy by
deploying Shakespeare and consciousness together, allowing scholars to
explore the synergies that emerged. We are not saying that considerations
of consciousness have been absent from critical inquiries in Shakespeare
before this collection, as the numerous works on inwardness, self, self-
consciousness, embodied self, and emotions attest to the interest that
scholars have lavished on the mind-body problem. We are suggesting,
however, that considering consciousness from multiple perspectives—as
a historically phenomenal process, a materially determined product, a
4 P. BUDRA AND C. WERIER
And second, the science of the cognitivist critics is often suspect or applied
in the loosest of fashions. Like Werier, Pechter sees in literary criticism’s
eager, and sometimes sloppy, appropriation of these scientific theories a
rationalization of its methods in an attempt to reclaim its cultural author-
ity. A turn toward consciousness and/or cognition will not, in Pechter’s
view, solve the problem of the decline of the humanities, but “nothing else
will either.” Instead he turns to a book that he admires, William Flesch’s
Comeuppance, which he calls “especially successful in its conjunction of
cognitive ideas with Shakespeare study” largely because Flesch does not
claim “determining privilege” for his theoretical arguments and use of psy-
chology. Rather, he “subordinates its cutting-edge evolutionary theory to
an earlier tradition of romantic commentary.”
In “Hamlet in the Bat Cave,” Paul Budra assesses the ontological
dilemma raised by the attribution of consciousness to literary characters
using Hamlet, the poster child for the literary depictions of conscious-
ness, as a test case. He surveys the philosophical literature about literary
characters and juxtaposes it with a long-standing test for the presence of
consciousness, a conjunction that yields an enticing paradox: Hamlet, as
a literary character, has no consciousness, yet for hundreds of years critics
and theatrical practitioners have had no problem imagining and writing
about this nonexistent quality. Budra deconstructs this paradox in two
ways: first, he demonstrates that Hamlet’s consciousness exists “only in
the truth of propositions expressible in human language” and that the
language which defines Hamlet’s consciousness is not only the text of
Hamlet, but the supplementary textual, historical, or theoretical contexts
that are brought to bear on it; second, he argues that Hamlet in particu-
lar has become the nexus for consciousness studies because Hamlet does
display a self-consciousness of the type that was discussed and analyzed in
early modern England. What is not clear is whether this self-consciousness
is synonymous with self-aware consciousness, that elusive defining step in
the evolution toward modern subjectivity, or is an ahistorical attribution.
The next group of essays examines the conjunction of conscious-
ness studies and early modern theatrical practice. Amy Cook, in “King
of Shadows: Early Modern Characters and Actors,” places the scholar-
ship surrounding literary characters within the context of embodied and
distributed cognition and cognitive linguistics. She describes art as a
“cognitive artifact” that illuminates how societies deal with what Andy
Clark has called “representationally hungry” issues, and focuses on how
Shakespeare’s plays make us aware of our own experience of consciousness
INTRODUCTION 9
in which individual actors did not possess the entire play script and so
would be in a state of active anticipation, she demonstrates through
a close reading of the first two scenes of Hamlet how the “Carefully
constructed holes in knowledge combine with contradictory impres-
sions to give both characters and audience a complex phenomenological
experience.”
The next papers return to the age-old puzzle of the mind-body rela-
tionship by focusing on the somatic, the out-of-body experience and
time-consciousness. In “‘Being Unseminared’: Pleasure, Instruction, and
Playing the Queen in Anthony and Cleopatra,” Andrew Brown argues
that in Anthony and Cleopatra, Shakespeare draws upon humoral psy-
chophysiology to present characters who are defined “not by individual-
ized subjectivity but by radically expansive, fluid, and generative forms of
conscious experience.” Perhaps more than any other play, Anthony and
Cleopatra embeds its characters in networks of dramatic practice to prob-
lematize Shakespeare’s age’s increasing interest in the distinction between
inward reflection, what we may call consciousness, and the physical
world. Brown illustrates this embeddedness by focusing on the character
of Mardian the eunuch and the boy actor who must have played him.
Starting with the culturally loaded image of the eunuch’s mutilated body,
he parallels the training of boy actors in the early modern professional
theater with the understanding of the consciousness of another self and
ties both to the somatic embodiment of consciousness and the distributed
cognition required by early modern theatrical practice.
In “Bodies and Selves: Autoscopy, Out-of-Body Experiences, Mind-
wandering and Early Modern Consciousness,” Jan Purnis considers the
complex history of and contemporary theorizing about such altered states,
how they are often portrayed in literary and dramatic texts, and how they
shed light on early modern conceptions of embodiment and selfhood.
This divisibility of consciousness is expressed in autoscopic experience
through the phenomenon of the double, an experience described by
Aristotle’s story of Antipheron, and explored in a number of twin plays,
including Twelfth Night and The Comedy of Errors, where the division of
a self is repeatedly imagined. Citing Lacan, Purnis links autoscopic illu-
sions to mirror ideas in Hamlet, and turning to out-of-body experiences,
Purnis offers historical examples linked to the early modern idea of ecstasy
and “being beside oneself” in The Winter’s Tale, Antony and Cleopatra,
and The Merchant of Venice. Finally, the phenomenon of mind wandering
is examined as a subcategory of ecstasy and daydreaming through both
Random documents with unrelated
content Scribd suggests to you:
Twigs they shook, | and blood they tried:
Rich fare in Ægir’s | hall they found.
[140]
[141]
Thor spake:
6. “May we win, dost thou think, | this whirler of
water?”
Tyr spake:
[143]
[144]
17. . . . . . . . . | . . . . . . . .
Fain to row on the sea | was Veur, he said,
If the giant bold | would give him bait.
Hymir spake:
Hymir spake:
[146]
26. . . . . . . . . | . . . . . . . .
Joyless as back | they rowed was the giant;
Speechless did Hymir | sit at the oars,
With the rudder he sought | a second wind.
Hymir spake:
27. “The half of our toil | wilt thou have with me,
[147]
And now make fast | our goat of the flood;
Or home wilt thou bear | the whales to the house,
Across the gorge | of the wooded glen?”
Hymir spake:
38. Not long had they fared | ere one there lay
Of Hlorrithi’s goats | half-dead on the ground;
In his leg the pole-horse | there was lame;
The deed the evil | Loki had done.
[150]
[138]
[Contents]
NOTES
[139]
13. Eight: the giant’s glance, besides breaking the beam, knocks
down all the kettles with such violence that all but the one under
which Thor and Tyr are hiding are broken.
14. Hymir’s wrath does not permit him to ignore the duties of a host
to his guests, always strongly insisted on.
19. Many editors combine lines 3 and 4 with stanza 20. Fortress,
etc.: the ox’s head; cf. introductory note concerning the diction of this
poem. Several editors assume a lacuna after stanza 19, but this
seems unnecessary. [145]
21. Lord of the goats: Thor, because of his goat-drawn chariot. Ape-
begotten: Hymir; the word “api,” rare until relatively late times in its
literal sense, is fairly common with the meaning of “fool.” Giants were
generally assumed to be stupid. Steed of the rollers: a ship, because
boats were pulled up on shore by means of rollers.
23. Warder of men: Thor; cf. stanza 11. Worm’s destroyer: likewise
Thor, who in the last battle slays, and is slain by, Mithgarthsorm; cf.
Voluspo, 56. The foe of the gods: Mithgarthsorm, who lies in the sea,
and surrounds the whole earth. [146]
29. Snorri says nothing of this episode of Hymir’s cup. The glass
which cannot be broken appears in the folklore of various races.
31. The loved one: Hymir’s wife and Tyr’s mother; cf. stanza 8 and
note. The idea that a giant’s skull is harder than stone or anything
else is characteristic of the later Norse folk-stories, and [148]in one of
the so-called “mythical sagas” we find a giant actually named Hard-
Skull.
38. According to Snorri, when Thor set out with Loki (not Tyr) for the
giants’ land, he stopped first at a peasant’s house (cf. stanza 7 and
note). There he proceeded to cook his own goats for supper. The
peasant’s son, Thjalfi, eager to get at the marrow, split one of the
leg-bones with his knife. The next morning, when Thor was ready to
proceed with his journey, he called the goats to life again, but one of
them proved irretrievably lame. His wrath led the peasant to give him
both his children as [150]servants (cf. stanza 39). Snorri does not
indicate that Loki was in any way to blame.
40. The translation of the last two lines is mostly guesswork, as the
word rendered “gods” is uncertain, and the one rendered “at the
autumn-time” is quite obscure. [151]
[Contents]
LOKASENNA
Loki’s Wrangling
[Contents]
Introductory Note
The Lokasenna is found only in Regius, where it follows the
Hymiskvitha; Snorri quotes four lines of it, grouped together as a
single stanza.
The poem is one of the most vigorous of the entire collection, and
seems to have been preserved in exceptionally good condition. The
exchange or contest of insults was dear to the Norse heart, and the
Lokasenna consists chiefly of Loki’s taunts to the assembled gods
and goddesses, and their largely ineffectual attempts to talk back to
him. The author was evidently well versed in mythological lore, and
the poem is full of references to incidents not elsewhere recorded.
As to its date and origin there is the usual dispute, but the latter part
of the tenth century and Iceland seem the best guesses.
The prose notes are long and of unusual interest. The introductory
one links the poem closely to the Hymiskvitha, much as the
Reginsmol, Fafnismol and Sigrdrifumol are linked together; the
others fill in the narrative gaps in the dialogue—very like stage
directions,—and provide a conclusion by relating Loki’s punishment,
which, presumably, is here connected with the wrong incident. It is
likely that often when the poem was recited during the two centuries
or so before it was committed to writing, the speaker inserted some
such explanatory comments, and the compiler of the collection
followed this example by adding such explanations as he thought
necessary. The Lokasenna is certainly much older than the
Hymiskvitha, the connection between them being purely one of
subject-matter; and the twelfth-century compiler evidently knew a
good deal less about mythology than the author whose work he was
annotating.
[Contents]
Eldir spake:
Loki spake:
Eldir spake:
Loki spake:
Then Loki went into the hall, but when they who were
there saw who had entered, they were all silent.
Loki spake:
Bragi spake:
Loki spake:
Othin spake:
Bragi spake: