Affective Color CHI 2017
Affective Color CHI 2017
Results
As our data distribution was marginally non-normal, we used
the Kruskal-Wallis test for significance (the rank-based non-
parametric equivalent of an ANOVA for multi-factor data).
Affect had a significant effect on both lightness X2 (7, 8607)
=199.6250, p<.0001 and chroma X2 (7, 8607) =387.7106,
p<.0001. Calm, Playful and Exciting images were lighter
than Disturbing and Negative. Negative and Calm were less
saturated than Playful and Exciting. We see patterns in Calm
palettes containing a larger concentration of light and desatu-
rated blues and greens. Trustworthy also has blues, purples
and some greens. Playful and Exciting use highly saturated Figure 2. Study 2 interface
colors like reds, vibrant greens and blues with Exciting having
relatively more dark reds. Disturbing has a larger distribution
of dark browns, blues, reds and black. Negative used more
grey and muted browns. With the exception of Positive, these
overall balance by adjusting a slider to control alpha. When
results mirror what color psychology would predict. We were
the participant was finished with the color selection, s/he rated
surprised by the amount of browns and dark colors in Positive.
satisfaction using a slider between 0 and 10 to indicate how
successfully s/he felt the colors expressed the concept. At the
STUDY 2: USER-DESIGNED PALETTES
beginning of each trial the initial alpha value was set to 1 and
We used these results to design colors for a user study. We
the visualizations were colored in neutral gray.
asked a visualization color expert to design a smaller set for
use in our next study. She reduced the set to 36 colors, using a Because we were interested in the relative locations of color
combination of k-means clustering to combine similar colors, groups in the PAD space, we paired endpoint tasks of the
and filtering to remove colors that were either too light to offer affect model (Calm-Exciting, Positive-Negative) to establish
sufficient contrast with the background or too dark for the hues differences (if any), drawing from the core theoretical work
to be easily visible when small. She then used visual tools and in mapping affect. Similarly, we considered these differences
visualizations of the associated color metrics to establish that in the pragmatic case (Serious-Playful) with clear semantic
the resulting colors offered a balanced mix of CIELAB hue, opposites. Where there was not a clear opposite (Trustworthy,
chroma and lightness values. Disturbing) we did not pair the tasks but presented them
singly. This provided a relative rather than an absolute measure
We ran a pilot study where people with design experience
of how palettes might differ with respect to comparative affect.
created palettes based on affect. The goal of our study was
We used this design because we are not expecting to be able
to see whether they would consistently assign different color
to absolutely quantify all possible palettes with respect to
sets for each of our 8 affective categories in simple visualiza-
affect, but rather to understand what might make a desired
tion tasks. The results from the pilot showed affect strongly
affective impression comparatively stronger or weaker. In
influenced colors chosen, similar to those in Study 1. We then
the cases of Trustworthy and Disturbing (both affects with
expanded our study to a wider population, to see if the results
strong ecological validity that are not clear opposites), we
were specific to designers and to get a larger data sample. We
simply sought to see how they might relate to the other affect
extended the color set to 41 colors, adding more dark, because
groupings.
our results indicated that the original palette did not provide
quite enough different colors for some of the darker affects. Each affect task was independent and required separate color
We show the detailed results for this larger experiment. assignments, alpha and satisfaction rating. Participants could
reassign the 5 colors in the palettes until satisfied: they were
Method not constrained to mapping a color to a particular spatial loca-
Participants completed categorical coloring tasks for two sim- tion or size in the visualization. The experiment began with
ple visualizations (bar chart or map) using the interface shown a training task in which participants colored Happy and Sad
in Figure 2 to select palettes of 5 colors for each task from a affects. No time limit was set on training; participants pro-
set of 41. We chose a map and a bar chart as they are familiar ceeded to the main study when ready. The experiment was
information representations. Participants were instructed to hosted on our university web server. People could stop at any
select colors to convey an affect from our eight affective cate- time, and login to the system later to complete the study: the
gories. They were not told what the data represented other than system retained state. We report only on results where all tasks
affective intent. They could additionally modify the palette were completed.
Factors and metrics
We had two independent variables: Affect (8) and Visualiza-
tion (2: bar chart, US map). Our raw dependent variables were
color metrics (lightness, chroma, hue), alpha, and satisfaction
rating.
DISCUSSION
What do these results tell us? First and foremost, we are able
to reliably associate color and palette properties with affective
response even in the limited scope of color selection in simple
information visualization representations, extending previous
work in individual color-affect relations. Our studies show
that both the perceptual properties of small color sets, and
the composition patterns of how they are used together, are
affectively distinct. From our studies in image analysis (S1)
and user-designed palettes (S2), we were able to algorithmi-
cally define a simple metric of palette weight based on color
frequency use for each affect that proved a reliable predictor
of preferred affective palettes in a final validation study (S3).
We see consistent patterns in lightness related to affect across Figure 8. The L,C,H distributions for each affect. Hue is rotated by 60°to
all studies. Calm, Playful, Positive and to a lesser extent group warm and cool colors, all values are normalized to a common
Trustworthy are lightest, while Serious, Disturbing and range
Negative are darker. This confirms H1. Similarly, we also
saw consistent use of higher chroma colors for Playful, Ex- about which colors go with others, as we are exploring in the
citing, Positive and Disturbing: where Calm, Serious and network diagrams.
to a lesser extent Trustworthy were less saturated. These
While we cannot reliably profile each affect, even being able to
comparisons are significant, and confirm our hypotheses that
say what colors to avoid for specific affects can be useful. For
lightness and chroma are linked to affect. This indicates that
example, we can say with some authority that highly saturated
any color set intended for use in expressing these affects must
provide enough variation in both lightness and chroma. light colors will NOT be appropriate for Serious or Trust, or
Calm; light blues, beiges and greys are never likely to convey
One of our primary goals in this is to create guidelines for de- Playful; dark red and browns are not Positive; and light colors,
sign and design evaluation simple enough to be programmed, particularly green, do not communicate Negative affect.
similar to work by Bartram et al. [1]. While these will never
The network models help us visualize palette composition
replace the skill of an experienced designer, they can reduce
patterns: groupings that show what colors are likely to be used
the time spent exploring options that simply don’t work and
provide reasonable starting grounds for further refinement. together. Certain affects showed different palette composition
patterns, Calm, Exciting and Playful palettes tightly clus-
Figure 10 summarizes the mapping between affect and color tered around the main colors, confirming high co-occurrence
metrics for each affect. Here, each of lightness, chroma and of these colors in most palettes. In contrast, certain colors in
hue have been mapped to a common range, then plotted. For Positive were only used in combination with bright yellow.
hue, the important metric is whether the colors are warm or Greens form an important cluster in Positive. Serious, Nega-
cool. We make this easier to see by rotating the hue angle by tive and Disturbing palettes show loose sub clusters anchored
60 degrees so that low values are cool, high values are warm. by a central set of core colors. Trustworthy shows a clear
The resulting metrics are labeled (NL, NC, N SH). The box example of two thematic strategies (blue-gray, green-gray)
plots show the distributions. Our hypothesis of hue association bridged by a single common color (yellow). In future work,
with affect is supported by these distributions. we hope to better understand what these patterns mean and
how they might be applied.
Based on the results of study 3, we speculate that if a palette
contains colors near the median of L,C,H for a specific affect, We sought to see if there were general color associations with
then the resulting palette is likely to be judged as matching the more abstract dimensions of valence (Positive-Negative)
that affect, especially if the distribution is tight. Some affects and arousal (Calm-Exciting) that would translate (lend color
have clearer L,C,H “profiles” than others. Of the Core affects, elements) to the specific expressions of Playful, Serious,
Calm is the strongest, requiring high L, low C and cool colors. Trustworthy and Disturbing. We certainly saw evidence
Negative has similar properties for C and H, but dark colors of these crossovers in our limited set of results. Establishing
predominate. Exciting and Positive, however, while distinctly such reliable associations leads to the question of if and how
different than Calm and Negative, are not very different from we might algorithmically determine color selection based on
each other, or from the pragmatic affect Playful. The profiles the desired affect’s location in the PAD affect space. In other
in the other pragmatic affects show similar overlaps, as we words, given a desire to enhance a visualization as more “re-
might expect. This is why we need more nuanced analysis assuring”, for example, can we quantify where “reassuring”
plots between Calm-Exciting and Positive-Negative and de- forms and data contexts. While this is not enough to clearly
termine the relative hues, chroma and lightness values as a distinguish all affects, it helps limit the design space, which
weighted contribution from each? In future work, we plan to can be of significant practical value. For example, one could
explore how the color patterns we see in the core affects may extend Colorgorical[11] to include affect by using L, C and H
generalize to other pragmatic affects. filters.