Disgusted and Afraid Consumer Choices Under The Threat of Contagious Disease
Disgusted and Afraid Consumer Choices Under The Threat of Contagious Disease
Disgusted and Afraid Consumer Choices Under The Threat of Contagious Disease
CHELSEA GALONI
GREGORY S. CARPENTER
373
374 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH
overwhelmingly suggests that, when an individual is con- on the basis of how the situation is judged. For instance,
fronted with the threat of contagious disease, the dominant Smith and Ellsworth (1985) identified six dimensions on
emotional response is disgust (Han, Lerner, and which emotions can be classified and differentiated: atten-
Zeckhauser 2012; Rozin and Haidt 2013; Rozin, Haidt, and tion (whether people are driven to pay attention to, ignore,
McCauley 2008; Tybur et al. 2013). When a consumer or avoid the situation), certainty (whether the situation is
senses the presence of contaminants in the environment predictable or not), control (whether the situation is con-
and feels the emotion of disgust, the dominant behavioral trolled by the person, someone else, or something else),
tendency is to reject the contaminant and to isolate the self pleasantness (whether the situation is pleasant or unpleas-
from potential infection by reducing consumption (Han ant), responsibility (whether the situation was brought
et al. 2012; Lerner and Keltner 2000; Lerner, Small, and about by the self or others), and anticipated effort (how
Loewenstein 2004). much the person has to work in the situation).
can simultaneously experience fear and disgust, even Regardless of whether this cue signals a real risk of harm
though the two emotions conflict on appraisals of certainty to the observer (e.g., if they are immune), the salience of
and attention (Morales et al. 2012). the cue nevertheless allows for an appraisal that there are
Despite some research documenting the phenomenon of contaminants present. This appraisal elicits a disgust re-
mixed emotions with conflicting appraisals, the appraisal sponse. In addition, the contagious disease cue often
tendency framework provides little insight and guidance affords an appraisal that there is a real threat imminent in
on how or whether conflicting appraisals of mixed emo- the environment that is out of their control (e.g., the lack of
tional states resolve within the individual, or how the expe- control over getting sick). This appraisal elicits a fear
rience of multiple emotions might influence behavioral response.
tendencies. This has led to a call for an increased focus on
the psychology of these mixed emotional states and how Conflicting Appraisals in a Joint Fear and
situations where appraisals of certainty or uncertainty can be Resolving Attention: Avoiding Pathogens and
directed toward both the source and outcome, the conflicting Approaching Control
certainty appraisals of disgust and fear may not cancel each
other out, and people could thus feel both disgusted and Another appraisal dimension on which fear and disgust
afraid simultaneously. More formally: do not align is that of attentional activity. The experience
of disgust is accompanied by an intense desire to avoid and
H1a: When confronted with a contagious disease cue, con- expel the source of contamination (i.e., a strong appraisal
sumers feel disgust from appraising a certain source of of attentional avoidance). Indeed, research on the neuro-
threat and fear from an uncertain outcome of that threat. physiological response to disgust has shown that people en-
But how might these diverging certainty appraisals af- gage in significant and immediate repulsion behaviors
fect subsequent behavior? We propose that the appraisal of when confronted with disgusting stimuli (Marks and Nesse
1994; Neuberg et al. 2011). Attention narrows to the
empirical results show that participants do indeed avoid the tendencies toward regaining control over the outcome. The
source of pathogens (e.g., by spending more of the dirty joint experience of disgust and fear thus leads to greater ac-
currency and de-valuing products that the currency may tion tendencies than fear alone and greater approach behav-
have come into contact with; Di Muro and Noseworthy iors than disgust alone (hypothesis 1c), thus
2013; Galoni and Noseworthy 2015), and also actively ap- asymmetrically increasing preference for the familiar (hy-
proach and increase the value they place on products that pothesis 2). We test these hypotheses across six studies.
help restore a sense of control over the outcome—in this
case, cleansing products (Galoni and Noseworthy 2015). STUDY 1: CONSUMER PANEL DATA
Thus, the joint fear and disgust response to contagious dis-
ANALYSIS
ease cues might narrow avoidance behaviors to objects as-
sociated with the source of threat, specifically, but increase Our initial analysis examines whether cues of contagious
FIGURE 1
AVERAGE ILLNESS INDEX BY WEEK OF THE YEAR ACROSS THE UNITED STATES.
batteries and paper towels) during the same shopping trip, families enjoy eating them over the holidays, or more soup
each of these products would have its own line in the data, because it is just generally colder in December than in
and we control for any dependency by including a trip con- April. To control for this potentially shared seasonality, we
trol in our model. However, all units purchased of the same included a measure of how sick people were, on average,
product in the same shopping trip (e.g., a purchase of five in any given week of the year (an effect of illness within
cans of private label chicken noodle soup in the same shop- week of the year). Also, the datasets we use were newer to
ping trip) would be included in the same row. states, retailers, and hospitals in 2009 than they were in
Assessing Familiarity. The panel data provides the 2014, and companies often introduce more products over
household’s entire purchase history in each category in our time. We therefore included a variable that controls for in-
time frame. We operationalize familiarity based on the creased compliance in reporting over time as well as
share a given product takes up within the household’s pur- increases in sales volume over time (an effect of time). We
STUDY 2: CONTAGIOUS DISEASE, emotion words were four items that measured disgust
DISGUST, AND FEAR (“Unclean,” “Disgusted,” “Dirty,” “Revolted;” a ¼ .89).
Likewise, five items measured fear (“Scared,” “Anxious,”
We designed study 2 to test whether cues of contagious “Afraid,” “Nervous,” “Vulnerable;” a ¼ .95).
disease elicit a joint disgust and fear response in consumers Measuring Preference. We measured preference for
(hypothesis 1a) and to assess whether this joint emotional the familiar under the guise of a second study interested in
response causes the asymmetric increase in preference for grocery shopping and food habits. First, all participants
familiar products (hypothesis 2). In addition, we use a non- were shown a list of 14 common grocery store items and
contagious disease as a secondary control condition to rule were asked to choose (by dragging and dropping) the five
out the alternative possibility that the pattern of results items they would be most likely to buy on their next shop-
found in the empirical analyses represents consumer re- ping trip. The 14 items were presented all together in a ran-
condition (M ¼ 1.74, SD ¼ 1.03; t(199) ¼ 2.14, p ¼ .034, was not the behavioral response to illness in general, but
d ¼ .46). Importantly, however, participants reading about rather the fear and disgust arising from contagious disease
the flu experienced significantly more disgust than partici- cues driving the effect. However, heart disease is an illness
pants reading about heart attacks (t(199) ¼ 3.66, p < .001, that can be managed through diet and it is possible that par-
d ¼ .57), which supports hypothesis 1a. ticipants in this condition are choosing more non-familiar
products because they are perceived as healthier.
Basket Composition. To assess whether asymmetric
preference for the familiar changes as a function of which
news story participants read, we first formed an index STUDY 3: USING A DIFFERENT
wherein we computed the proportion of culturally familiar CONTROL
items participants put in their baskets during the shopping
task. From the demographics collected, we had 180 self- We designed study 3 to replicate and extend study 2 us-
.90). We included an additional five emotion words (M ¼ 1.44, SD ¼ 0.81; t(317) ¼ 2.73, p ¼ .007, d ¼ .51).
(“Relieved,” “Lucky,” “Grateful,” “Fortunate,” “Secure”) There were no significant differences in disgust between
designed to measure how relieved participants might have participants in the control and unemployment fear condi-
felt after reading the news reports. We included this mea- tion (M ¼ 1.74, SD ¼ 1.15; t(317) ¼ 1.51, p ¼ .132, d ¼
sure, as well as questions in the demographics assessing .32). Thus, the unemployment reminder elicited more fear,
whether participants had received a flu shot and whether but not disgust, over the control, whereas the flu reminder
they were currently employed, to control for any variation elicited significantly more disgust and fear over the control
in actual circumstances that could have been influencing (hypothesis 1a).
the results. The measure of relief had no effect on results
Preference. We tested whether the flu news report led
and is thus not included in the analysis.
to taking greater action toward the threat (hypothesis 1c)
Measuring Preference. We measured preference for and an asymmetric preference for familiar products (hy-
linearly decreased in familiarity to a North American audi- response—and not other features of the flu specifically—
ence (see web appendix), so we treated restaurant choice as that are driving the behavioral response to contagious dis-
a continuous variable where lower values represent a pref- ease cues. To do so, we use a different disease (shingles),
erence for familiarity. A one-way analysis of variance on hold disease constant, and orthogonally manipulate
restaurant choice with reminder condition as the predictor whether news about the disease affords appraisals of salient
revealed a marginally significant effect of condition (F(2, contaminants and uncertainty over outcomes. Second, we
317) ¼ 2.58, p ¼ .077). The planned contrasts revealed assess the generalizability of our effect to nonfood catego-
that participants reading about the flu did indeed choose ries. To do so, we examine preference for a pair of head-
more-familiar restaurants (M ¼ 2.09, SD ¼ 1.01) over par- phones that are either framed as a familiar and trusted
ticipants in the control condition (M ¼ 2.49, SD ¼ 1.14; brand or as a novel and innovative brand. We expect that
t(317) ¼ 2.27, p ¼ .024). However, participants reading when reading about the shingles causes appraisals of out-
These participants also read that a close coworker arrived attention check. The remaining participants (N ¼ 516, Mage
at work complaining of symptoms, but that their doctor ¼ 37.69, 49.6% females) are included in the analysis
had recently told them they did not have the dormant below.
disease.
Preference. We expected participants appraising both
We ran a pretest (N ¼ 160, Mage ¼ 36.98, 55% females)
outcome uncertainty and salient contaminants to show an
to establish that the manipulation of uncertainty in these
asymmetric preference for the Beats headphone framed as
scenarios elicited fear whereas the manipulation of symp-
traditional. To assess this hypothesis, we ran a three-way
tom visibility in these scenarios elicited disgust. A two-
analysis of variance on the average rating of the three lik-
way ANOVA revealed a significant main effect of symp-
tom visibility on perceived disgust (F(1, 155) ¼ 123.18, p ing items (a ¼ .91). None of the lower-order interactions
< .001, g2 ¼ .44) such that visible symptoms produced or main effects were significant except for a marginal out-
come uncertainty familiarity interaction (F(1, 506) ¼
Method
unit sales from one national cookie brand (OREOs) and therefore more popular, there was an effect of familiarity
one entire product category (canned soup) across more such that people bought fewer non-familiar products than
than 35,000 retail outlets in the United States.1 familiar products on average (b ¼ .32, SE ¼ 0.008, t ¼
We do not have individuals’ shopping histories on which 39.31, p < .001). However, both of these effects were
to build an individualized familiarity variable in this data qualified by a significant familiarity illness interaction (b
set, so we operationalize familiarity in this analysis by us- ¼ .08, SE ¼ 0.002, t ¼ 45.20, p < .001). This interac-
ing the market leader in the category as the familiar brand. tion is pictured in figure 3 and indicates that in line with
In the soup category, we used sales of the market-leading the hypothesis, people asymmetrically preferred
national brand (Campbell’s) as familiar and sales of any Campbell’s national brand and asymmetrically punished
private label product as less familiar. We coded sales of private label brands when they feared getting sick. While
the traditional OREO as familiar and sales of any product soup sales generally increased as people became sicker, a
FIGURE 3
THE EFFECT OF PATHOGEN PRESENCE ON SALES VOLUME OF FAMILIAR AND LESS-FAMILIAR SKUS ACROSS CANNED SOUP
AND OREO CATEGORIES.
preference, but rather due to new, less frequent consumers disease can have real consequences on shopping habits and
of soup entering the market when they are sick (e.g., cus- product choice.
tomers who only buy soup when they are sick and buy
Campbell’s simply because they have less expertise in the GENERAL DISCUSSION
category and therefore default to the national brand). It is
less likely (both conceptually and empirically given the Across two large empirical data analyses and four
lack of a main effect of illness on OREO sales in this anal- experiments, we find that cues of contagious disease have
ysis) that there exists a parallel segment of consumers who a meaningful and systematic impact on consumption be-
only buy OREOs when they have the flu and prefer tradi- havior. Our results suggest that cues of contagious disease
tional OREOs over nontraditional OREOs because they are evoke appraisals of outcome uncertainty and appraisals of
less familiar with the category. Instead, we can infer from salient contaminants, leading to a joint fear and disgust re-
the increase in unit sales of familiar and decrease in unit sponse in consumers. The joint disgust and fear response
sales of non-familiar OREOs that there is indeed some amplifies the unique behavioral tendencies of each emo-
amount of consumer switching away from the non-familiar tion, producing an asymmetric preference for familiar
and toward the familiar when people are afraid of becom- products and sometimes increasing purchase quantity over-
ing sick. all. Unlike disgust alone, which motivates rejection of the
contaminant, feeling fear and disgust motivates consumers
to seek familiar and helpful products to reestablish a sense
Discussion of control. Unlike fear alone, which can lead to inaction,
feeling disgust and fear together motivates taking action,
Across two product categories, one within-category amplifying the tendency to regain control, and resulting in
comparison and one within-brand comparison, the present an asymmetric preference for more-familiar alternatives.
analysis indicates that the presence of illness can systemat- Our findings contribute meaningfully to understanding
ically alter consumer behavior in a very real way that is how emotions and mixed emotional states can influence
practically relevant to marketers. While an empirical anal- consumption behavior. Most prior research on contagious
ysis lacks a direct measure of consumer emotion, taken to- cues examines the role of disgust specifically on consump-
gether with the results of the laboratory experiments, the tion behavior (Han et al. 2012; Lerner et al. 2004).
present analysis clearly highlights that cues of contagious Likewise, most prior research on emotions has studied
GALONI, CARPENTER, AND RAO 389
situations where discrete emotions diverge and how they in- Beyond suggesting that mixed emotions can have unique
dividually impact behavioral tendencies (Lerner and Keltner behavioral tendencies and calling for an increased effort in
2001; Lerner et al. 2004). Little research, however, exam- understanding joint emotional states, our findings suggest
ines the behavioral consequences of situations in which peo- that the specific appraisal dimensions that differentiate be-
ple feel both disgust and sadness or both anger and fear. tween discrete emotions in the laboratory might be thought
While the common notion might be that these emotions con- of differently when consumers name and assess their own
tribute discretely to behavior, here we contribute to the liter- emotional experience in daily life. For example, past re-
ature showing that there can be unique and emergent search has shown that consumers can isolate disgust from
behavioral tendencies when multiple emotions converge. fear on the basis of the appraisal dimension of uncertainty:
Cues of contagious disease—by eliciting both fear and dis- people call how they are feeling “fear” when the situation
gust—amplify the behavioral tendencies of each emotion is uncertain, whereas they call how they are feeling
why comforting products seem to be in high demand when the data from the CDC FluView Interactive service in July
consumers are confronted with contagious disease cues. 2016. The first author aggregated and analyzed these data
Our results have important implications for marketing along with the Nielsen retail scanner data under the guid-
practitioners. Many contagious diseases share the same ance of the second and third authors between September
seasonal patterns as the flu, wherein people are usually 2016 and September 2017. The first author received autho-
sicker in the winter than they are in the summer months. rization for access to the Nielsen consumer panel data
The present research suggests that understanding this sea- along with the retail scanner data in July 2016. The con-
sonality might be important for promotion decisions and sumer panel data were analyzed by the first author under
message framing. In particular, if a product is novel, mar- the guidance of the second and third authors between
keters might be better served promoting the product in the September and December 2018. The first author designed,
summer months when people are less likely to encounter collected, and analyzed studies 2 and 3 under the guidance
Morales, Andrea C., Eugenia C. Wu, and Gavan J. Fitzsimons Rozin, Paul, Linda Millman, and Carol Nemeroff (1986),
(2012), “How Disgust Enhances the Effectiveness of Fear “Operation of the Laws of Sympathetic Magic in Disgust and
Appeals,” Journal of Marketing Research, 49 (3), 383–93. Other Domains,” Journal of Personality and Social
Morales, Andrea C. and Gavan J. Fitzsimons (2007), “Product Psychology, 50 (4), 703–12.
Contagion: Changing Consumer Evaluations through Smith, Craig A. and Phoebe C. Ellsworth (1985), “Patterns of
Physical Contact with ‘Disgusting’ Products,” Journal of Cognitive Appraisal in Emotion,” Journal of Personality and
Marketing Research, 44 (2), 272–83. Social Psychology, 48 (4), 813–38.
Nemeroff, Carol, and Paul Rozin (1994), “The Contagion Concept So, Jane, Chethana Achar, Da Hee Han, Nidhi Agrawal, Adam
Adult Thinking in the United States: Transmission of Germs Duhachek, and Durairaj Maheswaran (2015), “The
and of Interpersonal Influence,” Ethos, 22 (2), 158–86. Psychology of Appraisal: Specific Emotions and Decision-
Neuberg, Steven L., Douglas T. Kenrick, and Mark Schaller Making,” Journal of Consumer Psychology, 25 (3), 359–71.
(2011), “Human Threat Management Systems: Self- Susskind, Joshua M., Daniel H. Lee, Andree Cusi, Roman Feiman,
Protection and Disease Avoidance,” Neuroscience & Wojtek Grabski, and Adam K. Anderson (2008), “Expressing