A Systematic Review of The Literature On The Effects of School Bu
A Systematic Review of The Literature On The Effects of School Bu
A Systematic Review of The Literature On The Effects of School Bu
2011
This work is made publicly available by the City University of New York (CUNY).
Contact: [email protected]
A SYSTEMATIC REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE ON THE EFFECTS OF SCHOOL BULLYING
FROM THE FRAMEWORK OF JURGEN HABERMAS’S THEORY OF COMMUNICATIVE
ACTION
by
GARY KOGAN
2011
ii
© 2011
Michael Fabricant
Date Executive Officer
Darryl Wheeler
Elizabeth Danto
Supervisory Committee
Abstract
by
Gary Kogan
This project is a systematic review of the US quantitative, empirical studies on the effects of
school bullying for the purpose of determining the degree to which Jurgen Habermas’s social
theory, the theory of communicative action, can be used to understand the constellation of
measured effects. School bullying is defined as a systematic abuse of power: the empirical
literature on school bullying, therefore, provides a large data set on the abuse of power. The
review finds strong consistency between the theory and the results of selected studies suggesting
that Habermas’s theory of communicative action can explain and predict the mechanisms through
which the bullying experience can affect the targeted child.
v
Acknowledgements
This dissertation would not have been possible without my wife, Rachel Epstein, whose support
has been unending and whose editing skills carried this project through. Thanks to my son,
Huaquan, for being a good sport about the time I spent away from him on weekends, for being
my key informant on playground play, and for teaching me how to throw a spiral. Great thanks to
Professor Mimi Abramovitz who has guided this project since its inception and whose social
justice commitment and ability to see the simple solution at the heart of a problem is an
inspiration. Special thanks to Committee members Professor Elizabeth Danto and Professor
Darrell Wheeler for enriching and deepening this project. Thanks also to Professor Irwin Epstein
for showing me how existing data can be used in creative ways and to Professor Frances Fox-
Piven for showing how powerful ideas can change the world. A special thanks to the librarians of
the CUNY and Westchester library systems for tracking down countless out-of-print books and
obscure materials. This project is dedicated to everyone who has ever stood up to a bully.
vi
TABLE OF CONTENTS
INTRODUCTION…………………………………………………………..…………………1
APPENDIX I…………………………………………………………………….……..……258
REFERENCES…………………………………………………………………….………...267
vii
INDEX OF FIGURES
INDEX OF TABLES
INTRODUCTION
This study will build theory on the problem of school bullying1, defined for the
purpose of the study as the enduring impingement of the autonomy of one or more
children by one or more peers with greater power such that it is not feasible for the
victims to either stop or change the interaction2. The social theory of German
will be used as the conceptual framework for understanding the nature of bullying and its
effects. This project will attempt to determine whether and to what degree his theory can
has been studied, occurring at similar rates across social and economic strata (Nansel et
al., 2001). Estimates of the prevalence of bullying vary from 7% to 35% based on
prevalence most likely due to local school and community effects (Olweus, 1993; Sharp,
school is the venue where children, by legal mandate, congregate, thus school becomes
the focus of social scientific research for several academic disciplines. Children are not
free to withdraw from school of their own volition if they are being bullied, and most
parents have few schools they can choose from to send their children to. For many
children school is the only venue in which they interact with non-family members or
1
The term ―bullying‖ will be used in this paper in lieu of ―peer victimization.‖ The two terms are used
synonymously in the literature, but ―bullying‖ is the more commonly used term.
2
This definition was distilled from the various definitions and aspects of the bullying phenomenon
elaborated in the Definitions section below.
2
people outside their religious or ethnic communities, making school the primary non-
family venue for social reproduction, the process of instilling social values, language, and
behaviors that occur among schoolchildren for a variety of motivations (Arora, 1996).
Bullying takes place at different levels of social interaction, from one-on-one aggression
to, more frequently, group levels where a regular audience to the bullying behavior
gathers (Bukowski & Adams, 2005; Salmivalli, Lagerspetz, Björkqvist, Österman, &
Kaukiainen, 1996). Two factors distinguish bullying from other forms of peer aggression:
an imbalance of power between the bully and victim and the endurance of the bullying
over time (Arora, 1994; Craig & Pepler, 2003; Rigby, 2002b; Rivers, Duncan, & Besag,
2007; P. K. Smith & Sharp, 1994). The aggressive acts of bullying all share the quality of
domains. The techniques of bullying are evolving with the availability of new technology
Three decades of research have shown that bullying is associated with an array of
debilitating consequences (Hawker & Boulton, 2000; Mynard, Joseph, & Alexander,
2000; Nishina, Juvonen, & Witkow, 2005; Olweus, 1980; Rigby & Slee, 2001; Erling
Roland, 2002; Sharp et al., 2000; Shellard & Turner, 2004) for both victims and their
bullies. Bullying contributes to problems with school climate, depressing academic scores
for the individual child and the school (Juvonen, Nishina, & Graham, 2000; Schwartz,
Hopmeyer Gorman, Nakamoto, & Toblin, 2005; S. W. Twemlow, 2004; Wentzel &
Asher., 1995). Bullying experiences have been found to be associated with depression
3
(Haynie et al., 2001; Hunter, Boyle, & Warden, 2007; Morrow, Hubbard, Rubin, &
McAuliffe, 2008; D. J. Pepler & Craig, 1995; Erling Roland, 2002), anxiety (Baldry,
2004; Craig, 1998; Slee, 1994) ; aggression (Berthold & Hoover, 2000); and suicidal
ideation and attempts (Brunstein Klomek, Marrocco, Kleinman, Schonfeld, & Gould,
2008; Carney, 2000; Kaltiala-Heino, Rimpela, Marttunen, Rimpela, & Rantanen, 1999;
Omigbodun, Dogra, Esan, & Adedokun, 2008; Rigby, 1998; Erling Roland, 2002); and
avoidance of school for the victim (B.J. Kochenderfer & Ladd, 1996; Omigbodun et al.,
2008). Limited evidence indicates that bullying experiences, particularly on the part of
the aggressor, contribute to future violence (2007; Garbarino & Bedard, 2002; Vossekuil,
This study will build a theory about the mechanism of effect in bullying both to
guide social work practice in the specific problem of bullying and to contribute
knowledge to the field about dealing with other forms of oppression. By examining the
processes by which experiences of victimization affect children over time, this study will
explain how victimization affects development. Finkelhor uses the term ―developmental
victimology,‖ but he himself has not pursued this theoretical project. The research into
school bullying shows that many children who experience frequent abuse and humiliation
at the hands of someone more powerful than themselves will also experience changes in
their self-perception, sense of safety, mood or social life. These experiences can be
understood as forms of oppression which social work as a profession, and critical social
The desire to end oppression is found within critical theory and the related critical
social work. Walter Benjamin, an early critical theorist, felt that the social world should
be constituted in a way that allows people to construct their selves and relationships free
that can adversely affect children’s development. Intervening with bullying fits the
mandate of critical social work (Fook, 1993, 2002; Ife, 1997) which is an important
This writer began his social work career working in programs that practiced
structural social work (Moreau, 1990; Mullaly, 1997), a model of social work that
integrates the analysis of power into the ecological model of social work; however, I
found little guidance about how to use the structural model in casework practice. Further
reading and coursework into postmodern writers such as Michel Foucault and Jacques
Derrida suggested to me directions and ways to make the power relations within social
arrangements visible for clients and the social work profession; however, nothing
appeared to provide an overriding theory that could be applied to social work practice. In
fact, most contemporary postmodern theorists argued against any project of theory
One difficulty that arises from postmodern relativistic thinking is its challenge to
the absolutes of ethical codes, such as those of the social work profession. In particular,
the structural social work idea of increasing the power of marginalized communities,
often called ―empowerment,‖ raises concerns about the social work profession’s
diminish the power of others, as in the unwarranted use of violence (Fook, 1993, 2002;
Ife, 1997).
Kondrat (1995) proposed that the German philosopher Jurgen Habermas had solved this
described a model of social functioning that requires social actors to recognize and
1971), Kondrat laid out a theoretical model by which human beings act through a set of
rational interests. Kondrat’s article led me into reading Habermas’s work in depth. In his
affect the personality of the child. This article and Habermas’s subsequent theory of
dynamics are at the core of his project, in that he suggests a just society is only possible
in circumstances where individuals are able to consider the position of ―the other‖ and to
maintain communication in the face of conflict – skills learned and promoted in families
and communities. For Habermas, the ability to communicate in the face of conflict is the
Habermas bases his theory of communicative action on the idea that genuine
from speech and action designed to produce a specific action. In Habermas’s theory, this
6
writer saw a theory that could explain how children develop a set of social competencies
to create the conditions for socially just communication. Habermas, despite his critics’
assertions to the contrary (Allen, 2007), also appeared to offer a way to understand how
power gets translated into personal and communal pathologies. Habermas, in his two
action that, to this writer, provided a comprehensive guideline for intervening with
bullying and other forms of interpersonal oppression which had been part of my social
work experiences.
This project will test the applicability of Habermas’s theory to explain the
psychologists and educators; however, scientific inquiry into the topic is currently in
low of 136 peer-reviewed studies in 2005 (Stassen Berger, 2007). Despite a lack of
consistent scientific data, educators and social workers are constrained to act, sometimes
in the face of legislation, to end bullying (Limber & Small, 2003), with few scientific
tools to guide practice (Stassen Berger, 2007). Bullying is virtually invisible within the
sociology discipline and has only recently become a focus of research in the social work
settings. School social workers come into direct contact with bullying within the host
setting of schools and non-school social workers encounter the phenomenon in child and
7
family treatment settings. Yet Mishna (2003, 2007) acknowledges that bullying has
received little attention within the social work profession until very recently. In recent
times, fewer social workers have been employed by school districts, and those that are
employed may be providing services to more than one school (NASW, 2011). This
paucity of resources limits the ability of many school social workers to spend time and
resources on bullying interventions. School violence, on the other hand, has been a
significant issue for school social work, with overt acts of violence that invoke
disciplinary and juvenile justice intervention absorbing a great deal of school social
Bullying prevention has not been part of school social work’s mandate because
the problem has not been universally accepted as consistent with the traditional mandate
of school social work to intervene only when an issue directly affects academic
functioning (Miller, Martin, & Schamess, 2003). Evidence is emerging, however, that
bullying does in fact depress academic functioning both for individual children as well as
classroom groups (S. W. Twemlow, 2004; Wentzel & Asher., 1995; Woods & Wolke,
2004); therefore, intervening in bullying should fall within the traditional school social
A strong case, based on the academic and personal effects of bullying, can be
made for an expanded mandate for clinical professions to provide direct services to
children and their families within schools affected by bullying, including prevention and
primary treatment (Adelman & Taylor, 1998; Frey et al., 2005; SAMSHA, 2003). Such a
mandate, however, will require funding for social workers and faculty time for teachers
8
to implement anti-bullying and social skills building interventions into every day
curriculum.
Mishna (2003) asserts that social workers are in the best position to provide
counseling services to children, and consultation and training for school personnel to deal
specifically with school bullying. The social work profession is already established
within the host setting of the schools, and it has a set of conceptual skills with which to
analyze multiple social contexts as well as a set of practical skills with which to intervene
justice. Intervening against bullying is consistent with the mandate of Critical Social
Work. Critical social work (Fook, 1993; Healy; Ife, 1997), a dominant paradigm in
Canada, shares with critical theory an emancipatory mandate that requires taking direct
action against oppressive forces and ameliorating the effects of that oppression.
that reducing bullying is seen as a way to reduce marginalization among adult citizens
(Roland, 2000).
For some children, the experience of bullying may not be harmful. Most research
however, there is some evidence that some children who make the transition away from
the victim role, called ―escaped victims,‖ find that the bullying experience actually
strengthened their character (Dixon, Smith, & Jenks, 2004; P. K. Smith, Talamelli,
Cowie, Naylor, & Chauhan, 2004). Bullying experiences during childhood may also lead
9
to the social reproduction of adult roles of submissiveness and domination (Dixon et al.,
2004; A. Sutton, Smith, & Swettenham, 1999), which may be considered as positive or
negative depending on one’s standpoint on roles of social control. One study (S. Brown
& Taylor, 2008) shows that adults who were victimized as children tend to be employed
in subservient roles more frequently than those who bullied or those who were neither
capitalist societies which require people to take roles with varying degrees of coercion:
Kogan and Chandan (2004) in their qualitative research into school violence
found a small number of teachers and parents who expressed opinions in favor of
bullying. Most of this group felt that bullying was an important part of childhood and
helped the children become tougher and more prepared for an adult world. One father
threatened to hit his child himself if he did not learn to stand up to his tormentor.
No research studies have yet been published that examine the relationship
between childhood bullying and adult violence and aggression3; however, German
educators during the Nazi era promoted bullying, without using that specific term, as a
way of preparing children to take on adult roles of oppression such as concentration and
death camp workers. Kamenetsky (1996) in her analysis of Nazi children’s literature
shows how bullying was promoted and taught as a way to reproduce adult roles of
(Cowie, Naylor, Rivers, Smith, & Pereira, 2002; Hoel, Rayner, & Cooper, 1999; P. K.
Smith, 1997). However, it is not yet clear that workplace and school bullying are related.
Longitudinal studies begun in Scandinavia in the 1980s and 1990s are just beginning to
measure adult roles to determine whether children who were bullies or victims continue
their roles into adulthood: the longitudinal research cohort is just entering the labor
Social competent figures in several studies in this systematic review and is also a
attempt to capture the common feature of all social skills, defines social competence as
the ability to interact in a way that is appropriate and effective. Within the empirical
literature, social competence – or social skills--is measured in several ways including the
child’s interaction, the ability to negotiate, sense of humor, perceived attachment, among
others.
Despite the fact that bullying has been the subject of research for 30 years,
bullying researchers have not yet coordinated definitions or methodology. To date, there
have been no attempts to develop conventions that would coordinate definitions and
11
methods to allow studies to build logically one on the other. This lack of coordinated
With governments and states implementing policy and laws against bullying
(Limber & Small, 2003), intervention programs are proliferating (Limper, 2000;
McGrath, 2007; Scaglione & Scaglione, 2006); however, there is scant evidence for the
effectiveness of these programs (Rigby, 2002a, 2003; J. D. Smith, Schneider, Smith, &
Ananiadou, 2004; P. K. Smith, Pepler, & Rigby, 2004; Stevens, De Bourdeaudhuij, &
Van Oost, 2000). Three meta-analytic studies concluded that there is no evidence to show
that any bullying intervention program has been effective and there is even some
evidence that certain interventions may actually increase bullying (D.J. Pepler, Craig,
Ziegler, & Charach, 1994; Vreeman & Carroll, 2007). By contrast, there is evidence that
violence, as disaggregated from bullying, can be both prevented and reduced (Mytton,
DiGuiseppi, Gough, Taylor, & Logan, 2006). This leads to the conclusion that bullying is
Dubin (1978) suggests that interventions in the applied sciences are unlikely to
phenomenon. This appears to be the case with bullying. Many bullying intervention
programs are created with little or no reference to any supporting scientific literature
Abuse of power is the defining element of bullying interaction, yet none of the
theoretical models currently used in the bullying literature explicitly consider the
dynamics and effects of power. Power abuse is the single variable that distinguishes
feature of a theory that explains the bullying phenomenon and forms the basis of effective
attempt to mine the existing empirical literature to begin the process of building such a
theory.
13
This study will examine whether and to what degree Jurgen Habermas’s theory of
mechanisms of effect of school bullying. The project will build a theory using data drawn
element and defining feature of bullying (Farrington, 1993; P. K. Smith & Sharp, 1994),
yet the processes by which this abuse of power affects children have not been explored
communicative action does not use the discrete term ―power,‖ it does explore how action
is coordinated through differing means, including direct and indirect coercion and
conceptual framework with which to understand the complexities power and influence
social competencies and social conditions that make knowledge and social action
and explain the social world with direct reference to social processes without the need for
compares the human environment with biological ecology, break down when applied to
easily demarcated as the ecosystems of separate plant and animal species (McCarthy,
1984).
state. It might appear that the issue of school bullying, with its local, individual,
community level focus is remote from Habermas’s expansive goal; however, Habermas
shows that the basic competencies for building a just state are acquired in childhood.
Scandinavian anti-bullying projects are often predicated on claims that reducing bullying
will decrease the problem of marginalization among adult citizens (Roland, 2000).
of ideal speech conditions, he views the skills of meaning-making and consensus building
action into ―instrumental‖ and ―communicative‖ action; and he separates the ―lifeworld,‖
the part of the social world in which meaning and conventions are formed through
consensus and discussion, from the ―systems‖ such as economic and administrative
institutions that are invented to solve problems of distribution in complex societies. How
these two sets of distinctions will form the basis for understanding the effects of bullying
Instrumental action, on the other hand, is an effective tool to meet specific ends and is, by
nature, low in autonomy. To Habermas, systems and instrumental actions are neutral
15
concepts signifying the necessary steering mechanisms required to organize the lifeworld;
however, instrumental action misapplied can cause an array of social ills (Habermas,
1987). These abstruse concepts will be more fully explored in the theory chapter of the
of bullying actions that have emerged from a large body of studies that explicate and
delineate the bullying phenomenon. Free social play among children, in contrast to
instrumental action, shares with communicative action the defining feature of an absence
of instrumental motivations (Lever, 1978). These concepts will be briefly described in the
following sub-section.
Bullying is distinct from the larger set of behaviors that constitute ―peer
aggression.‖ The latter term is used to connote the full range of aggressive and coercive
actions among children that may or may not include bullying (Dupper & Meyer-Adams,
2002). In the theoretical and empirical research, the terms ―bullying‖ and ―peer
denotes the verbal forms of bullying. For this paper, the term ―bullying‖ will be used to
peer harassment.
16
Term Definition
power (similar to instrumental action) social action oriented toward achieving a goal
rough and tumble play children’s play behavior that mimics aggressive and
bullying activities
(Table 1 continues on next page)
17
victim or target child a child who is the recipient of bullying acts despite
a desire or attempts to stop these acts
greater power against a person with lesser power (Farrington, 1993), or, as more
generally as the systematic abuse of power (P. K. Smith & Sharp, 1994). The aggressive
acts of bullying repeat or endure over time despite the victim’s attempt or desire to stop
the abuse.
Bullying is distinguished from rough and tumble play (P. K. Smith & Boulton,
1990) and playful teasing, both of which have features that resemble bullying in content
but do not cause harm and are accompanied by clear signals of support, such as warm
communication that calls attention to an attribute of the recipient (Keltner et al., 2001).
Both rough and tumble play and teasing can move into direct aggression should the target
child misunderstand the intent of the play or should the play or teasing become too rough
and hurtful. Rough play, then, can turn into aggression or may be understood as
Bullying involves the deliberate selection of a target with fewer resources of self-
defense. Bullying is different from ―aggression,‖ hostile or violent behavior which can
18
disparities, or ―power bases,‖ occur when the bully is superior in ―physical size, strength,
number,‖ or ―social status.‖ This writer has also observed, in the context of school work
practice, physically small children using power bases not covered in the bullying
literature. These smaller children may bully stronger or higher-status children through
audacity, their willingness to break social rules, or ferocity, their willingness to take
physical risks by being physically or verbally cruel. In one instance, a very small child in
a younger grade terrorized children three grades above him and over one foot taller in
Bullying children may also use their own powers of observation to find ways to
gain a power advantage. Craig & Pepler (2007) point out that knowledge of a victim
distress.
does not use that term, is action taken within the social world to achieve a goal. The
framework section as will the implicitly rational nature of Habermas’s social theory.
19
Turner, & Hamby, 2005a; Ladd & Troop Gordon, 2005; Morrison, 2006; Rigby, 2003)
attest that there has been very little theory development into the nature of bullying, or into
the particular mechanisms of effect that operate on the children involved in various
bullying roles. The theories that have been most commonly used to explain the
phenomenon are social learning theory, social information processing theory, and
1995, 2007a; Finkelhor et al., 2005a) offers a tentative initial model for explaining the
mechanisms of effect on children of all power abuses, including bullying by peers and
abuse at the hands of adults. A small number of new or more obscure theories have also
been associated with bullying research; these and the theory of communicative action are
Social learning theory proposes that children are aggressive for the rewards that
accrue to them or are aggressive due to aggressive behavior learned in their families
(Bandura, 1979). Bullies may act out to assure their own safety, attain and maintain status
in their peer group (Hazler, Hoover, & Oliver, 1992); this theory tends not to be applied
to victims and the ill effects of bullying, although it can be assumed that some children
Social information processing theory (Crain, Finch, & Foster, 2005; Crick &
Dodge, 1994; Dodge, 2004; Dodge & Frame, 1982; Gifford-Smith & Rabiner, 2004) is
the social world. Children may bully because they misunderstand the cues available in the
20
social environment; this inaccurate cognitive representation of the social world causes
them to attribute negative motivation to neutral or benign social actions which in turn
leads to aggressive and violent acts (Dodge & Frame, 1982; Peets, Hodges, Kikas, &
(Blackburn, Dulmus, Theriot, & Sowers, 2004; D.L. Espelage, Bosworth, & Simon,
2001; Swearer & Espelage, 2004). To study bullying in its social context, Rodkin and
Hodges (2003) use the term ―peer ecology‖ for the various situations in which children
interact. A child’s particular friendship group may intersect with several contexts as
children form relationships with others encountered in different school and community
contexts, such as in the school classroom or in religious groups. The classroom itself is a
peer group which may include nested peer groups in the form of cliques or informal
friendship networks. While ecological theory helps understand the social contexts of
Table 2 lists the major and minor theories that have been applied to bullying.
21
1977a) to explain the educational system, explores the structure of power inherent in the
organization of mass education. He argues that the present United States school system
emerged from an organizational model similar to that of prisons and mental asylums:
institutions that enforce rules, regimentation, punctuality, and order. His central thesis is
that vestiges of the social control element of these institutions remain extant in the
structure and culture of most schools today despite the efforts of well-meaning principals
and teachers who want to interact humanely with children. Noguera suggests that the
There have been no major attempts to explain the reasons and mechanisms
through which children are affected by bullying. The theories and ideas discussed thus far
in this section explain the etiology of the bullying phenomenon and allow us to put
22
bullying into the context of the social environment, yet none of the theories suggest a
Finkelhor (1995), noting that the lack of theory explaining the effects of power
abuses, including bullying, suggests that there may be elements that affect children
common to all forms of abuse. Finkelhor (1995) constructed a model which he calls
―developmental victimology‖ that proposes two axes of influence: effects that take place
in the present environment and those that affect the future. He explains that a child who is
abused may experience stress contemporaneously, and that this present stress may
impede the child’s ability to negotiate developmental stages which then affects her future
adjustment. While Finkelhor thus outlined the idea of developmental victimology, he has
posit that trauma arises from the strong emotions that pertain to experiences of
powerlessness resulting in a set of physical and psychological symptoms that can endure.
One of the hallmarks of traumatic stress is its potential to evoke an instantaneous and
monumental shift in the traumatized person’s understanding of the world. In that way,
trauma theory fits with other cognitive theories but it also shows how the body is affected
mechanisms are at play when different forms of power imbalance occur in bullying. The
authors show that relational bullying, where the bully’s identity is unknown (strategic
indirect bullying), is associated with higher levels of post-traumatic stress, while physical
bullying is associated with lower self-worth. Mynard and Joseph’s work appears to
23
indicate that there are discrete mechanisms of effect at play in bullying experiences
depending on the nature of the bullying. In the years since Mynard and Joseph published
their study, however, no studies have either replicated or built on this discovery. This
in decline.
This writer’s exhaustive archival search in the fields of social work, education,
urban studies, psychology, sociology, and social psychology has yielded no theoretical
literature dealing with the question of the power differential specific to bullying4. As has
been said, the theories to date used to explain bullying largely concern cognitive
processes (Crick & Dodge, 1994) and social learning theory (Bandura, 1977, 1979; Craig,
1998; O’Connell, Pepler, & Craig, 1999), but do not attempt to explain the salience of the
development which is more expansive than most of the theories discussed above. Like the
school, as predictive of future outcomes. The product of a 20-year longitudinal study, this
theory finds that positive bonding to a school is the most powerful resiliency factor in
determining positive outcomes for students later in life. It proposes a four-part model of
socialization that includes the development of social competencies, the perceived and
actual opportunities for interactions with others, and the rewards that accrue from
interactions. Social Development Theory has, to date, been applied only to the prevention
and treatment of delinquency but it may be able to explain some of the effects of
4
The field of geography has also neglected the study of bullying, but a small number of geographers have
begun setting a research agenda for the topic (Andrews & Chen, 2006)
24
bullying, and, in fact, uses similar concepts to the Habermasian theory being used in this
project. Both bonding and social competencies are key Habermasian ideas which will be
applied to the problems associated with bullying in this project. Habermas’s idea, as will
be explored in the next section is able to explain both the etiology of bullying as well as
its effects.
25
Theory: The theoretical frame that will be used to explain the findings
thought called critical theory. Critical theory is strongly identified with the Institute for
Social Research which was founded in Frankfurt, Germany, in the 1920s, and is also
called the Frankfurt School, as are the group of thinkers directly or indirectly associated
with the Institute (R. H. Brown & Goodman, 2001). The term ―critical‖ derives from Karl
Marx’s critique of the political economy in his Das Kapital (Marx & Engel, 1955).
Within critical theory, the word ―critical‖ refers to the study of the mechanisms or laws
through which modern society was formed, with an emphasis on ameliorating the
deleterious effects of capitalism on the social world (Marx & Engel, 1955).
The central objective that critical theory advances is human emancipation through
a reordering of the social sciences into a unified project that makes ―sense of concretely
(Habermas, 1984, pp. 537-538). This goal of emancipation arose likely as the result of the
oppression and injustices that occurred through Nazi genocide and oppression in their
native Germany.
Germany. He openly discusses his membership in the Hitler Youth and the fact that he
briefly served as a child soldier in the German army posted on the Western defenses at
the end of the World War II (Habermas, 1992). Habermas dates his political awakening
to the immediate postwar period when he listened to the Nuremberg trials and saw
televised images of the Nazi concentration camps. His project can be summarized as an
attempt to determine the conditions upon which a democratic and constitutional state can
26
be built that will protect the rights of minorities. The evolution of Habermas’s theory will
be outlined in the next section, including the method he used to develop his theory.
The theory of communicative action has never before been applied to the
problem of bullying or, in fact, social casework practice. Habermas himself applied it to
family dynamics in an early paper but generally applies the theory to the formation of just
and democratic constitutional states. This project will use his theory to derive concepts
theory that explains how abuse of power can damage children both in their
framework that can deepen the understanding of how social forces affect the individual
(Borradori, 2003; Habermas, 1984, 1987; Honneth, 1992). Habermas proposes a set of
mechanisms though which action in the social world can be coordinated. In the following
section the system that he used to derive these concepts – universal pragmatics – will be
outlined.
Universal Pragmatics
a process he calls ―universal pragmatics,‖ or the search for the specific set of logically
irreducible competencies and conditions that make knowledge and social action possible
and allow people to reach understanding in ways that are least distorted (Habermas, 1984,
1987, 2001a; Heath, 2001). Habermas sees consensual communication present in the
27
social fabric in everyday acts of solidarity, friendship, and family life. As ordinary people
engage in daily conversation about life and the world, provide mutual support, and
meaning. This often happens through a consensual process that functions reflexively
within the social world. According to Habermas, efforts to reach consensus are the ideal
form of communication and necessarily require the recognition of different points of view
(Habermas, 1984).
Habermas bases his analysis of social pathology on the ways that communication
diverges from the ideal of consensus (Habermas, 1987; Honneth, 1992). He proposes an
ideal set of interests at the core of social cognition and an ideal situation for consensual
communication (1987). For example, Habermas’s ideal speech situation is one in which
all speakers are recognized and have equal opportunity to express themselves. However,
should not be deduced from this discussion that he believes the ideal condition is easily
Speech/Acts
utterances, written language, and actions intended to convey meaning and induce or
coordinate action, since, within his schema, not all speech/acts qualify as genuine
speech/acts intended to achieve a goal, while the latter includes the speech/acts intended
social work (Antonovsky, 1996; Saleebey, 1996). The strengths perspective challenges
social workers to privilege the strengths and resources of the individuals and
communities with which they work and marshal those resources to solve social problems.
Habermas suggests that the solutions to the ill effects of oppressive power involve
creating the conditions for consensus and solidarity, and applying them to the political
processes at play in everyday life constitute, in themselves, the world of human beings:
he calls this the ―lifeworld.‖ Ritzer and Smart (2001), in interpreting the theory of
communicative action, define the ―lifeworld‖ as ―those interpretive patterns that are
group identities and the development of individual personality‖( p.208). The lifeworld
includes the material, emotional and intellectual processes that comprise living organisms
and communities.
competencies, or social skills, through which an individual negotiates the lifeworld. The
concept of lifeworld may be better understood in light of the French language translation
as ―monde vecu‖, or the ―world as it is lived‖ (Ferry, 1991), and in which the world of
(Crossley, 1996). Ferry suggests that identity is formed in our daily interactions within
the lifeworld including in schools, and not exclusively in our intimate relationships.
Habermas makes a distinction between the lifeworld and the systems that are
created to organize activities in the lifeworld. Systems are created to fulfill a function
such as provide food or exchange. Habermas asserts that problems occur when the
constraints of a system supersede the needs the lifeworld. Nazism is an extreme example
of what can occur when a system uncouples from the lifeworld that created it. The
framework for the strengths perspective in social work. Communicative action is thought
to contain myriad acts of support and repair. In this project, some of the mechanisms of
effect by which children have been harmed by bullying involve their alienation from
sources of support: this alienation will be more fully discussed in the systematic review
below.
There is some research that shows how mechanisms of support and solidarity
work in the real world, lifeworld. Granovetter (1973), though not citing Habermas or
critical theory, supports the notion of the importance of informal relations. He suggests
that there is significant value in weak ties in the forms of acquaintanceship and informal
networks of relationship had been underestimated in social theory which favored deeper
community found that families supported each other in small but significant ways that
concretely improved the lives of individuals in that community. Kogan and Chandan
30
(2004) also found patterns of support that improved the lives of families in a lower-
income community and among children to support the victims of violence and bullying.
underestimated the importance of these weak ties. Within the theory of communicative
action the informal networks, weak ties, are the interstices through which all human
relations are formed. It is through the medium of acquaintance and social contacts that
Forming and maintaining weak ties, however, requires a considerable set of social
competencies. Several skills allow easy maintenance of weak ties including such qualities
as a sense of humor, respect for others’ time, the ability to exchange valuable or
presentation of self. The person with good social skill, regardless of such life conditions
maintain better marriages and friendships. These skills may be acquired in family and
recognition: without these, it is simply an interaction. Habermas does not privilege one
form of social action over another, stating that instrumental and systemic solutions are
necessary to organize an efficient society in which food and resources are distributed;
however, Habermas posits that instrumental action leads to less deeply-held foundations
of action than does communicative action. To explain this, he proposes the concept of the
―binding force.‖
31
Binding Force
the route through which much knowledge gets turned into action. To Habermas, the force
that comes from instrumental action, often coercion or reward, is weaker than the binding
force that comes from communicative action. Where instrumental action operates through
social and cognitive mechanisms that result in compacts, conventions, and commitments
for future action. Habermas suggests that this mechanism is based on an internal
cognitive process. He sees no discontinuity between his theory of social action and the
developed consensually in innumerable acts of love and solidarity. This idea contradicts
the idea of behaviorism (Skinner, 1969) and developmental biology that proposes that all
convention is stability. This stability is responsible for the tenacity of the binding force to
evoke social action. Actions based on commitment or conventions are more likely to
Lifeworld and system, and communicative and instrumental action are discrete
components in Habermas’s theory, yet he allows that in real life the divisions are not
always so crystalline: people can abuse the trust of others by giving the false appearance
existence of manipulation and lies, he makes the point that dishonesty can succeed only
that the person is being honest (Habermas, 1984). Likewise, some forms of bullying may
mimic consensual action, and there is some evidence that this deceptive form of bullying
is more destructive than bullying that is being done overtly (Mynard, Joseph, et al.,
2000).
Distortion occurs when people are manipulated into taking action that is not in
their own interest and is not generated through consensual process. An example of this is
the domination of the US food industry which has largely replaced healthy nutritional
distortions in both the bully and the victim themselves and may create a distortion in the
peer culture of the school when bullying becomes an accepted function within the school
culture as a child’s thoughts, emotions and identity become shaped by repeated and
enduring experiences. Bullying within the peer ecology can become a significant source
In this project the various components of the theorized model of effects will be
brought together into a comprehensive model. The constructs and effects sizes and any
Particular attention will be paid to any data that appear to disconfirm Habermas’s theory
Habermas’s concepts will be explored in this section including the idea of the
binding force, cumulative continuity, and the dynamics of respect to see if they can shed
light on the process that occurs when some children are affected by bullying and other
Table 3 contains a number of predictions based on the review of the literature and
deprivation of social free play less (more circumscribed) rehearsal of adult roles
(Pepler) less enjoyment of social world
less physical activity through play
mechanisms of endurance (ie. repetition and
reproduction)
cumulative continuity social skills and aggression that continue due to the
(Kokko) child’s choice of friendships and social environment
While the goal of this project is to test the theory of communicative action, it is
possible that the project will result in a modification of that theory, or the building of a
In this section, the research on bullying will be reviewed and analyzed. Bullying
on the history of the research, the characteristics of the bullying phenomenon, the
characteristics of bullies and victims, intervention strategies, and the effects of bullying.
Research into bullying began in the 1970s with a small number of seminal
articles. Since that time researchers have concentrated on determining the actions that
constitute bullying (Arora, 1996; Bjorkqvist, Lagerspetz, & Kaukiainen, 1992; Mynard,
Lawrence, & Joseph, 2000); the characteristics of both children who bully and their
victims (Bosworth, Espelage, & Simon, 1999; Coie, Dodge, Terry, & Wright, 1991;
Olweus, 1980; Perry, Williard, & Perry, 1990; Schwartz, Dodge, & Coie, 1993); the
2000; J. D. Smith et al., 2004); and the effects of being bullied (Ambert, 1994; Mynard,
Joseph, et al., 2000; Roland, 2000) As has been stated above, no research has been done
to determine the latent, unseen, processes and mechanisms at play when bullying affects
children (Ladd & Troop Gordon, 2005; Morrison, 2006; Rigby, 2002b; Rivers et al.,
2007).
A large part of the bullying research attempts to delineate and define the bullying
phenomenon itself (Mynard, Lawrence, et al., 2000; Rigby, 2000; P. K. Smith et al.,
1999; P. K. Smith & Sharp, 1994). In early research (Olweus, 1978, 1980), the concept of
36
bullying was not differentiated from peer aggression in general. The variety of gross and
subtle behaviors that comprise bullying and the nature of the bullying interaction
emerged as researchers and clinicians became more familiar with the phenomenon.
Bullying behavior is also evolving and with new communications technology, new types
studies of bullying among boys (1973, 1978), limited his operational definition of
bullying to physical aggression and verbal threat without concern for power disparities
(Olweus, 1978). Olweus’s initial definition has been used as the operational definition in
a great many subsequent research projects which has led to the conflation of bullying
with other forms of aggression in many studies.5 Since the publication of Olweus’s early
work, several researchers, including Olweus himself, have expanded the definition of
bullying to include power disparities and a range of harmful actions taken strategically
and indirectly (Arora, 1994; Crick & Grotpeter, 1995; Crick, Grotpeter, & Bigbee, 2002;
including a wider range of indirect and strategic actions including ganging up on children
and being rude about skin color. Lagerspetz, Bjorkqvist, and Peltonen (1988) furthered
the understanding of the actions that constitute bullying by including relational forms of
continued to elucidate subtle forms of relational aggression (Crick, Casas, & Mosher,
1997; Crick & Grotpeter, 1995; Owens, Slee, & Shute, 2000; Simmons, 2000) and the
5
This conflation of bullying and aggression compounds itself throughout the early bullying literature as the
definition used in early Olweus studies is repeated uncritically in later research.
37
use of contemporary technology such as cell phones and the Internet (Kowalski &
Limber, 2007; Li, 2006; Mishna, 2007) that bullies can use to coordinate elaborate
The Internet and texting capacities of cell phones provide a new forum for
unsupervised communication among and between children which has led to this new
phenomenon called ―cyberbullying‖ (Kowalski & Limber, 2007; Mishna, 2007; Strom &
Strom, 2005; Williams & Guerra, 2007). Cyberbullying refers to bullying done through
any electronic communication medium. Some of the technology available is new and
leads to hitherto unknown forms of bullying. For example, social networking technology
allows peers to rate each other using graphic indicators on such variables as popularity
her social system can now literally watch her own popularity plummet on a bar graph
within minutes,
The quality and nature of bullying is not stable over the developmental stages of
childhood (P. K. Smith & Levan, 1995). Ample evidence shows that bullying behavior
begins in the early school years, reaches a peak in middle school, and then drops off
sharply in later adolescence (Long & Pellegrini, 2003; Nansel et al., 2001; A.D.
Pellegrini & Bartini, 2000; Salmivalli & Lagerspetz, 1998; P. K. Smith & Levan, 1995;
P. K. Smith, Madsen, & Moody, 1999). Bjorkqvist, Lagespetz, and Kaukiainen (1992),
Smith and Levan (1995), and Long and Pellegrini (2003) found that the bullying
ages 14 and 15, and then sharply drops off in later adolescence. Younger bullying
38
children were found to use more direct physical aggression and threat, gradually
increasing their repertoire of actions as they age to include orchestrated social exclusion
(Stanley & Arora, 1998), assaults on reputation, and strategic manipulation of other
children to carry out bullying activities. The drop off in bullying in late high school may
be due to simple proportion: the fact that older teens have a diminishing cohort of older
From the beginning of the research into bullying, the phenomenon was described
however, research shows that bullying most often occurs in the context of a group
2003; D. J. Pepler & Craig, 1995; Salmivalli et al., 1996). Bullying is increasingly
typified as an interactional phenomenon and much of the bullying research being done in
the United States (Blackburn et al., 2004; D. L. Espelage, Holt, & Henkel, 2003)
phenomenon that occurs over time as the result of the interplay between and among
intrapsychic and environmental variables. While these authors show how bullying
behavior affects the school community, they do not suggest an explanation of how
Pepler and Craig (1995), using videotapes of children in play and classroom
contexts, reported that in 85% of observed incidents of bullying at least one uninvolved
peer was present. Salmivalli and her colleagues (1996) undertook the first systematic
study into the ancillary roles children take in bullying situations. In addition to the direct
bully and victim roles, these authors found children taking such roles as the ―assistant,‖ a
child directly abetting a bully through behaviors such as making suggestions to the bully
or holding the victim; the ―reinforcer,‖ a child who comes to watch, laugh, and actively
incite the bully; the ―defender,‖ a child who attempts to help the victim by actively
attempting to stop the bullying or by comforting the victim after the incident; and the
―outsider,‖ the child who intentionally absents him or herself from the bullying. A later
40
study by Sutton and Smith (1999) found that younger children tended to be more actively
involved in the bullying experience with fewer children taking the outsider role or no role
at all.
bully may victimize others as a form of mimesis, the deliberate imitation by one group of
another group’s behavior--in this case, children imitating adult roles in a process similar
to theatrical productions, sporting events, or pageants. Like theater, bullying comes with
assigned roles and coordinated action. The victim plays the protagonist role, as his or her
A large number of studies attempt to identify the factors and characteristics that
put children at risk for being involved in bullying either as victim or bully (Ahmed &
Braithwaite, 2004; Baldry, 2004; Bowers, P.K., & Binney, 1994; Ladd & Burgess, 2001;
Loeber & Dishion, 1984; Olweus, 1980; Perren & Hornung, 2005; Smetana, 1995).
Research into the correlates and characteristics of bullies and their families has been
uncoordinated and contains some very significant gaps. For example, researchers have
only recently begun to explore the co-occurrence of other forms of victimization in the
lives of bullied children (Finkelhor, Ormrod, & Turner, 2007a, 2007b; Legkauskas &
Jakimaviciute, 2007) and the association between victimization at school and family
Personal attributes may put some children at risk for being chosen as targets of
bullies (B. J. Kochenderfer-Ladd & Skinner, 2002; Ladd & Troop Gordon, 2003; Perry et
al., 1990; Schwartz et al., 1993). Kochenderfer and Ladd have done significant research
41
into the interplay between such variables as aggression and withdrawal with peer
rejection and bullying. Their body of work includes a number of longitudinal studies
intended to develop causal models of processes that put children at risk for bullying and
are associated with being victimized by bullies (Salmivalli & Helteenvuori, 2007).
class boundaries. Both genders bully, although there is a mildly significant difference in
the rates at which girls and boys use relational and direct bullying (Lagerspetz et al.,
1988).
Lagerspetz and Bjorkqvist and their colleagues (Lagerspetz, et al., 1988; Bjorkqvist, et al.
1992) included both genders in their samples and expanded the definition of bullying to
include relational aggression. The authors found only one minor gender difference – a
greater likelihood that boys will engage in direct physical aggression than would girls.
Despite attempts by the authors to clarify the purpose of their research and dispel
misunderstanding (Bjorkqvist et al., 1992), their studies have been widely misinterpreted
to mean that girls and boys bully in categorically different ways (Crothers, Field, &
Kolbert, 2005; Owens, Shute, & Slee, 2000; Owens, Slee, et al., 2000). One particular
study (Storch et al., 2003) finds no significant differences in the nature and extent of
bullying by gender.
The studies that address bullying among different ethnic communities also have
contradictory results. Moran, Smith, Thompson and Whitney (1993), in studying children
42
ages 9 to 15, found no differences in the ethnic affiliation of children who are involved in
bullying. Nansel et al. (2001) found a slightly higher level of bullying among Latino and
African- American children as compared to the dominant culture. Peskin and colleagues
(2007) found slightly higher levels of bullying among African-American children, while
Estell, Farmer, and Cairns (2007), in a study of rural African-American youth, found no
communities. DeVoe (2004) found that white children were slightly more likely to be
bullied than African-American or Latino children. Spriggs, Iannotti, Nansel & Haynie
(2007) surveying 11,033 children ages 11 to16 found lower rates of bullying among
comparing definitions, methods, and results among these seemingly contradictory studies
The variation in rates of bullying among ethnic, rural, and linguistic communities
is not as significant as the variation among schools in individual studies (Sharp et al.,
2000)6. Olweus (1990) found that bullying occurs less frequently in schools that do not
tolerate it. The degree of ambient violence--the range of behaviors that constitute
bullying -- fluctuates across context and depends, to some degree, on adults’ attitudes and
responses.
Family Characteristics
Most of the studies into the family characteristics of children involved in bullying
focused on the social context of the bully rather than the victim. Bullying behavior is
family violence (Ahmed & Braithwaite, 2004; Baldry, 2004; Bowers et al., 1994; Loeber
& Dishion, 1984; Smetana, 1995). Olweus (1980) in a study of boys, ages 9 to 11, found
a link between mothers’ negativity and fathers’ power assertive methods and boys’
aggression. Again, as with many of the early bullying studies, the definition of bullying
used in this study did not distinguish bullying from other forms of aggression.
style and a lack of warmth in the parent. In that study, no association was found between
bullying and maternal negativity, however. In a similar vein, Bowers, Smith, and Binney
(1994) found that bullies perceived their parents as distant, lacking in warmth, and either
within their own families (Bowers et al., 1994). Loeber and Dishion (1984), looking at
aggressive children in general, reported that the children of parents who used both
such as low substance abuse (Baumrind, 1991), social and psychological adjustment, and
social competence, are associated with such positive parenting practices as warmth,
Ahmed and Braithwaite (2004) examined a wide variety of family and school
variables that could contribute to a child’s role as a bully or victim. The authors found
that a combination of family and school factors predicted 61% of bullies and 76% of
44
bully-victims, the children who are both bullies and victims; however, the authors do not
attempt to determine whether some of the school factors such as ―school liking‖ and
Aside from the power differential, victims are not always chosen because of a
particular attribute or quality, as many in the public believe. Some children are chosen as
targets at random, or because they are thriving socially and academically and are
children are selected as victims at random this dilute the statistical likelihood that family
characteristics will be strongly associated with victimization, since many random victims
Bullies vary in terms of popularity among other children (L.D. Hanish & Guerra,
2004). Aggressive children in general are not usually popular (Lopez & DuBois, 2005;
Mynard, Joseph, et al., 2000; Mynard, Lawrence, et al., 2000; Wentzel & Asher., 1995);
however, some bullies are considered popular among their peers (Estell et al., 2007; P. C.
Rodkin, Farmer, Pearl, & Van Acker, 2000) and this group appears to have the largest
aversive effect (Mynard, Lawrence & Joseph, 2000; Lopez & DuBois, 2005). Estell et al.
(2007) found that both bullies and victims tend to be socially marginalized in schools;
however, a subset of bullies was also identified, in their study, as popular. The authors
argue that deficits of social learning and social skills do not explain the incidence of
indicates that the characteristics of the school environment may determine much of the
bullying, including rapid and firm adult response to bullying, positive, non-punitive
and corridors (Olweus, 1992, 1994; Boulton, 1991). This is supported by studies that
compare the rates of bullying among schools (Boulton, 1991; Sharp et al., 2000). Sharp
and her colleagues (2000) found that the prevalence rate of bullying varied by as much as
Gumpel and Meadan (2000) show that the prevalence of bullying is sensitive to
and possibly even determined by the response of adults within the school community:
bullying will increase where adults systematically ignore it. Dixon and colleagues
qualitative research about the integration of a class of hearing impaired children into one
and minimizing instances of bullying (Dixon, Smith, & Jenks, 2004). Thus, the attitudes
and prejudices of parents and teachers should be the focus of a coordinated research
Noguera (1995) proposes that the structure of the school system replicates an
oppressive power structure in that it is organized using the same model as prisons with an
emphasis on surveillance and order (Foucault, 1977a). Noguera suggests that this
humanely with children. Twemlow, Fonagy, Sacco & Brethour Jr. (2006) note that some
46
teachers themselves are using bullying tactics in the classroom and that this reinforces
Community factors in school bullying have been studied less than family and
individual factors (Bowen, Bowen, & Ware, 2002). In the one study that specifically
looks at community violence, Schwartz and Proctor (2000) found no correlation between
Bullies do ―not distribute their aggression evenly across all available peer targets
but instead selectively direct their attacks toward a minority of peers who serve
consistently in the role of victim‖ (Perry et al., 1990, p. 1310). There is a popularly held
belief that victims of bullying are chosen for this role because of a particular physical or
personal quality, such as obesity or wearing eyeglasses (Olweus, 1993), or being gifted.
One study (Janssen, Craig, Boyce, & Pickett, 2004) did find, contrary to other research,
that overweight teens were somewhat more likely to be chosen as a target. However,
Sunde-Peterson and Ray (2006) found that such ―outstanding‖ features were evenly
distributed between bullies and victims. In other words, a child with glasses, obesity, or
braces is as likely to fill the role of bully as the role of victim, but these children are not
certain behavioral and social characteristics put children at higher risk of being bullied.
These include passivity (Schwartz et al., 1993) and aggression, particularly reactive
aggression (Schwartz, Proctor, & Chien, 2001), and the tendency toward social
interests (Egan & Perry, 1998). The victim may also reinforce the bully by easily
Ladd and Kochenderfer-Ladd have conducted a large number of studies into the
quality of children’s social relations (B. J. Kochenderfer-Ladd & Skinner, 2002; B.J.
Kochenderfer & Ladd, 1996; Ladd & Troop Gordon, 2003) and found that social skills
were both a cause and a consequence of bullying and that both were associated with a
downward spiral of self-esteem among victims. In other words, children who started their
school career with poor social skills were found to be at higher risk of bullying, and
bullying experience damage social skills even for those children who started school with
good social skills. Social skills, then, need to be understood as dynamic and evolving
Low self-esteem also appears to function as both a risk factor for victimization as
well as a consequence of victimization (Egan & Perry, 1998, Troop-Gordon & Ladd,
2005) (Egan & Perry, 1998; Ladd & Troop Gordon, 2005). Egan and Perry (1998) found
that children who were ―protected‖ by a high self-regard were victimized less than
children with low self-regard. The authors also found that the experience of victimization
Children who are either withdrawn socially or reactively aggressive are at higher
risk for bullying than children with better social skills (L.D. Hanish & Guerra, 2004; B.
J. Kochenderfer-Ladd & Skinner, 2002; B.J. Kochenderfer & Ladd, 1996; Ladd &
48
Burgess, 2001; Ladd & Troop Gordon, 2003). The victimized child may lack the social
skills that are valued in children’s peer relations, such as a sense of humor or familiarity
Swiss researchers Perren and Hornung (2005) provide confirmation that victims
tend to have lower levels of peer support than either bullies or non-bully-involved
children. Having and keeping friends protects children from victimization (Davies, 1982;
Ladd, Kochenderfer, & Coleman, 1997). Friendships may function to decrease the
Certain minority groups such as gay, lesbian, and bisexual youth in contemporary
America tend to suffer more bullying than other groups, but it is not yet clear whether the
or homophobia (Poteat & Espelage, 2007; Warwick, Chase, Aggleton, & Sanders, 2004;
Young & Sweeting, 2004)Further research needs to be done to identify children who
otherwise have few risk factors and rich resources of resilience and are still targeted by
Figure 1: Role Transitions Over Two Years(adapted from Smith, Talamelli, Cowie,
Naylor, & Chauhun, 2004)
Time 1 Time 2 (two years later)
Non Victims
Non Victims
New Victims
Continuing Victims
Victims
Escaped Victims
literature is the simple mathematic formula that determines this large jump in prevalence
in middle and especially junior high school: the proportion of children on the lowest end
of the age range is one-third in middle school and fully one-half in junior high: this
compares with one-sixth of children in a K-5 school. Middle and junior high school
children find themselves suddenly on the smaller end of the school after being on the
As was discussed above, the acceleration of growth at middle and junior high
school age explains the statistical jump in bullying prevalence in middle school.
Children’s bodies may increase as much as 35% in bone and muscle mass over a two-
year period (Ruff, 2003). In addition to the likelihood of becoming a victim increasing
with the proportion, children may experience extreme variation in physical size. In some
cases, children who were formerly the smallest in their cohort will grow in size and
strength to equal or surpass their peers: conversely, a child who was a bully in elementary
school may suddenly find himself in middle school dwarfed in size by his former victim.
50
As with the peak in middle school, the fall off of victimization in high school may also be
the result of simple mathematics: as children reach the end of their high school years, the
pool of potential bullies vanishes as the older and larger cohort graduates or leaves high
school.
Little research has sought to determine whether and to what degree children
2007b; Legkauskas & Jakimaviciute, 2007). Legkauskas and Jakimaviciut (2007) found
that for both girls and boys the experience of abuse at home was highly correlated with
the perpetration of bullying behavior; emotional and physical abuse at home correlated
with both victimization and bullying at school. In several studies, Finkelhor and his
colleagues (Finkelhor et al., 2007a, 2007b; Finkelhor, Ormrod, Turner, & Hamby, 2005b)
surveyed a national sample of over 1,000 children for the occurrence of different types of
into the same category despite the fact that they are different phenomena, albeit with
similar characteristics. The authors found that bullied children often experience several
forms of abuse including sexual, physical, and emotional abuse and peer victimization as
well as being witnesses to violence. While victimization was correlated with other forms
of abuse, 50 per cent of the children who were bullied did not have other forms of abuse
or aversive family characteristics. This finding lends credence to the idea that there is a
cohort of children who are chosen at random as targets of bullying. Holt, Finkelhor, and
Kaufman-Kantor (2007) confirm the existence of a group of youth victims who have not
Effects of Bullying
A great deal of research has been done into the association between bullying and
its negative effects7. It has been established that bullying and other problems within the
peer system are antecedents of global problems with adjustment and psychopathology in
children (Bukowski & Adams, 2005). Children who have been bullied or who themselves
engage in bullying for an extensive period of time are at risk for problems during
childhood such as lower academic performance and school attendance (Clarke &
Kiselica, 1997; Cunningham, 2007; Holt et al., 2007; Sharp et al., 2000; Shellard &
Turner, 2004; Wentzel & Asher., 1995); lower self-esteem (Mynard, Joseph, et al., 2000;
O’Moore & Kirkham, 2001; Rigby & Slee, 2001; Salmon, James, & Smith, 1998);
depression (Haynie et al., 2001; Erling Roland, 2002; Seals & Young, 2003); generally
poorer mental health (Petersen & Rigby, 1999; Rigby, 1999, 2000); anxiety and poor
physical health (Baldry, 2004; Craig, 1998; Slee, 1994); suicidal ideation and gestures
(Brunstein Klomek et al., 2008; Carney, 2000; Kaltiala-Heino et al., 1999); and
climate, often depressing academic scores for an entire school (Ballard, Argus, &
Remley, 1999; Clarke & Kiselica, 1997; S.W. Twemlow et al., 2001). Bullying has a
negative impact on learning for children with all active roles in bullying, including
bystanders for whom the bullying of peers may be at best a distraction from learning and
at worst a source of ongoing anxiety (Lumsden, 2002). Victims were more often absent
7
The terms ―effects‖ and ―sequelae‖ are used interchangeably. The term ―sequelae‖ will be used in this
paper when the term ―effect‖ is used to denote effects other than bullying sequelae.
52
from school than non-victims and expressed less enjoyment in life (Ballard et al., 1999;
formation (A. van Hoof, Raaijmakers, van Beel, Hale, & Aleva, 2008); however, to date,
no research has been done into the formation of identity in children experiencing
bullying. Nevertheless, the totality of the effects literature shows a close corollary to
identity formation as the items in identity formation questionnaires involve most of the
same measures of social life, cognition, emotion, and behavior as are used in the effects
literature.
The physical effects of bullying have been firmly and clearly established (Baldry,
2004; Faust & Forehand, 1994; Hodges & Perry, 1999; Legkauskas & Jakimaviciute,
2007; Paquette & Underwood, 1999). The immediate physical effects include stomach
aches, headaches, sleeping problems, and other physical complaints. Srabstein (2006) in a
national study found that victims as well as bullies report a wide set of physical problems,
including headaches, stomach aches, sleep disturbance, and backaches. These symptoms
avoid school.
immediate physical consequences that have a negative impact on the quality of the
child’s life. Empirical evidence supports an association between victimization and a set
bullying. This is a clear indication that children are suffering as a result of bullying, and
Even in the absence of other risk factors in the life of a child, a single or short-
term experience of bullying and peer rejection can negatively affect the child’s self-
esteem and emotional well-being (Arora, 1996; P. K. Smith & Levan, 1995). For
example, the victim of orchestrated relational aggression may experience the sudden loss
of a peer network after a lifetime of stable and healthy peer relations, precipitating a
Sourander, Helstela, and Piha (2000) found that victimization and bullying were
both associated with lower levels of social competencies and high externalizing behavior,
such as aggression and hyperactivity in children who bully; and, in victims, high
Youth Behavior Checklist (Achenbach, 1991; Achenbach & Rescoria, 2001). The
Victims were found to exhibit both high levels of psychotic symptoms and internalizing
Children involved in bullying are at higher risk for suicide than their non-bully-
involved peers (Cleary, 2000; Finkelhor et al., 2007a, 2007b; Kaltiala-Heino et al., 1999;
Kim & Leventhal, 2008; Omigbodun et al., 2008; Rigby, 1998; E. Roland, 2002). In a
systematic review of the 39 studies on bullying and suicide, Kim and colleagues (2008)
found that, despite the methodological problems of comparing studies that used differing
54
measures of victimization, both bullies and victims are at greater risk for suicidal
thoughts and attempts, with children who are bully-victims at the highest risk.
Kaltiala-Heino et al. (1999) also found that children at highest risk for suicidal
symptoms were bully-victims, with a 12:1 odds ratio. An odds ratio is used to evaluate
risk in epidemiological studies; it is the number of events divided by the number of non-
events (Cook, 2002). Bullies were seen as at risk with an 8.7:1 odds ratio; victims were
5.7:1 times more at risk8. A fuller study might gather data on other risk factors in the
children’s environments, such as abuse and neglect, that may also explain these elevated
levels of suicide and depression (Finkelhor et al., 2007b; Legkauskas & Jakimaviciute,
2007).
(2008) studied 2,342 high school students and found that both victims and bullies are at
higher risk for depression and suicidal ideation as well as actual suicide attempts.
Bullies and victims were found to be more frequently referred for psychiatric
Kumpulainen, Rasanen, & Puura, 2001; Rigby, 1998, 2000), with bully-victims almost 7
times more likely to be referred for psychiatric consultation (Kumpulainen et al. 1998).
adolescence (Kumpulainen & Rasanen, 2000) for both bullies and victims. Roland (2002)
physical health and social support. His study investigated bullying among 2,088
Norwegian children in the 8th grade. Roland selected a sample of children who identified
8
While the lower rates for victims appear counterintuitive, they may result fromthe significant percentage
of victims who are chosen at random and may have significant resources for protection.
55
themselves as having been bullied within the past week: 5% met the criteria; this narrow
victimization from those whose bullying had been ongoing. However, within that small
group, the author found a very strong association between peer victimization ( as Roland
tends to term the phenomenon of bullying) and both anxiety and depression. Bullies and
their victims scored higher on depression and measures of suicidal ideation and gestures
compared to a group that was not involved in bullying. The sample of those
those children being bullied may experience symptoms of stress, such as trouble sleeping
Few bullying researchers have looked at bullying from the perspective of its
spatial effects. An exception is the mapping of high school spaces by social work
researchers in Michigan (Astor & Meyer, 1999; Astor, Meyer, & Behre, 1999; Astor,
Meyer, & Pitner, 2001). These authors identified ―unowned spaces‖ where children are at
higher risk for violent encounters; the authors, however, did not distinguish bullying from
other violent acts. A small number of geographers have started to look at bullying as
creating ―tyrannical space,‖ referring to the changes that bullying and other forms of
victimization make in the environment (Andrews & Chen, 2006; Percy-Smith &
Matthews, 2001). Several studies, without explicitly defining the issue as spatial,measure
safety or anxiety about being in school (Berthold & Hoover, 2000; Glew, Fan, Katon,
56
Rivara, & Kernic, 2005; Juvonen et al., 2000). This element of bullying will be more
Social Space
Where physical space is bounded by clear, physical limits, social space is more
diffusely bounded and involves social skills and social bonds without physical
manifestations that can be detected directly, though researchers can infer lack of
bullying experiences and leads to, or is correlated with, problems of social connection
and social competence. Several separate studies (Mouttapa, Valente, Gallaher, Rohrbach,
& Unger, 2004; A. D. Pellegrini, Bartini, & Brooks, 1999) as well as a canon of studies
conducted by Gary Ladd and Becky Kochenderfer-Ladd (Buhs & Ladd, 2001; Buhs,
Ladd, & Herald, 2006; B.J. Kochenderfer-Ladd & Wardrop, 2001; Ladd & Burgess,
2001; Ladd et al., 1997; Ladd & Troop Gordon, 2003, 2005) shed a great deal of light on
Social skills, is another diffuse concept that refers to a large set of qualities,
actions, and perceptions that defy easy definition (Segrin, 2000). Not only is the concept
diffuse, it is highly context-sensitive and changes over time. A person may need to use a
different set of social skills depending on her role and that role’s orientation to various
social contexts (Brunkhost, 1986), so a child may need to use certain skills with adults
and another, far more dynamic set of skills with peers. Peer contexts vary and fragment
as children create communities of interest that become richer as the children progress
57
through adolescence: each context may have a jargon, a dress code, and a set of behaviors
Victimization and social competence are highly entwined concepts as the one
affects the other. Problems with social competence and social connectedness are both a
cause and a consequence of victimization (B.J. Kochenderfer & Ladd, 1996). Pellegrini
and colleagues (1996) found that victimized children were significantly more isolated:
they had the fewest reciprocated friendships (the independent agreement between two
children that they recognize each other as friends), and were generally least liked by their
peers. An isolated child is at higher risk for being chosen as a target, and the
victimization limits or eliminates the child’s opportunity to learn social skills. Since
social skills within childhood are constantly evolving, even a child who enters school
with a good set of social skills may quickly fall behind her peers if she finds herself
The research of Ellis and Zarbatany (2007) finds a similar pattern of withdrawal,
and the authors also conclude that there is a social cost to befriending or maintaining a
friendship with a targeted child: the friend of the target may be putting herself at risk of
becoming a target as result of the friendship and may experience a lowering of social
status.
bullying (L.D. Hanish & Guerra., 2000). Kogan and Chandan (unpublished data)
observed victims using this strategy as well as non-victims who quietly and routinely
absented themselves from situations in which they were at risk for being bullied.
58
Several studies examine the role bullying experiences play in the formation of
later problems, particularly later aggression (Coie, Lochman, Terry, & Hyman, 1992;
Newman, Fox, Harding, Mehta, & Roth, 2004; Vossekuil et al., 2002). The cohort of
children taking part in the longitudinal studies begun in the 1980s in Scandinavia is now
entering late adolescence, and the first study has recently been published (Sourander et
al., 2007); it links early bully involvement with criminality in late adolescence and early
adulthood. Earlier US studies found that early peer rejection contributes to adolescent
underachievement (Chen, Chang, Liu, & He, 2008; Glew et al., 2005; Ialongo, Vaden-
Kiernan, & Kellam, 1998; Schwartz, Gorman, Duong, & Nakamoto, 2008; Schwartz et
al., 2005; Wentzel & Asher., 1995). Both peer rejection and aggressiveness are predictors
of several disorders that appear during adolescence (Coie et al., 1992). From an inverse
Persons who were bullied and/or peer rejected as children may experience
negative sequelae that endure into adulthood (Crick, 1996; Kokko & Pulkkinen, 2000).
While there are, as yet, no longitudinal studies that have followed victimized children
fully into adulthood, Ambert (1994) coincidentally found evidence showing that bullying
experiences in childhood are still deeply felt in early adulthood. The author gathered
public university. Students were asked to describe what made them happy and unhappy
during their childhoods, pre- and post-puberty. The students reported that negative peer
59
interactions, including bullying, racist, and sexist actions, had more detrimental impact
lasting into the present day compared to abuse at the hands of a parent. While many
students disclosed other forms of abuse, including physical and sexual abuse, the
consensus was that the bullying experiences were more emotionally damaging.
A few studies show some evidence that bullying and other negative peer
experiences have sequelae that last into adulthood. In an oft-cited longitudinal study,
Caspi and colleagues (1998) delineated several variables associated with chronic adult
unemployment and underemployment in the United Kingdom. The authors found general
that girls who were indirectly bullied tended to exhibit perfectionist tendencies as young
adults, and Sourander, et al. (2007) found that early bullying behavior was associated
with criminal behavior in youths 16 to 19 years old, while victimization was not
about associations and correlations between early bullying and peer rejection and later
sequelae; they suggest that the mechanisms of ―interactional cumulative continuity,‖ the
selection of relationships and environments that support and propagate anti-social (or
constricting and punitive disciplinary actions; these children tend to maintain the same
friends or similar aggressive friendships over their entire life span. Kokko and Pulkkinen
60
say that the process by which these children are affected is likely to be continuous rather
than discrete.
no association between the experience of being bullied and youth crime and some
evidence that the bullies were at higher risk for adult criminality. Garbarino (2002) and
Gilgun (2001) both claim that school bullying is a contributing factor to the mass murders
of students by other students. Both authors base their assertions on qualitative research
done with the perpetrators of highly publicized school shootings as well as the survivors
of the shootings who describe the murderers as having been extensively bullied for years
that bullying was a significant factor in the lives of many children who perpetrated school
shootings. The study, commissioned by the US Secret Service (Vossekuil et al., 2002),
psychologists, teachers, and social workers with 41 children who perpetrated school
shootings between 1974 and 2000. The content was coded and analyzed in an attempt to
uncover the intrapsychic or familial variables that could help identify potential shooters
and thereby prevent future school shootings. The most salient variable discovered in that
study is that 71% of school shooters were victimized by school bullies. The authors
conclude that educators need to make each school a place where students feel safe and
valued, and that neglecting problematic social interaction among students could yield dire
consequences.
61
bullying in the media, stated that parental neglect may be a more significant factor than
bullying in school shootings, pointing out that the parents of some of the child murderers
claimed they did not know that their child had a collection of assault weapons though the
not always acute, environmental stressor. Finkelhor (1995) states that bullying is a source
of ongoing stress for the victim as well as an obstacle to adequate development of social
skills and cognition for both the bully and the victim.
review and analysis of children’s literature during the years that the National Socialist
(Nazi) party dominated Germany and much of occupied Europe. German educators of the
Nazi era directly and explicitly encouraged bullying, while not using that term itself.
Kamenetsky points out that this was most evident in the curriculum they used to train
children enrolled in Hitler Youth groups9. The Nazis ―successfully‖ used the Hitler
Youth groups to develop a corps of adults to take jobs preparing countries for Nazi
occupation or in the death camps (Office of United States Chief of Counsel, 1946); it can
be assumed that the children’s literature was developed and disseminated expressly to aid
in that goal.
through the Hitler Youth, in a children’s book whose title was translated as Never Trust a
9
Kamenetzky (1996) demonstrates that all children’s literature of that period was developed according to
specific guidelines from the Nazi Department of Education.
62
Jew on the Green Heath (Kamenetsky, 1996), cartoon depictions show children how to
bully and exclude Jewish children on the playground. These illustrations bear a striking
resemblance to cartoon depictions of bullying Smith and colleagues (2000) showed their
subjects in order to study bullying across linguistic and national barriers. Kogan and
Chandan (2004) found that some children, parents, and teachers support childhood
bullying, in part for its perceived positive role in preparing children for adult roles of
enforcement and domination. While an analogy to the Nazis may seem distasteful, their
idea that bullying prepares children for adult roles of coercion is identical to the ideas of
Anti-Bullying Interventions
With federal governments and states implementing policy and laws against
bullying (Limber & Small, 2003), intervention programs are proliferating (Blakely, 2004;
McGrath, 2007; Scaglione & Scaglione, 2006); however, there is scant evidence for the
Pepler, et al., 2004; Stevens et al., 2000). Some evidence suggests that certain
interventions may actually increase bullying or simply make it less visible to adults (D.J.
the prevalence of bullying, only a small number of bullying intervention models have
been in existence long enough to have been evaluated, and none of the bullying programs
al., 2004; J. D. Smith, Stewart, & Cousins, 2003). Rigby (Rigby, 2002a) in his meta-
analytic study of anti-bullying intervention programs shows that none of the studies
63
included in his analysis sought to determine the relative effects of their multiple
components or their dosage; evaluation methodology varied in quality and criteria; and
many evaluation studies were applied in only one geographical area often with a
homogeneous population.
While there are several models of anti-bullying interventions, most utilize three
program components: whole school policies, social skill-building curricula, and social
This section will review a few of the major intervention models that have been
implemented over the past three decades and recent models that offer a significant
variation or innovative program component. All programs reviewed have one or all of the
abovementioned three components, and some recent programs have added new
components, for example, legal accountability, in states where schools can be held liable
Olweus (1991, 1994), surveying the results of several intervention programs using
his Olweus Anti-Bullying Program (now called the Olweus Bullying Prevention
Program) over the previous decade, reported a reduction of bullying and an increase in
teachers’ willingness to intervene. Since most of the data were gathered before the more
general aggression is conflated with bullying in the results. Several future evaluations of
the same program model were unable to replicate Olweus’s results (D.J. Pepler et al.,
1994; Vreeman & Carroll, 2007); however, all these used a contemporary definition of
bullying as opposed to the original study’s wider definition. The Olweus model was
implemented and evaluated in four Canadian schools (Pepler, Zeigler, & Charach, 1994)
64
over a period of a year and a half. The results were mixed at best with a small decrease in
the number of children reporting abuse within the week previous to the post-test, but with
a small increase in the proportion of children who reported being frequently bullied. No
show that, while the programs may reduce the number of bullying incidents, the number
of bullies and victims may still remain stable or actually increase. These seemingly
children who were formerly only overtly aggressive had started to bully in a de facto
changing of the guard, or children may have changed the way they bullied in order to
One US study purports to show that the Olweus model significantly reduces
bullying. Black and Jackson (2007) report observing a very large reduction of bullying in
one school after four years of intervention. The authors used an interrupted time-series
decrease in observed bullying. While the authors claimed to be implementing the Olweus
model, they added significant program components, including the separation of ages and
genders and the inclusion of highly adult-supervised and organized social activities at
lunch and recess. These new program components would logically account for the large
reduction of observed bullying simply because of the limitations they place on the
autonomy of all children including bullies. The Black and Jackson model may in fact be
iatrogenic, with the cure being more costly than the problem, due to the children’s lack of
model in a different way, by including program components that are intended to limit the
ancillary roles of bullying. In a posttest after six months of intervention, students reported
a 15% decrease in bullying. Frey, et al. (2005) studied a similar intervention program that
incorporated Salmivalli’s approach which also showed some success in reducing the
incidence of observed bullying. These studies suggest that intervention programs that aim
to reduce children’s ancillary roles in bullying are promising and worthy of future study.
Ducharme, Folino, and DeRosie (2008) suggested that the minimal effectiveness
of social- skill building intervention programs (J. D. Smith et al., 2004; P. K. Smith, D. J.
Pepler, et al., 2004) result from too weak a dosage, and, in response, the authors
developed an intensive social-skills program which they implemented with two groups of
four children with severe bullying involvement. The authors theorized, after intensive
time-series observation of children at play, that the ability to give in to the will of others
was the core component skill for positive peer interaction which they term
broader range of positive social skills and positive interpersonal experiences. While the
authors show some moderate improvement in bullying, the intensity of the intervention
makes it costly both in terms of labor and time, with 60 academic hours taken up by the
Legal considerations are also entering the field of bullying prevention and
McGrath’s (2007) model uses the whole school policy and social-skills building
components and adds in procedures to protect the schools from liability in jurisdictions
66
where there are State laws against bullying. McGrath, a lawyer, adds program
components that require the schools to maintain procedures and records that will protect
the children against bullying and protect the schools in any future legal procedures.
widely marketed --nor have any outcome studies been published to date.
The few program models that show positive results demand a tremendous
concerted effort on the part of school staff, parents, and students. This puts anti-bullying
and social-skills promotion in direct competition with academic curriculum. In the US, at
the time this paper is being written, schools are under pressure to raise standardized test
scores as part of the No Child Left Behind initiative and schools may not be amenable to
introducing programs that would take time away from academic teaching unless the
program positively affects test scores. While there is a correlation between bullying and
low academic performance, none of the intervention programs’ effectiveness studies cited
test scores.
powerful policies and well-funded national intervention efforts. Due (2005) directly
studied the effects of bullying cross-nationally and found that US and Israeli children
experienced more severe levels of symptoms than countries with long standing anti-
bullying efforts. The same comparison can be extrapolated from the results of Wolke’s
67
(2000)10 UK study that shows markedly less severity of anxious and depressive
symptoms than the results for those variables in this review of US studies.
children are supported and, if necessary, treated as they are in the UK, Scandinavia and
Commonwealth nations in which bullying has been seen as a significant social problem
Conclusion
The effects of bullying are diffuse. A cursory look at the results of the studies
reviewed below prior to the more systematic analysis of the results show that bullying is
widely that making comparisons between studies is not always plausible. Bullying is
measured variously by direct observation, peer report, self report, teacher report;
of a range of bullying actions; and the temporal criteria for determining the degree to
up to three years. Measures used to investigate variables such as depression and anxiety
range from standard instruments such as the Achenbach Child Behavior Checklist (1991)
of bullying would lead a reader to conclude that being bullied is associated with a
J. D. Smith et al., 2004). Dubin (1978) points out that for interventions in the applied
required. A theoretical model that can explain both the etiology of bullying and the
mechanisms through which bullying affects children is also likely to result in more
effective interventions .
There has been very little research seeking to build or test a theory within the field
of bullying research. None of the theoretical models currently reflected in the bullying
literature explicitly consider the dynamics and effects of power abuse; yet, this single
variable distinguishes bullying from aggression. The next section will survey the
literature on power with the aim of finding parallels to the bullying phenomenon by
exploring the links between the literature on bullying and the literature on power.
The power qualities of bullying are the central concern of this writer’s project, yet
a thorough archival search of the bullying literature has yielded no study specifically and
directly linking the literature on power with the literature on bullying. In this section, the
power literature will be briefly reviewed and then related to the topic of bullying.
Theory and research of power involve a range social processes and are important
fields of study in several social science disciplines, such as sociology (Dahrendorf, 1958;
Parsons, 1969), organizational theory (Kearins, 1998), political science (Gaventa, 1980;
Giddens, 1995; Luhman, 1990), social and community psychology (Rappaport, 1987),
69
marriage and family studies (Safilios-Rothschild, 1970; Straus & Gelles, 1979, 1990),
and social work (Guittierrez, 1990; Guittierrez & Ortega, 1991; Kondrat, 1995; Levy,
1990). Contemporary power theorists and researchers tend to cross disciplines such as
philosophy and psychology (Foucault, 1973, 1977a, 1977b, 1978, 1997); sociology,
philosophy, and linguistics (Habermas, 1971, 1979, 1984, 1987, 1996, 2000, 2001a); and
Definitions of Power
The first problem in the study of power is that there has yet to be a consensus on
the definition for the concept of power itself (Dahl, 1957; Keifer, 1984; Lukes, 1974;
Parsons, 1969). Just as the concept of bullying is inclusive of a great many qualities and
bullying, the physical aspects of power can be more easily quantified by examining the
control and distribution of resources, personal attributes, and access to the political
process, than can the socially constructed aspects of power that lie within our subjective
experience of the world (Bachrach & Botwinick., 1992; Foucault, 1973, 1978, 1997;
Parsons (1969) says that there are three contexts that explain the difficulty in
developing a consensus for the definition of power: the concept itself is diffuse; the
relation between the coercive and consensual aspects of power has not been clearly
established11; and the notion that power is a zero-sum problem poses difficulty. In
11
Parsons wrote this before the ideas of Foucault, Derrida, and Habermas, among others, enriched the
dialogue on power.
70
reality, certain aspects of power can be seen as having finite properties, called zero-sum,
Parsons (1969) subscribes to the zero-sum idea of power, which is consistent with
his structural functionalist idea that uses as its dominant metaphor for social relations the
flow of goods and services in the marketplace. The zero-sum idea proposes that, like
money and most resources, there is a finite supply of power, and that increasing the
power of one group or person will, somewhere in the system, decrease the power of
another. In fact, Parsons equates money and power as similar symbolic media in that
Weber (1947) defines power as "the probability that one actor in a social
relationship will be in a position to carry out his (sic) will despite resistance, regardless of
the basis on which this probability rests" (Weber, 1947, p. 152). Weber provides a
schema through which power is exercised through authority based on different qualities
framework for how complex social systems function by adapting to new situations, by
attaining goals, by integrating or harmonizing with other systems, and by including a set
technology of power, he rarely uses the term ―power‖ itself. When he does, he defines
that promote or resist change in human systems (Foucault, 1977b). Habermas also rarely
uses the term ―power‖ but, in elucidating the instrumental and communicative sources of
social action, he covers the gamut of power relations (as will be fully explained below).
71
Luhman (1990), while acknowledging the objective manifestations of power, points out
that power is often situated in social relationships in which at least some part of the action
could have been different, no matter what the state of the power relations; with this
symbolic or subjective aspect of power, power would not be considered a finite quantity.
Bachrach and Baratz (1962) developed a model with two dimensions of power
that is still widely used in the political theory literature today and will be helpful in
locating the various forms of bullying catalogued by bullying researchers. The first
dimension of power is the most easily quantified: it includes observable behavior, such as
overt use of physical strength and resources. The second dimension of power includes
techniques that are employed to deter access to political power by keeping issues off the
political agenda; these less obvious uses of power include ―non-decision-making‖ and
―mobilization of bias.‖
constructed (Burger & Luckman, 1966) plane, as in Foucault’s analysis of how power
1973), mental health (Foucault, 1976), justice (Foucault, 1977a), and sexuality (Foucault,
1978). For example, in Foucault’s analysis of the mechanics of state power, he traces the
evolution of the techniques of power: from coercive forms exercised directly on the body,
through public displays of torture and execution, to more remote and subjective
Foucault (1977) examines the evolution of the mechanics of power over time across
the dimensions described by Bachrach and Barantz (1962). Initially, power was
72
exercised directly by the monarch and his proxies against the individual body, the first
power dimension. Before there were codified laws, local lords meted out justice usually
in the form of capital punishment: torture, bodily humiliation, or execution. With the
codification of laws, the sovereign began to be distanced from the body, and power
plan in which prison barracks are built around a central atrium from which the inmates
can easily be watched --as one of the most significant inventions in Western history. The
people. Subsequent civil engineering used the panoptikon principle in city planning, as in
Napoleon III’s redesign of Paris in which miles of medieval alleys and crooked streets
were razed and replaced with wide straight avenues amenable to surveillance and easy
access of troops (Saalman, 1971). Foucault sees the panoptikon and the dossier as a
describes that power is exercised through knowledge (Foucault, 1977b) and can extend to
our own bodies in the form of sexual constraints and stereotypes (Foucault, 1978) and the
Habermas’s early work closely mirrors Foucault’s in two meaningful ways but
differs in an even more significant way: Habermas, like Foucault, investigated how
body of literature. Foucault, however, exhibits a flare for dramatic narrative, drawing the
73
reader into his highly abstract philosophy with stories taken from history; at times,
Foucault’s enthusiasm for narrative leads him, at times, towards doubtful associations
though his work may lack narrative verve. Habermas abandoned his interest-based
Derrida’s contribution to the study of power derives from his textual project.
Derrida changed the frame of literary analysis by making all characters in narratives
text that reveal the social forces acting on the writer to constrain the narrative, the actions
can help social workers and their clients see the world differently.
Perhaps Derrida’s most profound contribution to social work will be the idea of
anything that exists in reality. It is a pun on the French terms ―to differ‖ – differer – and
the English word ―to defer,‖ and refers to things that exist out of time, that are deferred,
but that would exist if not for constraints in the socially constructed world (Derrida,
1988). While Derrida avoided theory and method in his work, it is this writer’s opinion
that his literary analysis techniques will be able to provide the social work profession
with a very clear way of helping clients to analyze the social world in order to detect and
on the work of Weber and Parsons and purports to solve the problem of how systems can
74
intersubjective paradigm, in the form of the idea of the lifeworld, Habermas shows how
systems designed to solve problems in can develop a life of their own and diverge from
The idea of power entered the social work dialogue in the form of empowerment
(Guittierrez & Ortega, 1991; Kondrat, 1995; Moreau, 1990). The meaning of
detail below; however, one can say that the common feature of the various definitions
power of groups that are in some way oppressed. Kondrat (1995) cautions, though, that
social workers can ensure that empowerment is mutual by including the interests of other
lay groundwork for the study of power qualities in bullying. The methods discussed range
from studies of community power structure (Gilbert, 1972); observation of the activities
of small groups (Gamson, 1982; Milgram, 1974); use of survey instruments with
authority that has a right to dominate others. Bachrach and Botwinick (1992) suggest
that there is a strong current in society that sees compliance as essential to the stability of
the society; that belief mitigates the movement towards participatory democracy. Several
During and after World War II, a number of critical theorists living in the US
technology to develop an instrument that they hoped would help identify individuals at
risk for becoming dangerously oppressive. The so-called ―F-Scale‖, or Fascist Scale, was
updated by Aletmeyer and his colleagues (1981) by including items from cognitive
Timothy Leary (1956) integrated some of Adorno’s items into the MMPI
its time, to include measures of interpersonal power. Leary developed two sub-scales to
measure dominance globally, the DOM and LOV: the DOM scale measures -- items were
scale measures behaviors such as acceptance of others, affection, and the tendency and
desire for affiliation. Leary’s scale has not been used widely12 but is one of the few
12
In fact, the scale was only available through the Harvard Library which only owns one copy. This may be
largely due to done to Leary’s academic reputation by his later activities promoting the use of psilocybin
and LSD for which he lost his academic position.
76
examples of early power research that is predicated on a neutral rather than either a
experiments that were subsequently determined to have put their subjects at risk for
intended to test the limits of obedience in which subjects were made to believe they were
researcher at Yale University, enjoined the research subjects to administer greater and
greater levels of shock to ―learners‖ whenever they made mistakes in learning word pairs.
The experimenter assured the subjects that there would be no permanent damage that
could result from any shock they administered. Subjects believed they were actually
administering shocks, while in fact, the learners were actors paid to sham reactions of
pain and protest. Despite clear labeling on the devices used to administer the ―shock,‖
fully two-thirds of the subjects administered shocks at levels that would have been deadly
had the shocks been real. The degree to which people were willing to obey authority was
recruited subjects to act as prisoners and prison guards and created a make-shift prison in
which the roles were played out. Zimbardo found that acting out the role of prison guard
the subjects-- some of whom have lived for decades with the guilt that they are capable of
issues of power and, although unintentionally, its psychological effects in that some
a fictional trial in civil court. One-third of the participants were requested to argue in
favor of an oil company’s position and attempted to sway the others to make false
statements on videotapes that they were told would be used in a civil trial. The researcher
then left the room for a period of time. Gamson was trying to create a scenario conducive
to rebellion but not easily spotted by his subjects as a hoax. A successful rebellion career
was seen as a unanimous decision not to sign the affidavit allowing the fictional oil
company to use the videotape in the fictional trial. Gamson then analyzed and classified
the rebellion activities into seven categories of relevant action: compliance, evasion, rim
talk (questions and discussions about the scenario), dissent, resistance, direct action, and
preparation for future action. The author was originally attempting to study the actions of
defiance against authority; the high levels of compliance to authority he found had not
been anticipated.
bullying among children due to the obvious ethical constraints of putting children into
potentially damaging situations. However, the entirety of the literature on the effects of
bullying can be viewed as research into the effects of power imbalance on target children.
through the study of the secondary data into the social, emotional, physical, and cognitive
effects of bullying. Finding ethical and safe ways to study the conditions under which
children resist oppressive and abusive authority could help in the design of bullying
In a study more aligned with today’s ideas of protecting human subjects, Keiffer
(1998) looked at the process of community activism and the emergence of individual
Keiffer chose a sample of 15 community activists, all of whom acknowledged that their
participants were active in more than one issue area, took a proactive rather than a
reactive position vis-a-vis social action, and had an ongoing commitment to social action.
Keiffer conducted interviews with the participants using the technique of dialogic
their own data and elaborating the meaning of their personal transformation.
Keiffer found that there are a set of skills that include an awakening consciousness
through which actions evoke new understanding, which then provoke new and more
effective action,‖ and a set of skills to deal with constructive conflict. The author
participatory competence‖ (p29). Keiffer suggests that further research with larger
the experience of people who have successfully negotiated bullying and other oppressive
power experiences. This could lead to intervention programs that promote the safe
This section will discuss the history of empowerment and look at the research that
universally accepted way. Keiffer defines empowerment (as cited above) as the
personal, interpersonal, or political power so that individuals can take action to improve
their life situations‖ (Guittierrez, 1990, p. 149); Rappaport states that empowerment is ―a
over their affairs‖ and that social problems arise from the inequality of access to
resources and power among different groups in society (Rappaport, 1987). Simon (1994)
operation of power blocks, or, at least, to diminish their effects. Empowerment, in these
authors’ definitions, is the use of power in ways that address inequality across groups and
balancing power, they do not address how that power can be increased or balanced in an
80
ethical way. As has been discussed above, the symmetry inherent in Kondrat’s (1995)
idea of ―mutual emancipation‖ provides a constraint: to increase power for one group
Power is essential to make change in the social world. Gutierrez (1990) claims that
social workers can best create change by sharing power with their clients. She suggests a
number of strategies and principles to apply to that end: accepting the client’s definition
of the problem; identifying and building upon existing strengths; engaging in a power
analysis of the client’s situation; teaching specific skills; and working with the client to
mobilize resources. The goal is to reduce self-blame while increasing a sense of efficacy
Gutierrez and Ortega (1991) studied empowerment strategies among Latino college
1991) developed their Change Strategy Scale based on subjects’ responses to an anecdote
variation on Milgram’s and Gamson’s small group technique, in that, rather than having
subjects participate in a hoax, the authors gave the subjects an anecdote and asked a set of
questions designed to elicit what power strategies they would use to deal with that
situation. This methodology could be more easily adapted for use with children,
adolescents, in particular.
81
indirectly, on issues of power. In this subsection, these few studies will be discussed.
& Levinson, 2004; Mynard, Joseph, et al., 2000), and the element of power abuse may
increase or even determine the toxic effect of an aggressive action (Mynard, Joseph, et
accidents, and social misunderstandings may be far less traumatic than aggressive events
sessions led by a therapist. They examined the strategies children used to ―transform‖
themselves or others. The authors coded behaviors that they consider to reflect four
conceptual levels of social skills development and were rated as either self-transforming
of the research subjects are observed intermittently for brief periods, two observers,
trained to identify negotiation strategies, took detailed notes of their observations from
behind a two-way mirror. The verbal content was transcribed, coded, and analyzed. The
authors offer this insight into the process of these two children:
In our view any strategy, regardless of its orientation or its developmental level,
represents an attempt to exercise some kind of control over a situation. A self-
transforming strategy is a particular way of controlling a situation in which the
medium through which control is achieved is self-adaptation; and conversely, for
82
children’s power tactics, including instances of bullying and the responses that bullying
evoked.
Mynard, Joseph, and Lawrence (2000), while not making explicit links to the
imbalance and bullying. The authors studied 331 adolescents at one English secondary
school in an attempt to determine whether specific types of bullying are associated with
specific adverse effects. In particular, the authors tested the effects of relational, direct,
and indirect bullying on self-esteem and post-traumatic stress symptoms. All the
(Harter, 1985), and were also asked to respond ―yes or no‖ to the question of whether
they had been bullied. Forty percent of respondents were identified as having been
victims of bullies, fairly evenly divided between boys and girls. When the data were
analyzed for this group, bullying was found to be correlated with posttraumatic stress.
Relational bullying and social manipulation were found to predict posttraumatic stress.
Physical bullying and low self-worth were also found to be associated, with the subscale
Mynard and colleagues’ results would need to be replicated in order for one to
draw definitive conclusions based upon them; yet, their evidence that different bullying
83
experiences correlate with ill effects supports two important assumptions: 1) that
aversive.
Raven and his colleagues (Raven, 1993; Schmidt, Raven, Pastorelli & Caprara,
1993; Schmidt & Raven, 1985) attempted to apply Raven and French’s (1960) adaptation
of Weber’s (1947) power analysis to children’s peer relations and bullying. The authors
undertook pilot studies using a cartoon instrument that depicts French and Raven’s
categories of administrative power in ways that apply to the world of children’s peer
relations (Raven, 1993; Schmidt, Raven, Pastorelli & Caprara, 1993; Schmidt & Raven,
1985). The authors conclude that there are parallels between the power used in childhood
and adult forms of power but did not pursue their research further.
skill shares important qualities with Ducharme, Folino, and DeRosie’s (2008) more
skill required for all negotiation and cooperation. Bullying may be an other-transforming
strategy that children use when they are lacking in or disdainful of higher order skills: in
In the following section, ideas that shed light on bullying effects indirectly or
Almost all the theory discussed in the bullying literature concerns the etiology of
bullying rather than the nature of its effects. In this section, ideas and theories from a
84
variety of sources that could potentially be adapted to explain the mechanisms of effect of
Categorical Thinking
Heyman, 1999; Giles, 2003) explains how bullies identify a child to victimize and may
also explain how bullying experiences can be internalized by the victim, thereby causing
Categorical thinking is a stable set of ideas and beliefs through which the individual
activity in the neo-cortex-- these ideas are learned slowly over time and tend to resist
change; flexible thinking, possibly located in the hippocampus, involves faster learning
Macrae and Bodenhausen explore the ways that categorical thought processes can
lead to stereotyping when they state that ―…perceivers may use the contents of the
judgmental outcomes (Macrae & Bodenhausen, 2000, p. 95).‖ That children in general
exhibit the features of categorical thinking was established by Gelman (2003) who
into four groups: two groups were described as ―children who eat carrots,‖ and ―children
85
who believe in creatures,‖ thought to be neutral stimuli; and two groups were labeled
―carrot eaters‖ and ―creature believers,‖ non-neutral, categorical, stimuli. Both labeled
groups, carrot eaters and creature believers showed evidence of stereotyping but the
descriptive groups, children who eat carrots and who believe in creatures, did not.
victimization. That some bullies choose a target based on a categorical perception seems
Poteat & Espelage, 2007; Warwick et al., 2004), but categorical thinking may also
explain some of the effects of bullying. The child who is affected by the bullying and
categorical thinking of victims, but it can be hypothesized that both bullies and victims
will exhibit less flexible thinking than their non-bully-involved peers. The presence of
systematic review below, variables such as low self-esteem and low self-worth may serve
internalization and victimization; however, there are most likely positive outcomes of
categorical thought processes as well. It seems likely that categorical processes are
86
Commitments, covenants, and contracts have a great deal of force within human relations
as breaking an oath or bond means jeopardizing one’s own social connections. Much
Resilience is another concept that has implications in this review. The construct
of resilience, like power, social competence, and bullying, comprises a wide array of
behaviors, qualities, and conditions (Keyes, 2004; Luthar & Cicchetti, 2000), and the
study of resilience is in its infancy. Resilience factors are those whose presence in a
child’s life will predict positive outcomes in adulthood: social competence is one of the
The primacy of social skills in the lifeworld gets empirical support from John
psychologist as well as a physicist with highly developed mathematical skills, coded the
speech/acts of couples as they interacted over a series of taped interactions collected over
decades. Gottman and his colleagues discovered a simple mathematical formula that
predicted divorce: couples who committed more than one negative speech/act in every six
interactions were statistically likely to divorce; those that were able to maintain positive
skills stayed together. These coded speech/acts included a range of interactions from
Gottman’s work can be seen as evidence of the importance of primary social skills in
work that children able to gain and maintain social skills will also have success in
major resilience factor. There is also a cohort of resilient children who have been bullied
but do not experience ill effects: these children will likely show attributes and actions that
virtually absent. Research on the factors that protect a child from victimization or the
factors that mitigate the effects of victimization would be very helpful in designing
effective interventions.
Resilience, in the form of social support and social competence, is at the heart of
Habermas’s theoretical project. His conceptualization of ethical action is based not on the
interaction. The most competent people will be able to put themselves into the standpoint
of others, no matter which culture she is part of. In fact, evidence will be sought from the
existing empirical literature for mechanisms of effect that alienate the social sources of
With one exception (P. K. Smith, L. Talamelli, et al., 2004), bullying research has
been conducted using a medical frame and based on the assumption that bullying
perspective would not require the assumption that bullying, the experience of multiple
humiliations and aggressive acts, is beneficial; but rather would make visible the many
13
Habermas tends to use the term ―universal,‖ yet is very clear that he is studying contemporary Western
cultures. This writer uses the qualifier ―possibly‖ before ―universal‖ because a great deal of research
would need to be undertaken to determine whether the social skills described by Habermas are truly
universal or are constrained by Western ideas of human rights that have emerged post Enlightenment.
88
positive social experiences that bullying displaces, replaces, or prevents. In fact, a large
part of the effects of bullying explained by the theory of communicative action and
derived concepts involve the systematic alienation of the victim from sources of support
Deeper bonds, such as family relations and personal friendships, may survive
bullying experiences, if only because they are not primarily based in school where the
bullying occurs; weaker bonds, however, within the peer environment, may be instantly
decimated depending on the type and severity of the bullying. Granoveter’s (1973) notion
of the importance of weak bonds, discussed above, will be more fully elaborated within
The concept of play, for this age group meaning forms of social free play, will
figure prominently in this paper. Play and Habermas’s key theoretical concept of
The term ―play‖ was defined by Lever in 1978 as ―a cooperative interaction that
has no stated goal, no end point, and no winners (1978, p. 473); formal games in contrast,
473). Later definitions echo Lever’s and suggest that, at higher levels of socialization,
play includes such behaviors as the rehearsal of adult roles and serves as a venue in
which children can practice a wide set of social skills, including the ability to influence
each other without resorting to coercion (Christie & Johnsen, 1987; Cole-Hamilton,
Harrop, & Street, 2005; Frost & Jacobs, 1995; A.D. Pellegrini & Smith, 1998; Rubin,
Fein, & Vandenberg, 1983; Rubin, Maioni, & Hornung, 1976; Spinrad et al., 2004).
89
features and may be essentially the same phenomenon14. Attempts to influence another
influencing others based on logic, norms, and relationship. Victimization puts an affected
child into what Habermas would call an instrumental state; for some children, this may
last only in the minutes they are being bullied, but for other children, the bullying may
The play behavior of victimized children is yet another important area of research
only marginally featured in the bullying literature (Boulton, 1999; A.D. Pellegrini,
Blatchford, Kato, & Baines, 2004). Prominent bullying researchers Pellegrini and Smith
performed a meta-analysis of studies into the benefits to children of play and found that
the only benefits of play that can be empirically demonstrated are physical strength and
flexibility (A.D. Pellegrini & Smith, 1998). This lack of finding is likely due to the
methodological difficulties of tracking change over time. This project makes the case that
much can be learned from bullying research based on the fact that bully-involved
While play deprivation has been identified in the conceptual literature at the time
of this writing, there has been no empirical study attempting to measure the effect of play
deprivation. This writer’s project may shed some light into the effect of play deprivation
by showing that some of the ill effects of victimization result from play deprivation by
virtue of the theorized idea that victimized children experience less communicative social
contexts which can be shown by demonstrating that the children play less.
14
A research protocol directly based on Habermas’s theory of communicative action would need to be
implemented to determine whether and what kind of play is similar to communicative action.
90
Autonomy
The concept of autonomy has not figured in any theory of bullying or research. It
common feature of all bullying interactions. Threat and coercion are overtly physical but
relational bullying, including status attacks, effectively limits the autonomy of the
victimized child within the social world. Although autonomy is implicitly subsumed into
Habermas’s theory of communicative action, it was the cornerstone of his earlier idea of
of autonomy that assumes that an actor is operating in a complex social environment and
(Bertschinger, Olbrich, Ay, & Jost, 2006). In other words, autonomous action does not
mean taking action that is self-derived without consideration of the effect one will have
within the social world. A successful interaction will not only leave the environment
Shapiro (1981, 2000) sees the restriction of autonomy as the essential component
Yet another example of the interdisciplinary fragmentation Shapiro does not cite or
ideas from which a focus on autonomy clearly belongs. Shapiro shows how the
obstruction of autonomy distorts character in rational ways that fit perfectly with and
extend Habermas’s ideas of cognitive distortion. This also meshes with Kokko’s (2000)
notion of cumulative continuity in that these experiences accrete over time and pervade
91
the individual’s choice of social life. For example, Shapiro would predict a strong
correlation between childhood bully involvement and adult sadistic and/or masochistic
children of curative and supportive experiences. This idea will be explored at length as it
theorized to be more globally impaired. The existing literature will be plumbed for
prevention program in an elementary school, found data that both confirmed and
challenged the conventional wisdom present in the bullying literature. The authors found
that community violence was a factor in how one goes about reducing and preventing
bullying. The cousins, sibling, friends, and even parents of victimized children would
frequently directly intervene with the bully or his family, sometimes directly threatening
violent retaliation. There appeared to be a code of conduct toward violence, although this
researcher was unable to find an informant willing to discuss this frankly, that
stigmatized the brutalization of weaker children. The study took place in an urban
92
community that, despite a rapidly decreasing crime and murder rate, still had many
research and non-research venues to express vehement opposition to one aspect of the
violence prevention program being implemented in classrooms. Both youth and adults
made it clear to our staff that teaching children to go to a teacher for help if they were
being bullied or threatened was an unwise and potentially lethal strategy in their
community that had a viable drug trade. They explained that they did not trust that the
young children being taught to report bullying behavior to teachers would not do the
same in the more dangerous community context such as the drug trade. Community
members who have complained to police or had given evidence against drug dealers had
categorical thinking (Macrae & Bodenhausen, 2000). One child who had been bullied
extensively revealed her deeply-held belief that some unknown quality inherent in her
nature was to blame for the bullying. Individual, classroom, and group interventions, as
well as an arts-based recreational program, helped her find a unique identity and adopt a
The foregoing review of the bullying literature suggests several gaps in the
literature and directions for future research. Despite the primacy of power imbalance in
the definition of bullying, no research has specifically focused on the power bases used
by bullies. Rigby (2004) suggests that bullying can be understood as a socio-cultural
93
phenomenon; however, he does not suggest a specific social theory that might be applied
to the phenomenon
Other ideas or paradigms do not rise to the level of an expansive social theory but
may also elucidate the bullying phenomenon. For example, research into the idea of
interactive and cumulative continuity (Kokko & Pulkkinen, 2000) could elucidate the
processes of bully involvement: in particular, how children who enter school at risk for
bullying involvement may be less likely to transcend their victim or bully role. Kokko’s
theory of cumulative continuity would predict that children involved in bullying, in
whatever role, might repeat their experiences throughout childhood, staying with the
same friends or friends who share the same tendency toward bully-involvement.
The bullying literature does not provide a comprehensive picture of the entry into
and exit from the victim and bully roles. The literature does imply that some children
with few social skills are at risk for bullying the moment they enter the school system.
Although there is evidence that bullying is a stable phenomenon, it is also apparent that
children enter and exit bullying at different times in their school careers. Future research
may collect data on entry and reentry into victimization to determine the differences in
context and constitution of symptomatic victims from those children who do not become
symptomatic after being bullied. As touched on above, community violence has been
largely neglected as an environmental variable that may affect the quality and quantity of
bullying.
cohort of bully victimized children who were not poly-victimized and determine bullying
Very little research has been done to determine any benefits or perceived benefits
of bullying. No study directly or indirectly asked parents and teachers whether they
and older siblings of children may well support bullying as a way for the children to gain
a competitive advantage while other adults may value the submissiveness of victimized
children. As described above, Nazi policy appeared to directly promote bullying as a way
of preparing young people for adult roles of coercion. An historical study of pedagogy
and children’s literature may unearth other examples of techniques used to promote the
Only two studies in the literature investigate whether teachers themselves have a
tendency to bully or scapegoat children (Omigbodun et al., 2008; S.W. Twemlow et al.,
2006). It seems likely, given the fact that local factors often determine the prevalence of
bullying, that the power tactics of teachers and school administrators could determine the
high and low bullying schools could shed light on these local factors15.
Play is another neglected area of bullying research. It is likely that both bullies
and their victims are deprived of social play given the isolating nature of the problem.
While there is research to show that bully-involved children tend to be less successful
socially, there has been no research specifically correlating bullying involvement and its
effects on play behavior. Buhs and Ladd (2001) contend that children with compromised
social systems and poor social skills lack the opportunity to play. Aside from rough and
tumble play, which mimics bullying and aggression in a play context, bullying cannot be
considered play. Children involved in bullying are not playing, at least while the bullying
involvement deprives children of play during and after the bullying occurs. Play
15
Such a research project would present some methodological challenges. In a host setting it would be
challenging to collect data that has the potential to embarrass the host of that setting.
95
deprivation may, in fact, explain some part of the negative effects of bullying. If, as
theorists predict, play helps children learn social skills and rehearse contemporaneous
situations and adult roles (Cole-Hamilton, Harrop & Street, 2007; Frost & Jacobs, 1995),
Research could determine whether bullying changes the quality or quantity of play
activities.
To date, no research has been done to adapt critical theory to the study of
bullying. The theory has largely been used in legal and political theory (Habermas,
1996); however, Habermas’s theory of communicative action offers a full social theory
that is intended to explain a full range of social actions and could as easily be applied to
the life of the individual as to the life of a nation. In fact, Habermas himself often
illustrates his ideas with mundane examples of interpersonal relations – his mechanic, or
a person staying late at a party (1984) within his own lifeworld (in addition to giving
Theory-building has been identified by Morrison (1994) and Rigby (Rigby, 2003)
as a serious gap in the bullying literature. Despite the lack of a theoretical foundation,
school boards are mandated to intervene to end bullying. As Dubin (1978) makes clear,
96
In light of Dubin’s observation, this writer’s project will attempt to tackle this
lack of theory building in the bullying literature by reviewing the current knowledge base
and by applying Habermas’s conceptual framework to see whether and to what degree his
In this project, the German philosopher and critical theorist Jurgen Habermas’s
understand the effects of bullying. This section includes a brief summary of the aspects of
Habermas’s social theory that will be applied in this project. In the subsequent research
design section, the theory will be adapted to the study of bullying and used to derive a
conception of some of the mechanisms through which children are affected by bullying
experiences. An in-depth analysis will be undertaken in that section which will include
While Habermas does not tend to use the terms ―power‖ or ―power imbalance,‖
his project can be understood as an analysis of different forms of power as applied to the
schema of social action, as instrumental action that limits the autonomy of the target (and
the other children involved). Different types of bullying limit the children’s autonomy in
one or more domains of life, physical, emotional and social domains in particular. By
contrast, consensual activities such as those that occur in free play fit the schema of
97
communicative action in which children are highly autonomous and influence each other
in non-coercive ways.
play deprivation; however, that victims of bullying and other bully-involved children
have lower levels of social play can be deduced from studies that show children involved
in bullying are more isolated socially: at least during the minutes when the bullying is
occurring, the children involved are not ―playing.‖ This deprivation of play may explain
some of the continuing and cumulative effects of bullying in that the bully-involved
action (coercive) than their peers. The children, through this lack of communicative
action, therefore, may not be learning essential social skills negotiation, compromise and
meaning creation. Bullies’ and victims’ limited access to this domain of interaction may
explain a large part of the negative sequelae of bullying and other forms of power abuse.
In light of the fact that there is no empirical literature showing the positive effects of
limiting the actual opportunity to interact in a consensual environment, bullying may also
ideas of Dodge and Frame (1982) in which bullies are often considered to be acting on a
98
hostile attribution of social cues due to an inaccurate internal representation of the social
world. Distortion of cognition may explain some of the effects of bullying, not just the
reasons for bullying, in that a child who internalizes a negative self-concept as the result
of bullying will have distortions of self-concept, identity, mood, and have fewer
opportunities to learn and practice social skills. Several studies show that such variables
as self-esteem, identity, and self-beliefs mediate the effects of bullying and other forms of
aggression (Burt, Obradovic, Long, & Masten, 2008; Schmidt & Bagwell, 2007). These
mediating variables, then, can be examined to see if they are consistent or not with
Habermas’s theory.
The following section will clarify the research design and methods chosen for this
project.
99
Opening Statement
This dissertation is a systematic review of the research literature into the effects of
school bullying published in the United States within a ten-year period, with the goals of
developing a model of these effects and of testing German philosopher and critical
theorist Jurgen Habermas’s theory of communicative action with the real world social
problem of school bullying. Systematic reviews and meta-analyses for theory building
are relatively new techniques done in the fields of organizational development (Hassard,
1991; Lewis, 1999; Viswevaran & Ones, 1995; Yang, 2002), psychology (Cheung & Au,
2005), and, under the term ―conceptual ontologies data mining‖ (Blagasklonny & Pardee,
2002), in the fields of information technology and biomedicine (Gottgtroy, Kasabov, &
MacDonell, 2003). Dubin’s (1978) schema of theory building is being used because it is
In this section, the research design and methodology will be outlined as will the
rationale for the design. The goals of the project and the feasibility will be briefly
outlined. The systematic review technique will be described and related to the problem of
The research into the effects of bullying, as with most bullying research, has been
methodologies (Grills & Ollendick, 2002), also called ―shared method variance‖ (Haynes
& Hayes O'Brien, 2000). Both the dependent and independent variables in effects studies
100
have been measured with a variety of techniques. In consequence, the studies into the
effects of bullying present a confusing picture of differing results. While there is a small
number of theories consistently applied in the bullying research, these theories have
tended to explain only the etiology of bullying involvement. No significant work has
been done to create or adapt a theory to explain the effects of the bullying phenomenon.
adequately understood the practitioner does not know how to effectively intervene
(Dubin, 1978; Kaplan, 1964; Van de Ven, 1989). In spite of this lack of theory, bullying
consistently effective, while some evidence shows that certain intervention programs
actually increase bullying or help bullies hide their actions from adults (Dixon et al.,
the feature of power imbalance inherent in the bullying phenomenon, no research has
been done to determine the similarities and differences between bullying and other forms
of power. Nevertheless, bullying studies can be seen as a rich data for the study of the
effects of power, particularly those studies which were conducted using the contemporary
researchers into power and power relations, only Schmidt and Raven, reviewed above
power.
101
Research Question
The essential question for this project, ―What are the effects of bullying on the
target children (victims16)?‖, will be asked at several levels of conceptualization from the
implicit or stated theory in the research through the component concepts, the variables
measured, and the instruments and techniques used. Jurgen Habermas’s theory of
Goals
The goal of building knowledge within critical social work, as within critical
theory, is to emancipate people from active oppression in the present and to promote the
conditions under which they can fully participate in the lifeworld. The primary critical
victimization in order to free children from the oppressive experience of being victimized
test the efficacy of Habermas’s theory of communicative action in reference to the study
It is this writer’s contention that the emancipatory goal of this project is identical
to Jurgen Habermas’s goal of creating the conditions for a peaceful democratic society.
Whereas Habermas applies his theory to nation building and the creation of constitutions,
in this project, this writer looks at the conditions under which children can become free of
oppression at a local level. This writer contends that children able to handle peer
16
For the research project, an attempt will be made to avoid using the terms ―bully‖ and ―victim‖ to refer to
children as the categorization itself may be iatrogenic.
102
oppression effectively will grow into adults able to participate and contribute positively
The review of US empirical studies will be used to create a typology of the effects
of bullying on target children and to propose at least one mechanism by which children
are affected by school bullying. The measures and definitions used in the research will be
organized into typological categories in the interest of conceptual clarity, since, as noted
above, definitions and measures used in bullying research vary widely. This writer
intends that this project will make a contribution to the theoretical understanding of the
ways that the bullying phenomenon and, perhaps, other forms of power abuse, affect the
target child.
This project will attempt to build a theory by using data from research studies
set of studies (Petticrew & Roberts, 2006)--will be conducted on research studies into the
effects of bullying to build a model that can explain the mechanisms of effect. As Lehnert
(2007) suggests, this review will organize the array of variables already studied in the
McGuire (1997) asserts that reorganizing an existing body of literature can itself
reconciling studies with conflicting outcomes. In the case of the bullying literature, this
will entail clarifying the concepts used in the research studies that were conducted using
Only US studies are being selected for this review for several reasons. The
primary reason is that US schools do not yet have significant anti-bullying initiatives is a
Commonwealth study result. US children, in the absence of such programs, are likely to
experience the aftereffects of bullying more strongly than their European counterparts as
the US children are much less likely to have access to informal and professional sources
of support. The research of Wolke and colleagues (Wolke et al., 2000) indicates that
victimized children living in places where there is a stigma against bullying and services
are less symptomatic than children living in places where bullying is largely ignored (i.e.,
the US): the results of their cross-national study show that children in countries where
there is a great deal of service and support in the schools for bullied children.
victims of bullies from 28 nations and found that the victims in the US and Israel ranked
significantly higher in symptom rates and severity than those in the European countries
with which they were compared. While it would be hard to determine all the factors that
explain this sharp difference, the US and Israel share two features that constitute
confounding variables: (1) both countries had significant wars during the period in which
most of the studies were conducted and published (US—Iraq and Afghanistan; Israel—
Lebanon and Palestine); and (2) both countries occupied other countries during this
period, which has raised human rights’ and other concerns. The US engaged in
systematic methods of torture in Iraq (Bybee, 2002) amid a media climate that was, in
some cases, tolerant of torture and other oppressive tactics carried out by government
officials, and Israel has used its military to attack terrorists (or freedom fighters using
104
terror tactics depending on one’s frame of reference on the Middle East) in the West
Another reason for limiting this review to US studies is that the term ―bullying‖
languages and cultures (P. K. Smith & Brain, 2000; P. K. Smith, Cowie, Olafsson, &
understand the term in the same way, while speakers of other languages understand
Meta-analyses are often paired with systematic reviews. The research studies used
in meta-analyses need to measure the same phenomenon, using the same or similar
techniques. Various meta-analytic techniques are often used to determine the most
techniques can be applied depending on the nature of the data and the question being
asked: the cumulative effect size can be generated; the homogeneity of the collected
results can be determined; compatibility with a theory or model can be measured using
complex factor analysis and path analysis techniques both separately and together in the
homogeneity of method is that the use of several studies can dramatically can increase the
sample size and, therefore, increase the power of the statistical relationship.
tremendous heterogeneity among the variables in the selected studies. All of this project’s
selected studies report data on the correlation between victimization and at least one of its
hypothesized effects but very few use the same instrument or methods with a comparable
victimization, different studies among those selected sought to determine the presence of
bullying during different time periods. Some asked only if the bullying was happening
immediate effects of bullying but may miss some of the longer term effects by
106
eliminating escaped victims, children who were no longer being victimized and who may
be asymptomatic.
which simply tabulates and compares the relative effect sizes on their face value without
using a statistical technique to determine aggregated effects sizes. This review will use
this vote-counting technique to examine the results on their face value. This method
allows for simple, descriptive techniques. The range, significance, and direction of the
study results for each variable will be presented in table form, and these scores will be
averaged. Where feasible, averages will be generated to allow a comparison between age
groups. The method will be more fully elaborated in the data analysis section below.
Vote counting does not allow the researcher to weight effect sizes according to
the number of subjects used or the type and acuity of the instrument. More advanced
techniques will have to wait for a more homogeneous set of studies to be undertaken.
Lipsey and Wilson (2001) coined a term for the variation in context and method
among different studies purporting to measure the same variables: ―the apples and
oranges effect.‖ This project by aggregating and analyzing the various measures and
concepts in the disparate studies, intends to bring some clarity to the apple and orange
effect of the results of the studies. The writer’s main method for bringing about this
Constructed Typologies
A systematic review can be used to determine the state of knowledge and to begin
and rethinking the variables used in various studies, in this case, to develop a model to
The call for conceptual clarity is consistent with the evolution of the social
to describe and explain the same social action in different contexts (and used sometimes
used the same concept to describe different phenomena). Lazarsfeld (1937) credits
Hempel and Oppenheim (cited in Lazarsfeld, 1937) with describing and codifying the
ways social scientists borrowed techniques from the hard sciences by developing ―types‖
Lazarsfeld (Lazarsfeld & Henry, 1968; Lazarsfeld & Rosenberg, 1955) and McKinney
(1966) adapt classification methodologies from the hard sciences through the use of
The use of powerful computers has extended this process by freeing conceptual
of human action, including numbers of factors beyond the capacity of most human beings
to manipulate (Blagasklonny & Pardee, 2002; Gottgtroy et al., 2003; Gruber, 1993); these
conceptual connections are then made intelligible to humans through concept mapping
(Trochim, 1989).17 This project does not have data sources large enough to require
sophisticated data- processing techniques, but the process of building a theory is the
same. Given the current public interest in the topic of bullying and the availability of
17
Conceptual ontologies data mining is beyond the scope of this project due to the limited scope of
available data; however, with web technology, the available data could rise exponentially in the near
future.
108
―crowd sourcing technology‖-- the use of the Internet to gather large amounts of data
very quickly--it is possible that this and other projects like it could be the beginning of a
action as the conceptual framework for the project is based largely on Habermas’s
the types of indirect bullying that involves the manipulation of the social environment
that have emerged from over 30 years of research. Briefly, Habermas proposes a set of
categories of instrumental action, including direct action and strategic action, in which
others are recruited to elicit an action: this is consistent with the ways that bullies
Social Action
Communicative Instrumental
Action Action
Conscious
Deception
Unconscious
Deception
109
action or a derived theory can also provide an explanatory typology--a typology of the
concepts and relationships between concepts that explain a phenomenon (Lazarsfeld &
Henry, 1968). To that end, the concepts and measures used in existing bullying studies
will be catalogued and abstracted into typologies. The creation of a typology involves the
compression of large amounts of data into a smaller set of concepts. These typologies
will be compared with Habermas’s theory of communicative action. This will entail
organizing the many variables studied in the disparate studies into logical categories.
Theory building and testing is largely the work of the imagination (McGuire,
1997; Weick, 1989), often the imaginations of multiple contributors (Kaplan, 1964)
formation:
necessarily visible (Brante, 2001). These processes may be ―latent variables‖, either
unseen or unnoticed, whose relationships are laws or rules that can explain and predict
relationships among variables and properties cannot be perceived, the researcher must
rely on the creative imagination, albeit based on real world experience. As Dubin (1978)
110
states, all theory begins with our understanding of the world, whether this takes the form
and recordings of events and conversation. For the research into bullying, processes exist
Dubin (1978) suggests that there is a dynamic process that occurs among theory,
lived experience (for which he uses the German term verstehen, or understanding of
human activity), and empirical research. Each one informs and enriches the other. He
processes, and empirical research. He states that there are three results possible in the
testing of a theory or model: 1) the theory and empirical results are identical; 2) the
theory is not sufficient to explain all the results; and 3) the results go beyond the
boundaries of the theory. In this case, the empirical literature on the effects of bullying
Theory itself can bring phenomena into human consciousness. Research and
theory serve to make seemingly obscure processes visible. Latent, theorized, variables
are often considered to be invisible, but in fact, the visibility of theorized variables is a
relative phenomenon (Price, 2007) as are the ways that theory can make social processes
visible. Marx and Freud and their colleagues and, later Einstein created revolutions in
thought by theorizing about invisible processes in the social world and the physical
universe, respectively. For example, Einstein rethought the nature of light asking himself
what if light were neither a particle nor a wave but instead had qualities of both. This was
a theoretical idea up until 1919 when observers found that light from a distant star
passing the sun during an eclipse was bent by the sun’s mass. Not only did the light
111
curve, it curved exactly as Einstein’s theory predicted. Light clearly had a small amount
In the case of bullying, despite the calibration of bullies which maintains their
activities sub rosa (Gumpel, 2000), adults also substantially ignored the bullying, perhaps
assuming that it was a rite of passage that could not be stopped. One can say that
process, are invisible; yet Rensis Likert (1932) developed a simple method of asking
people to rate their subjective process on a numeric scale: the Likert Scale makes
subjective processes visible by simply asking the individual to rank their subjective
experience. In an example from the bullying literature, the cognitive effects of bullying
occur within the mind of the child and are accessible only to the degree that the child is
developmentally able to express herself; however, any lessening of social play is visible
to the researcher who can observe and record the child’s and others’ actions.
and long-term effects. The studies selected for this review measure a large number of
effects which may or may not have a temporal relationship with each other. It is likely
that a logical model will emerge from this review showing how being victimized affects a
child over time. It is hoped that such a model will be the first step in answering
Finkelhor’s (1995) call to enrich our understanding of how victimization affects the
Though the methods adapted for this research are varied and drawn from different
sources, they have been chosen for their capacity to answer a series of questions that will
give a comprehensive picture of the present state of knowledge in the field of bullying
and how that knowledge relates to Habermas’s theory of communicative action. The
research will involve two stages: a systematic review of selected existing studies, and the
This stage of the research will analyze selected existing studies to create a
typology and model(s) of the effects of bullying. Conceptual clarity will be sought in
order to reconcile or differentiate data gathered using different definitions and methods to
The second stage of the research will relate the concepts and research results to
Habermas’s theory of communicative action and derived concepts and constructs. This
Can the results of existing studies be compared with a conceptual model based on
Habermas’s theory?
Are there data that disconfirm Habermas’s theory?
Are there data that suggest an alternative model to Habermas’s?
Are there data that deepen or extend Habermas’s theory?
Hawker and Boulton’s (2000) review of 20 years of studies into the adjustment
effects of victimization very much inspired this project. The overall method of this
project is similar in several key ways. Like the Hawker and Boulton review, this project
converts study results to a common metric; also like this project the authors used the
vote-counting method18, presenting the results and a group mean for each category that
was not weighted according to the sample size. Hawker and Boulton limited their
descriptive analysis to concurrent studies and grouped their selected studies according to
the type of bullying definition used. Hawker and Boulton’s contribution was a milestone
as it helped to clarify and summarize the disparate definitions that had been used in early
bullying research.
Kim and Leventhal (2008) performed a systematic review of the research studies
that looked at bullying and the single effect of suicidal ideation and behavior. For the
same methodological reasons as are used in this project, the authors felt that a full meta-
analysis was not feasible as the dependent and independent variables were measured in
The theory-building aspect of this project has close parallels to the work of Bai
Yin Yang (2002) and Lewis and Grimes (1999) who proposes the use of meta-analysis as
18
Hawker and Boulton use the term meta-analysis in their study. As in this study the studies available to
Hawker and Boulton lacked homogeneity. Hawker and Boulton can be said to have provided a set of
statistics describing the body of study results including their ranges and averages.
114
shows the literature in that field is rich in theory. A pilot study reviewing the bullying
literature in preparation for this project showed that the bullying literature is very thin on
theory. Where Yang and Lewis and Grimes were building on a rich tradition of
theorizing, this projects is taking place at the preliminary stages of theory building,
identifying potential latent constructs and processes within the empirical literature and
The theory analysis aspect of this project will be modeled upon three studies:
Margolin and Goldis (2000), Painter and colleagues (2008), and Abramovitz (1981).
Margolin and Goldis’s (2000) systematic review of the literature on the effects of
studies that show a range of effects (although not nearly as rich a source of data as the
bullying literature provides). The authors, however, do not include any theory in their
review. Painter and colleagues (2008) surveyed the theories used in a selected review of
empirical research articles into behavioral health, the field that promotes a healthy
lifestyle. They found that less than 5% of researchers clearly articulated any theory in
portraying their research. This project will similarly survey the selected studies for the
direct use of theory, but will also attempt to detect latent theories at play.
business played on the political processes and alliances that supported or thwarted
worker’s compensation and health care reform in the US. She reviewed literature from
project will survey the (little) theory that exists and propose a particular theoretical frame.
However, the studies in this project are not homogeneous, and only one theory, not three,
will be used for the conceptual framework. The homogeneity of the documents in
Abramovitz’s study lent themselves to a more systematic approach in the review since
the concepts and results were more able to be organized into tabular form.
A few studies indirectly parallel this project by using existing data sources to
build a theory and knowledge in general. Hripcsak, et al. (retrieved September 2007)
examined over 45,000 paper medical files in order to create a protocol for automated
error detection and error prevention that could search medical files stored on computer
medical error were drawn from case files, and the patterns were then translated into
searchable terms. The intention was to create an automatic search mechanism that could
medical error.
Another project that marries case file data mining and automated searches of
databases is the domestic surveillance campaign carried out by the US National Security
Agency. While the project is secret, evading even Congressional oversight19, much of the
technology and methodology used is in the public domain (Department of Defense, 2005;
Arkin, 2007). The data-mining aspect of the domestic surveillance project does not
actually monitor the verbal content of telecommunications, but rather searches enormous
electronic databases for patterns that resemble the patterns of communications mined
from FBI and CIA files. These patterns may include a mapping of the communication
19
This section was written during the second term of US President GeorgeW. Bush, whose administration
authorized domestic surveillance.
116
that known-terrorist plotters used to coordinate past failed or completed attacks (Bulletin
project, tests social capital theory using secondary data available through the Education
Longitudinal Study (ELS) which gathers a wide range of educational data from all US
schools. Keshvala begins with a theoretical model and tests to see how well the theory
can explain the existing data. Keshvala uses structural equation modeling to build a
theory about the effect of social capital on children’s academic success. This project will
limit the theoretical analysis and an examination of the face value of the effect sizes
as a conceptual framework with which to understand the effects and etiology of bullying.
While Habermas does not use the terms ―power‖ or ―power imbalance,‖ his whole project
action. Bullying will be conceptualized according to this schema of social action. Within
the schema, bullying can be viewed as instrumental action that limits the autonomy of the
target (and the other children involved). Consensual activities such as those that occur in
free play, by contrast, fit the schema of communicative action i.e., the action of involved
No study has operationalized the concept of play deprivation; however, the fact
that bullied children have lower levels of play can be extrapolated from studies that show
children involved in bullying are more isolated socially and less active: at least while the
bullying itself is occurring the children involved are not playing. This deprivation of play
may explain some of the continuing and cumulative effects of bullying in that the
children experience less communicative action and more instrumental action. They may,
therefore, not be learning essential social skills that could enrich their lives.
that consists of a myriad of, often small, acts of solidarity, consensus-making, and
meaning creation. Limiting access to this domain of interaction may explain a large part
of the negative sequelae of bullying and other forms of victimization. In light of the fact
that there is no empirical literature showing the positive effects of communicative action
a systematic review of the bullying literature may be able to show the deleterious effects
corresponds with Dodge and Frame’s (1982) idea of cognitive distortion within the
internal representation of the social world. Distortion of cognition might explain some of
the effects of bullying by adversely affecting self-concepts, identity, mood and the
opportunity to learn and practice social skills. Several studies show that such variables as
self-esteem, identity and self-beliefs mediate the effects of bullying and other forms of
aggression (Burt et al., 2008; Schmidt & Bagwell, 2007). These mediating variables,
118
then, can be examined to see if they are consistent with Habermas’s theory. That theory
In this section, Habermas’s theory of communicative action will be set into its
historical and intellectual context, and will be fully elaborated and applied to the problem
of bullying.
The term ―critical theory‖ refers to a diverse body of social and political thought
that shares common interests; an interest in crossing intellectual and research disciplines,
and an interest in assuring that social science acts to emancipate rather than oppress
Critical theory began in the 1920s in Frankfurt, Germany, at the Institute for
Social Research, ―the Frankfurt School,‖ as the proponents of critical theory inside and
out of the institute came to be known. The Frankfurt School was funded by private capital
to encourage Marxist scholarship and to provide a venue for Marxist intellectuals who
had been denied a place in mainstream academia (Jay, 1973). The Frankfurt School is
associated with the University of Frankfurt which provides a professor to act as the
institute director and was itself funded by the City of Frankfurt so it could exist outside of
the conservative and, at the time, anti-Semitic, state university system (von Friedeburg,
2007).
In the years after the Russian Revolution, Germany lost its position as the home
of Marxist scholarship to the Soviet Union, although the political forces within the Soviet
the Soviet state. The Frankfurt School provided an opportunity for social scientists to
explore and expand Marxist theory (Jay, 1973) in a way that could not be done within the
Soviet Union. Another rationale for the creation of the Institute was to overcome the
120
the Frankfurt School sought to unify philosophy, political economy, social psychology,
context of its time: the Frankfurt School began at the same time as the National
Socialists, or Nazis, were organizing politically in Germany (Ritzer & Smart, 2001).
Many of the Institute’s members were Jewish and, despite the fact that it was still hard
for a Jewish academic to rise within the German university system and that Jews were
often denied public service positions, the Frankfurt School members felt that the
moderately socialist Weimar Republic in power at the time of the inception of the
Institute was proof that German anti-Semitism was coming to an end (Jay, 1973). Jay
points out that the Jewish Institute members had more experience of anti-Semitism, in the
form of social exclusion, while in exile in the US than they did in Weimar Germany.
Though the faculty of the Franklin School perceived that German anti-Semitism
was waning, the Nazi party, well before it took power in the early 1930s, had already
begun using systematic acts of terror against German-Jewish citizens and others they
identified as political opponents and potential political opponents; trade union leaders, for
example, were being beaten and murdered (Office of United States Chief of Counsel,
1946), Jewish academics were losing their positions, initially through a campaign of anti-
Semitism and, after the Nazis gained power, through the Nuremberg Laws of 1935 which
officially revoked the citizenship and civil rights of Jews and, subsequently, other groups
supposed coalition government, German gay, lesbian, and bisexual people began to be
sexual relations with Jews. These concentration camp inmates were routinely sterilized,
castrated, and killed (Kaplan, 1961). People with developmental disabilities were
sterilized as well, and, as the system of extermination camps developed, killed (United
After the Nazi takeover of Germany in 1933, the early critical theorists, many of
whom were Jewish and all of whom were progressive and committed to social justice,
had either personal experience of Nazi repression or had family and friends directly
affected. Most of the Frankfurt School faculty members emigrated from Germany to the
US during the Nazi period, although Walter Benjamin, a critical theorist with a wide-
ranging set of interests, took his own life in 1941 in Spain while trying to immigrate to
the US. The Frankfurt School itself went into exile in 1933, initially to Geneva,
Switzerland, and then to New York, where it continued to be very much concerned with
The experience of state repression continued to be the central focus and impetus
for critical theorists (Frankfurt School members and others) in the postwar period. The
Nuremberg trials in 1945 and 1946, conducted to adjudicate war crimes after the end of
hostilities, thoroughly documented the techniques used by the Nazis to gain and maintain
power, and exposed the extent of Nazi crimes and the bureaucratic nature of their
carefully planned campaign of terror, torture, and genocide (United States Office of Chief
122
communicative action will be elaborated below, had regrettably himself been a member
of the Hitler Youth and briefly served as a child soldier on the Western defenses at the
end of the war (Habermas, 1992). Habermas specifically dates his political awakening to
the immediate postwar period when he listened to the Nuremberg trials on the radio and
observed the reactions of denial and minimization among the adults in his environment
(Habermas, 1992).
Critical theorists Arendt (2006) and Adorno (1982) have addressed fascism
explicitly in their research into authoritarian tendencies, while Horkheimer (2003) and
Habermas (Borradori, 2003; Habermas, 1979, 1996) struggle with the processes of
preferring speculative theorizing and hermeneutic research (How, 2003). The reasons for
In the 1930s, certain Nazi academics had developed systems to identify ―non-
Aryan‖ traits based on the ―science‖ of eugenics, the study of animal husbandry applied
to human beings (Hawkins, 1997). Jewish and other peoples were classified by these
academics and the state as ―sub-humans‖ and were systematically tortured and murdered
as a ―solution‖ to a perceived social problem, that Germany’s economic and social ills
123
were caused by the presence of Jewish and other ―non-Aryan‖ influences. Critical
theorists shied away from empirical research for fear of falling into any classification
pitfalls that would reify their research subjects and cause similar harm.
After moving to the US, however, the Frankfurt School absorbed the American
bias in favor of empirical research and produced much original research using survey
techniques (Adorno et al., 1982; Jay, 1973). Nevertheless, one of the consequences of the
testing some of the basic assumptions of critical theory, including Habermas’s theory of
theory in order to ensure that social science plays no role in oppression. Intersubjectivity
assumes an equality of position among human beings who work together to create a
world of meaning. The idea of emancipation has been integrated into the definition of
critical theory in that it is constrained to reflect on the social world in ways that dissolve
addition to providing an explanation of the social world and a basis for predicting social
action, contains a mandate to promote social justice: in this way it shares a mandate with
social work.
and a prominent critical theorist, suggests that empirical research can remain consistent
with critical theory’s emancipatory mandate even when it undertakes traditional empirical
tasks such as verifying the validity of concepts and testing the salience of a theory in
attempts to dissolve what he sees as an unnecessary boundary between the qualitative and
traditional quantitative research. Habermas himself laments that his ideas have not been
academic disciplines (Horkheimer, 1982). The central objective of critical theory is the
reordering of the social sciences into a unified project that promotes emancipation by
making visible and understandable the life experiences that obstruct personal freedoms
Emancipation/Autonomy
The fascist and anti-Semitic agenda of the Nazi regime was predicated on the
classification of human beings into discrete groups have certain character traits. The
process of objectifying human beings and human activity, called ―reification‖ (Lukacs,
1971), is seen as a precursory mental process to oppression (Heath, 2001), both within
the social world and the social sciences. Marcuse (1974) defines reification as the
perception of human beings and their relationships as having the quality of an object or a
thing. The perception of a human being as a thing, or an other, who is not part of a valued
used to condone and exhort genocide in Nazi Germany (Goldhagen, 1996) and more
process by which people believe that certain attributes are ―rooted in the nature of‖
(Gelman, 2003, p,7), or an essential part of, the person, and that these essences explain
aggression. Internalized essentialist beliefs may also explain some of the ill effects of
bullying: the victim may believe that his role as a victim and the reasons that he was
chosen as a victim are essential to him and will not change over time. (As has been
development.)
Critical theory was an attempt to develop a social theory that avoided the pitfalls
oppressive processes and may, when internalized, be a key to the self-oppression that
occurs when people internalize colonialist and other oppressive thought forms.
The Oxford English Dictionary defines the term ―emancipation‖ as ―setting free
from social and political restrictions‖ (2002). Max Horkheimer, a key critical theorist,
defines ―emancipation‖ as the liberation of all human beings ―from the circumstances that
Habermas uses the term ―autonomy‖ in place of ―emancipation‖ to avoid the idea
20
The bibliographies of Gelman’s book and articles show no reference to critical theorists or Lukacs.
126
How (2003) defines autonomy as orienting oneself towards determining one’s own life,
McDermott, 1975).
the theory as we have adapted it, will have specific sequelae for the victim, as well as for
other bully-involved children. This writer’s project will examine the bullying literature
for the insight it offers into such components if Habermas’s critical theory.
discussed.
modernist promise of reason and science’s ability to ensure human freedoms no longer
which reason was to elucidate universal truths (Rasmussen, 2003) was replaced by the
idea that power relations, existing as structures within society capable of oppression,
were active in direct and indirect ways, guiding the course of science to the exclusion of
which extends the ecological model to include power structures in the social world that
approach. Moreau (1990) attributes power to an unspecified group of the ―rich and
127
textual analysis, hermeneutics, in particular, through which the discourses of power could
be understood in the lacunae, the gaps or missing portions, of text (Ritzer, 2000).
Through the work of Derrida specifically, the main character and plot lines of literature
can be seen as guided by the dominant ideology of the time which privileges some
characters and issues while rendering others invisible. By focusing off the center of the
text, called ―decentering,‖ one can consider the invisible or less visible characters. For
example, a decentering approach to the book Gone With the Wind might move the focus
onto the lives of the enslaved peoples depicted as minor characters in the original novel.
analyzing text for logical pitfalls, contradictions, and assumptions that may not be part of
the conscious writing process but are inherent in the dominant ideology of the writer in
Employing these literary techniques one considers that interwoven narratives also
construct the social world through discourse. Within US mass culture, the narratives of
the dominant culture are frequently privileged at the expense of the narratives of minority
popular US media.
128
constraint‖ (Foucault, 1980). Habermas attempts to elucidate the conditions and interests
that create the social world. Through the method he calls ―universal pragmatics‖ he looks
for the social processes and competencies that any social group uses to create meaning.
on building theory and his belief in the power of reason; he is, however, most directly
Habermas’s theory derives from the solution he proposes to the theoretical difficulties of
reconciling the actions of an individual actor with the action of the ―structure,‖ or
discussed in detail, proposes that structures or systems operate according to a different set
Despite the stated goal of critical theorists to integrate ideas from all the social
sciences, the profession of social work and its literature are not visible within the body of
critical theory. While social, developmental, and clinical psychology frequently inform
the work of critical theorists, as does the work of linguists, philosophers, and sociologists,
the social work profession is not presented or discussed by the major theorists writing in
or translated into English21. It is ironic that a body of work and theory that is predicated
on discovering and promoting invisible narratives has entirely ignored the narratives from
21
This writer’s thorough reading of critical theorists published in English and several theorists published in
French shows no references to social work theory or research.
129
social work history as the social work skill set is uniquely poised to actualize the mandate
Social work, among all the applied social sciences, has the practical skill base
with which to engage individuals, groups, and communities in reflection on the social
world. If social work has been invisible to critical theory, critical theory has become
visible to social work, particularly social workers in Canada, Australia, and Great Britain
who are embracing the tenets of critical theory and relating it to the profession. Within
the social work profession, Habermas’s theory of communicative action has been applied
to the roles and ideologies underlying the child welfare system (Blaug, 1995), as a way to
promote perspective-taking and diversity in social work education (Healy, 2001) and as a
tool of policy analysis (Reading, 1998). The theory, however, has not yet been applied to
The fact that Habermas’s theory has not been applied to child and family research
is surprising in light of the fact that Habermas himself stated over 30 years ago that:
familial environments are filtered and transmitted to the personality system (1974, p.
132)‖.
In this section, both Habermas’s early and later work will be described and
applied to the problem of bullying. This description will begin with a discussion about an
Habermas abandoned this project, many of its features remain extant within the
―universal pragmatics‖ will then be more fully described, and the final sections will
elaborate the theory of communicative action and synthesize the aspects of the theory that
Knowledge-Constitutive Interests
behavior, including conscious and less conscious processes, and suggested a direct link
between cognitive structures controlling behaviors and social skills, which he called
Swindal, 2003) within the individual and the social group which forms the basis of action
knowledge formation and are themselves the product of social interaction (Ottman,
1982). Ottman best captures the reciprocal nature of this concept of interests by
indicating that the Latin origin of the word ―interest‖ - inter esse -- actually translates as
of attraction between a person and a desired thing or state, or the advantages or benefits
that motivate human beings to form their ideas of the world based on those benefits that
accrue to them when they act on these ideas. This knowledge can be communicated
22
Habermas’s work resembles the extensive work of Foucault although their methods differ. Habermas
surveys theory and ideas, while Foucault surveys the history of institutions. See p. 66 of this document for a
further comparison.
131
between the environment --the lifeworld – and the personality structure itself in the form
While there is a myriad of human interests, Habermas distilled all interests into
human interests can be understood. These can be described more informally as the need
for food and shelter; the need for human companionship and communication; and the
Morals (1969), Habermas proposed that a balancing of the three universal interests would
decisions smoothly yield to the welfare of the whole‖ (Horkheimer, 1934, p.27).
Habermas continued this search for an ideal speech situation in his later theory of
communicative action.
paradigm, Habermas does not so much refute the teleological idea of action as much as
132
subsume it into a more complex theory that assumes a source of action not exclusively
coordination based on the need to create meaning and build solidarity even in the absence
of reward23.
and several preliminary and subsequent volumes and articles (Habermas, 1984, 1987,
2000, 2001a). In the following section, this theory will be more fully outlined with an
emphasis on those aspects that will be used to derive a theory that explains the effects of
bullying. Where possible Habermas’s own words will be used followed by an exegesis24.
In the section that follows the next one, these ideas will be directly applied to the topic of
bullying25.
23
Habermas’s theory would not be used to refute ideas of evolutionary biology but would classify these
ideas as part of the instrumentally motivated actions described in greater detail below.
24
Habermas has a dense, challenging writing style and presents the breadth of knowledge in his work. In
attempting to develop an interdisciplinary theory, he refers frequently to philosophers such as Kant,
Husserl, Hegel, and Nietzsche; sociologists and social theorists such as Marx, Parsons, Mead, and Weber;
linguists such as Searle and Chomsky, and psychologists such as Freud, Piaget, Kohlberg, and Selman. His
texts are almost completely theoretical and conceptual (Healy, 2000); however, he meticulously
summarizes each theory he uses being careful to make sure the reader understands the concepts and ideas
he is portraying.
These challenges can also be seen as strengths. Habermas rarely assumes a reader’s prior knowledge and
rewards the persevering reader with succinct summaries of the ideas of the other thinkers he discusses. His
lack of illustration allows the social work reader to apply his theory to the social work profession and such
social conditions as bullying and other forms of abuse.
25
This writer decided to separate the explanation of Habermas’s theory itself and the way that theory is
being applied to the problem of school bullying. This was done because the original theory is complex and
because the application of communicative action to the problem of bullying involves particular
interpretations of the meaning of the theory as well as the derivation of concepts not directly suggested by
Habermas.
133
processes rationally. He begins his two-volume work about his theory by explaining in
depth his views on rationality (Habermas, 1984). This extensive explanation cannot
adequately be represented here, but his project intends to increase our understanding of
For this writer’s project, rather than speaking of rationality, it is more appropriate
to use the term ―visibility‖ and to look at the ability of the theory of communicative
action to make human processes visible. The idea of visibility will be revisited when
speaking of the ability of a social theory to make human processes that are currently
obscure more visible. The conditions and processes that give rise to meaning and action
are the areas that the theory of communicative action tries to make rational, including the
―communication distorted by power‖ (1979) and aims to discover the tools with which
opposed to being imposed by a will, even the will of a majority (Habermas, 1984, 1987).
This dissolution of distortion relates to the Marxist idea of praxis, action taken that is
free of ―externally motivated behavior‖ (Jay, 1973, p. 4). This project will show how,
using Habermas’s ideas, some of the effects of bullying can be understood as arising
The core technique which Habermas uses, universal pragmatics, is described and
Universal Pragmatics
pragmatics,‖ or the search for the specific set of logically irreducible competencies and
conditions that make knowledge and social action possible--Habermas terms these the
refinement of concepts and processes that link language to the objective and subjective
reality. These rules apply equally to the verifiable nature of reality and to the subjective –
intersubjectively created – experience until they are conceptually irreducible and contain
anthropologists, and compare these to life processes (Habermas, 1984, 1987, 2000). In
his encyclopedic survey of ideas and theories, Habermas attempts to create a social
While he does not specifically describe his project as such, Habermas tries to
build a social theory that does not rely on metaphor but is actually based on a set of rules
and conditions through which actions arise and knowledge is built. This project is similar
to Marx and Engel’s concept of dialectical materialism (Engels, 1954), but with the
135
early Marxists. Habermas examines the conditions under which communication is both
effective – the ideal speech condition – and ineffective and misleading, and how action is
includes the ability to make one’s self as well as one’s idea understood and to negotiate
meaning with one’s hearers (D. M. Rasmussen & J. Swindal, 2002). Knowledge, then, is
intersubjectively created.
Habermas can be understood to mean that his theory can be used to understand
local human processes such as bullying when he says that ―the universal-pragmatic rule
system reveals the restrictions that the external reality of nature and society, on the one
hand, and the internal reality of the cognitive and motivational make-up of the human
organism, on the other, place on language (Habermas, 2001c, p. 78).‖ Habermas clearly
indicates that the theories derived from universal pragmatics will apply equally to the
society.
that represent distinctions in the real world. These distinctions have hitherto been largely
from instrumental speech/acts. Habermas uses the term ―speech/acts‖ to denote the
utterances and actions intended to convey meaning and induce or coordinate action.
Within the theory of communicative action, speech/acts are divided into two categories:
136
instrumental action and communicative action. The former includes speech/acts intended
to achieve a goal, and the latter includes speech/acts intended to achieve shared meaning
communication, which, for him, is a reciprocal process. According to Habermas, the term
speech/acts that limit, suppress, and pervert true communication and subtly or grossly
coerce others into taking actions that are often not in their own interest: as will be
makes visible the fact that instrumental speech/acts give rise to little or no actual
communication.
everyday acts of solidarity, friendship, and family life. As ordinary people engage in
daily conversation about life and the world, provide mutual support, and negotiate
and build the social world. This often happens through a consensual process that may not
consensus are Habermas’s ideal form of communication (Habermas, 1986), and they
necessarily require the recognition of different points of view, an essential condition for
socially just communication. This mutual recognition is central to the ideal form of
―perlocutionary‖:
action, Habermas indicates that the different forms of action coordination change the
state of the social environment. While Habermas himself does not elaborate on the
throughout his work. This idea of states will be an important key to understanding the
effects of bullying in that instrumental and communicative states have unique and
of the social world, creates a state of being ―instrumentalized.‖ An instrumental state can
Habermas acknowledges that people can be manipulated into believing they are acting
consensually but the instrumental effects remain latent in the speech/act (Habermas,
1984).
…there are countless cases of indirect understanding, where one subject gives
another something to understand through signals, indirectly gets him to form a
certain opinion or to adopt certain intentions by way of inferentially working up
perceptions of the situation; or where, on the basis of an already habitual
communicative practice of everyday life, one subject inconspicuously harnesses
another for his own purposes, that is, induces him to behave in a desired way by
manipulatively employing linguistic means and thereby instrumentalizes him for
his own success (Habermas, 1984, p. 288).
reputation, will create an instrumentalized state; involved children who may think they
are making an autonomous choice to stop being friends with the target are, in fact, having
their own autonomy curtailed by the bully’s speech/acts. Likewise, an overt bully may
coordinate action and acquire henchmen through instilling fear among his friends that
unless they take part in the bullying process they may become a target themselves.
Habermas does not imply that the distinctions between communicative and
instrumental action are always clear in the real world. In explicating his theory, he
assertion he fully supports; however, these distinctions may not always be self-evident.
While deception is a frequent occurrence and would explain how people can be
manipulated into taking actions that benefit others while weakening themselves,
Habermas goes to great lengths to explain that deception only works because there is a
139
rare enough in the social world that an unethical person can benefit from the expectation
of honest interaction.
but what comes to pass manifestly through outside influence or the use of violence cannot
actually serve to limit or entirely eliminate choice, or the autonomy of individuals and
participants harmonize their individual plans of action with one another and thus pursue
their illocutionary aims without reservation (Habermas, 1984, p. 294).‖ The term
―to share or join with,‖ whose literal root is ―within the walls‖ – con munis.
Communicative processes account for innumerable interactions that take place free of
support, solidarity, and repair, and occur in such institutions as child rearing and
friendship networks.
devalue all teleologically-induced, instrumental actions since some of these, like some
systems, are necessary tools for organizing complex tasks. Problems arise when
organization, or corporation which are at odds with the needs of the other members of the
community.
140
represents a paradigm shift similar to social work’s adoption of the strengths perspective
(Antonovsky; 1996; Saleebey, 1996) in which social workers are challenged to privilege
the strengths and resources of the individuals and communities with which they work and
distinction between the ―lifeworld‖ (Lebenswelt), the world of both organic life as well as
constructed meaning, from the steering mechanisms that operate within the lifeworld with
1984).
provides a tool that can be used to explain the ways that administrative and economic
systems work in favor of or against lifeworld needs for survival and well-being. The
lifeworld necessarily involves organic and psychological processes, while systems may
function in more linear and logical ways. Pathology arises when systems become
recognized‖ (Habermas, 1984, pp. 69-70). The lifeworld is the repository of culture and
translation of the German, which is monde vecu, or the ―world as it is lived‖ (Ferry,
1991). Lifeworld processes are often constrained by the organic nature of life, such as the
human need for survival, comfort, and social connectedness. The lifeworld operates
according to the various needs and interests of living organisms. For human beings, these
needs and interests include materials for survival and well-being, the creation of meaning,
and the maintenance of social relationships. For children and their development, these
The ―generalized competencies for action‖ to which Habermas refers have direct
relevance to this writer’s project on bullying and will be more fully elaborated below.
From a social work perspective, these social competencies have a direct effect on the
individual’s quality of life, as the ability to form and maintain relationships, even through
conflict, is essential to the maintenance of marriage, the social network, and career. By
―responsibility of persons‖ Habermas means the ability to understand the truth of the
other person: this ability to take the position of the other Habermas sees as the apotheosis
of social skills.
Systems are invented and implemented by human beings within the lifeworld in
where direct barter and subsistence farming is not possible. Monarchies, capitalism, and
justice and ensuring some form of distribution of goods using hereditary, military,
The political system produces mass loyalty in both a positive and a selective
manner: positively through the prospect of making good on social-welfare
programs, selectively through excluding themes and contributions from public
discussion. (Habermas, 1987, p. 346)
As stated previously, Habermas does not favor lifeworld over systems but sees
assumptions that occur when a system becomes uncoupled from the lifeworld, by which
Habermas means that the system is no longer responsive to or synchronized with the
This categorical distinction between lifeworld and systems solves the problem of
how and why individuals can act against their own interests and the interests of
humankind, the environment, and other parts of the lifeworld. Acting on ideology, people
are capable of undertaking roles that are not oriented to the needs of the lifeworld, rather
they can ally themselves with a system that has uncoupled from the lifeworld
(Brunkhorst, 1986). Actors taking a role within a system that is uncoupled from the
lifeworld, such as the Nazi system, will follow the system’s rules even though this may
Another example of this uncoupling process is the stripping away of local cultural
the detriment of the general population. Habermas also points out that when systems
143
(Habermas, 1987).
Habermas (1987) suggests, for example, that local systems of meaning are
stripped away when multinational corporate systems colonize the lifeworld with
political authority. This replacement of narratives results in the loss of local identities
and local wisdom that served the purpose of helping the particular social group negotiate
their environment, perhaps for centuries. Traditional stories that were meant to instruct
children may get forgotten or buried in the onslaught of television narratives that promote
the sale of superhero toys. The narratives that are prevalent on US television are ones
that at worst support aggressive social roles and at best portray successful aggressive
In the following section, the building block of the consensual process, which
fundamental unit which he calls the ―validity claim,‖ the implicit or explicit claim based
on the appeal to logic, justice, and sincerity. The validity claim is the method with which
some kind for some future action, if only to support the proposition. Agreement rests on
common convictions. The speech/act of one person succeeds only if the other accepts the
claim that is in principle able to be criticized. Both ego, who raises a validity claim, with
his utterance, and alter (this writer’s italics ), who recognizes or rejects it, base their
decisions on potential grounds or reasons (Habermas, 1984, p. 287). The validity claim is
simply says that when we communicate a proposition, we base our claim on appeals of
various types. Through pragmatic analysis, Habermas categorizes the basis of claims as
technical, appeals based on logic; normative, appeals based on the conventions of law
and general practice; and sincerity, appeals based on an emotional connection to the
speaker. In other words, we accept a proposition from another person because we accept
their logic, we accept that the proposition fits with normal practice, and/or because we
Habermas sees the validity claim as the fundamental unit of the creation of the
lifeworld and therefore, the development of language and culture. Conventions, such as
norms and languages, develop through the action of enormous numbers of validity claims
language in early human society, we can imagine a scenario in which a word becomes
associated with an object in the physical world. An early human may have made a
particular vocalization in reference to a particular edible plant, say a wild onion, in order
to share the nourishment with others. That vocalization amounted to a proposition for
others to associate that sound with that plant in the future. Such an event would be
considered a validity claim. If the person making the claim had credibility based on
expertise, perhaps as an adept provider or cook, it is more likely that the claim would be
accepted, and others would continue to use that sound to represent that plant.
As a group continues to use that sound for that plant, through successive waves of
validity claims, that sound becomes a word in a nascent language which is passed on to
the next generation or onto another group. As language develops, it is likely that
conventions also form, such as preferred cooking methods and foods that are pleasing
with wild onion. Conventions may occur as enterprising early humans found ways to
cultivate the onion, thus ensuring a steadier supply and contributing to a change from a
hunter-gathering society to an agricultural society. The names and uses of the onion come
have a multiplicity of possible meanings depending on the social context and the uses to
which knowledge is being put (Gottgtroy et al., 2003); however, the result of social
language occurs in a way that mixes consensual and instrumental processes. Religion, for
example, can instrumentalize eating habits with both proscriptions against eating certain
foods and the ritualization of eating others26; commodity fetishization, the accretion of
value, of a rare food product, such as black truffles, may make it financially out of reach
of most buyers, and thereby out of reach of most lifeworld members. In this way,
economic system.
Validity claims and other forms of action coordination are predicted to have
longer lasting effects within the lifeworld than action compelled through instrumental
action through an invisible, internal process Habermas calls ―illocutionary binding.‖ This
Illocutionary Binding
Validity claims that are accepted lead to further action through what Habermas
action operates through interest, communicative action operates through a set of cognitive
mechanisms that include conventions and commitments for future action. ―With the
illocutionary force of an utterance a speaker can motivate a hearer to accept the offer
contained in his speech/act and thereby to accede to a rationally motivated binding (or
26
These may have had roots in the lifeworld in that proscriptions against eating certain foods may have
arisen out of health concerns such as parasites and food spoilage.
147
developed consensually in innumerable acts of love and solidarity are more powerful
binding agents than those that arise from instrumental actions based on the desire to attain
a specific goal. While this idea appears utopian given the volume of violence and
oppression in human history, one needs to consider that innumerable acts of healing and
creativity repair human communities during and after times of oppression and should be
Acts of repair may be essential components of the natural world. Both string
theory and its more contemporary variant ―M-theory‖, in physics, contain a process by
which tears in space/time that occur in extremely minute spaces are easily repaired by a
symmetrical twin (Aspinwall et al., 1994). The complex mathematics of these theories is
well beyond the discipline of social work; nevertheless, if only by metaphor, we can
propose that repair processes are part of the fundamental nature of the universe.
Ecological and biological repair and support processes, such as the body’s innate healing
In the social world, Gottman and his colleagues, in a previously mentioned study,
marriage by the couple’s ability to generate five speech/acts of support to every one
critical or negative act(1994). While Gottman doesn’t reference Habermas, his research
years of coded speech/acts and whose conclusion shows how support and solidarity are
Social history, history from the perspective of ordinary people, looks at the
actions of numerous people making change over time. E.P. Thompson (1966), a founder
of the social history movement, examines labor history from the perspective of laborers
instead of the perspective of the political elite. Thompson finds that the labor movement
began with myriad acts of sacrifice and creativity by ordinary people. Those people’s
actions are not found in the ordinary historical record. Acts of solidarity and support
undergirded the effort to feed and clothe families during strikes and to provide emotional
and physical support in the face of violent counteractions. These acts, for Habermas, are
The binding process operates through each successful validity claim which results
in a commitment to take action. The ―speaker makes an offer that he is ready to make
good insofar as it is accepted by the hearer‖ (Habermas, 1979, p. 61). When acting on
coercive power, the action may be dependent on the proximal range of the authority, the
distance after which the authority is no longer enforceable; conversely, the actions based
on validity claim remain present in the lifeworld. Habermas, then, sees communicative
endure over time and may be generalized across different situations. It is through this
binding process that knowledge gets turned into action. The ideas of binding are implicit
in the work of attachment theorists who propose that much human suffering is caused by
problems in the development of nurturing relationships (Bowlby, 1999) and are also
consistent with Catalano’s related theory of social development (Catalano et al., 2004).
149
Habermas, in his one foray into intra-psychic processes, applies the theory of
communicative action (D.M. Rasmussen & J. Swindal, 2002). He shows how unequal
which vary in quality among differing cultures. The theme of conflicts which is
prominent in his analysis, Habermas says that ―the ego’s strength increases to the same
extent that the ego is able to do without such (repression) strategies and to process its
ability to express oneself and one’s ideas and to take in the other and her ideas in light of
―pseudoconsensus.‖
Habermas stresses that identity is the stable quality of the self. Identity is formed
within the constraints of the resources available in the lifeworld, as well as in the face of
stressful, contradictory role systems. Habermas lays out an argument for the ways that a
character, with its unique configuration of interests, abilities, limitations, and tastes, and
the child’s experience of both the lifeworld and the systems within it. The goal of
identity-building is to form a functional adult identity that can help the person
The ego-identity of the adult proves its worth in the ability to build up new
identities from shattered or superseded identities, and to integrate them with old
identities in such a way that the fabric of one's interactions is organized into the
unity of a life history that is both unmistakable and accountable. (Habermas,
1987, 98)
processes in which he sees the individual taking responsibility for the formation and
adaptation of her own identity. Identity is the essential condition for recognition, by
which he means the enduring condition in which the individual remains in a stable
adult forms the basis through which the person interacts with the world. This idealized
self-realized ego would be able to form lasting relationships precisely because it can be
process, comparing the formation of identity with the transformative nature of narrative
through which life events acquire unique overlays of meaning (Ferry, 1991). An
observable event may appear to have a singular meaning on a concrete level, but in
reality will have innumerable potential meanings when recounted in language. This idea
of identity being formed on the basis of repeated and enduring experiences will be more
fully explored in terms of the experience of bullying victimization and the ways this can
―communication‖ diverges from the ideal (Habermas, 1987; Honneth, 1992). Habermas
theorizes that healthy identity is best formed under ideal speech conditions that, for
children, include free play as well as other autonomous interactions with peers and adults.
communicative action) provoke action orientations; they are aimed at filling in the spaces
communication, children can explore their interests, proclivities, tastes, skills, and
weaknesses within their own cultural and sub-cultural contexts contributing reciprocally
There is a very strong association between behavior and social skills, Habermas
The idea that communication needs to be maintained even through conflict is key
to Habermas’s view of this link between behavior and social competence. He privileges
this process of reaching mutual understanding and makes it the keystone of his project of
discourse ethics in which he proposes that the constitutional state be built based upon a
consensus among all groups on essential issues. Consensus can only be arrived at if and
when all involved parties are able to hear each other even when their positions are in
competence that should take place in societies that have a commitment to equality for its
citizens28. At the top of the set of communicative competence is the ability to take the
perspective of the other person. This is Habermas’s key argument against the cultural
27
The term Aufbau also translates as ―development‖ which may be more consistent with Habermas’s own
view of the process of acquiring communicative competence.
28
Habermas argues that societies that do not have universally accepted conventions for fundamental human
rights social competencies that lead to universal participation may be obstructed or suppressed. To test this
assumption would require an extensive ethnographic and anthropological study and is not germane to the
topic of bullying.
153
will develop the advanced competencies that will allow leadership by consensus in the
absence of systemic constraints against taking the perspective of others into account. It is
this constraint against taking into account the perspective of others that is at the heart of
oppressive regimes such as Nazi Germany: genocide would not take place if all people,
including the oppressed group, were allowed to take part in the decisions that affected
with the ability to take the perspective of the other person as the highest level of
competence. While intelligence and goal-oriented behavior are keys to academic and
vocational achievement, social skills are the key to a successful social life for all
members of society. The ability to form and maintain relationships vastly improves the
quantity and quality of an individual’s life experience entirely apart from career,
education, and intelligence and will apply equally to people with significant
Instrumental experiences that are decoupled from the lifeworld, which includes
victimization and abuse, can distort identity and give rise to pathologies:
In the following section the above theory will be applied and adapted to explain
action and the topic of school bullying will be explored. The philosopher’s concepts will
be applied directly to bullying and new concepts will be derived following through on the
logic inherent in his social theory. An important caveat must be added here in that what is
presented here is this writer’s interpretation of Habermas’s concept. This writer was
unable to communicate with Professor Habermas and is not educated in either of the two
disciplines associated with the theory of communicative action: philosophy and sociology
There has been no research in the English and French databases and search
Germany (Brunkhorst, 2003 personal communication),but their results are not yet
will be explored conceptually and the related to the results of studies in this systematic
review.
With the exception of his essay on family communication (D.M. Rasmussen & J.
Swindal, 2002), Habermas applies his theory to the building of a just state and his
research involves constitutional law and statecraft more than individual psychology. In
this section, the connections between Habermas’s macro level theory and the topic of
The starting point for this collection of typologies is Habermas’s own (1987)
descriptive typology of social action (Figure 3), which this writer will then adapt to
Social Action
Communicative Instrumental
Action Action
Conscious
Deception
Unconscious
Deception
In exploring Habermas’s theory of communicative action for the first time, this
writer had a sense that it could be applied directly to explain the breadth of the bullying
phenomenon, including all or most of its effects. Habermas’s ideas about the nature and
context of communication seemed to hold promise in explaining how the abuse of power
in bullying would diminish a target’s social competence and distort her cognition. In this
section, this theory will be explored for both its direct application and for the derivation
of concepts that can explain the effects of bullying. Where possible, these ideas will be
the use of latent variables, variables based on theoretical concepts, such as illocutionary
binding, that are not visible or directly measurable (Borsboom, Mellenbergh, & van
156
Heerden, 2003; Lazarsfeld, 1937; Lazarsfeld & Henry, 1968; J. C. McKinney, 1966; J.C.
McKinney & Kerckhoff, 1962). The concept of invisibility is not monolithic. Human
processes are invisible for a number of reasons. Some invisible human processes, like
other processes in the physical world, can only be perceived through their traces; others
states, for example, had been seen as invisible to researchers until Likert (1932)
developed a method to measure Freudian concepts. Likert makes subjective states visible
inaccessible child is not able to represent herself and her anxiety and depression, so these
invisible states would need to be discerned from their visible traces: the child may stop
having fun, have somatic complaints, or wake up with nightmares. For these research
techniques that involve observable aspects of hidden phenomenon, I have borrowed the
term ―perturbative‖ from physics because it represents a term that can be applied to social
sciences. Using a particle accelerator, physicists cause a reaction among particles, and try
to learn about invisible particles through the effects of the collision on more visible
particles. Social scientists are ethically constrained not to cause harm by evoking
researchers can be said to be examining in their subjects the perturbation that the bullying
_______________________________________________________________________
Table 4: Levels of Visibility of Human Processes
Time and space are important considerations in a theory of the social world and
are often neglected by those building a theory (George & Gareth, 2000). Temporal
considerations are especially relevant to the study of children considering the changes
that occur as they develop physically, mentally, and socially over a relatively short period
or endurance over time; it also has a strong spatial effect in that it shrinks the space in
which a child feels secure (Andrews & Chen, 2006) and in which she is able to
autonomously develop. The effects of bullying are also temporal, ranging from the
affective processes that produce long-lasting effects on her psyche and social status.
Temporal and spatial elements will be considered in applying and adapting the theory of
exceptions, these typologies are used to describe phenomena rather than explicate theory
(Habermas, 1984, 1987). In this section, the theory will be applied to the phenomenon of
bullying. Examples from the bullying literature and other forms of power abuse have
been added to each box to illustrate their fit with Habermas’s theory. These connections
will also be outlined in the following paragraphs as will a set of derived concepts with
which to build a theoretical understanding of the mechanisms involved when children are
bullied.
examples from the bullying literature. As has been discussed above, Habermas divides
instrumental action into direct and indirect actions: actions taken by an actor directly
toward another actor (or actors) and actions taken through intermediaries, respectively.
Direct instrumental action includes a set of speech/acts that are intended to affect another
person directly, including commands and incentives on one end of the spectrum of
coercion to threats and violence at the other end. Strategic instrumental action includes a
set of speech/acts that, when used covertly, are carried out by other people without the
knowledge of the target; for example, in relational bullying, the spreading of rumors to
159
diminish a child’s social network is both strategic and concealed: the victim may never
Actions that are both open and strategic are not concealed. Neutral forms of open
strategic activities include theatre, pageantry and religious or spiritual ritual. Some forms
of bullying share features with both theater and ritual in that they take place in front of an
considers legal and military procedures to be, at times, open, insofar as the rules of the
interaction are predetermined, and strategic insofar as the framers of the law or policy are
not present.
Conscious Deception-Pseudo-
Communicative Action (mimicking
communicative action by
manipulating others into excluding
others)
Unconscious Deception
(this results from cognitive
distortions/deficits)
160
In this section, the mechanisms of effect of bullying will be examined. First will
responsible for a variety of ill-effects. When other sources of stress and trauma in the
lives of bullying targets are controlled for, bullying still appears to be a potent force;
however, many bully-involved children have likely experienced other forms of trauma
and victimization.
Each of the following subsections ends with a list of specific effects that would be
Being bullied shrinks the space that is safe for the victimized child as well as
many of her peers (Andrews & Chen, 2006; Percy-Smith & Matthews, 2001): it changes
the child’s lifeworld, using Habermas’s term. Bullying may alter the way the child
experiences the bus to school, its hallways, bathrooms, locker rooms, and classrooms.
The geography of childhood is relatively circumscribed; in most cases, the young child’s
world consists of her home, a small part of the social space of her community, such as its
parks and stores, the homes of a small number of relatives and friends, and the school.
Bullying has a spatial effect within the lifeworld, further limiting the already
narrow space in which the target child lives. The target experiences a narrowing of the
safe space in her world. Depending on the nature of the child, this will have an immediate
effect on her physical organism: she may become hyper-aware of her surroundings and
161
will likely be scared in those unsafe environments; this fear and arousal may make it hard
for her to relax and enjoy other experiences. Being continually or repeatedly afraid, she
may engage in less social and physical activity and may experience a diminishment of
Some children may experience their entry into the victim role as a trauma and
experiencing the traumatic event, nightmares, and recurring physical effects, such as
increased heart rate. Bullying, however, differs from trauma in that it is by definition an
ongoing, repetitive and enduring condition. Bullying then may have two distinct
but the majority of children will be affected through a slower, more cumulative process.
Enduring bullying experiences in the environment may start to create ongoing and
enduring problems some of which may continue even if the bullying ceases. Finkelhor
A child who is experiencing anxiety and fear on an ongoing basis may start
thinking and feeling differently about her world than she did previously and compared to
non-bullied peers: these effects can be described under the rubric of cognitive and
affective distortion. For this project, these internal processes can be measured and can be
classified as temporal because due to the ongoing and cumulative nature of bullying over
time. These include temporal cognitive and affective processes, such as negative self-
The physical symptoms associated with fear and anxiety may start to turn into
physical complaints, and a child may have more excused and unexcused absences from
school. Within the empirical literature on bullying, one would expect to see evidence of..:
somatic symptoms
generalized anxiety (as opposed to fear of the school environment only)
depression
low self-worth, low self-esteem, high self-derogation
externalization, such as aggression or conduct difficulties
absenteeism from school
lowering of academic performance
continued and worsening patterns of social isolation and/or withdrawal
Some of the mechanisms involved when children are affected by bullying are not
as easily explained as the above variables. The mechanisms through which bullying
impede the acquisition of a full set of social competencies are not immediately apparent
in existing theory. In the next section, Habermas’s theory of communicative action will
victimization and both mood and cognition. Repeated and enduring stressful and
humiliating experiences will distort a child’s thinking and feeling, but a large part of the
effects of bullying victimization derives from the target’s lack of supportive and anodyne
experiences available within a rich lifeworld. The subtle and less visible mechanisms
through which the experiences of bullying victimization undermine healthy processes can
lessening of autonomy; distortion of identity; and the distortion of cognition based on the
experience of bullying.
While adapting Habermas’s ideas for use in social work with children and
schools, it becomes clear that children overall experience a great many instrumental
children have to walk, talk, and sit when told by adults, are put into lines and ranks, and
have their behavior monitored in almost all settings. This institutional arrangement
narrows the social space available for autonomous states such as free play.
largely ineffective. By ineffective, it’s meant that children involved in bullying are not
learning effective skills of instrumental action through the bullying in which power is
exercised in ways that positively affect the lifeworld; rather, they are learning skills that
are consistent with adult abuses of power, including coercion of innocents and
164
humiliation in its various forms. While the effects of bullying on the bullies and
bystanders are outside the purview of this project it is clear that these non-victimized
children also experience an array of ill-effects. It is not effective for any children
involved.
the Hitler Youth movement, the society deems it desirable to reproduce brutal and
ruthless adults, as Kamenetsky (1996) asserts was done by the National Socialists. In the
case of the educational policy aimed at the Hitler Youth movement, the reproduction of
Bullying and communicative action are mutually exclusive within a single social
interchange. In general, children have relatively few opportunities to shape their own
social environment. In school, this can take place most often only during recess and lunch
breaks. When bullying, as instrumental action, enters the play environment at school, the
day.29 hildren who have restrictive home and community environments may have no
Many of the effects of bullying can be understood to arise from the breach of
support and learning. This includes the deprivation of play which can be understood as
the crucible in which children learn and practice the widest range of social skills.
29
Actions that appear to have the features of bullying, such as rough-and-tumble play and face-maintaining
teasing, are still part of communicative action and may help children to develop successful strategies for
dealing with bullies.
165
supportive words and gestures to sustain a marriage; from this can be extrapolated the
As was touched upon in the review of the theoretical literature, Children’s play
and communicative action share the common feature of speech/acts with no stated or
among adults to be an end state after childhood (D.M. Rasmussen & J. Swindal, 2002),
and it can be taken as given that these skills are practiced and learned in childhood. What
5 includes the nature and features of both. The first box shows the validity claim. A
167
validity claim can be understood as any attempt to convince others to act in a particular
Children try to influence their social world in myriad ways, but only among their
peers are the power dynamics sufficiently level to allow for mutual influence. Within
imaginative play, children will recreate adult roles. They learn adult skills through this
play, including convincing others to act in a certain way. When children make
suggestions and explain the logic, albeit in a childlike manner, of their suggestions these
meet the criterion for validity claims. A research agenda directly based on Habermas’s
ideas could shed light on the links between children’s pretend play and the development
of communicative competence.
The nature of the validity claim (Box 2) is that it allows a hearer the freedom to
simply ignoring the original actor. Children’s responses are limited only by their
developmental stage and limited life experience; however, their imaginations may make
The next step (Box 3) in the schema of social action is the development of
meaning and norms that lead to future action. Habermas’s idea of a consensual speech/act
is a speech/ act jointly formed and mutually agreed upon; it will likely have a lasting
effect, possibly enduring in some form across multiple generations. This is true in the
an imperative, unlike a validity claim, is finite. The intention of the action may vary
―Distribution of resources‖ is the term being used here as it can encompass such abstract
commodities as recognition and status, as well as concrete resources, such as money and
food. Roles and identities adhere to differing forms of instrumental action (Brunkhorst,
1986); the roles of bully and victim are those under discussion in the current project.
The final box on the right side (Box 8) shows the range of responses to an
meet the needs for which it was originally formed. Boxes 4 and 8 show how social action
tandem.
In the following section, these ideas will be applied to the mechanisms of effect
type of effect and ends with a list of specific and measurable effects as well as a concept
map of the mechanism. The separate mechanisms will be related in a complete model in
Spatial Effects
The initial adverse effects of bullying are related to the shrinking of the physical
and social space of the target. A child who is being bullied will likely react with fear and
anxiety that is specific to the locale of the bullying. She may also, sometimes for the first
time, begin to fear people if only the bully and those children that collude with the
bullying. For the target who is bullied relationally, her social life may be suddenly
changed, and she may experience the loss of some or most of her companions. This
experience may be sufficient to catalyze a depressive episode, even a suicide attempt, and
may spark the development of an anxiety disorder that generalizes beyond the school
environment. The targeted child will almost certainly stop enjoying at least part of her
school experience and may begin to be less attentive in class: it is likely, however, that if
the bullying is stopped early enough, the contemporaneous effects will clear up quickly.
As has been discussed above, victimization may begin at any age throughout
childhood, so a target child who has experienced bullying before may develop more
resilience resources over time; however, a teenager, for whom peer relationships are of
primary importance, may experience the destruction of her rich social life as a
cataclysmic loss.
The expected contemporaneous effects of being bullied on the victim would be:
anxiety related to the specific locations of the bullying (school, bus, bathroom,
etc...)
physical symptoms related to stress, including somatic complaints
symptoms of post-traumatic stress
social withdrawal or isolation
Figure 6 below shows a concept map of the contemporaneous effects of being bullied.
170
The term ―breach effects‖ has been chosen to describe the set of bullying effects
inherent in communicative contexts. These breach effects include lack of opportunity for
solidarity and support experiences that that entails. Such continuous deprivation
identity and the acquisition of a wide range of social skills. Habermas stresses the role of
171
social skills in the creation of a just state; and social skills are also seen as the key to a
rich and satisfying life for the individual. The primacy of social competence in
the ways that bullying affects children’s development by depriving children of a major
communicative contexts the target experiences. The bullying affects the child’s social
world in two ways: it usually isolates the child from her peers through a systematic
undermining of the social network, and it may also change the quality of peer interactions
over time. As a result, the child will not learn the social competencies required to take
part in adult consensual processes, will not be able to take in the perspective of the other,
and will not develop an identity that fits with her individual nature. Instrumental contexts
also shape the child’s identity through the use of power resulting in an inauthentic,
Not only can bullying change the cognitive and affective experience, it can
change the actual and objectively quantifiable experience of the child. The child forms a
world view based on the quality of her personal experiences. Where Burger and Luckman
(1966) propose that individuals can construct their own social world, Kokko’s idea of
states can determine the actual quantity and quality of those experiences. Two children in
the same social environment, then, can have two diametrically opposed experiences of
their own childhoods and form different identities and worldviews based on those
different experiences.
172
The damage bullying causes may be predicated on the degree to which the
which is the corollary to the deprivation of communicative social contexts – will have
A child with other social venues outside of school in which to experience consensual
interactions may well be less affected by the bullying than the child who experiences
bullying studies, showed that victimized children did tend to have more authoritarian
parenting, but that variable has not been studied in more recent effects studies.
Controlling for these multiple contexts will be important in future research to determine
Erikson (1974) and, later, Ferry (1991) points out, identity formation is a dialectical
process requiring the freedom to test out one’s own preferences and predilections within
a complex social life. In instrumental contexts, the child does not explore a wide set of
preferences and experiences, and her identity is less likely to be adaptive to the
potentially ambiguous and conflictual aspects of the adult social world. Problems of
identity formation, as with early onset depression, are co-morbid with adult psychiatric
next section, identity can be adversely affected by bullying as well as directly affected.
Based on Habermas’s theory, one would predict that the bonding that arises from
bullying would be less effective than that arising from consensual communicative
173
contexts. Attachments in school and with peers may be measurably weaker and would
result in less deeply felt commitments to the school and peer group.
Within the empirical literature on bullying, one would expect to see evidence of:
Direct effects are those effects that directly confirm Habermas’s theory of
Habermas’s ideas within this systematic review as none of the studies explicitly base
their research on his theory. These direct effects include the verification of his concepts
such as illocutionary bonding and the acts of repair and solidarity that he asserts make up
communicative action. Direct effects also include the continuing exposure to maladaptive
instrumental social skills which may be directly limiting and distorting. The most
significant source of distortion most likely comes from the role and label victim itself.
The child begins to think of herself in light of that limited role and well-meaning anti-
bullying strategies may perpetuate this label by identifying children as victims and
In children’s peer relationships, power may have direct effects inherent in its
nature. The children obeying a more powerful child are not exercising their own interests.
A child habitually bullied will have fewer chances to take leadership roles and practice a
variety of social skills. While there has been no published study quantifying the amount
of play engaged in by bullies and targets, it can be assumed that both the bully and victim
roles take away from the time available for free play. If bullies and targets are playing
less, then they may acquire fewer of the benefits of play, such as rehearsal and skills
addition to the research into children’s play to characterize the dynamic flow of power
relations within free play situations, coding the speech/acts as either instrumental or
Instrumental states are not negative in themselves; they are necessary elements of
a complex social world. The instrumental actions that constitute bullying, however,
involve coercion, manipulation, and humiliation. These behaviors are not adaptive to
175
success in the lifeworld and do not prepare children for a sufficiently rich variety of adult
social roles. Neither the bully nor the target learns effective instrumental leadership
strategies through the process of bullying. Bullying, then, is a distorting mechanism that
propels children away from consensus and toward coercion. Bully-involved children are
less likely to learn effective instrumental techniques, such as allocating tasks and
resources to optimum effect and issuing imperatives in respectful and logical ways.
Children involved with younger siblings and schoolmates have ample opportunity to use
effective instrumental techniques. As has been discussed above, children often act to keep
other children safe from cars, strangers, and potentially dangerous adults.
repetitive scenarios; communicative action, on the other hand, maximizes choice. This is
an important distinction in that there are many circumstances where choice needs to be
limited and others where choice is essential. In complex and dense social environments,
such as schools, it is necessary to use power in ways that allow smooth flow of activities
that maximizes safety. These are inherent, as Noguera (1995) points out, in the power
primarily to reach a specific goal, usually involving the apportioning of finite resources,
where those resources are social or material. The person in power assigns roles and
distributes resources. This is as true for complex social organizations as it is for children
need to rehearse roles that they see around them and that children exposed to a surfeit of
power within their environment might try to exercise power in their own social contexts.
176
However, for power to be exercised wisely, the person in power needs to have a great
deal of information and skill both to determine the circumstances under which power
should be exercised and the appropriate ways to apportion resources. A child who bullies
For many children, the victim role is stable over time. While some children have
short or multiple episodes of being bullied, most children in victim roles, particularly
those who have internalized that role, continue to accumulate victim experiences even in
discrete and unrelated social environments. A child may reflexively repeat behaviors and
signals that trigger bullies and may not have the social skills to join cohorts of children
Figure 8 below depicts a model of the active effects of bullying within the theory
experiences including iterations of negative thoughts and moods; the second box
situations leading to distortions in social bonds. The third box describes the
children) of bullying across social and geographic contexts. The result of this
The physical symptoms associated with fear and anxiety may start to turn into
physical complaints and a child may have more excused and unexcused absences from
school. Within the empirical literature on bullying one would expect to see evidence of:
somatic symptoms
generalized anxiety (as opposed to fear of just the school environment)
depression
low self-worth, low self-esteem, high self-derogation
externalization such as agg
or school social work, with overt acts of violence that invoke disciplinary and juvenile
justice intervention absorbing a great deal of school social workers’ limited resources
Bullying prevention has not been part of school social works’ mandate because
prevention has not been universally accepted as consistent with the traditional mandate of
school social work of intervening only where an issue directly affects academic
functioning (Miller et al., 2003). Evidence is emerging, however, that bullying does in
fact depress academic functioning both for individual children as well as classroom
178
groups (S. W. Twemlow, 2004; Wentzel & Asher., 1995; Woods & Wolke, 2004);
therefore, intervening in bullying should fall within the traditional school social work
A strong case, based on the academic and personal effects of bullying, can be
made for an expanded mandate for clinical professions to provide direct services to
children and their families within schools affected by bullying, including prevention and
primary treatment (Adelman & Taylor, 1998; Frey et al., 2005; SAMSHA, 2003). Such a
mandate, however, will require funding for social workers and increased faculty time to
Mishna (2003) asserts that social workers are in the best position to provide
counseling, consultation, and training for school personnel to deal specifically with
school bullying. The social work profession is already established within the host setting
of the schools and has a set of conceptual skills with which to analyze multiple social
contexts and a set of practical skills with which to intervene to improve the school
environment.
justice. Intervening against bullying is consistent with the mandate of critical social work.
Critical social work (Fook, 1993; Healy; Ife, 1997) is a dominant paradigm in this
writer’s native country Canada and shares with Critical Theory an emancipatory mandate
that requires taking direct action against oppressive forces and ameliorating the effects of
Related Issues
malignant phenomenon; however, there is some evidence that some children who make
the transition away from the victim role, called ―escaped victims‖, find that the bullying
al., 2004). Bullying experiences during childhood may lead to the social reproduction of
adult roles of submissiveness and domination (Dixon et al., 2004; A. Sutton et al., 1999)
social control. One study (S. Brown & Taylor, 2008) shows that adults who were
victimized as children are employed in subservient roles more frequently than those who
bullied or those who were neither bullies nor victims. Percy-Smith makes the point that
systematic abuses of power are endemic to capitalist societies which require people to
Kogan and Chandan (2004) in their qualitative research into school violence
found a small number of teachers and parents who expressed opinions in favor of
bullying. Most of this small group felt that bullying was an important part of childhood
and helped the children become tougher and more prepared for an adult world. One father
threatened to hit his child himself if he did not learn to stand up to his tormentor.
180
No research studies have yet been published that examine the relationship
between bullying and adult violence and aggression30; however, the example of Nazi
tyranny may extend this notion into the reproduction of coercive and oppressive adult
roles. Kamenetsky (1996) shows how bullying was promoted and taught as a way to
reproduce adult roles of domination and even cruelty. Bullying among adults in the
workplace is another emerging topic of research (Cowie et al., 2002; Hoel et al., 1999; P.
K. Smith, 1997). However, it is not yet clear that workplace and school bullying are
related. Longitudinal studies begun in Scandinavia in the 80s and 90s are just beginning
and victimization and their research cohort is just entering the labor market.
competence is a diffuse concept that comprises a wide range of behaviors. Segrin (2000),
in an attempt to capture the common feature of all social skills, defines social competence
as the ability to interact in a way that is appropriate and effective. Within the empirical
literature social competence – or social skills- are measured in several ways including the
others.
30
There has been a longitudinal study that has followed Norwegian children into young adulthood.
181
Despite the fact that bullying has been the subject of research for 30 years,
bullying researchers have not yet coordinated definitions or methodology. To date, there
conventions that would allow studies to build logically on one another. The result of this
lack of coordinated activity is a very confusing picture of bullying and its effects.
With governments and states implementing policy and laws against bullying
(Limber & Small, 2003) intervention programs are proliferating (Limper, 2000; McGrath,
2007; Scaglione & Scaglione, 2006); however, there is scant evidence for the
Smith, D. J. Pepler, et al., 2004; Stevens et al., 2000). Three meta-analytic studies
concluded that there is no evidence to show that any bullying intervention program has
been effective and there is even some evidence that certain interventions may actually
increase bullying (D.J. Pepler et al., 1994; Vreeman & Carroll, 2007). By contrast, there
is evidence that violence, as disaggregated from bullying, can be both prevented and
reduced (Mytton et al., 2006): this leads to the conclusion that bullying is an intransigent
Dubin (1978) suggests that interventions in the applied sciences are unlikely to
phenomenon. This appears to be the case with bullying. Many programs are created with
little or no reference to any supporting scientific literature (Stassen Berger, 2007). None
of the theoretical models currently used in the bullying literature explicitly consider the
dynamics and effects of power abuse; yet, power abuse is the single variable that
182
distinguishes bullying from other forms of aggression. Imbalance of power clearly needs
to be a significant feature of a theory that explains the bullying phenomenon and form the
In this section, the systemic review will be discussed including a discussion of the
data collection and data analysis methods employed. The results will be reviewed and
DATA COLLECTION
This section will elaborate the tools employed for gathering data and how these
data will advance the understanding of the effects of school bullying. The systematic
review of bullying will be a process of isolating the variables used in the existing
literature as well as the results found. Data were gathered on the measures and results on
the selected literature and as well as on the claims and theoretical assumptions being
Inclusion Criteria
studies and articles in the bullying literature. This is the American sub-set of the literature
reviewed above and includes articles from US databases. Specific information on the
databases used are identified in Appendix I. All studies that met the requirement were
The decision to include longitudinal and cross-sectional studies was made based
on convenience in that there would not be enough studies in this review if one or the
other sets were eliminated. As well, both types of studies will capture a somewhat
different peer context: longitudinal studies will capture more of the effects of longer-term
bullying. Since the goal of the study is to determine the range and type of effects the
contextual factors are less important. Where possible, the time comparisons will be
limited to one year or less in longitudinal studies due to the fact that children who are no
longer being victimized will begin forgetting their experience (this is the same reason that
Exclusion Criteria
A number of studies were excluded because they lacked data that would make
it possible to convert to a standard effect size. In one case the means were
presented in graphic form without either the SDs, standard deviations, or the
effects of bullying were excluded because they did not provide correlation data. In
studies that present regression or path analyses the correlations could not be
extrapolated from the data presented. Several studies reported the results of latent
class analyses or structural equation modeling; such studies were included as long
as the correlation data were also included (authors of studies that lacked these
data were contacted but were not able to provide the correlation data).
specifically for this project. Textual data on the theory used in the study were tabulated in
a word processor as, likely due to the limited space in journals, very few studies related
their results to a comprehensive social theory. The database used to collect data will be
discussed below.
Quantitative Database
the theories informing the studies as described in Table 2 (where these are made
explicit)
the actual measures used
186
The project has been subject to review by the Hunter College Institutional Review
Board and data was gathered under the protocol #HC-11091453. The project did not
gather data directly from human subjects but from secondary sources and is considered to
subjects.
concerns due to their lack of identifying information secure storage is not an issue. No
data containing identifying information, such as survey data with identifying material,
Since the unit of observation for this meta-analysis is the individual study, issues
of reliability and validity have been dealt with by each set of authors. Reliability is the
(Yang, 2002). In several studies existing measures were used whose validity and
187
reliability of the instrument had already been established. In other cases the authors of the
selected studies borrowed and adapted items from existing measures. In only a few cases
did the authors develop original measures for their study; and in these cases the items
were not substantially different than those items from established studies.
DATA ANALYSIS
A single question is being asked of each level of data: what are the proposed or
implied mechanisms that link the actions of bullies to the effects on bullying? This
question is asked at different levels of conceptualization from the level of theoretical and
conceptual analysis to the empirical level at which research has been performed.
with the analysis of the theoretical data, the qualitative data and the theory of
communicative action.
The purpose of any analysis of multiple studies, as Yang (2002) points out, " is to
synthesize and organize the existing empirical findings into a coherent pattern…and to
seek general conclusions across studies" (p. 297). This review takes all the measurements
and attempts to find the commonalities and differences in order to begin to build a model
that can explain the effects of victimization as they pertain to the dynamics and nature of
bullying.
To accomplish this using existing literature as a starting point the first order of
business is to bring some clarity to the many disparate concepts used in the selected
review. Some concepts are similar. While the statistical possibilities of this review are
limited, using the typological method (Hegar & Yungman, 1989; Lazarsfeld, 1937;
188
Lehnert, 2007; J.C. McKinney & Kerckhoff, 1962), it is possible to determine the
consistency of concepts, variables and propositions inherent in the empirical studies with
the theoretical framework and to begin the process of determining a coherent model of
The different concepts used for the variables measured were classified and
categorized according to their qualities. The variables were classified was rated by four
independent raters to establish consistency and reliability. Each rater was given a list of
all the variables as they were termed by the authors themselves and were asked to put
them into logical categories. The classified variables were be turned into typological
categories based on shared features and, where feasible, consistency with the
communicative conceptual frame. For example, all the measures of self-esteem, self-
category and all mood related process were put into an affective category.
Temporal considerations were added since none of the raters used spatial or
temporal considerations in their ratings. As will be seen below, fears and avoidant
behaviors that were specifically related to school were put into a spatial category and
repetitive cognitive and effective processes were put into temporal categories. These
considerations were considered to have face validity and not be open to interpretations.
As was discussed in the analysis section, a plan had been put in place to use
qualitative methods and a qualitative software program to gather, code and analyze a
great deal of textual content to determine stated and implicit theory. In reality the selected
189
studies were very thin on theory to explain bullying and negligible in explaining the
effects of bullying and could be collected almost verbatim in a few pages of prose.
In the following section the choice and rationale for the use of the vote-counting
The statistical technique that will be used in this systematic review is an informal
the rationale for that choice will be re-iterated, other statistical techniques that were
Due to the variation in measures, also called the ―shared method variance‖
(Hawker & Boulton, 2000; Haynes & Hayes O'Brien, 2000) used for both victimization
and all the effects outcomes several statistical methods were eliminated. For example,
such statistical analyses as measuring the homogeneity of results, the degree to which the
results of separate studies of the same phenomenon appear to be consistent, and changing
the power of the result as according to the cumulative sample (Glass, McGaw, & Smith,
1981) are not feasible due to the lack of homogeneity of method and measures. In this
systematic review that cumulative sample size is over 90,000 so that, if traditional meta-
analytic techniques were possible, the statistical power would be increased exponentially.
Typically this kind of meta-analysis is done on studies that were conducted using the
same measures and under similar conditions making the results more easily compared.
The results of the separate studies selected for review are presented in different
effect size metrics. An effect size metric is simply the choice of a statistical metric that is
used to compare the degree and strength of an association between two variables. The
190
selected studies depicted their results using different effect size metrics, the most
common being the Pearson’s r. Other studies compared the means between comparison
groups, used chi square comparisons or Odds Ratios (the standard technique in suicide
studies). ―Pearson’s r‖ correlation coefficient, the most commonly used metric, measures
the degree to which two continuous variables correlate; ―Cohen’s d‖ measures the degree
to which the standard deviation of two continuous variables differs; and an ―Odds Ratio‖
horse race. (A continuous measure allows a range of scores between whole numbers as in
scales; dichotomous effect sizes measure exclusive, either/or, categories that do not allow
scaling: in the case of suicide ultimately the dichotomous choice is between alive or dead
or an attempt to take one’s own life or no attempt.) A few selected studies presented
descriptive statistics only and were included if they presented sufficient data for
conversion to a common metric; specifically, the means, the number of subjects in each
In order to make it possible to compare results, each discrete result of all the
studies was converted to a common metric, Cohen’s d. An exception was made to include
studies using Odds Ratios for which data conversion is not feasible: this exception was
made so that suicide studies could be included in this review. An on-line effect size
calculator was used to convert the data into Cohen’s d (Lyons & Morris, 2010).
The ―Cohen’s d‖ effect size was chosen because this metric allows easier visual
comparison than other measures of effect size. It is a 3 decimal point metric that has a
above or below a mean, By contrast, while Pearsons r varies between -1 to + 1 with one
191
being an absolute correlation. The Odds Ratio metric is a far more unwieldy measure of
effect size; it presents effect sizes as a ratio, a confidence interval – an upper and lower
range of likelihood that a particular score will fall within that range (Hopkins, 2001). For
example, Brunstein (2008) shows that victimized girls are at higher risk for suicidal
ideation measured as 27.38/1 ratio with a lower limit of 8.93/1 and an upper limit of
83.53/1 95% of the time. This is represented as 27.38 (8.98-83.53) 95%: an unwieldy
Studies using Odds Ratios were included in this review in order to include suicide
in the model of effects. As is standard in suicide research all but one study measuring
suicide used the Odds Ratio metric. Odds Ratios can also be converted to r and d if the
standard deviations are known (Chinn, 2000). In fact, Cohen’s d and Pearson’s r effect
sizes can also be converted into Odds Ratios. This was not done as the Odds Ratio is such
an inefficient metric for the purpose of comparing effect sizes visually: they are not as
easily to compare as they use more space; and almost no suicide studies provided the
Where possible the direction of effect sizes was changed to further assist
quality in the form of a plus or minus (+ -). For example, a measure of self-derogation
was changed to a negative direction and included with studies of self-esteem and self-
worth which were determined to be essentially measuring the same cognitive process (see
the following section). In a similar way peer acceptance, a positive direction, was
terms of direction and strength, and allow for the identification of outliers, scores that are
radically different from others in a set. An unweighted mean score will be created by
averaging the scores in each dimension. An unweighted mean is calculated by adding the
scores and dividing by the number of scores; a weighted mean would take into account
the size of the sample and other considerations such as the measures used: weighting is
being avoid again due to the lack of homogeneity between studies. As will be evident
below the homogeneity and consistency of some results such as those for loneliness and
significant were turned into significant results. This was only done when the studies
being compared showed low levels of shared method variance (where variables were
measured in similar ways) and when the particular non-significant score fell within a
range of significant scores. In all cases the smaller but significant correlation came from a
study with a larger sample than the one with higher correlation and non-significant result.
This was not done in cases where there was high shared method variance since the non-
The original studies collected data on several different factors. Some presented
data on gender, others distinguished between scores and data collected from different
informants such as peers, teachers and parents. For this study none of these distinguishing
factors were retained as the fundamental similarity between genders and between raters
has been amply established in previous research (Hawker & Boulton, 2000; O’Connell et
193
al., 1999) . Hawker and Boulton (2000) distinguished between informants in their meta-
The only distinguishing factor of the sample that has been maintained has been
the general age of the subjects of the study. These have been grouped into two groups
designated L for early school and U for later school. (Middle school children are often
included in samples of high and elementary school student and therefore span both
categories.)
Results will be presented in three ways. The range of scores for each dimension
will be presented; the unweighted average of scores will be presented; and, for
dimensions with 2 results from at least 2 studies or 3 or more discrete results, the
unweighted mean scores for the two age cohorts will be presented. This will allow the
reader to determine whether the scores appear consistent and whether there is a change
The acuity of the victimization measures was collected but did not play a
significant role in the data analysis. The vote-counting method was used to show that low
acuity studies were more likely to produce insignificant or less significant results. Future
detailed statistical exploration would need to be done to develop a system for weighting
the results to compensate for the lack of instrumental acuity but that would require a
larger number of studies where other variables were measured using the same methods.
The vote-counting method chosen for this review is sufficient for an early
exploration of the typology in that an informal analysis of the results as they are
aggregated into typological category would be able to rule out the salience of the
194
category. Vote-counting meta-analysis can identify outliers, and determine whether all or
most findings are significant and in the same direction (Bangert-Drowns, 1986).
A table for each dimension will show the range of results, the percentage of
results deemed significant, the mean unweighted result, and, where possible, the means of
the scores for the lower and upper grades. Several sets of scores showed 100%
significance which is an indication that all studies measuring a particular variable showed
a significant correlation; the range of scores indicates the relative strengths of the
The methodology has a few drawbacks. The most significant obstacle is that the
research is constrained by the data available in existing studies. Variables for which no
research has been conducted, such as the play behavior of bully-involved children or the
state of their social and emotional bonds, cannot be directly measured using this
opposed to individual scores: since the unit of analysis in a systematic review is the
individual study rather than the individual children that are the subjects of the studies it is
possible that some results can show relationships that exist solely between studies rather
and among different variables is not possible as the individual scores are not available.
European studies both of which have longer and better-funding bullying research projects
including longitudinal studies that have followed groups of children into young
195
adulthood. Some effects such as the physical and post-traumatic stress symptoms are
accept data only from certain sanctioned sources, ignoring ―grey‖ data, data which does
(Rothstein, Sutton, & Borenstein, 2005). In the case of the bullying literature, there does
not appear to be a large body of grey data in that most published material on the web or
in non-scholarly books are oriented toward supporting victims rather than quantifying the
bullying phenomenon and its effects. The potential bias against non-significant results
may be mitigated by that fact that many studies present data on effects that are not
necessarily the main focus of the research. The results of this systematic review will need
to be tested with a real population before the results can be shown to be generalizable.
(Slavin, 1995). Slavin (1995) makes the point that virtually all study criteria and effect
size measures are incomplete and that the only way to handle bias is to be as overt as
possible about the choices made. In this case, bias is avoided by including all studies that
In the following sections the systematic review will be described and the results
analyzed. The concepts inherent in the studies will be organized and categorized using
Thirty-eight research articles comprising 39 separate studies met the criteria for
inclusion in this review. Table 5 shows the authors, the variables measured (represented
as dimension, which will be more fully elaborated below), the number of subjects, and
The total number of subjects in the review is 92,179 and the ages of the sample
span the stages of childhood but, due to the variation in ages, were coded into groups: L –
lower age, U – upper age, and A – all ages. Middle and junior high school children were
part of the younger end of studies of high school students and at the older end of studies
included a considerable middle school cohort. A few studies limited their sample to that
(2003)
Totals 92,179 *
* The number of subjects is not included in this list for the following reasons: Finkelhor
2007 and 2007 b report data on the same sample; Study 1 of Schwartz 2008 appears to
use the same sample as Schwartz, 2005 as the number, mean age and gender proportion
are identical; Srabstein et al., 2006 reports data from the same group as Nansel, 2003.
Prevalence
between 9% and 35%. Only 15 studies presented prevalence data in the form of a
percentage; the rest of the studies presented the prevalence data in terms of the scores on
victimization scales from which the percentage could not be inferred31. Victimization
was measured using several types of techniques: the most common technique was peer
and self-nominations in which the children themselves identified those among them who
had been victimized; standard instruments were also common (B. J. Kochenderfer &
Ladd, 1997; Neary & Joseph, 1994; Olweus, 1996) as were original instruments whose
items were substantially the same as those from standardized scales. All victimization
questionnaires and the textual preambles that frequently accompany them include such
the major features of the bullying phenomenon such as a perceived imbalance of power,
the measures of prevalence is the variation in the temporal aspects of the individual study
design. Some studies asked about the occurrence of bullying as far back as three years;
other studies used a contemporaneous criterion such as acts of bullying observed and
31
Requests were sent to several authors for the prevalence rates or the data from which those rates could be
extrapolated.
200
videotaped. Studies that showed prevalence rates as high as 35% are likely to include a
significant number of children who successfully negotiated their encounters with bullies
and may be correspondingly less symptomatic: that cohort of former victims would likely
show lower rates of anxiety and depression. Despite the fact that repetition, the other
temporal aspect of bullying, is now a universal part of the definition of bullying, the
authors of the selected studies neglected to provide information on how and whether
repetition was measured—in other words, none of the authors specified whether and to
victim within the studies reviewed were categorized as high and low acuity. Low acuity
measures were most frequently used in large scale studies such as those conducted by
Nishina (2005) and Adelmann (2005) and were usually comprised of four or less items to
determine victimization. High acuity measures require significant researcher time and
were more common in smaller scale studies; they used elaborate surveys, in-depth
which interviewers asked children to sort photographs of their peers to identify those
The wide variation in prevalence of bullying among the studies does not appear to
be uniquely the result of differences in the measures employed. Studies that used similar
methods and instruments still yielded different prevalence rates among the different
samples. As has been noted by earlier researchers (Boulton, 1991; Sharp et al., 2000),
local factors appear to account for a large part of the variance in prevalence rates. Other
factors may be at play for which there is no data; for example, some school subcultures
201
may make it less or more likely that a child would be willing to nominate a peer or
abuse their own power by hitting or humiliating children (Omigbodun et al., 2008) there
behaviors, that extend to children; in such environments the prevalence rates may appear
lower.
This writer’s assumption that there was little or no theory underpinning the
studies was borne out by the literature review. However, the assumption that the claims
supporting the studies would be largely based on the victims’ risk for perpetrating future
violence was not borne out. While that impression was the result of reading dozens of
articles published in the US, many of which were indirectly or directly advocating for
resources and policy changes; researchers did not tend to make claims that went far
beyond the parameters of their own projects: the claims on which the studies were
predicated largely reflected the variables being examined and were generally concerned
with the well-being of the victimized children. Researchers did not base their claims on
the fear that the victims of bullies would become violent young adults themselves.
Only four studies provided any theoretical framework for their research; yet, none
provided a theory that explained the mechanism of effect of bullying per se with the
exception of Cunningham (2007) which used Catalano’s social development theory. This
importance of social skills and social connectedness and the idea that diminishing a
None of the authors predicated their research on the strengths perspective but
rather looked for negative sequelae of the victim experience. The embedded assumption
common to all the selected studies was that bullying was a problem requiring eradication.
None of the authors attempted to discover any positive aspects of the phenomenon nor
did they seek to elicit the opinions of adults or children who support or justify bullying.
The results of the studies, however, show little evidence for a positive effect of
victimization, although it can be assumed that there are cohorts of children among the
samples studied who were victimized but did not experience ill effects, and possibly a
cohort of children for whom escaping bullying was associated with positive effects (these
that some of the worst effects of bullying would come from its ability to separate the
All the results were turned into the covariation metric – Cohen’s d, aside from
those presented as Odds Ratios, and all but one of the significant results were in the
expected direction. As has been discussed, the methodological variation among studies
makes it difficult to compare the effects sizes since the research design and operational
definitions are so different between studies. These results will be described below and
The idea that lower acuity victimization instruments would result in lower levels
of significance in outcome measurements was only marginally borne out by the data.
203
58% of results from studies using lower acuity instruments bore significant results versus
68% for studies using higher acuity instruments. This 10% difference was not sufficiently
victimization by peers and a total of 236 separate outcomes. These measurements ranged
from single items that indicated the presence of a single discrete effect, such as the
instrument that includes indicators of symptoms of both depression and anxiety (Hodges
& Perry, 1999). These 236 outcome variables have been divided into eight categories
based on the general topic investigated. In this section these measures and the logic of
To make sure that this writer was not imposing an organization onto the variables
that was influenced by his theoretical readings, independent raters were recruited. Four
independent raters were given the list of variables in the form of the actual concepts used
by the study authors. Raters were asked to put these variables into categories that
intuitively or logically fit together. While none of the raters considered temporal or
spatial features in their categories, there was almost complete agreement among the raters
about the nature of the variables32. These categories are summarized in Table 6.
32
Areas of disagreement were explained by difference of interpretation. For example, one rater put almost
all social variables into cognitive and affective categories. When asked, that rater explained that this was
because of the emotional and cognitive effects of social dimensions such as loneliness and isolation.
204
Table 7 shows these dimensions ranked according to the variables by which they
were measure.. For example, measurements of social consequences of bullying, the most
only one study directly measured play although information about play can be
Typological Dimensions
Social
Loneliness
Social Competence
Social Connectedness
Bonding
Cognitive
Self-Esteem
Self-Blame
Behavioral
Aggression
Aggressive Friends
Conduct
Dominance
Externalization
Substance Abuse
Academic
Attendance
Test Scores and GPA
(Table continues..)
Classroom Participation
School Liking
School is Hard
Academic Problems
PTSD
various levels of visibility. For example, some of the data on play and social connections
206
were done through observation, as were some of the measures of bullying and other
anxiety. Bullying and other complex phenomena tend to be hidden from adult eyes and
are only made visible when they are labeled and become subject to general attention.
Spatial
Five studies measured the degree to which victimized children find their school
environment aversive. While these items measured fear and insecurity and were placed
with other measures of anxiety by all the independent raters, the decision was made to
disaggregate them from anxiety because they related specifically to the school
environment.
Variable % Significant Range of Scores and Mean Means for Upper and Lower
Grades
Feel Unsafe at 100% 0.3504 to 0.4706 n/a no upper grade data
School 0.4347
School 100% 0.2417 to 0.4945 n/a ―
Avoidance One Odds Ratio: 2.0 (1.6 -
2.6)
0.3744
Anxiety in the face of a genuine threat is a rational reaction and a separate phenomenon
from generalized anxiety which would be present even in the absence of aversive
stimulus. The studies measured the children’s perception of safety in their school as well
as the desire to avoid the school (Berthold & Hoover, 2000; Buhs & Ladd, 2001; Buhs et
207
al., 2006; Glew et al., 2008; B. J. Kochenderfer-Ladd & Skinner, 2002). Feeling unsafe
at school was measured using four separate but similar scales with only one measurement
using the same scale; all but one scale were measures of school adjustment and one scale
measured social anxiety and included a subscale for social anxiety. School avoidance was
The results show a high degree of consistency between studies in that all studies
showed significant results in the same direction. All the studies were done at the lower
grades so no data were available on the safety of older children. As would be consistent
with the age of the children studies the feeling of lack of safety was higher than their
level of school avoidance: smaller children are not as free as older children to avoid
school. In the next section, however, it will be seen that these children also experience a
number of aversive physical symptoms that may be directly related to their fear of going
to school.
While none of the authors theorized about the spatial characteristics of the
bullying phenomenon, the seven studies provided measurements that demonstrate that
conceivable that being afraid of the school environment will turn out to be an effective
indicator of bullying, particularly for younger children who may not yet have formed a
concept of bullying and are not as able as older children to miss school. In other words,
by simply surveying children on their level of fear in school as well as perhaps, their
loneliness, educators and social workers can identify children who are being bullied or
are at risk.
208
Physical Dimension
The two selected studies that measured physical effects looked at looked at a large
variety of effects, although with very little duplication among studies. These included:
stomach ache, dizziness, backache, headache, and obesity; as well as general morbidity
measure the physical effects of bullying: the HBSC, Health Behavior in School-aged
Children Families & Learning, 2001) and one original instrument; one instrument was
(Wiggins & Wender, 1961). The HBSC includes an aggregate score of the degree of
victimized children are experiencing more than one physical symptom and perceive
themselves to be experiencing these symptoms at a more severe level than do their non-
bullied peers. The morbidity score is an aggregate score that includes the individual
209
scores and allowed for a rating of severity. While this single score comes from one study
the sample was over 15,000 and the scale used was the World Health Organization
Health Behavior in School-children (HBSC) Check List (WHO, 2009) whose reliability
and validity have been amply established through very large cross-national studies
Obesity was based on a body mass index of the HBSC and the result of over 2.0 is
the highest result in this review. The result does not indicate the degree to which obesity
was present prior to the bullying. While most of the results of this study were published
as Odds Ratios this result was published as means, standard deviations and number of
subjects for bullied only and non-bullied comparisons group. As Hopkins (2001)
suggests the standard deviations of the two group were averaged when converting to a
Cohen’s d metric, but this did not substantially lower the covariation.
The high score for obesity compared to non-bullied and bullying groups seems to
fly in the face of research that shows obese children at equal risk for both bully and
victim roles (Janssen et al., 2004). This result clearly indicates that victimized children
have higher body mass but this result is inconsistent with the quite low association within
the same large study between poor nutritional choices and victimization: it is hard to
understand how the body mass could be so high if the nutritional choices are not terrible
unless the increase in body mass were the result of the markedly less activity that
victimized children show. This decreased activity will be shown in the play section
below.
The fact that all measures of physical strength correlated negatively with
a positive result in physical strength would almost certainly obviate any effects result as
the score would not likely be for a child who was genuinely victimized according to the
traditional definition.
bullies.
Both studies measuring PTSD used the same measure the Trauma Symptom
Checklist for Children, TSCC (Briere, 1996). While all four measures show significant
results the range of scores from 0.2417 to .4945 is mild to moderate in significance.
The 100% significant rate of PTSD symptoms, all studies found significant results
for these variables, among the exclusively lower grades tested came as a surprise as a UK
study of PTSD in older victims of bullying did not show anything like that rate (Mynard,
Joseph, et al., 2000). It may be that the trauma of victimization at the hands of peers is
significantly lower in cultures that have taken universal stances against bullying as
opposed to the US culture which still largely ignores the problem; another explanation of
this difference may be that older children have been exposed to bullying both in school
and in the media thereby removing the sudden shock dimension of trauma.
211
The fact that all measures showed a significant result does not indicate that all
children who were victimized experienced trauma. It would be important to test an older
children over time. It would seem that a cohort of victimized children exhibit symptoms
typical of post-traumatic stress including intrusive thoughts about the traumatic event and
other symptoms such as irritability and sleeping difficulties. It is clear that many young
another group of victimized children may experience damage due to cumulative stress
Activity and play behavior were joined into one dimension since much voluntary
physical activity involves some form of play, ranging from informal or organized sports
to fantasy play with imaginative and creative interaction. Four of the measurements were
from observation and two from sub-scale drawn from the Teacher Checklist (Dodge &
Coie, 1987) and the Teacher Rating Scale of School Adjustment (Birch & Ladd, 1997).
One study, (Boulton, 1999), directly measured play behavior including the size of the
playgroup, conversation during play, and solitary play. The results show ample evidence
that victimized children play less than their peers. The size of the play network is strongly
positively correlated.
212
The one significant and positive result that is in an unanticipated direction is play
finding is the single significant anomaly in this review and was recognized as an anomaly
by the author (Boulton, 1999). The item was for boys and indicated that they had been
other very high correlations within that one study for solitary play and very low social
play. Boulton suggests that further study is required to determine whether this result
Social
The social dimension is the most frequently investigated outcome of bullying and
loneliness, and bonding. Dimensions were organized based on shared features which
measured social sub-dimension. The Child Behavior Scale (Ladd & Profilet, 1996) was
the most frequent measure used in six discrete measurements, five measures were
213
original to the study, two measurements were made with the Metropolitan Area Child
Study survey (Metropolitan Area Child Study Group, 2002); and two were drawn from
direct observation of peer interaction. All the variables relate in different ways to the
quantity and quality of the victimized child’s social life, albeit from different
such as the number of friends, peers’ nominations of popularity, and perceptions of social
acceptance or rejection.
Measures of social distance were kept in this category but may need to be
disaggregated as a separate but related variable in future research: it is possible that some
children who are distant and withdrawn may still be considered popular; however, it
seems more likely that withdrawal and unpopularity are closely related in most cases.
with the idea that unpopular children may not be perceived as romantically appealing for
results in any of the several studies that measured loneliness. This is likely due to the low
method variance as many authors used the same scale (Cassidy & Asher, 1992) or items
that are very similar. Loneliness, based on the different means for the two age groups,
and differs from other outcome measures in the social dimension in that loneliness is a
distress felt about social isolation. Loneliness, because of its subjective nature may not
always act as an indicator of actual social isolation: some children may experience
loneliness despite a high number of friends and positive social interactions; conversely,
some withdrawn and rejected children may not experience social isolation as aversive,
preferring solitude or the company of one chosen friend to more expansive social lives.
Jobe and colleagues, however, in a study of college students (Jobe & Williams White,
2007) found that adults with autism report high levels of loneliness despite a
Social exclusion also, not surprisingly, comes out high both on the percentage of
studies showing significant results and the strength of the correlations for both lower and
upper grades. These measures were changed to be uniform in direction as they measured
both peer preference and peer rejection. While there appears to be an increase between
lower and upper grades the difference is not significant: what can be said is that
Four studies measured some form of attachment and commitment which were
categorized as bonding, defined as a force or feeling that unites people. These included
measures of attachment, commitment, closeness, conflict and school liking drawn from
the Teacher Rating of School Adjustment instrument (Birch & Ladd, 1997) and a similar
(2004). The research results show that children who are victimized are also less bonded
to others. While only 60% of bonding measures were significant, the size of those effects
was quite high. This difference is very likely due to method variance and will require
further study. Teacher-student conflict was kept in the social bonding category as it was
used as a corollary of teacher-student closeness within Ladd and Burgess’s study and the
authors proposed it as an indicator of bonding (Ladd & Burgess, 2001): the authors take a
nuanced view of conflict and propose that the conflict can be interactional in that it can
be the result of social distance and not always or exclusively the cause of social
distance33. Further study would be required to determine whether and to what degree
and interpersonal problems. Three scales: the Social Behavior Rating Scale (Schwartz,
Farver, Chang, & Lee-Shim, 2002), the Self-Perception Profile for Children (Harter,
1999) and the Self-Report Coping Scale (Causey & Dubow, 1992) were used in all 14
33
Ladd and Burgess (2001) suggest that conflict arise in the vacuum that is created when there is social
distance.
216
Kindergarten with low social skills is at risk for victimization (B.J. Kochenderfer &
Ladd, 1996) but a child with adequate social skills who is targeted by bullies may have
Several measures looked at the numbers of friendships or the size of the social
group of victimized children, the vast majority using similar peer nomination methods in
which children identify peers and rate or describe their friendships. The aggregated
results show that victimized children tend to be more socially isolated at all ages. The
studies measured the size of friendship groups as well as the number of best friends.
While there was a 30% rate of non-significant result there was no result in the contrary
direction: no study showed that victimized children had the same or higher numbers of
friends compared to non-victimized children. The mean for the upper grades was
somewhat higher indicating that social exclusion may worsen over time.
The self-perception of romantic appeal among older grades was also significant
with a high negative correlation with victimization. This is taken from a sub-scale of the
Social Experience Questionnaire (Crick & Grotpeter, 1996). This negative perception
process indicates hopelessness for future social relationships and should be checked to
see if it correlates with depression and suicide in a larger sample. While the victimized
sample is shown to be physically less strong than its non-victimized cohort many of these
children may possess physically attractive features. It is interesting that the two measures,
217
which were separated by gender, are substantially the same because it would be expected
that the lack of physical strength would be an asset at best and neutral at least for girls
The picture that the data provides shows a cohort of lonely and isolated children.
While social competence and the number of friends can be related, a child may be
isolated even if she were to possess measurably high social competence. In fact, some
popular children are selected as the target of relational aggression precisely because they
have a rich social life. Relational aggression, including cyberbullying, often very
effectively damages the reputation and social status of a child with a rich and varied
social network.
The next section looks at the affective results for these isolated children.
Cognitive Temporal
self-esteem, self-worth, and self-efficacy. While these are very separate concepts, they all
share the quality of cognitive self-appraisal that is variable in negative and positive
directions. The directions of measures were made uniform and the dimension was divided
into two main categories self-worth and self-blame. A single measurement of self-
efficacy for aggression did not fit either category and was maintained in the cognitive
perception.
Variable % Sig- Range of Scores and Mean Means for Upper and Lower
nificant Grades
Self-efficacy one non- one measure for the lower grades
for aggression significant
Self-Perception Profile for Children (1996) was used in 11 discrete measurements with a
sub-scale of the Child Depression Inventory being the next most frequently used with
three. The items in all three scales are similar giving this dimension a very low level of
method variance. Low self-worth is particularly significant for older children as there is
over six-fold decrease in self-worth between upper and lower grades. This indicates
clearly that children who remain in the victim role will have their self-esteem
teenagers who experience victimization for the first time in the older grades will
experience a fast undermining of their self-esteem. Oddly, self-blame does not appear to
Self-blame and self-derogation, which was folded into the self-blame sub-
dimension, were both seen as essentially the same phenomenon and were measured by
two separate instruments: an original scale accounts for 8 of the measurements and the
Social Experience Questionnaire (Crick & Grotpeter, 1996) was used once. Self-blame
was only measured in the upper grades and did not show as strongly significant as other
measures. This indicates that many victimized children are not ascribing blame to
themselves for their bullying even though the bullying is undermining their self-esteem.
determine whether the cohort of self-blaming children suffer the same or, as seems likely,
measure self-blame in the earlier grades to see if that corresponds to the relatively low
cognitive assessment indicating that the child does not rate herself capable of acting out
aggressively. The one result was for a study of a lower grade cohort and did not show a
significant result in either direction. That study cohort, however, did not consider itself
capable of acting aggressively which is not surprising given the powerlessness inherent in
Affective Temporal
These measures used the children themselves, their parents, or peers as informants and
asked about the victimized child’s subjective emotional state. These include items on
specific moods, anxiety, and more global measures of anxiety and depression.
Internalization is a general term for emotionally aversive states that include and conflate
anger, depression and anxiety, the inwardly directed symptoms. Table 14 shows the
(Kovacs, 1992) inventory was used 14 times with the Beck scale (Beck & Steer, 1993),
the Child Behavior Check List (Achenbach, 1991), the Trauma Symptoms in Children
Checklist (Briere, 1996) and the Health Behavior in Schools Checklist (WHO, 2009) and
original items used in one to three of the measurements. One hundred percent of the
anhedonia - low mood and unhappiness - were significant. A comparison of the means
for depression scores in younger and older children show a 60% increase over time: by
far the largest increase between age cohorts in any measurement within this review.
While some part of this variance may be due to the difference in measures and capacity
of children to accurately report their mood, it would be very unlikely that this explains a
60% increase. Coupled with a slight decrease in anxiety (.3505 to .3029) this seems to
bear out Parker and Ashgari’s (1997) prediction that anxiety in young children turns into
early onset depression, which they show is co-morbid with other life difficulties.
221
One single item on emotional problems in general was separated into its own sub-
category and showed a high degree of significance. Emotional lability was also unique
and also showed a significant positive result. A single item on irritability did not show a
Anxiety was quite common for victimized children although the means remain
relatively steady over time. Anxiety was also measured using similar scales as for
depression (HBSC, TSCC and the MASC Multidimensional Anxiety Scale – Children
(March, Parker, Sullivan, Stallings, & Conners, 1997). Given the rise in depression over
time, it seems likely that one group of victimized children develops generalized anxiety
symptoms while a larger group becomes increasingly depressed over time. Some, of
the Peer Nomination Inventory (Wiggins & Wender, 1961), and the Strengths and
anxiety as well as cognitive problems and was highly significant with an average - of
1.25. Measures of internalization were given only to lower grade children and showed an
Academic
The academic dimension includes such typical academic measures as grades and
test scores, and teacher, self, and peer ratings of academic achievement; measures of
attendance and tardiness are also included in this dimension as are perceptions of school.
Schwartz (2008) reported on both math and reading and found that victimized children
scored significantly lower on math but did score in a positive direction, although not
222
Autonomous participation refers to a sub-scale that measures the independent and self-
directed activity: this measure, while in a negative direction, was not at a significant
Variable % Significant Range of Scores and Means for Upper and Lower
Mean Grades
Marks and scores 85% -0.1201 to – 0.6521 Lower -0.28105
-0.2990 Upper - 0.4734
Learning Significant 0.3242 n/a
problem
Attendance 33% -0.02 to -.3242 n/a no upper grade data
-0.1120
School is hard Significant 0.3914 single n/a
Participation 67% -0.3660 to -0.1001 n/a no lower grades data
-0.2021
Marks and test scores for victimized children were significant in 85% of the
results at moderate levels for younger grades and significantly higher levels for older
grades indicating the tendency for victimized children’s academics to slide over time.
perception: this single result is also significant and close to other scores within the range.
Attendance was significantly lower but this measure is not that sensitive in the lower
grades as children don’t have as much freedom to skip classes or play truant. Most of
Victimized children are seen, at older grades, to like school less, to find school
hard and to participate less in class. Cooperative participation was not folded into general
participation because the authors made the distinction and because this item retains
and a decrease in comfort and enjoyment of the academic setting. Finding school hard is
the highest co-variation in this dimension which is intuitively logical in that this would
also capture the victimized children who are academically successful. Victimized
a more sensitive measure for the upper grades. This may be due to the increasing
workload and complexity of material in the older grades. Measures of perception indicate
that victimized children find school less rewarding, less pleasant, and experience more
The positive direction for reading scores among victimized children is only one of
two anomalies found in this review. While not significant it nevertheless was in an
children as well as socially isolated children in general. It can be hypothesized that some
low play children spend more time reading, perhaps as a way to retain a semblance of
social contact.
Behavioral
behavior problems. Included are measures of delinquent behavior, anger as rated by peers
and teachers, and violent and delinquent behaviors including carrying weapons and
fighting.
Variable % Significant Range of Scores and Mean Means for Upper and Lower
Grades
Aggression 62% -0.1001 to 1.0361 n/a only one result from upper
mean 0.4493 grades
Aggressive 60% 0.02 to 1.093 Lower 0.1928
friends 0.3728 Upper n/a
Conduct 50% 0.3115 to 1.0078 n/a
0.4971
Externalization 100% 0.5608 to 0.9256 No upper grade data
0.8437
Substance Use 100% 0.1202 to .899 No lower grade data
mean 0.5130
Dominance one non-
significant
Results within this review dimension are less homogeneous than other
dimensions. Aggression itself was measured using 7 separate scales and one peer
nomination protocol and scores range from -0.1 to over +1.0 which is over one standard
deviation difference. All but one result was from the upper grades. This wide range is
likely explained by the fact that some studies separate out bully/victims while others do
not: this would result in higher aggression levels in those that include bully/victims, a
separate subset of aggressive victims34. Another likely explanation is that some habitual
victims tend to be targeted precisely because of their emotional lability. One cohort of
victimized children, therefore, may be quite compliant and non-aggressive while another
may be aggressive.
34
It is this writer’s contention that self-report of victimization among bullies can be an exculpatory strategy
or an example of hostile attribution, the attribution of hostile motive to neutral or benign actions.
225
was included in this category, but may function separately if it were found in future
research that some children endorse aggressive acts without actually performing them
themselves.
While the data shows that victimized children are not as physically strong as their
cohort, after the first years in each educational institution --elementary, middle (and
junior high), and high school– they are likely to be more powerful than the younger
cohort and therefore do have access to a cohort that they could themselves bully.
Externalization, the global scalar measure for all externally visible problematic
behaviors was measured with the Child Behavior Checklist (Achenbach, 1991) and the
Peer Nomination Inventory (Wiggins & Wender, 1961) is the highest average score in
behavioral dimension at 0.8437. Conduct problems, while only significant in half the
results is the second highest co-variation in this dimension at 0.4971. Only one study
Adelmann’s study, with over 50,000 middle school children, it is the largest
included in this review. The victimization measure used in the Adelmann study is
considered to be low acuity and the prevalence rate of 35% is the highest in this review.
Substance use was very high among victimized teens with measures of smoking, alcohol
and drug use folded into one sub-dimension and having a 100% significance and an
The data in this dimension were not collected uniformly in upper and lower
grades and with high shared method variance to the extent that it would be difficult to
226
draw conclusions: further research should be done on a wide sample using the same
refute the commonly held perception that victims tend to be compliant and non-violent.
Rather, it would appear that they lack the physical resources to defend themselves, and,
Suicide
comparison in the way that the other studies. The methods differ and the statistical
technique of Odds Ratio is significantly different than the continuous measures discussed
above. Nevertheless, this section indicates that the risk of suicide is a significant effect of
The shared method variance within this dimension and between the other
dimensions in this review poses difficulties in comparing and contrasting results. As had
been pointed out above, all but one suicide study was done using Odds Ratio effect sizes
and there is an apparent lack of homogeneity in scores for similar phenomena within the
suicide dimension: scores range from a low of 1.69/1 for suicidal behaviors to 27.38 for
227
suicidal ideation. The results for suicide attempt and ideation are ten times more
significant than the result for suicidal behaviors which is too high a shared method
variance to conflate. It is not immediately evident why the results would be so different.
Suicide attempt was measured using three separate scales the Youth Risk
Behavior Survey (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 1998), the Minnesota
Student Survery (Minnesota Department of Children Families & Learning, 2001) and the
Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children (Shaffer, Fisher, Lucas, Dulcan, & Schwab-
Stone, 2000). Suicidal ideation was measured using one instrument, the Suicidal Ideation
Questionnaire (Reynolds, 1988). Since none of the suicide studies attempted to determine
other forms of trauma and victimization it is unclear how much victimization alone
contributed to the suicidal thoughts. This can be said of all the studies including the two
Finkelhor studies since they put sibling and peer abuse in the same cohort (2007a,
2007b).
Despite the method variance it is clear that victimized children are at high risk for
suicide and, perhaps, suicidal and homicidal combinations. All of the studies reported
behavior, and suicidal ideation. The odds of a victimized child having suicidal ideation
and suicide attempts are particularly high but have very large confidence intervals for all
four results which are typical of studies with a relatively small sample. Violent and
suicidal behavior was maintained as a separate category as the two phenomena may not
be related. Some children may be suicidal without having any intention of harming others
while some combine both. This again gives credence to the notion that victimized
children can pose risk for violence including school shootings (Vossekuil et al., 2002).
228
These teens may be particularly dangerous for such behaviors as vehicular homicide
which may frequently be deliberate suicides that go uncounted if there is no note or other
While all the studies included for review used adolescent samples, Holt and
colleagues (2007), in a study that couldn’t be included due to the lack of convertible data,
also found statistically significant rates of suicidal ideation among all ages, including
young children.35
As this project is being completed, the suicides of two teenagers have been getting
significant media attention. One, a boy, was bullied with homophobic epithets and
35
The author was contacted but was not able to furnish the standard deviations required to convert the
means and Ns to a co-variation metric.
229
In this section the findings will be synthesized. The findings will be analyzed for
their fit with Habermas’s theory, a model of these effects will be depicted, and the two
discrete anomalies found in the review will be discussed. The final section will include a
discussion of the possible positive or neutral effects of bullying experiences and will give
suggestions for future research and interventions. The section ends with a brief discussion
of the place this dissertation may play in the eventual creation of a theory that can be
The results of this review suggest a logical order to the temporal and spatial
effects of bullying that start with the initial spatial and physical reactions which lead to
changes in the quality of the social environment. This social and spatial experience leads
to repeated affective and cognitive processes that lead to an academic slide, depressive
symptoms and generalized anxiety, and destructive behaviors up to and including suicide.
no study isolated peer victimization from other forms of trauma and victimization. The
two related studies by Finkelhor and colleagues (2007a, 2007b) do isolate bullying from
most other forms of victimization but they include sibling victimization with bullying -
the two phenomena may in many cases be unrelated; nevertheless, some of the processes
victimization. For example, sibling abuse would make the home as unsafe as the school
This review does not imply causation. The results of this review show a set of
associations that appear to coalesce into a single model: no definitive conclusion about
230
causation could be made at this stage of scholarship. This review also presents a, perhaps,
his academic positions and this writer was not able to find a way to correspond with him
communicative action can explain a large part of the effects of bullying. Most of the
theoretical concepts relevant to the topic of bullying correspond closely to the dimensions
presented in the previous section. In this section the core concepts of the theory will be
compared to the results of the systematic review. In terms of Dubin’s schema it appears
that the theory and empirical results are very close if not identical and is sufficient to
self-esteem and self-blame - and social competence were measured directly, and the both
binding force and play deprivation were measured indirectly in that both these concepts
can be extrapolated from the data that show victimized children to be socially isolated,
play less, have fewer social attachments and are less physically active.
While the effects of bullying on identity formation was not directly or explicitly
measured, despite the availability of instruments that measure identity in older children,
there is plenty of passive evidence for distortions of identity within this review. A direct
measure of identity would include such things as preferences and interactional styles such
others through communicative and consensual means – of children who are bullied.
While two studies measured bonding this was not taken to be synonymous with
Habermas’s binding energy since that concept hasn’t been directly investigated at this
point: it would be hard to make the case that bonding and attachment were the same as
232
binding energy when that concept has not been empirically verified. This lack of
Table 18 relates the mechanisms of effect and the predicted results described
above. The first two columns are the predicted mechanisms through which the bullying
experience can damage a child, the third column answers whether or not there is evidence
in the studies reviewed. Evidence that refutes Habermas’s theory would have been
placed in that column as well but no such contradictory evidence was found.
several theories and links intrapsychic motivations with socio-cultural forces acting, as
Ritzer (2001) suggests, as a meta-theory. While it is true that Habermas distills many
existing theories and explains the generation of theory as part of the process of
extremely broad in its boundaries rather than subsuming other theories. The theory of
communicative action is simply a good social theory that is able to explain the
thinkers in many disciplines. Considering that all the social scientists contributing to this
review were operating at least in part within the intellectual traditions Habermas
elucidated, it may be inevitable that the results adhere to these essential concepts.
234
visible and cohesive. Theory gives focus to analysis and helps to adapt or create language
that is helpful in grasping the social situations and contexts of bullying. Habermas’s
terminology is precise and hard to replace but may lack the type of poetry that it takes to
Anomalies
There were two anomalies within the review, far fewer than had been anticipated.
Of the two anomalies found only one was significant; they were, however, related. The
significant finding was that victimized children were observed talking during play time
which appears to contradict the data that shows that they tend to play less and be more
socially isolated; the other anomalous finding was that they scored, as a group, in a
positive direction for reading scores in comparison to universally depressed scores for all
other academic areas among victimized children. It may be that victimized children are
using decoding skills in order to protect themselves in the social environment; they may
also derive some secondary social benefit from reading; and they may be conversing with
peers in a non-play way, perhaps commiserating with other children who are also
The theory of communicative action makes visible the nature of instrumental and
communicative states. As had been shown in Table 4 above, various research methods
look at human processes that have varying degrees of visibility. For this systematic
review those obscured processes include internal emotional states of depression and
235
anxiety and cognitive processes and the visible processes include the quantitative
measures of a child’s social life and behavior. As was discussed above such latent
variables as bonding and cognitive distortion were actually well measured. The link
between play and communicative action is still tenuous but there does appear to be a
The systematic review data have confirmed enough of the effects predicted by
The purpose of the model is to understand the patterns and temporal processes
applied to show that the data work together. The data in this review confirms the passive
and play deprivation. Active effects of bullying would need to be confirmed by new
The data suggest a temporal sequence in the effects of bullying. The initial,
narrowing of social space. The theory of communicative action suggests that social
236
effects would mediate the other effects of bullying. Those children who can maintain
consensual relationship and retain a space for free play would be predicted to remain
confirmed through a research protocol that tests the direct effects of the theorized
variables.
imbalance, it is likely to start earlier for children entering the system constitutionally
smaller and meeker. Some children have a continuous or episodic, long-term experience
of bullying, while others have a sudden bullying experience after years of successful
social interactions.
different ways. Children with successful social lives can suffer a sudden and cataclysmic
change in their social life; long-term targets will experience a delay in their social skills
development; children with episodic experiences will have periods of remission, in which
their symptoms diminish or disappear. A large part of the latter two groups begin to
physical problems, attentional and academic problems, and affective problems up to and
including suicidal ideation and suicide attempts. The processes by which children are
The ability to rise above conflict, whether petty or crucial, is for Habermas the
highest form of social competence, but suggesting that we rise above conflict does not
mean he is asking us to remain passive in the face of injustice. He is, however, asking us
to stop it from affecting our life globally. As is evident from the relatively low scores for
237
the victimized cohort some children who are bullied, even repeatedly, may not be
affected globally. They are finding a way to endure the experience without having to
internalize it.
The children who are badly affected by these continuous and repeating
These may be understood as a rational response to real experience rather than arising
from an inherent weakness within the child. A large part of the victimized cohort
experience diminished self-worth and some experience a process in which they blame
themselves for the bullying. It can be surmised that self-blame will be closely associated
with the more severe effects and levels of effects: this should be the subject of future
research.
The concept map in Figure 8 shows a preliminary model for the effects of
bullying almost all of which is directly or indirectly supported by the research. The model
shows how the initial bullying results in a present effect of lessening pleasure and
including isolation from peers which in turn leads to the deprivation of play and
communicative states and results in weaker social bonds and fewer social skills. Without
other strong supportive environments the child may then experience repeating negative
cognitive and affective process unless protected by internal resources that prevent the
developmental victimology would like to find those effects that are common to the other
major forms of victimization of children. A further research project reviewing the effects
of emotional, physical and sexual abuse could identify any common features as well as
In this section sources of error will be explored. In the statistical sense, the term
―source of error‖ refers to the measure of the estimated difference between the observed
effect and the theorized effect. In this case, the largest likely source of statistical error
would be the group of children who remain unaffected by bullying experiences. In the
social sciences error can be understood as all latent, theorized, variables that can explain
the difference in scores. This would include random error which arises from simple
mistakes in orthography or comprehension. In this case the ―error‖ would be the reasons
Some children who have been bullied appear to be unaffected by the experience
spectrum disorders that make children resistant to social interaction. These children might
have been subject to bullying and would score high on measures of victimization but may
score low on the effects measurements if they do not mind being isolated. Research into
the characteristics, attributes, and strategies of these children will be helpful to design
Another source of error relates to children who have escaped the victim role. At
any point bullying may end. While bullying tends to be stable, there are always children
whose victim status ends for a variety of reasons: an intervention may help a child learn
to be less affected by bullying, a child may experience a growth spurt that has him
suddenly towering over his former tormentor, or a child may enter a higher status social
group that is less vulnerable to bullying. Further, a bullying child may learn a more
effective set of social skills and thus stop the bullying themselves. It would be expected
that some symptoms of victims would stop after the bullying ceases.
span that the researcher uses in her survey (e.g., a question like, Have you been bullied
within the past year?). An escaped victim would endorse herself as a victim if she had
been bullied within that time span but, depending on her resilience, may no longer show
any symptoms related to that bullying event. The longer the retrospective span used, the
higher the source of error. This error could be controlled by asking children who escaped
the victim role to self identify and using regression techniques to determine the
characteristics and effects that differ from children still in that role.
240
As was stated above, some children may be positively affected by their bullying
experiences. Smith and Talamelli (2004) documented a cohort of children who ascribe
retrospective span, could endorse themselves as having been bullied but may show higher
children not distressed by being bullied may be children raised in highly oppressive and
states.
Perhaps the most significant source of error in the social dimension is the variable
entry and exit from victimization roles. In order to control for this error longitudinal
methods should be used to track both the well-being of children as well as their initial and
None of the selected studies specifically looked for positive effects of bullying;
however, the evidence that bullying has a global negative impact is overwhelming. It is
likely, as Smith and Talamelli (2004) found, that a proportion of victimized children
having escaped the victim role derived benefit from the bullying experience. It is also
possible that some multiply victimized children derive a benefit from being victimized in
as yet unforeseen ways: for example, a submissive stance may be adaptive for a child
who is being victimized at home and who would be put at risk if she became more
powerful. There is also some possibility that adult masochism may have roots in
childhood experiences and that some children enjoy the bullying as a form of negative
attention.
241
some proportion of victimized children would score higher on positive traits and lower on
The above review leads to many suggestions for future research. These will be
Creating Standards
that will make it easier to build knowledge in concert. Studies using comparable
techniques. It may be time for bullying researchers to come together in person and over
the Internet to discuss issues of standardization and to set an agenda for future directions
in bullying research.
perspective. The bullying research in this review looks at the absence of or alienation
from experiences of support, repair, and solidarity in victimized children’s lives; direct
research into children’s interactions needs to be done to identify acts of solidarity. This
writer conducted a three-year research study, Searching for Safety, into violence and
bullying in an inner city school and identified many minute36 interactions of support and
repair that fit into the schema of communicative action (Kogan & Chandan, unpublished
data).
36
The term minute is used because the acts that were observed tended to be subtle within the context of
violence and bullying: rather than directly confronting the bully, children showed solidarity with victims
through sharing of toys and food, touch and symbolic play, such as talking about superheroes saving the
day.
243
Future research could observe and document repair processes and compare the
important also to explore the social world of children who escape the victim role or those
who are unaffected by bullies in order to develop novel interventions that prevent or
The systematic review shows that there is no further need to study the social
connectedness of victimized children. It is clear from multiple studies that they have
smaller friendship networks and are significantly lonelier than their non-victimized peers.
Loneliness may be present in most or all victimized children and factor analysis research
impact. If loneliness were a good indicator that bullying was occurring, surveying
children to determine their loneliness would be an efficient way for educators and social
within childhood for the quality of life of both the child in the present day as well as the
adult that will emerge. A rich social world can be compared to a rich constellation
shimmering stars on a moonless night. A single warm interaction can open an individual
life to infinite possibilities. A chance encounter can spark a relationship that can expose a
young adult to new ways of thinking, music, arts, job opportunities and marriage
prospects. By contrast a narrow social world is a darker world, albeit comfortable for
244
serendipity.
It has already been shown that essentialist thinking contributes to the acts of
bullying (S.A. Gelman, 2003; Giles, 2003). The theory of communicative action would
cognitive distortions that victims suffer. It would be important to test children affected
with anxiety, depression, and low self-esteem to determine whether they, as a group, have
instrument as the studies done by Giles and Gelman and Heyman have been qualitative in
nature. If it turns out that essentialist thinking is a precursor to any or all ill effects of
distortion for victims the best intervention may be curricular. Children who can stop the
essentialist thinking will have an easier time developing productive social skills and
being protected from the ill effects of trauma and stress. Schools may be able to teach
flexible thinking and could modify their recess and physical education time to allow
autonomous activities while ensuring that they are relatively free of bullying.
the formation of identity per se; however, the aggregated data can be seen as synonymous
with identity using the definition of identity as stable characteristics over time and space
(A. van Hoof & Raajimakers, 2003). The study cannot draw a conclusion about any
245
individual child’s identity but can certainly be used to confirm that victimization distorts
identity in negative directions affecting the cognition, behavior, mood and social life of
the child.
Low play and cognitive distortions such as low self-esteem together with greater
social isolation and diminished social skills can be expected to compromise the formation
of identity, those set of verbal and physical actions and reactions that repeat over time in
such a way that these characteristics can identify the individual. Future research would be
helpful in particular looking at how different forms of bullying can affect identity. It is
possible that researchers will find a pattern to the identity of some victims such that they
Developmental Victimology
there are common effects between and among different forms of victimization. Mynard,
likely that there will be common effects to the qualities of the different forms of
victimization. For example, forms of family violence that rely on isolation will likely
result in similar effects as social bullying; similarly, it is likely that emotional abuse by
adults will result in diminishing self-worth in similar ways that verbal victimization does
The value to the social work profession for testing Habermas’s theory is to guide and
explain the nature of a social phenomenon including its effects it follows logically that it
would then be able to form the basis for a novel and effective intervention. For bullying,
a comprehensive theory should prevent bullying from occurring, stop it when it does
Such a project to really end deleterious bullying would require intervention on several
levels. In the US legislation at the State level requires district-level intervention; but an
change in the national education policy as well as a change in the culture in favor of
genuinely egalitarian and inclusive practices. Such a vision, at this time, may seem
chimeric; however, Habermas’s theory provides a roadmap for future social work policy
advocacy.
The above review suggests several interventions that can be formed from the theory
of communicative action. Some of these interventions would not differ from existing
anti-bullying interventions, whereas others would look radically different. What follows
programs emphasize the acquisition of positive social skills, which would be considered
emphasize promote the acquisition of instrumental skills. In line with Habermas’s neutral
definition of instrumental action such a program would teach children how to initiate
instrumental action effectively and respectfully. As had been pointed out above, children
may take informal and formal instrumental roles such as team captain, school prefect or
hall monitor, and older relatives and friends often keep an eye out for the safety of
smaller children safe from cars or unknown adults; some children may humanely help
teaching the skills of instrumental action in order to divert the bullying impulse into
248
positive directions. Teaching and promoting instrumental skills would involve teaching
children how to use social power while taking into considerations the ability and desires
of others, rather than riding roughshod over the autonomy of others. Such an approach
would have the added advantage of changing the framework from a bully/victim
dichotomy to one in which the instrumental aspirations at the heart of a bully’s actions
can be accepted and respected. Such aspirations, however, require considerable resources.
desire to entertain can be provided with creative opportunities to build on that desire
desire to enforce norms could become involved in student government. The latter
raising the level of social skill, particularly for the bullying child. A program based on the
theory of communicative action would also seek to improve social skills but would do so
equally for both victims and bullies. This is because the theory and the research in this
review show that victimization delays and impedes the acquisition of social skills.
Social skills have primacy within the theory because they lead to deeper and more
effective social bonding which, in turn, opens up an infinitely variable richness of the
249
social world. These social bonds may be deep and intimate but others may be more
superficial and temporary. Social bonds lead to healthier social action than does the
and covenant describe the bonds underlying very strongly motivated action; these
malign authority.
to consider the standpoints of others and requires that the child has had a rich and diverse
experience of other children in social contexts with sufficient autonomy for self-
exploration. These contexts are most frequently found in free or freer play activities.
The systematic review above suggests that the deprivation of play is one of the main
free play contexts. After bullying has become part of a school culture, however, it would
autonomous contexts.
The promotion of free play presents a paradox for social workers and educators
when applied to the problem of bullying. The theory would suggest that victimized
children still need an autonomous environment yet helping and teaching professionals
tend to increase structure and adult supervision for children experiencing any kind of
often comprised of the least needy and most academically accomplished children, and
250
many such activities are available only to middle and upper class children. This writer
was unable to find the quantitative data that would support the former statement; but it
has certainly been true in his practice experience spanning three countries and two
continents.
development of a healthy identity; yet bullying occurs unchecked where adult supervision
is sparse. The social skills curriculum, Second Step **, includes regular discussion about
events that occur that day on the playground and how the children applied social skills
contexts. Adult supervision, however, is essential so long as the adults limit their
autonomy. An effective policy would allow more opportunities for marginalized and
distressed children to experience more, not less, autonomy. It would involve training
seeing arts and special interests as a universal right for all children rather than a finite
commodity for a privileged few. Such a policy would also need to pay attention to non-
academic aspects of the school system in general including how transitions and
The systematic review in this dissertation is on the topic of school bullying, yet,
school bullying has led him to the conclusion that Habermas’s social theory has the
potential to provide the basis for an effective and comprehensive social work theory
making a significant contribution to social work’s major methods. Habermas’s theory not
only adds considerably to our understanding of the social world, it is also able to subsume
and integrate a number of existing theories across disparate disciplines. In this section,
this writer will explore the assertion that Habermas’s theory of communicative action
As has been reiterated in this paper, the articles selected for the school bullying
review, with few exceptions, offered no coherent theory about how victimized children
are affected by bullying. Through this project, this writer found that Habermas’s theory
of communicative action is able to subsume the theories that explain how bullying arises
and also shows how bullying affects children. The theories that Habermas’s theory
attachment theory, which suggests that the ability of a child to form secure bonds is an
the actions of support which constitute the anodyne power of supportive acts similar to
The data from this review of bullying, interpreted from a Habermasian theoretical
frame, show what happens when children experience a surfeit of instrumental social
states. The victimized children frequently react through their bodies with anxiety and
school avoidance. Those children who continue to be adversely affected are significantly
lonelier, and their social skills development is arrested. It is clear from the data that these
affected children play less and are generally less physically active. With time, this social
isolation has a global effect on mood and academic functioning for children that
children will act out with behaviors that injure themselves and others; those most
method, is rigorously based on life processes. Universal pragmatics functions for social
science in a way that is similar to the null hypothesis in hard science, eliminating all ideas
whose opposite cannot be proven to be true in the social world. While universal
pragmatics is most often described as the search for the conditions for mutual
understanding, this description misses the larger scope of Habermas’s work with its
ability to explain both meaning making and action—a far wider sphere of theorizing that
includes most or all of the social world, including the connection to the subjective and
intrapsychic experience. Universal pragmatics also looks for ideas that are capable of
crossing disciplines: this is similar to the way the social work profession finds ideas from
Habermas describes the nature of human speech and the coordination of social
allows the social work profession to have a deeper understanding of the nature of social
environments. An instrumental social context, by its nature, limits the autonomy of the
instrumental social context that can damage communicative social contexts in the form of
This writer’s project was limited to school bullying, yet adult experiences of
abuse and oppression may affect people through similar mechanisms, although without
violence perpetrators, for example, tend to isolate their partners from social supports and
systematically undermine their partners’ self-esteem in ways that are similar to child
Europe will presently have data about the quality of the marriages of the bullies and
school bullying transfers into adulthood in the form of domestic abuse or community
violence.
action, Habermas’s theory of communicative action could be used to explain not only
bullying but other problems within the social work field. Habermas’s theory is broad
enough to explain the etiology of large social forces and their connection to minute
254
internal processes. By linking intrapsychic processes with larger social forces, the theory
covers the same territory as such social work models as the ecological and bio-
psychosocial models.
behaviorism, evolutionary biology, and economic theories that are predicated on rewards
that accrue to a person through particular behaviors. Habermas, indeed, extends social
theory into an intersubjective dimension that acknowledges a set of behaviors that people
engage in even in the absence of reward: these behaviors include meaning making,
altruism, and acts of solidarity and support that can’t always be reciprocated, and may, at
Habermas allows that much human activity is verifiably motivated by rewards, but under
subsume various social work theories confirms Ritzer’s notion that the theory is a ―meta-
theory.‖ This writer, however, prefers to see Habermas’s theory of communicative action
from Dubin’s schema of theory building in which the theory can be seen to have a very
large boundary: it is able to explain and predict intrapsychic effects such as cognitive and
Habermas’s theory explains all non-genetic sources of social action, in other words, all
notion of the lifeworld contains the biological needs and constraints that human
255
communities must furnish to flourish. Systems that are in synch with these human needs
communicative action be coined for social work. This writer will call this the ―Lifeworld
Theory.‖ The Lifeworld Theory would explain the connection between larger social
phenomena and the cognitive, affective, and even physical elements of the individual
human being. The Lifeworld Theory would absorb social work ideas about non-genetic
sources of distortion, such as family influences, social forces that limit organic drives,
are not simple: within the same social group, sub-groups may have very different
seen as an ideal utopian context that would exist if not for nefarious faceless conspirators,
although there is room in Habermas’s theory for that degree of social manipulation;
rather, his theory should be understood as a way of analyzing the social world. Within the
same geographical area, the nature of the social community could vary greatly as positive
sub-groups coalesce around individuals and communities with good social skills.
A Lifeworld Theory could be used for all major social work methods:
casework, group work, community organization, administration, and policy. It also has
256
the potential to explain the nature of human strength and resilience as part of a body of
derived action explain the effectiveness of group work as a treatment and mutual support
communicative and instrumental states, and social work administrators practicing from a
Lifeworld Theory would have very specific skills and tools to analyze the informal and
formal communication within agencies and make sure the systems stay aligned with the
interventions that could uncover and mobilize strengths and social skills.
what degree a Lifeworld Theory could help social workers intervene effectively. The
apparent applicability to all social work methods, however, indicates that this theory
would be a good candidate for a general social work theory that could be taught to social
Bibliographic Databases:
BiblioBranchee (France and Quebec)
Cochrane Collaboration
Campbell Collaboration C2-SPECTR
Database of reviews of effectiveness (DARE online),
Dissertation Abstracts International (DAI)
ERIC
MEDLINE
NCAAN Information (Child Abuse and Neglect)
Psychological Abstracts (PsycINFO, PsycLIT)
SCOPUS
Social Care Institute for Excellence (SCIE) (UK)
Social Work Abstracts
Social Science Citation Index
Social Sciences Abstracts
Social Service Abstracts
Sociological Abstracts (Sociofile)
Urban Studies Abstracts
Search Engines:
Biblioline
De.licio.us (conceptual ontologies website)
Francite
Google/Google Scholar
Lexus Nexus
Lycos (France)
Keywords and subject headings were searched systematically (searches were modified
according to the specific database):
Bullying
Child
Child Abuse
Cyberbullying
Harassment/Harcellement
Identite/Identity
Intervention
Intimidation
Peer Victimization
Peer Harassment
Power/Pouvoir
Prevention
School
258
REFERENCES
Abramovitz, M. (1981). Business and health reform: Worker's compensation and health
insurance in the progressive era. Columbia University, New York.
Achenbach, T. M. (1991). Manual for Child Behavior Checklist/ 4-18 and 1991 Profile.
Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Dept. of Psychiatry.
Achenbach, T. M., & Rescoria, L. (2001). Manual for the ASEBA school-age forms and
profiles. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Research Center for Children,
Youth and Families.
Adelman, H. S., & Taylor, L. (1998). Reframing mental health in schools and expanding
school reform. Educational Psychologist, 33, 134-152.
Adorno, T. W., Frenkel-Brunswik, E., Levinson, D. J., & Nevitt Sanford, R. (Eds.).
(1982). The Authoritarian Personality. New York: Norton & Company.
Ahmed, E., & Braithwaite, V. (2004). Bullying and victimization: Cause for concern for
both families and schools. Social Psychology of Education, 7, 35-54.
Allen-Meares, P., Washington, R. O., & Welsh, B. L. (2000). Social work services in
schools. Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
Alsaker, F. D. (1993). Isolement et maltraitance par les pairs dans les jardins d'enfants:
Comment mesurer ces phenoomenes et quelles sont leurs consequences? Enfance,
47(3), 241-260.
Ambert, A. (1994). A Qualitative Study of Peer Abuse and Its Effects: Theoretical and
empirical implications. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 56, 119-130.
Andrews, G. J., & Chen, S. (2006). The production of tyrannical space. Children’s
Geographies, 42(2), 239–250.
Arora, C. (1994). Measuring bullying with the life in school checklist. Pastoral Care in
Education, 12(3), 11-16.
Arora, C., & Thompson, D. (1987). Defining bullying for a secondary school.
Educational and Child Psychology, 4, 110-120.
Aspinwall, P. S., Greene, B. R., & Morrison, D. R. (1994). Calabi-Yau moduli space,
mirror manifolds and spacetime topology change in String Theory. Nuclear
Physics, B416, 414-480.
Astor, R. A., & Meyer, H. A. (1999). Where girls and women won't go: Female students',
teachers', and social workers views of school safety. Social Work in Education,
21(4), 201-219.
Astor, R. A., Meyer, H. A., & Behre, W. J. (1999). Unowned places and times: Maps and
interviews about violence in schools. American Educational Research Journal,
36(1), 3-42.
260
Astor, R. A., Meyer, H. A., & Pitner, R. O. (2001). Elementary and middle school
students' perceptions of violence-prone school subcontexts. Elementary School
Journal, 101(5), 511-528.
Bachrach, P., & Barantz, M. (1962). The two faces of power. American Political Science
Review, 56, 947-952.
Bachrach, P., & Botwinick., A. (1992). Power and empowerment: A radical theory of
participatory democracy. Philadelphia: Temple University Press.
Baldry, A. (2004). The impact of direct and indirect bullying on the mental and physical
health of Italian youngsters. Aggressive Behavior, 30, 343-355.
Ballard, M., Argus, T., & Remley, T. P., Jr. . (1999). Bullying and School Violence: A
Proposed Prevention Program. NASSP Bulletin, 38-47.
Bandura, A. (1979). Social learning theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Beck, A. T., & Steer, R. A. (1993). Manual for the Beck Depression Inventory. San
Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.
Berthold, K. A., & Hoover, J. H. (2000). Correlates of bullying and victimization among
intermediate students in the Midwestern US. School Psychology International,
21(1), 65-78.
Bertschinger, E., Olbrich, E., Ay, N., & Jost, J. (2006). Autonomy: an information-
theoretic perspective. Paper presented at the ALife X conference. Retrieved from
http://www.santafe.edu/research/publications/workingpapers/06-10-035.pdf
261
Birch, S. H., & Ladd, G. W. (1997). The teacher-child relationship and childrens' early
school adjustment. Journal of School Psychology, 35, 1-17.
Bjorkqvist, K., Lagerspetz, K. M. J., & Kaukiainen, A. (1992). Do girls manipulate and
boys fight? Development trends in regards to direct and indirect aggression.
Aggressive Behavior, 18, 117-127.
Black, S. A., & Jackson, E. (2007). Using bullying incident density to evaluate the
Olweus bullying programme. School Psychology International, 28(5), 623-638.
Blackburn, J. A., Dulmus, C. N., Theriot, M. T., & Sowers, K. M. (2004). Kids-parents-
school personnel: Few are talking and even less are listening Journal of Evidence-
Based Social Work, 1(2/3), 45-57.
Blagasklonny, M. V., & Pardee, A. B. (2002). Conceptual Biology: Unearthing the gems.
Nature, 416, 373.
Borsboom, D., Mellenbergh, G. J., & van Heerden, J. (2003). The theoretical status of
latent variables. Psychological Review, 110(2), 203-219.
Bosworth, K., Espelage, D. L., & Simon, T. (1999). Factors associated with bullying
behavior in middle school students. Journal of Early Adolescence, 19(3), 341-
362.
Bowers, L., P.K., S., & Binney, V. (1994). Perceived Family Relationships of Bullies,
Victims and Bully/Victims in Middle Childhood. Journal of Social and Personal
Relationships, 11(2), 215-232.
Bowlby, J. (1999). Attachment and Loss (2 ed. Vol. 1). New York: Basic Books.
Brown, S., & Taylor, K. (2008). Bullying, education and earnings: Evidence from the
National Child Development Study. Economics of Education Review, 27, 387-
401.
Brunstein Klomek, A., Marrocco, F., Kleinman, M., Schonfeld, I. S., & Gould, M. S.
(2008). Peer victimization, depression, and suicidality in adolescents. Suicide and
life-threatening behavior, 38(2), 166-180.
Buhs, E. S., & Ladd, G. W. (2001). Peer rejection as an antecedent of young children's
school adjustment: An examination of mediating processes. Developmental
Psychology, 37(4), 550-560.
263
Buhs, E. S., Ladd, G. W., & Herald, S. L. (2006). Peer exclusion and victimization:
Processes that mediate the relation between peer group rejection and children's
classroom engagement and achievement? Journal of Educational Psychology,
98(1), 1-13.
Burger, P., & Luckman, T. (1966). The social construction of reality. Garden City, NY:
Doubleday.
Burt, K. B., Obradovic, J., Long, J. D., & Masten, A. S. (2008). The interplay of social
competence and psychopathology over 20 years: Testing transactional and
cascade models. Child Development, 79(2), 359-374.
Bybee, J. S. (2002). Memorandum for John Rizzo Acting General Counsel of the Central
Intelligence Agency.
Carney, J. (2000). Bullied to death: Perceptions of peer abuse and suicidal behavior
during adolescence. School Psychology International, 25(2), 213–223.
Caspi, A., Moffitt, T., Entner Wright, B., & Silva, P. (1998). Early failure in the labor
market: Childhood and adolescent predictors of unemployment in the transition
to adulthood. American Sociological Review, 63, 424-451.
Cassidy, J., & Asher, S. (1992). Loneliness and Social Dissatisfaction Questionnaire. In
G. Kogan (Ed.). Croton-on-Hudson, NY.
Catalano, R. F., Haggerty, K. P., Oesterle, S., Fleming, C. B., & Hawkins, J. D. (2004).
The importance of bonding to school for healthy development: Findings from the
Social Development Research Group. Journal of School Health, 74(7), 252-261.
Causey, D. L., & Dubow, E. F. (1992). Development of a self-report coping measure for
elementary school children. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 21(1), 47-59.
264
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (1998). Youth risk behavior surveillance-US
1997. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report, 47(SS-3).
Chen, X., Chang, L., Liu, H., & He, Y. (2008). Effects of the peer group on the
development of social functioning and academic achievement: A longitudinal
study in Chinese children. Child Development, 79(5), 239-251.
Chinn, S. (2000). A simple method for converting an odds ratio to effect size for use in
meta-analysis. Statistics in Medicine, 19, 3127-3131.
Clarke, E. A., & Kiselica, M. S. (1997). A systemic counseling approach to the problem
of bullying. Elementary School Guidance and Counseling, 31, 310-324.
Coie, J., Dodge, K., Terry, R., & Wright, V. (1991). The role of aggression in peer
relations: An analysis of aggression episodes in boys’ play groups. Child
Development, 62, 812-826.
Coie, J., Lochman, J., Terry, R., & Hyman, C. (1992). Predicting early adolescent
disorder from childhood aggression and peer rejection. Journal of Consulting and
Clinical Psychology, 60(5), 783-792.
Cole-Hamilton, I., Harrop, A., & Street, C. (2005). Making the Case for Play: Gathering
the Evidence. Children, Youth and Environments, 15(2). Retrieved from
http://thunder1.cudenver.edu/cye_journal/review.pl?n=36
265
Cook, T. D. (2002). Advanced statistics: Up with odds ratios! A case for odds ratios
when outcomes are common. Academy of Emergency Medicine, 9(12), 1430-
1434.
Cowie, H., Naylor, P., Rivers, I., Smith, P. K., & Pereira, B. (2002). Measuring
workplace bullying. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 7(1), 33-51.
Craig, W. M., & Pepler, D. J. (2003). Identifying and targeting risk for involvement in
bullying and victimization. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 48(9), 577-582.
Craig, W. M., Pepler, D. J., & Blais, J. (2007). Responding to bullying: What works?
School Psychology International, 28(4), 465-477.
Crain, M. M., Finch, C. L., & Foster, S. L. (2005). The relevance of the social
information processing model for understanding relational aggression in girls.
Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 51(2), 213-249.
Crick, N. R. (1996). The role of overt aggression, relational aggression, and prosocial
behavior in the prediction of children’s future social adjustment. Child
Development, 67, 2317–2327.
Crick, N. R., Casas, J. F., & Mosher, M. (1997). Relational and overt aggression in
preschool. Developmental Psychology, 33(4), 579-588.
Crick, N. R., & Dodge, K. (1994). A review and reformulation of social information
processing mechanisms in children’s development. Psychological Bulletin, 114,
74-110.
Crick, N. R., & Grotpeter, J. K. (1995). Relational aggression, gender and social-
psychological adjustment. Child Development, 66, 710-722.
Crick, N. R., Grotpeter, J. K., & Bigbee, M. A. (2002). Relationally and physically
aggressive children's intent attributions and feelings of distress for relational and
instrumentla peer provocations. Child Development, 73(4), 1134-1142.
Crothers, L. M., Field, J. E., & Kolbert, J. B. (2005). Navigating power, control and being
nice: Aggression in adolescent girls' friendships. Journal of Counseling &
Development, 83, 349-354.
Currie, C., Gabhainn, S. N., Godeau, E., Roberts, C., Smith, R., Currie, D., et al. (2008).
Inequalities in young people's health: HBSC International report from the
2005/2006 survey. Edindburgh, Scotland: The University of Edinburgh.
Davies, B. (1982). Life in the classroom and playground: The accounts of primary school
primary school children. Boston: Routledge & Kegan Paul.
Derrida, J. (1974). Of grammatology, publ. s., 1974. Baltimore, Md: John Hopkins
University Press.
Derrida, J. (1988). Letter to a Japanese friend (A. Benjamin & D. Wood, Trans.). In D.
Wood & R. Bernasconi (Eds.), Derrida and differance. Evanston, Il: Northwest
University Press.
267
DeVoe, J. F., Peter, K., Kaufman, P., Miller, A., Noonan, M., Snyder, T. D., et al. (2004).
Indicators of school crime and safety: 2004.
Dijkstra, J. K., Lindenberg, S., & Veenstra, R. (2007). Same-gender and cross-gender
peer acceptance and peer rejection and their relation to bullying and helping
among preadolescents: Comparing predictions from gender-homophily and goal-
framing approaches Developmental Psychology, 43(6), 1377-1389.
Dixon, R., Smith, P. K., & Jenks, C. (2004). Managing difference and managing
bullying: A case study of one secondary school. Journal of School Violence, 3(4),
41-58.
Dodge, K. A., & Coie, J. D. (1987). Teacher rating of depression. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 53, 1146-1156.
Dodge, K. A., & Frame, C. (1982). Social cognitive biases and deficits in aggressive
boys. Child Development, 53, 620-635.
Ducharme, J. M., Folino, A., & DeRosie, J. (2008). Errorless acquiescence training: A
potential "keystone" approach to building peer interaction skills in children with
severe problem behavior. Behavior Modification, 32(1), 39-60.
Due, P., Holstein, B. E., Lynch, J., Diderichsen, F., Gabhain, S. N., Scheidt, P., et al.
(2005). Bullying and symptoms among school-aged children: International
comparative cross sectional study in 28 countries. European Journal of Public
Health, 15(2), 128-132.
Egan, S., & Perry, D. (1998). Does low self-regard invite victimization? Development
Psychology, 14(2), 299-399.
268
Ellis, W. E., & Zarbatany, L. (2007). Explaining friendship formation and friendship
stability. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 53(1), 79-104.
Engels, F. (1954). Dialectics of nature (C. Dutt, Trans.). Moscow: Foreign Languages
Publishing House.
Espelage, D. L., Bosworth, K., & Simon, T. R. (2001). Short-term stability and
prospective correlates of bullying in middle-school students: An examination of
potential demographic, psychosocial and environmental influences. Violence and
Victims, 16(4), 411-426.
Estell, D. B., Farmer, T. W., & Cairns, B. D. (2007). Bullies and victims in rural African
American youth: Behavioral characteristics and social network placement.
Aggressive Behavior, 33, 145-159.
Finkelhor, D., Ormrod, R., & Turner, H. A. (2007b). Polyvictimization and trauma in a
national longitudinal cohort. Development and Psychopathology, 19(1), 149-166.
Finkelhor, D., Ormrod, R., Turner, H. A., & Hamby, S. L. (2005a). Child Maltreatment,
10(1), 5-25.
Finkelhor, D., Ormrod, R., Turner, H. A., & Hamby, S. L. (2005b). Measuring poly-
victimization using the Juvenile Victimization Scale. Child Abuse and Neglect,
29, 1297-1312.
Fook, J. (1993). Radical casework: A theory of practice. St. Leonards, Australia: Allen
and Unwin.
Fook, J. (2002). Social Work: Critical theory and practice. London: Sage.
Forero, R., McLellan, L., Rissel, C., & Bauman, A. (1999). Bullying behaviour and
psychosocial health among school children in New South Wales, Australia: Cross
sectional survey. British Medical Journal, 319, 344-348.
Foucault, M. (1977a). Discipline and punish : the birth of the prison. New York:
Pantheon Books.
270
Foucault, M. (Ed.). (1997). Power. Essential Works of Foucault 1954 – 1984. London:
Penguin.
Frey, K. S., Hirschstein, M. K., Snell, J. L., Van Schoiack Edstrom, L., MacKenzie, E. P.,
& Broderick, C. J. (2005). Reducing playground bullying and supporting beliefs:
An experimental trial of the Steps to Respect program. Developmental
Psychology, 41(3), 479-491.
Frost, J., & Jacobs, P. (1995). Play deprivation: A factor in juvenile violence. Dimensions
of Early Childhood, 23, 14-17.
Gamson, W. (1982). Encounters with unjust authority. Homewood, Illinois: The Dorsey
Press.
Garbarino, J., & Bedard, C. (2002). Parents under siege: Why you are the solution, not
the problem in your child’s life. New York Simon and Shuster.
George, J. M., & Gareth, J. M. (2000). The role of time in theory and theory building
Journal of Management, 26(4), 657-684.
Giddens, A. (1995). Politics, sociology, and social theory: Encounters with classical and
contemporary social thought. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
Gifford-Smith, M., & Rabiner, D. L. (2004). Social information processing and children’s
social competence: A review of the literature. In J. B. Kupersmidt & K. A. Dodge
(Eds.), Children’s peer relations: From development to intervention to policy.
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Glass, G. V., McGaw, B., & Smith, M. L. (1981). Meta-analysis in social research.
Thousand Oaks: CA: Sage.
Glew, G. M., Fan, M.-Y., Katon, W., & Rivara, F. P. (2008). Bullying and school safety.
Journal of Pediatrics, 152, 123-128.
Glew, G. M., Fan, M., Katon, W., Rivara, F. P., & Kernic, M. A. (2005). Bullying,
psychosocial adjustment, and academic performance in elementary school.
Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine, 159, 1026–1031.
Gottgtroy, P., Kasabov, N., & MacDonell, S. (2003). Building evolving ontology maps
for data mining and knowledge discovery in biomedical informatics. Knowledge
Engineering and Discovery Institute, Auckland University of Technology.
Retrieved from
http://www.aut.ac.nz/resources/research/research_institutes/kedri/downloads/pdf/
gottgtroybsmtp.pdf
Gourevitch, P. N. Y. (1999). We wish to inform you that tomorrow we will be killed with
our families: Stories from Rwanda. New York: Farar, Straus & Giroux.
Graham, S., & Juvonen, J. (1998). Self-blame and peer victimization in middle school:
An attributional analysis. Developmental Psychology, 34(3), 587-599.
Grills, A. E., & Ollendick, T. H. (2002). Peer victimization, global self-worth, and
anxiety in middle school children. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent
Psychology, 31(1), 59-68.
Guittierrez, L., & Ortega, A. (1991). Developing methods to empower Latinos: The
importance of groups. Social Work with Groups, 14(2), 23-43.
Habermas, J. (1971). Knowledge and human interests (J. Shapiro, Trans.). Boston:
Beacon Press.
Habermas, J. (1979). Communication and the Evolution of Society (T. McCarthy, Trans.).
London: Heinemann.
Habermas, J. (1984). The theory of communicative action. Reason and the rationalization
of society (T. McCarthy, Trans. Vol. 1). Boston: Beacon.
Habermas, J. (1992). Political experience and the renewal of Marxist theory. In P. Dews
(Ed.), Autonomy and solidarity: Interviews with Jurgen Habermas. New York:
Verso.
Hanish, L. D., & Guerra, N. G. (2004). Aggressive victims, passive victims, and bullies:
Developmental continuity or developmental change? Merrill-Palmer Quarterly,
50(1), 17-38.
274
Hanish, L. D., & Guerra., N. G. (2000). Predictors of peer victimization among urban
youth. Social Development, 9(4), 521-543.
Harter, S. (1999). The construction of the self: A developmental perspective. New York
Guilford Press.
Hawker, D. S. J., & Boulton, M. J. (2000). Twenty years' research on peer victimization
and psychosocial maladjustment: A meta-analytic review of cross-sectional
studies. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41(4), 441-455.
Haynes, S. N., & Hayes O'Brien, W. (2000). Principles and practice of behavioral
assessment. New York: Kluwer/Plennum.
Haynie, D. L., Nansel, T., Eitel, P., Crump, A. D., Saylor, K., Yu, K., et al. (2001).
Bullies, victims and bully/victims: Distinct groups of at-risk youth. Journal of
Early Adolescence, 21(1), 29-49.
Hazler, R. J., Hoover, J. H., & Oliver, R. (1992). What kids say about bullying. The
Executive Educator, 20-22.
Healy, K. (2000). Critical Social Work: What is it? Critical Social Work, 2(1).
Heath, J. (2001). Communicative action and rational choice. Cambridge: MIT Press.
Hegar, R. L., & Yungman, J. J. (1989). Toward a causal typology of child neglect.
children and Youth Services Review, 11, 203-220.
Heinemann, P.-P. (1972.). Mobbning. Grupp'dld bland burn ocltyrrxna (cited in Pikas,
1975). Stockholm: Natur och kultur.
Hinde, R. A. (1976). Interactions, relationships and social structure Man, 2(1), 1-17.
275
Hodges, E. V. E., Malone, M., & Perry, D. (1997). Individual risk and social risk as
interacting determinants of victimization in the peer group. Developmental
Psychology, 33, 94-101.
Hodges, E. V. E., & Perry, D. G. (1999). Personal and interpersonal antecedents and
consequences of victimization by peers. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 76(4), 677-685.
Hoel, H., Rayner, C., & Cooper, C. L. (1999). Workplace bullying. International Review
of Industrial and Organisational Psychology, 14, 195-230.
Holt, M. K., Finkelhor, D., & Kaufman Kantor, G. (2007). Multiple victimization
experiences of urban elementary school students: Associatoins with psychosocial
functioning and academic performance. Child Abuse and Neglect, 31, 503-515.
Hunter, S. C., Boyle, J. M. E., & Warden, D. (2007). Perceptions and correlates of peer
victimization and bullying. British Journal of Educational Research, 77, 797-810.
Ialongo, N., Vaden-Kiernan, N., & Kellam, S. (1998). Early peer rejection and
aggression: Longitudinal relations with adolescent behavior. Journal of
Development and Physical Disabilities, 10(2).
Ife, J. (1997). Rethinking social work: Towards critical practice. Melbourne: Longman.
276
Janssen, I., Craig, W. M., Boyce, W. F., & Pickett, W. (2004). Associations between
overweight and obesity with bullying behaviors in school-aged children.
Pediatrics, 113(5), 1187-1194.
Jay, M. (1973). The dialectical imagination: A history of the Frankfurt School and the
Institute of Social Research 1923-1950. Boston: Little, Brown & Company.
Jobe, L. E., & Williams White, S. (2007). Loneliness, social relationships, and a broader
autism phenotype in college students. Personality and Individual Difference,
42(8), 1479-1489.
Juvonen, J., Nishina, A., & Graham, S. (2000). Peer harassment, psychological
adjustment, and school functioning in early adolescence. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 92(2), 349-359.
Kaltiala-Heino, R., Rimpela, M., Marttunen, M., Rimpela, A., & Rantanen, P. (1999).
Bullying, depression, and suicidal ideation in Finnish adolescents. British Medical
Journal, 319, 348-351.
Kaplan, A. (1964). The conduct of inquiry; methodology for behavioral science. San
Francisco: Chandler Pub. Co.
Keltner, D., Capps, L., Kring, A. M., Young, R. C., & Heerey, E. A. (2001). Just teasing:
A conceptual analysis and empirical review. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 229-
248.
Kim, S. Y., & Leventhal, B. (2008). Bullying and suicide: A review. International
Journal of Adolescent Mental Health, 20(2), 133-154.
Kowalski, R. M., & Limber, S. P. (2007). Electronic bullying among middle school
students. Journal of Adolescent Health 41, S22-S30.
Kumpulainen, K., Räsänen, E., Henttonen, I., Almqvist, F., Kresanov, K., Linna, S., et al.
(1998). Bullying and psychiatric symptoms among elementary school-age
children. Child Abuse and Neglect, 22, 705-717.
Kumpulainen, K., Rasanen, E., & Puura, K. (2001). Psychiatric disorders and the use of
mental health services among children involved in bullying. Aggressive Behavior,
27, 102-110.
Ladd, G. W., & Burgess, K. (2001). Do Relational Risks and Protective Factors Moderate
the Linkages Between Childhood Aggression and Early Psychological and School
Adjustment
Child Development 72(5), 1579-1601.
Ladd, G. W., Kochenderfer, B. J., & Coleman, C. C. (1997). Classroom peer acceptance,
friendship, and victimization: Distinct relational systems that contribute uniquely
to children's school adjustment? Child Development, 68(6), 1181-1197.
Ladd, G. W., & Profilet, S. M. (1996). The Child Behavior Scale: A teacher-report
measure of young children's aggression, withdrawn and pro-social behaviors.
Developmental Psychology, 32, 1008-1024.
Ladd, G. W., & Troop Gordon, W. (2003). The role of chronic peer difficulties in the
development of children’s psychological adjustment problems
Child Development, 74(5), 1344-1367.
Ladd, G. W., & Troop Gordon, W. (2005). Trajectories of peer victimization and
perceptions of the self and schoolmates: Precursors to internalizing and
externalizing problems. Child Development, 76(5), 1072 - 1091.
Lazarsfeld, P. F., & Henry, N. W. (1968). Latent Structure Analysis. . Boston: Houghton
Mifflin.
279
Lazarsfeld, P. F., & Rosenberg, M. (Eds.). (1955). The Language of Social Research: A
reader in the methodology of social research. New York: The Free Press.
Leary, T. (1956). A manual for the use of the interpersonal system of personality.
Berkeley, CA: Psychological Consultation Service.
Lever, J. (1978). Sex differences in the complexity of children's play and games.
American Sociological Review, 43, 471-483.
Lewis, M. W., & Grimes, A. J. (1999). Metatriangulation: Building theory from multiple
paradigms. Metatriangulation: Building theory from multiple paradigms, 24(4),
672-690.
Limber, S. P., & Small, M. A. (2003). State Laws and Policies to Address Bullying in
Schools. Child Development, 322.
Limper, R. (2000). Cooperation between parents, teachers and school boards to prevent
bullying in education: An overview of work done in the Netherlands. Aggressive
Behavior, 26, 124-134.
280
Loeber, R., & Dishion, T. J. (1984). Boys who fight at home and school: Family
conditions influencing cross-setting consistency. journal of Consulting and
Clinical Psychology, 52, 759-768.
Long, J. D., & Pellegrini, A. D. (2003). Studying change in dominance and bullying with
linear mixed models. School Psychology International, 32(3), 401-417.
Lopez, C., & DuBois, D. L. (2005). Peer victimization and rejection: Investigation of an
integrative model of effects on emotional, behavioral, and academic adjustment in
early adolescence. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 34(1),
25-36.
Lowenstein, L. F. (1978). Who is the bully? Bulletin of the British Psychological Society,
31, 147-149.
Luhman, N. (1990). Political theory in the welfare state. New York: DeGreyter.
Luthar, C., & Cicchetti, D. (2000). The construct of resilience: Implications for
interventions and social policy. Development and Psychopathology, 72, 857-885.
Macklem, G. L. (2003). Bullying and power: Social teasing in children's group. New
York: Kluwer/Plennum.
March, J. S., Parker, J. D. A., Sullivan, K., Stallings, P., & Conners, C. K. (1997). The
Multidimensional Anxiety Scales for Children (MASC): Factor structure,
281
Margolin, G., & Gordis, E. B. (2000). Effects of family and community violence on
children. Annual Review of Psychology, 51, 445-479.
Marx, K., & Engel, F. (1955). The communist manifesto with selections from the
Eighteenth Brumaire of Louis Bonaparte and Capital. Arlington Heights, Illinois:
Harlan Davidson.
McCarthy, T. (1984). Translators' note. The theory of communicative action. Reason and
the rationalization of society (Vol. 1). Boston: Beacon.
McGrath, M. J. (2007). School bullying: Tools for avoiding harm and liability. Thousand
Oaks, CA: Sage
McKinney, J. C. (1966). Constructive Typology and Social Theory. New York: Appleton-
Century-Croft.
Miller, J., Martin, I., & Schamess, G. (2003). School violence and children in crisis:
Community and school interventions for social workers and counselors. Denver:
Love Publising.
Miller, J., & Vaillancourt, T. (2007). The relation between childhood peer victimization
and adult perfectionism: Are victims of indirect aggression more perfectionistic?
Aggressive Behavior, 33, 1-12.
Mishna, F. (2003). Peer victimization: The case for social work intervention. Families in
Society, 84(4), 512-520.
Mohr, A. (2006). Family variables associated with peer victimization: Does family
violence enhance the probability of being victimized by peers? Swiss Journal of
Psychology, 65(2), 107-116.
Moran, S., Smith, P. K., Thompson, D., & Whitney, I. (1993). Ethnic differences in
experience of bullying: Asian and white children. British Journal of Educational
Psychology, 63, 431-440.
Morrow, M. T., Hubbard, J. A., Rubin, R. M., & McAuliffe, M. D. (2008). The relation
between childhood aggression and depressive symptoms. Merrill-Palmer
Quarterly, 54(3), 316-340.
283
Mouttapa, M., Valente, T., Gallaher, P., Rohrbach, L. A., & Unger, J. B. (2004). Social
network predictors of bullying and victimization. Adolescence, 39(154), 315-335.
Mullaly, B. (1997). Structural social work: Ideology, theory, and practice. Don Mills,
Ontario: Oxford University Press.
Mynard, H., Joseph, S., & Alexander, J. (2000). Peer-victimisation and posttraumatic
stress in adolescents. Personality and Individual Differences, 29, 815-821.
Mynard, H., Lawrence, H., & Joseph, S. (2000). Development of the multidimensional
peer-victimization scale. Aggressive Behavior
26, 169-178.
Mytton, J., DiGuiseppi, C., Gough, D., Taylor, R., & Logan, S. (2006). School-based
secondary prevention programmes for preventing violence. Cochrane Database of
Systematic Reviews, 3,
Nansel, T. R., Overpeck, M., Pilla, R. S., Ruan, W. J., Simons-Morton, B., & Scheidt, P.
(2001). Bullying behaviors among U.S. youth: Prevalence and association with
psychosocial adjustment. Journal of the American Medical Association, 285(16),
2094-2100.
Nansel, T. R., Overpeck, M. D., Haynie, D. L., Ruan, W. J., & Scheidt, P. C. (2003).
Relationships between bullying and violence among U.S. youth. Archives of
Pediatric Adolescent Medicine, 157, 348-353.
Neary, A., & Joseph, S. (1994). Peer victimization and its relationship to self-concept and
depression among Australian schoolgirls. Personality and Individual Difference,
16, 183-186.
Newman, K., Fox, C., Harding, D., Mehta, J., & Roth, W. (2004). Rampage: The social
roots of school shootings. New York: Basic Books.
Nishina, A., Juvonen, J., & Witkow, M. R. (2005). Sticks and stones may break my
bones, but names will make me feel sick: The psychosocial, somatic, and
scholastic consequences of peer harassment. Journal of Clinical Child and
Adolescent Psychology, 34(1), 37-48.
284
Nylund, K., Nishina, A., Bellmore, A., & Graham, S. (2007). Subtypes, severity, and
structural stability of peer victimization: What does latent class analysis say?
Child Development, 78(6), 1706-1722.
O’Connell, P., Pepler, D. J., & Craig, W. M. (1999). Peer involvement in bullying:
Insights and challenges for intervention. Journal of Adolescence, 22, 437-452.
O’Moore, M., & Kirkham, C. (2001). Self-esteem and its relationship to bullying
behavior. Aggressive Behavior, 27, 269–283.
Office of United States Chief of Counsel. (1946). Nazi conspiracy and aggression /Office
of United States Chief of Counsel for Prosecution of Axis Criminality.
Olweus, D. (1978). Aggression in the schools: Bullies and whipping boys. New York:
Wiley.
Omigbodun, O., Dogra, N., Esan, O., & Adedokun, B. (2008). Prevalence and correlates
of suicidal behaviour among adolescents in southwest Nigeria. International
Journal of Social Psychiatry, 54(1), 34-46.
285
Owens, L., Shute, R., & Slee, P. (2000). "Guess what I just heard!": Indirect aggression
among teenage girls in Australia. Aggressive Behavior, 26, 67-83.
Owens, L., Slee, P., & Shute, R. (2000). "It hurts a hell of a lot...": The effects of indirect
aggression on teenage girls. School Psychology International, 214(359-376.).
Painter, J. E., Borba, C. P. C., Hynes, M., Mays, D., & Glanz, K. (2008). The use of
theory in health behavior research from 2000 to 2005: A systematic review.
Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 35, 358-362.
Parker, G., Wilhelm, K., & Asghari, A. (1997). Early onset depression: the relevance of
anxiety Journal Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 32(1), 30-37.
Peets, K., Hodges, E. V. E., Kikas, E., & Salmivalli, C. (2007). Hostile attributions and
behavioral strategies in children: Does relationship type matter? Developmental
Psychology, 43, 889-900.
Pellegrini, A. D., Bartini, M., & Brooks, F. (1999). School bullies, victims, and
aggressive victims. Factors relating to group affiliation and victimization in early
adolescence. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91(2), 216–224.
286
Pellegrini, A. D., Blatchford, P., Kato, K., & Baines, E. (2004). A short-term longitudinal
study of children's playground games in primary school: Implications for
adjustment to school and social adjustment in the USA and the UK. Social
Development, 13(1), 107-123.
Pellegrini, A. D., & Smith, P. K. (1998). Physical activity play: The nature and funciton
of a neglected aspect of play. Child Development, 69(3), 577-598.
Pepler, D. J., & Craig, W. M. (1995). A peek behind the fence: naturalistic observations
of aggressive children with remote audiovisual recording. Developmental
Psychology, 314, 548-553.
Pepler, D. J., Craig, W. M., Ziegler, S., & Charach, A. (1994). An evaluation of an anti-
bullying intervention in Toronto schools. Canadian Journal of Community Mental
Health, 13(2), 95-110.
Percy-Smith, B., & Matthews, H. (2001). Tyrannical Spaces: young people, bullying and
urban neighbourhoods. Local Environment: The International Journal of Justice
and Sustainability, 6(1), 49 - 63.
Perren, S., & Hornung, R. (2005). Bullying and delinquency in adolescence: Victims’ and
perpetrators’ family and peer relations. Swiss Journal of Psychology 64(1).
Perry, D., Williard, J., & Perry, L. (1990). Peers’ perceptions of the consequences that
victimized children provide peer aggressors. Child Development, 61, 1310-1325.
Peskin, M. F. (2007). Bullying and victimization and internalizing symptoms among low-
income Black and Hispanic students. Journal of Adolescent Health, 40, 372-372.
Petersen, L., & Rigby, K. (1999). Countering bullying at an Australian secondary school.
Journal of Adolescence, 22(4), 481-492.
Petticrew, M., & Roberts, H. (2006). Systematic reviews in the social sciences: A
practical guide. Oxford: Blackwell Publishing.
Pikas, A. (1975). A treatment of mobbing in schools: Principles for and the results of the
work of an anti-mobbing group. Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research,
19, 1-12.
Price, R. (2007). The way things look. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Rasmussen, D. M., & Swindal, J. (Eds.). (2002). Jurgen Habermas (Vol. 2). Thousand
Oaks, CA: Sage.
Rasmussen, D. M., & Swindal, J. (Eds.). (2002). Jurgen Habermas (Vol. 1). Thousand
Oaks, CA: Sage.
Rigby, K. (1998). Suicidal ideation and bullying among Australian secondary school
children. Australian Educational and Developmental Psychologist, 15(1).
Rigby, K. (1999). Peer victimization at school and the health of secondary school
students. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 69, 95-104.
Rigby, K. (2000). Effects of peer victimization in schools and perceived social support on
adolescent well-being. Journal of Adolescence, 23, 57-68.
Rigby, K. (2003). Addressing bullying in schools: Theory and practice. Trends and Issues
in Criminal Justice, 259, 1-6.
Rigby, K., & Slee, P. (2001). Dimensions of interpersonal relation among Australian
children and implications for psychological well-being. Journal of Social
Psychology, 133(1), 33-42.
Ritzer, G., & Smart, B. (Eds.). (2001). Handbook of social theory. . Thousand Oaks, CA:
Sage Publications.
Rivers, I., Duncan, N., & Besag, V. E. (2007). Bullying: A handbook for educators and
parents. Westport, CN: Greenwood/Prager.
Rodkin, P., & Hodges, E. V. E. (2003). Bullies and victims in the peer ecology: Four
questions for psychologists and school professionals. School Psychology Review,
32(2), 384-400.
Rodkin, P. C., Farmer, T. W., Pearl, R., & Van Acker, R. (2000). Heterogeneity of
popular boys: Antisocial and prosocial configurations. Developmental
Psychology, 36(1), 14-24.
Rothstein, H., Sutton, A., & Borenstein, M. (Eds.). (2005). Publication bias in meta-
analysis: Prevention, assessment and adjustments. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley &
Sons.
Rubin, K. H., Fein, G., & Vandenberg, B. (1983). Play. In E. M. Hetheringon (Ed.),
Handbook of child psychology: Socialization, personality and social development
(Vol. 4). New York: Wiley.
289
Rubin, K. H., Maioni, T. L., & Hornung, M. (1976). Free play behaviors in middle and
lower-class preschoolers: Parten and Piaget revisited. Child Development, 47,
414-419.
Ruff, C. (2003). Growth in bone strength, body size, and muscle size in a juvenile
longitudinal sample. Bone, 33, 317-329.
Salmivalli, C., & Helteenvuori, T. (2007). Reactive, but not proactive aggression predicts
victimization among boys. Aggressive Behavior, 33(3), 198-206.
Salmivalli, C., Lagerspetz, K. M. J., Björkqvist, K., Österman, K., & Kaukiainen, A.
(1996). Bullying as a group process: Participant roles and their relations to social
status within the group. Aggressive Behavior, 22, 1-15.
Salmon, G. M., James, A., & Smith, D. M. (1998). Bullying in schools: Self-reported
anxiety, depression and self-esteem in secondary school children. British Medical
Journal, 317, 924-925.
Schmidt, M. E., & Bagwell, C. L. (2007). The Protective Role of Friendships in Overtly
and Relationally Victimized Boys and Girls. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 53(3),
439-460.
Schwartz, D., Dodge, K., H., & Coie, J. (1993). The emergence of chronic peer
victimization in boys’ play groups. Child Development, 64(6), 1755-1772.
Schwartz, D., Farver, J. M., Chang, L., & Lee-Shim, Y. (2002). Victimization in South
Korean children's peer groups. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 30, 113-
125.
Schwartz, D., Gorman, A., Duong, M., & Nakamoto, J. (2008). Peer relationships and
academic achievement as interacting predictors of depressive symptoms during
middle childhood. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 117(2), 289-299.
Schwartz, D., Gorman, A. H., Dodge, K., A., Petit, G. S., & Bates, J. E. (2008).
Friendships with peers who are low or high in aggression as moderators of the
link between peer victimization and declines in academic functioning. Journal of
Abnormal Child Psychology, 36(5), 719-730.
Schwartz, D., Hopmeyer Gorman, A., Nakamoto, J., & Toblin, R. L. (2005). Victimizatin
in the peer group and children's academic functioning. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 97(3), 425-435.
Schwartz, D., & Proctor, L. (2000). Community violence exposure and children's social
adjustment in the school peer group: The mediating roles of emotion regulation
and social cognition. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 68, 670-683.
Schwartz, D., Proctor, L., & Chien, D. (2001). The aggressive victim of bullying:
Emotional and behavioral dysregulation as a pathway to victimization by peers In
J. Juvonen & S. Graham (Eds.), School-based peer harassment: The plight of the
vulnerable and victimized. Boston, MA: Guilford Press.
Seals, D., & Young, J. (2003). Bullying and victimization: Prevalence and relationship to
gender, grade level, ethnicity, self-esteem and depression. Adolescence, 38(152),
735-747.
291
Search Institute. (2007). What kids need: Developmental assets. Retrieved July, 2004,
from http://www.search-institute.org/developmental-assets
Segrin, C. (2000). Social skills deficits associated with depression. Clinical Psychology
Review, 20(3), 379-403.
Shaffer, D., Fisher, P., Lucas, C. P., Dulcan, M. K., & Schwab-Stone, M. E. (2000).
NIMH Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children Version IV: Description,
differences from previous versions, and reliability of some common diagnoses.
Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 39, 28-38.
Shapiro, D. (1981). Autonomy and rigid character. New York: Beacon Books.
Sharp, S., Thompson, D., Arora, C., & Cho, S. (2000). How long before it hurts? An
investigation into long-term bullying. School Psychology International, 21(1), 37-
46.
Shellard, E., & Turner, J. R. (2004). Safe and Secure Schools. ERS Focus On . Arlington,
VA: Educational Research Service.
Simmons, R. (2000). Odd girl out: The hidden culture of aggression in girls. New York:
Harcourt.
Slee, P. (1994). Situational and interpersonal correlates of anxiety associated with peer
victimisation. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 25(2), 97–107.
292
Smith, J. D., Schneider, B. H., Smith, P. K., & Ananiadou, K. (2004). The effectiveness
of whole-school anti-bullying programs: A synthesis of evaluation research.
School Psychology International, 33(4), 547-560.
Smith, J. D., Stewart, R., & Cousins, J. B. (2003). Anti-bullying interventions: A survey
of Ontario schools. The CAP Journal, 12(1), 9-11.
Smith, P. K., & Boulton, M. J. (1990). Rough and tumble play, aggression and
dominance: Perception and behavior in children’s encounters. Human
Development, 33, 271-282.
Smith, P. K., & Brain, P. (2000). Bullying in schools: Lessons from two decades of
research. Aggressive Behavior, 26, 1-9.
Smith, P. K., Cowie, H., Olafsson, R. F., & Liefooghe, A. P. D. (2002). Definitions of
bullying: A comparison of terms used, and age and gender differences, in a
fourteen-country international comparison. Child Development, 73(4), 1119–
1133.
Smith, P. K., & Levan, S. (1995). Perceptions and experiences of bullying in younger
pupils. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 65(4), 489-500.
Smith, P. K., Madsen, K., & Moody, J. (1999). What causes the age decline in reports of
being bullied at school? Towards a developmental analysis of risks of being
bullied. Educational Research, 41, 267-285.
Smith, P. K., Morita, Y., Junger-Tas, J., Olweus, D., Catalano, R., & Slee, P. (1999). The
nature of school bullying: A cross national perspective. London: Routledge.
Smith, P. K., Pepler, D. J., & Rigby, K. (Eds.). (2004). Bullying in schools: How
successful can interventions be? Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
293
Smith, P. K., & Sharp, S. (1994). School bullying: Insights and perspectives. London:
Routledge.
Smith, P. K., Talamelli, L., Cowie, H., Naylor, P., & Chauhan, P. (2004). Profiles of non-
victims, escaped victims and new victims of school bullying. British Journal of
Educational Psychology, 74, 565-581.
Sourander, A., Jensen, P., Ronning, J. A., Niemala, S., Helenius, H., Sillanmaki, L., et al.
(2007). What Is the early adulthood outcome of boys who bully or are bullied in
childhood? The Finnish "From a Boy to a Man" study. Pediatrics Vol. 120 No. 2
August 2007, pp. 397-404, 120(2), 397-404.
Spinrad, T. L., Eisenberg, N., Harris, E., Hanish, L. D., Fabes, R. A., Kupanoff, K., et al.
(2004). The relation of childrens everyday nonsocial peer play behavior to their
emotionality, regulation and social functioning. Developmental Psychology,
40(1), 67-80.
Spriggs, A. L., Iannotti, R. J., Nansel, T. R., & Haynie, D. L. (2007). Adolescent bullying
involvement and perceived family, peer and school relations: Commonalities and
differences across race/ethnicity. Journal of Adolescent Health, 41(3), 283-293.
Srabstein, J. C., McCarter, R. J., Cheng, S., & Huang, Z. J. (2006). Morbidities associated
with bullying behaviors in adolescents: School-based study of American
adolescents. International Journal of Adolescent Mental Health, 18(4), 587-596.
Stack, C. B. (1975). All our kin: Strategies for survival in a Black community. New York:
Harper & Row.
Stanley, L., & Arora, C. (1998). Social exclusion amongst girls: Their self-esteem and
coping strategies. Educational Psychologist, 14(2), 94-100.
Steinberg, L. D., Lamborn, S. D., Darling, N., Mounts, N. S., & Dornbusch, S. M. (1994).
Over-time changes in adjustment and competence among adolescents from
authoritative, authoritarian, indulgent, and neglectful families. Child
Development, 65, 754-770.
294
Stevens, V., De Bourdeaudhuij, I., & Van Oost, P. (2000). Bullying in Flemish schools:
An evaluation of anti-bullying intervention in primary and secondary schools.
British Journal of Educational Psychology, 2000(70), 195-210.
Storch, E. A., & Esposito, L. E. (2003). Peer victimization and posttaumatic stres among
children. Child Study Journal 33(2), 91-96.
Storch, E. A., Roth, D. A., Coles, M. E., Heimberg, R. G., Bravata, E. A., & Moser, J.
(2003). The measurement and impact of childhood teasing in a sample of young
adults. Anxiety Disorders, 18, 681-694.
Straus, M., & Gelles, R. (1979). Measuring intrafamily conflict and violence: The
Conflict Tactic Scale. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 41, 75-85.
Straus, M., & Gelles, R. (Eds.). (1990). Measuring intrafamily conflict and violence: The
Conflict Tactic Scale. New York: Transaction Publishers.
Strom, P. S., & Strom, R. D. (2005). When teens turn cyberbullies. Education Digest,
71(4), 35-41.
Sunde Peterson, J. (2006). Bullying and the gifted: Victims, perpetrators, prevalence, and
effects. Gifted Child Quarterly, 50(2), 148-168.
Sutton, A., Smith, P. K., & Swettenham, J. (1999). Bullying and ―theory of mind‖: A
critique of the ―social skills deficit‖ view of anti-social behaviour. Social
Development, 8(1), 117-134.
Sutton, J., & Smith, P. K. (1999). Bullying as a group process: An adaptation of the
Participant Role approach. Aggressive Behavior, 25, 97-111.
Twemlow, S. W., Fonagy, P., Sacco, F. C., & Barbour, J. R. J. (2006). Teachers who
bully students: A hidden trauma. International Journal of Social Psychiatry,
52(3), 187-198.
Twemlow, S. W., Fonagy, P., Sacco, F. C., Gies, M. L., Evans, R., & Ewbank, R. (2001).
Creating a peaceful school learning environment: A controlled study of an
elementary school intervention to reduce violence. American Journal of
Psychiatry, 158(5), 808–810.
van Hoof, A., Raaijmakers, Q. A. W., van Beel, Y., Hale, W. W., & Aleva, L. (2008). A
multi-mediation model on the relations of bullying, victimization, identity, and
family with adolescent depressive symptoms. Journal of Youth and Adolescence,
37, 772-782.
van Hoof, A., & Raajimakers, Q. A. W. (2003). The search for the structure of identity
formation. Identity: An international journal of theory and research, 3(3), 271-
289.
Vanerkolk, B. (1994). The body keeps the score: Memory and the evolving
psychobiology of posttraumatic stress. Harvard Review of Psychiatry, 1, 253-265.
Viswevaran, C., & Ones, D. S. (1995). Theory testing: Combining meta-analysis and
structural equations modeling. Personnel Psychology, 28(4), 865-885.
von Friedeburg, L. (2007). History of the Institute for Social Research. Retrieved from
http://www.ifs.uni-frankfurt.de/english/history.htm
Vossekuil, B., Fein, R. A., Reddy, M., Borum, R., & Modzeleski, W. (2002). The final
report and findings of the Safe School Initiative: Implications for the prevention
of school attacks in the United States.
Warwick, I., Chase, E., Aggleton, P., & Sanders, S. (2004). Homophobia, sexual
orientation and schools: A review and implications for action.
Weber, M. (Ed.). (1947). The theory of social and economic organization. New York:
Oxford University Press.
Wentzel, K., & Asher., S. (1995). The academic lives of neglected, rejected, popular and
controversial children. Child Development, 66, 754-763.
Wiggins, J. S., & Wender, C. L. (1961). The Peer Nomination Inventory: An empirically-
derived sociometric measure of adjustment in preadolescent boy. Child
Development, 64, 643-677.
Williams, K. R., & Guerra, N. G. (2007). Prevalence and predictors of internet bullying.
Atlanta, GA: Centers for Disease Control and Prevention.
Wolke, D., Woods, S., Bloomfield, L., & Karstadt, L. (2000). The association between
direct and relational bullying and behavior problems among primary school
children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41(8), 989-1002.
Woods, S., & Wolke, D. (2004). Direct and relational bullying among primary school
children and academic achievement. Journal of School Psychology, 42(2), 135–
155.
Yitzhak, A. (1987). Belzec, Sobibor, Treblinka: The Operation Reinhard Death Camps.
Bloomington and Indianapolis, IN: Indiana University Press.
297