0% found this document useful (0 votes)
70 views44 pages

Chapter 1

Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
0% found this document useful (0 votes)
70 views44 pages

Chapter 1

Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
You are on page 1/ 44

LIGHTBOWN & SPADA

HOW LANGUAGES ARE


LEARNED
CHAPTER 1

LANGUAGE LEARNING IN
EARLY CHILDHOOD
What is this chapter about?
• How do children learn their first language?

• What enables a child not only to learn words, but to put


them together in meaningful sentences?

• What pushes children to develop complex grammatical


language?

• Does child language develop similarly around the


world?
The First Three Years

• high degree of similarity in the early language of


children all over world

• there are developmental sequences of first language


acquisition
The First Three Years (cont.)
(early weeks of life)
◦ involuntary crying

◦ cooing and gurgling

◦ babies can hear very little differences between the sounds of


human language
◦ Babbling (few months later)
The First Three Years (cont.)

• By the end of first year:


◦ understanding many frequently
repeated words
• e.g. waving when someone says ‘bye-bye’

• At twelve months:
◦ producing one or two words that everyone recognizes
The First Three Years (cont.)
• By the age of two:
◦ producing at least fifty different words
◦ beginning to combine words into simple sentences
(e.g. ‘mommy juice’ ‘baby fall down’).
• ‘telegraphic sentences’
• leave out articles, prepositions, or auxiliary words.

◦ What makes them ‘sentences’?


• the word order reflects the word order of the language children are
hearing
• combined words have a meaning relationship, not just a list of
words
The First Three Years (cont.)
(grammatical morphemes)
• predictable patterns (developmental
sequences/stages)
• these stages are related to children’s
◦ cognitive development
◦ their gradual mastery of linguistic elements (plurality)
• see ‘wug test’ on p. 2 (grammatical morp.) Gleason,
1950s
• see the study on p. 4 (developmental seq.) Brown, 1973
Acquisition of
Grammatical morphemes
Roger Brown’s study (1973):
- approximate order of acquiring grammatical
morphemes
◦ Present progressive –ing (running)
◦ Plural –s (books)
◦ Irregular past forms (went)
◦ Possessive -’s (daddy’s hat)
◦ Copula (am/is/are)
◦ Articles (a/an/the)
◦ Regular past –ed (walked)
◦ Third person singular simple present –s (he runs)
◦ Auxiliary ‘be’ (He is coming)

8
Overgeneralization
• Child will generalize grammar rules
so they apply the rules too broadly.
• Example: “I dugged in the sandbox”
rather than “I dug in the sandbox”
Overgeneralization
The First Three Years (cont.)
Negation & Questions

• see Pages 5-6 for the stages in the development of


‘negation’

• see Pages 6-9 for the developmental stages ‘questions’


Acquisition of Negation
Lois Bloom’s study (1991) – four stages
◦ Stage 1: ‘no’ – e.g., “No go”. “No cookie.”

◦ Stage 2: subject + no – e.g., “Daddy no comb hair.”

◦ Stage 3: auxiliary or modal verbs (do/can) + not


(Yet no variations for different persons or tenses)
e.g., “I can’t do it “, “He don’t want it.”
◦ Stage 4: correct form of auxiliary verbs
(did/doesn’t/is/are) + not
e.g., He didn’t go. She doesn’t want it.
But sometimes double negatives are used
e.g., I don’t have no more candies.

12
Acquisition of Questions
Lois Bloom’s study (1991):
Order of the occurrence of wh- question words
• “What” - Whatsat? Whatsit?
• “Where” and “who”
• “Why” (emerging at the end of the 2nd year and
becomes a favorite at the age of 3 or 4)
• “How” and “When” (yet children do not fully
understand the meaning of adults’ responses)
e.g., Child: When can we go outside?
Mother: In about 5 minutes.
Child: 1-2-3-4-5! Can we go now?

13
Acquisition of Questions
Lois Bloom’s study (1991):
Six stages of children’s question-making
◦ Stage 1: using single words or single two- or three-word
sentences with rising intonation
(“Mommy book?” “Where’s Daddy?”)

◦ Stage 2: using the word order of the declarative sentence


(“You like this?” “Why you catch it?”)

◦ Stage 3: “fronting” - putting a verb at the beginning of a


sentence
(“Is the teddy is tired?” “Do I can have a cookie?”)

14
Acquisition of Questions

Lois Bloom’s study (1991) – six stages (II)


◦ Stage 4: subject-auxiliary inversion in yes/no questions
but not in wh-questions
(“Do you like ice cream?” “Where I can draw?”)

◦ Stage 5: subject-auxiliary inversion in wh-questions, but


not in negative wh-questions
(“Why can he go out?” “Why he can’t go out?”)

◦ Stage 6: overgeneralizing the inverted form in embedded


questions
(“I don’t know why can’t he go out.”)

15
The First Three Years (cont.)

• What do all these examples of developmental


sequences mean?

◦ Children’s language develops systematically.

◦ Their language is not just a list of


memorized word pairs.

◦ In order to create new forms and structures,


children go beyond what they have heard.
The pre-school years
• mastering the basic structures of the language
• asking questions, giving commands, reporting real
events, and creating stories
• using correct word order and grammatical markers most
of the time
• learning several new words a day
• beginning to acquire more complex linguistic features
(e.g. passives).
The pre-school years (cont.)

• developing the ability to use language in a social


environment

• using the language in various social environments


The pre-school years (cont.)

• ‘metalinguistic awareness’

◦ the ability to treat language as an object separate from the


meaning it conveys
“drink the chair vs. cake the eat”

◦ knowing ‘about’ the language


The school years
• the language abilities expand and grow

• more sophisticated metalinguistic awareness is


developed

• an astonishing growth of vocabulary

• the acquisition of different language ‘registers’


i.e. a style or way of using language that is typical of or appropriate for a
particular setting
Explaining First Language
Acquisition
• Three main theoretical positions to explain first
language acquisition:
◦ The behaviourist perspective
◦ The innatist perspective
◦ Interactionist / developmental perspectives
The Behaviourist Perspective
◦ Children imitate the language and receive positive
reinforcement
• The reinforcement may be:
• praise
• just successful communication

◦ They imitate and practice until they form ‘habits’ of correct


language use (see examples on page 10).
The Behaviourist Perspective

◦ Important factors:
• quality and quantity of the language the child hears
• the consistency of the reinforcement

◦ Environment is the source of everything the child


needs to learn
The Behaviourist Perspective

• Fallacies of the behaviorist perspective:


◦ Children imitate selectively.
• They imitate what is new and useful to them, not simply what is
‘available’ in the environment (see example on Page 11).

◦ Many of the things children say show that:


• they use language creatively (overgeneralizing)
• not just repeat what they have heard (see examples on Pages 13-14)
The Behaviourist Perspective
• Therefore:
◦ Imitation and practice alone cannot explain some of the forms
created by children.

◦ Children pick out patterns and generalize them to


new contexts.

◦ They create new forms or new uses of


words.

◦ Classical behaviorism is not a satisfactory


explanation for the acquisition of complex
grammar.
The Innatist Perspective

• All human languages are fundamentally innate and the


same universal principles underlie all of them.

• Children are biologically programmed for


language.

• The environment makes the basic


contribution, and the child will do
the rest.
The Innatist Perspective
• Behaviorism cannot explain language acquisition because:
◦ Children know more about the language than they hear from the
environment.
◦ Children are sometimes exposed to incorrect use of language (e.g.
false starts, incomplete sentences) but they learn the correct use of
language.
The Innatist Perspective

• Therefore:
◦ Children’s minds are not blank plates to be filled
by imitation.

◦ Children are born with a specific innate ability


to discover the underlying rules of the language
they are exposed to.

◦ Children are born with a template which contains the principles


that are universal to all human languages –
UNIVERSAL GRAMMAR (UG).
The Innatist Perspective

• Critical Period Hypothesis

◦ There is a critical period for first language acquisition.

◦ Children who are not given access to language in infancy


and early childhood will never acquire language (see
examples on p. 19-21).
Interactionist/Developmental Perspectives
• Language learning is just another remarkable learning
ability of human beings.

• There is no need to think that there are specific brain


structures devoted to language
acquisition.

• Even though there is a powerful learning mechanism in


human brain, environment is also very important.
Interactionist/Developmental Perspectives

• Piaget:
◦ Children’s language is built on their
cognitive development.

◦ Their cognitive development would


determine how they use language.

◦ Language can be used to represent knowledge that children


have acquired through physical interaction with the
environment.
Interactionist/Developmental Perspectives
• Vygotsky:
◦ Language develops primarily from social interaction.

◦ Zone of Proximal Development


• In a supportive interactive environment:
• children can do more than they are
capable of doing independently
• they can advance to a higher level of
knowledge and performance
Interactionist/Developmental Perspectives

• Child-directed Speech:
◦ slower rate of delivery
◦ higher pitch
◦ more varied intonation
◦ shorter, simpler sentences
◦ stress on key words
◦ frequent repetition and paraphrase

• Child-directed speech is not universal, but in every


society children hear language that is meaningful to
them.
Interactionist/Developmental Perspectives
• The importance of interaction:

◦ See the example of Jim on Page 25

◦ One-to-one interaction gives the child access to language


that is adjusted to his or her level of comprehension.
Interactionist/Developmental Perspectives
• Connectionism:
◦ When children hear a word, a connection is created in the
child’s mind between the word and what it represents.

◦ Language acquisition is a process of


associating
• words with objects
• words with other words
• words with grammatical morphemes.

◦ All this is possible because of the child’s general ability to


develop associations
Childhood Bilingualism

• Simultaneous Bilinguals

• Sequential Bilinguals

• Subtractive Bilinguals

You might also like