Bensoukas 2012 MorphophonologyofTashlhitAgentiveNouns
Bensoukas 2012 MorphophonologyofTashlhitAgentiveNouns
Bensoukas 2012 MorphophonologyofTashlhitAgentiveNouns
net/publication/309386245
CITATIONS READS
2 676
1 author:
Karim Bensoukas
Mohammed V University of Rabat- FLHS
8 PUBLICATIONS 36 CITATIONS
SEE PROFILE
All content following this page was uploaded by Karim Bensoukas on 23 October 2016.
Karim BENSOUKAS
M5-Agdal University, Rabat.
1. Introduction
*
This paper is a synthesis of the analysis of agentive nouns in my D.E.S. and
Doctorat d’Etat dissertations (Bensoukas 1994, 2001, respectively) as well as
the treatment of labial dissimilation in Bensoukas (1999, 2004). I have
benefited from discussions with and/or comments from A. Adnor, A.
Benhallam, A. Boudlal, A. Bouhlal, P. de Lacy, F. Dell, M. Elmedlaoui, R.
Faizi, E. Iazzi, S. Imouzaz, A. Jebbour, J. McCarthy, K. Moktadir, S. Parker, J.
Pater, J. Saib, E. Selkirk, A. Youssi and the members of the phonology-
morphology research group of FLHS, Rabat. Remaining errors are my entire
responsibility.
1
accounted for in terms of an independently justified constraint
interaction driving labial dissimilation. The [a/i] alternation
affecting the initial inflectional vowel will also be addressed for the
sake of providing a complete analysis. The second hypothesis we
defend is the Augmented Stem Hypothesis (ASH). The ASH
explains the presence or absence of the prefinal vowel [a] in
surface AN forms. ANs will accordingly be subdivided into two
major classes: those that require augmentation via a prefinal vowel
and those that do not. Both the UMH and the ASH are couched in
parallel Optimality Theory (OT) (Prince and Smolensky
1993/2004; McCarthy and Prince 1993a, 1995, 1999).
The remainder of this paper is organized as follows.
Describing AN classes and allomorphs, section 2 provides a factual
background for the analyses in the paper. Section 3 characterizes
the AN morpheme as a unitary form by (i) providing an analysis of
the initial, inflectional nominal vowel and its allomorphs and (ii)
explaining the labial dissimilation affecting the AN morpheme.
Finally, section 4 deals with the augmentation involved in the
majority of AN forms.
2
loanwords with an AN meaning but without the native AN
morpheme (e.g. axrraz ‘shoemaker’, agzzar ‘butcher’, and asjjax
‘goldsmith’). Although expressing the same meaning as Tashlhit
ANs, these borrowed items are discarded due to their
morphophonological difference.
Par. C
1
Conventional IPA symbols are used in this paper, except for emphatics
transcribed with a dot underneath. Š and Ž are the emphatic counterparts of ʃ and
ʒ, respectively. Gemination is indicated by doubling the consonant.
3
our conception contains the items in paradigms (1A-B), while the
V-final one contains the items in paradigms (1C-D) above. A
further distinction between the items that are V-final can be
proposed on the basis of whether the final vowel is underlyingly /a/
or /u/. We have been unable to find roots ending in a vowel [i];
therefore, we will assume as provisionally correct that these
constitute a lexical gap in the grammar of Tashlhit.
Most of the irregularity characterizing Tashlhit morphology
hinges on this bipartition. V-final verb roots and C-final verb roots
behave distinctly in the various morphological classes of the
language as the following items reveal:
4
prefixing the derivational, AN morpheme {m+} to the verb root
and epenthesizing a prefinal vowel, generally [a] and sporadically
[u].2 All AN nouns bear an initial inflectional morpheme, generally
a prefix vowel {a+}. The AN morpheme shows allomorphy
between [m] and [n] (see 2.4 below). The initial vowel is also
subject to contextual variation [a]/[i] as in (4iv) below:
2
Root material is considered here to contain vowel elements as well as
consonantal elements. Espoused by many scholars working within the generative
tradition (Jebbour 1988; Moktadir 1989; Bensoukas 1994), this position is a
major departure from the belief that Amazigh roots consist of consonantal
material only (Basset 1929; 1952).
5
ii- tabʕ amtabʕ ‘follower/disciple’
ʕ(a)ʃr amʕaʃr/ amʕʃʃr ‘roommate’
iii- ddukkl amddakkwl ‘friend’
mmuddu anmmuddu ‘traveller’
iv- rẓa imrẓi ‘sb. with a fracture’
ṛʒu imṛʒi ‘hoper’
ħḍu imħḍi ‘hider’
ddullu imddilli ‘demeaned person’
6
ii- ANs with labial dissimilation
bdd anbdad ‘stand’
ʕḍb anʕḍab ‘injure’
frn anfran ‘sort out’
ḍfur anḍfur ‘follow’
gwmr angwmar ‘hunt’
ag m
w
anagam ‘draw water’
3
See for example the general works done within the colonialist tradition
(Aspinion 1953; Laoust 1936; Renisio 1932) and the more focused linguistic
approaches (Chaker 1978; Chami 1979; Elmoujahid 1981; Penchoen 1973).
7
alternation). In section 3.2, we provide an analysis of the AN
allomorphy (the [m/n] alternation). The analysis will be completed
when the provenance of the prefinal vowel, which we claim is not
part of the AN affix, is explained, the focus of section 4.
4
CS and plural formation facts in Amazigh are extremely complex (see Basset
1932; Chaker 1988; Elmoujahid 1982; Guerssel 1983; Jebbour 1988; Idrissi
2000; Saib 1982, 1986). For the sake of simplicity, we assume that the nouns in
(7) change their initial vowel [a] to [u] in CS and to [i] in the plural.
8
initial vowel is obligatory in ANs and that (ii) it is a prefix. The
former characteristic suggests that the constraint Max-AffixNInfl
(8a) is high ranking. The latter issue ensues from the high-rank of
the constraint Align (AffixNInfl, L, Stem, L) (8b) (on Alignment, see
McCarthy and Prince 1993b, 2004a-b-c)
9
is not completely undominated (see Bensoukas 2010 for details and
analysis.) 5
To rule out candidates in which the AN morpheme is away
from the left edge (*akmraz, *akrmaz...), we rely on the gradience
of violation which characterizes alignment constraints: the further
away from the left edge, the more alignment violations the AN
morpheme incurs. This positions the AN morpheme as closest as
possible to the left edge, specifically immediately after the initial
vowel. One argument we propose for this standpoint is that in the
absence of the initial vowel, the AN morpheme becomes word-
initial. A few putative frozen ANs exist that do not have the initial
vowel. In this case, Max-AffixNInfl must be dominated by another
constraint which bans the initial vowel in these items. Examples are
maf (>af ‘find’) in maf-aman ‘water-finder’, mslm (>slm ‘inhale’)
in mslm-aggwrn ‘moth’ (literally ‘flour-inhaler’), milluṭṭs/miṭṭus (>
ṭṭs ‘sleep’) and mixxi (>xxi ‘discharge excrement’). In the absence
of the intial vowel, the morpheme {m+} is aligned with the left
edge of the stem.
An additional issue with respect to the initial, nominal
vowel is its changing quality, as revealed by a group of derived and
non-derived nouns that appear with the vowel [i] instead of the
more regular [a]. Examples are given in (11):
(11)
Non-derived nouns Derived nouns
ifili ‘thread’ imddilli ‘demeaned person’
ifri ‘cave’ imkiri ‘hired person’
isli ‘bridegroom’ issgni ‘needle’
5
Finally, the fact that syllables can be exceptionally onsetless at the beginning of
the word calls for another alignment constraint (McCarthy and Prince 1993a,
32), which dominates ONS (the requirement that syllables have onsets) namely
Align-L: [Stem = [PrWd.
10
analysis. Concerning vowel feature markedness constraints, we
assume Feature Class Theory (Padgett 1995), according to which
features are members of various classes, and constraints on well-
formedness may target individual features as well as classes of
features. (12) lists the feature classes along with their individual
members that are of relevance in the present context.
(12) Feature classes (Padgett 1995, 399): Vplace: {high, low, back,
round…}; Height: {high, low}; Color: {back, round}
6
In this conception, vowel markedness constraints are considered along two
dimensions: First, on the basis of labiality being universally more marked than
coronality (*Labial » *Coronal) (Prince and Smolensky 1993/2004), the vowel i
should emerge as less marked than u. Second, on the basis of a language-
particular preference of the low vowel over high vowels, a emerges as less
marked than the remaining two vowels.
11
According to the precepts of Feature Class Theory, the
phenomenon of vowel copying is viewed as spreading the whole
vocalic constituent in an all-or-nothing fashion (see for example
Clements and Hume 1995; Hulst and Weijer 1995; Ní Chiosáin
1995). The result is a feature-sharing structure like the one in (15)
(b, h, and r stand for back, high and round respectively; the AN
imħḍi is derived from the verb ħḍu ‘hide’). As to the locality issue,
since vowels never occur contiguously in the language, copying is
distant. Note, however, that the vowel features associated with
glides or doubly articulated consonants never spread.
-b -h-r
12
(17)
13
In tableau (18), the candidate (18i), with the initial vowel a, incurs
a violation of the markedness constraint *a, which (18ii), the
copying candidate, does not. Since the constraint that decides is a
markedness constraint that is otherwise inactive, this is a case of
TETU in vowel copying.
To recapitulate, the changing quality of the initial nominal
vowel is shown to be the effect of vowel copying, itself driven by
markedness reduction, the copying pattern being a case of TETU.
7
This section draws heavily on the analyses in Bensoukas (1999) and (2004)
and references therein. We specifically adhere to the essence of the OCP » Faith
analysis (Selkirk 1995) and the self-conjunction analysis (Alderete 1997).
8
The essence of constraint conjunction is that two violations of the same
constraint are worse when they occur in the same domain. Smolensky (1993,
1995) defines local constraint conjunction as: “The local conjunction of C1 and
C2 in domain D, C1 and C2 is violated when there is some domain of type D in
which both C1 and C2 are violated.” Universally, the conjunction of C1 and C2
dominates the individual constraints conjoined, i.e. [C1 & C2] » C1, C2.
14
(19) *PL/Lab2Stem: Ban any stem containing two segments with
independent Place specifications [labial].
15
Second, if faithfulness is compelled to yield in satisfaction
of some higher ranked constraint, root faithfulness wins:
(23)
/a+m+lmd/ Ident-R(Lab) Ident(Lab)
i. anlmad *
ii. amlnad *! *
As (23) shows, the loss of the labial place of the affix is less serious
than that of the root. This behavior reveals the dominance relation
holding between Ident-Root(Lab) and Ident(Lab).
The last decision to be made concerns the interaction of
*PL/Lab2Stem and Ident-Root. In the OCP » Faith analysis, featural
markedness outranks faithfulness in order to ban the tauto-
morphemic co-occurrence of labial consonants. Taking this
consideration into account, we give in tableau (24) the constraint
hierarchy accounting for Tashlhit labial dissimilation (Selkirk
1995, 7), in which Correspondence Theory constraints are
substituted for the original ones:
(24)
/a+m+mgr/ *PL/Lab2Stem Ident-Rt(Lab) Ident(Lab)
i. ammgar *!
ii. anmgar *
iii. amngar *! *
9
The surfacing of the coronal place instead of the input labial one ensues from
two more considerations. First, all consonants should have place specified in
satisfaction of the constraint HAVEPLACE (Padgett 1995; Fukazawa 1999).
Second, the choice of the coronal feature instead of any other feature emanates
from the ranking of the featural markedness constraints proposed in Prince and
Smolensky (1993/2004): *Dorsal, *Labial » *Coronal.
16
picture is, as highly predicted, one of identity avoidance observing
positional faithfulness (for a full development of root faithfulness
effects in Tashlhit, see Bensoukas 2004).
With no further stipulation, the same ranking predicts that if
there are no inimical labial specifications in a word, the labial
consonant [m] of the morpheme will surface unscathed:
(25)
/a+m+krz/ *PL/Lab2Stem Ident-R (Lab) Ident(Lab) *Lab
i. ankraz *!
ii. amkraz *
17
maximal one of two (for example ut ‘hit’, mun ‘accompany’). A
weight distinction between syllables is motivated in which the
status of the nucleus as vocalic or consonantal plays a decisive role
(Jebbour 1996, 55-57). 10 A heavy syllable consists of a nucleus
vowel that is followed by a singleton or a geminate consonant as in
the words ut ‘hit’, mun ‘accompany’, ass ‘day’, and bidd ‘stand’. A
light syllable, on the other hand, is either an open syllable with a
nucleus vowel or any syllable with a consonantal nucleus as in the
following examples, in which the consonantal nucleus is
underlined: a ‘hey’, fi ‘suppurate’, g ‘be’, gn ‘sleep’, ls ‘wear’, krz
‘harvest’, ʃʃ ‘eat’, qzz ‘look’.
In case a syllabic consonant is followed by a coda, both the
nucleus and the coda of the syllable in question are dominated by a
branching mora. A leftover consonant in the case of a syllable
headed by a vowel is provided a mora by the Weight-by-Position
convention (Hayes 1989), whereas a leftover consonant in the case
of a syllable headed by a consonant is attached directly to the
syllable node. Finally, following Dell and Elmedlaoui (1988),
Jebbour proposes that the first element of a geminate cannot be the
onset of a syllable, forcing initial geminates to be split between two
syllables. A final geminate consonant can be hosted by the same
syllable.
Consideration of the agentive nouns in Tashlhit yields the
following generalizations, and the output prosodic structure of the
ultimate syllable of agentive nouns is summarized in the table in
(26):
1. Bi/Triconsonantal roots and vowel initial trisegmental bases
have corresponding ANs derived by prefixation and prefinal
vowel epenthesis.
2. Verbs containing a prefinal or medial vowel undergo
affixation only.
3. Verbs containing a final vowel undergo prefixation and
appear with a final vowel [i].
10
In terms of its prosodic structure, Tashlhit is notable for the fact that any
segment in its inventory, even voiceless stops, can behave as a syllable peak
provided it is the most sonorous sound in the string to be syllabified (Boukous
1987, 2009; Clements 1997; Dell and Elmedlaoui 1985, 1988, 2002; Jebbour
1996, 1999; Prince and Smolensky 1993/2004; Ridouane 2008).
18
(26) Agentive noun stem prosody:
Augmented stems Unaugmented stems
Root Stem σ# Root Stem σ#
a. mgr anm[gar] a. σµµ a. bur an[bur] a. σµµ
agwm ana[gam] b. aẓum ana[ẓum] b. σµµ
awj ama[waj] c. ʒlu imʒ[li] c. σµ
b. gn amg[gan] b. σµµ kla imk[li] σµ
c. nj am[naj] c. σµµ ggalla imggil[li]
d. matr anmma[tr] σµ
ddukkwl amddak[kwl] σµ
sijħ amsi[jħ] σµ
19
/agwm/ anagam ‘draw water (from a well)’
/aws/ amawas ‘help’
/gwmr/ angwmar ‘hunt’
/lmd/ anlmad ‘learn’
(28) Final Heavy Syllable (σµµ]): stems must contain a final heavy
syllable.
This constraint will rule out any stem that does not end in a heavy
syllable. On the other hand, given that the site of the epenthetic
vowel is consistently the prefinal position, this turns out to just
make the final syllable heavy, and the constraint σµµ] rules in
candidates which epenthesize a prefinal vowel to augment the AN
stem. In the spirit of OT, this constraint can be violated as long as it
is dominated.
The possibility of vowel epenthesis calls for a low ranked
faithfulness DEP-V constraint (29), which otherwise militates
against vowel epenthesis:
20
Through this ranking alone, we obtain both the application of
vowel epenthesis and its prefinal site.
Kager (1999, 105) makes the following prediction about
epenthesis. It always applies minimally, that is precisely to the
extent that is necessary to improve structural markedness.
Moreover, epenthesis never results in an increase, but rather a
decrease, of structural markedness. By providing a vocalic nucleus
to a consonant cluster, Tashlhit vowel epenthesis results in the
decrease of structural markedness by reducing the number of
consonantal-peak syllables. Epenthesis also applies minimally, as it
never affects syllables with a vowel.
21
consonant epenthesis and vowel lengthening is given in (33), where
NLV and DEP-C are not ranked with respect to each other.
22
Epenthesis not resulting in a final heavy syllable, candidate (35i)
incurs a gratuitous DEP-V violation in addition to a violation of
σµµ]. Though satisfying σµµ], candidate (35iii) violates both DEP-
C, fatally, and DEP-V. Only the faithful candidate (35ii) is optimal
in this case. So the ranking established so far shows how economy
of epenthesis is derived in the sense that epenthesis only applies
when needed.
So far, one possibility we have not considered via which the
final heavy syllable result may be achieved, but which should be
discarded immediately, is the deletion of some of the root material
in long roots. Examples are:
(36)
ddullu i-m-[d.dil.li] / *i-m-[d.dil] ‘be demeaned’
mmuddu a-n-[m.mud.du] / *a-n-[m.mud] ‘travel’
Candidate (38i) does not maximally parse root material with the
aim of achieving the final heavy syllable requirement; accordingly,
(38i) fails in the competition in front of the faithful candidate
(38ii).
23
To sum up, the driving force of epenthesis is blocked in
vowel final roots by either the markedness constraint NLV or the
faithfulness constraint DEP-C. These have no effect on consonant
final stems. No provable ranking holds between the constraints
NLV and DEP-C and MAX. Therefore, these will appear at the top
of the hierarchy: NLV, DEP-C, MAX »σµµ] »DEP-V
11
Note the exceptional form anḍalab derived from ḍalb (beg), a loan-word,
containing a medial vowel but nonetheless having a corresponding agentive noun
that does exhibit vowel epenthesis.
24
heavy syllable (ttdukkul, ttgunzur, ttmatar, and ttnakaṛ (see
Bensoukas 2001a for an account).
5. Conclusion
25
REFERENCES
26
Bensoukas, Karim. 2004. “Markedness, Faithfulness and
Consonant Place in Tashlhit Roots and Affixes.” Langues et
Littératures 18: 115-153.
Bensoukas, Karim. 2010. “Construct State Allomorphy in
Amazigh: What can Alignment Explain?” ms. UM5-Agdal,
Rabat.
Boukous, Ahmed. 1987. Phonotactique et Domaines Prosodiques
en Berbère. Doctorat d’Etat diss., Université Paris VIII-
Vincennes, Saint-Denis.
Boukous, Ahmed. 2009. Phonologie de l’Amazighe. Rabat:
IRCAM publications.
Chaker, Salem. 1978. Un Parler Berbère d’Algérie (Kabylie)-
Syntaxe. Doctorat d’Etat diss., Université Paris V.
Chaker, Salem. 1988. “L’Etat d’Annexion du Nom.” Encyclopédie
Berbère V: 686-695. Available at:
http://www.centrederechercheberbere.fr/tl_files/docpdf/annexion.pdf.
Chami, Mohamed. 1979. Un Parler Amazigh du Rif- Approche
Phonologique et Morphologique. Third Cycle diss., Université
Paris V.
Chomsky, Noam and Morris Halle. 1968. The Sound Pattern of
English. New York: Harper and Row.
Clements, Georges N. 1997. “Berber Syllabification: Derivations
or Constraints.” In Roca, Iggy ed., Constraints and Derivations
in Phonology, 289-330. Oxford: Clarendon Press.
Clements, Georges. N. and Elisabeth V. Hume. 1995. “The internal
Organization of Speech Sounds.” In Goldsmith, John. ed., The
Handbook of Phonological Theory, 245-306. Cambridge, MA:
Blackwell.
Dell, François and Mohamed Elmedlaoui. 1985. “Syllabic
Consonants and Syllabification in Imdlawn Tashlhiyt Berber.”
Journal of African Languages and Linguistics 7: 105-130.
Dell, François and Mohamed Elmedlaoui. 1988. “Syllabic
Consonants in Berber: Some New Evidence.” Journal of
African Languages and Linguistics 10: 1-17.
Dell, François and Mohamed Elmedlaoui. 2002. Syllables in
Tashlhiyt Berber and in Moroccan Arabic. Dordrecht: Kluwer.
Elmedlaoui, Mohamed. 1992/1995. Aspects des Représentations
Phonologiques dans Certaines Langues Chamito-sémitiques.
27
Ph.D. diss., Mohammed V University, Faculty of Letters,
Rabat. [Published version Rabat: Faculty of Letters.]
Elmoujahid, El Houssain. 1981. La Classe du Nom dans un Parler
de la Langue Tamazight: Le Tachelhiyt d’Igherm. Third Cycle
diss., Université Paris V, René Descartes.
Elmoujahid, El Houssain. 1982. “Un Aspect Morphologique du
Nom en Tamazight: L’Etat d’Annexion.” Langues et
Littératures 2: 47-62.
Fukazawa, Haruka. 1999. Theoretical Implications of OCP Effects
on Features in Optimality Theory. Ph.D. diss., University of
Maryland at College Park.
Guerssel, Mohamed. 1983. “A Phonological Analysis of the
Construct State in Berber.” Linguistic Analysis 11: 309-330.
Guerssel, Mohamed. 1986. “Glides in Berber and Syllabicity.”
Linguistic Inquiry 17: 1-12.
Hayes, Bruce. 1989. Compensatory Lengthening in Moraic
Phonology. Linguistic Inquiry 20: 253-306.
Hulst, Harry van der and J. van de Weijer. 1995. “Vowel
Harmony.” In Goldsmith, John. (ed.). The Handbook of
Phonological Theory, 495-534. Cambridge, MA.: Blackwell.
Idrissi, Ali. 2000. “On Berber Plurals.” In Lecarme, J.,
Lowenstamm, J. and U. Shlonsky (eds.). Research in
Afroasiatic Grammar, 101-124. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Itô, Junko. 1986. Syllable Theory in Prosodic Phonology. Ph.D.
diss., University of Massachusetts, Amherst.
Itô, Junko. 1989. “A Prosodic Theory of Epenthesis.” Natural
Language and Linguistic Theory 7: 217-259.
Itô, Junko, Armin Mester and Jaye Padgett. 1995. “Licensing and
Underspecification in Optimality Theory.” Linguistic Inquiry
26: 571-613.
Jebbour, Abdelkrim. 1988. Processus de Formation du Pluriel
Nominal en Tamazight. Tachelhit de Tiznit- Approche Non-
Concaténative. D.E.S. diss., M5 University, FLSH, Rabat.
Jebbour, Abdelkrim. 1996. Morphologie et Contraintes
Prosodiques en Berbère Tachelhit de Tiznit- Analyse
Linguistique et Traitement Automatique. Doctorat d’Etat diss.,
M5 University, FLSH, Rabat.
28
Jebbour, Abdelkrim. 1999. Syllable Weight and Syllable Nuclei in
Tachelhit Berber of Tiznit. Cahiers de Grammaire 24
“Phonologie: théorie et variation”: 95-116.
Kager, René. 1999. Optimality Theory. Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.
Lahrouchi, Mohamed. 2001. Aspects Morpho-Phonologiques de la
Dérivation Verbale en Berbère (Parler Chleuh d’Agadir).
Contribution à l’Etude de l’Architecture des Gabarits. Doctoral
diss., Université Paris VII.
Lasri, Ahmed. 1991. Aspects de la Phonologie Non-Linéaire du
Parler Berbère Chleuh de Tidli. Doctoral diss., Université de la
Sorbonne Nouvelle, Paris III.
Laoust, Emile. 1936. Cours de Berbère Marocain- Dialectes du
Sous, du Haut et de l’Anti Atlas. (2nd edn.). Paris: Société
d’Editions Géographiques, Maritimes et Coloniales.
McCarthy, John J. and Alan Prince. 1993a. Prosodic Morphology I:
Constraint Interaction and Satisfaction. Ms. University of
Massachusetts, Amherst and Rutgers University.
McCarthy, John J. and Alan Prince. 1993b. Generalized Alignment.
In Booij, G. And J. van Marle. eds., Yearbook of Morphology
1993, 79-153. Dordrecht: Kluwer.
McCarthy, John J. and Alan Prince. 1994. “The Emergence of the
Unmarked: Optimality in Prosodic Morphology.” North East
Linguistic Society 24: 333-379.
McCarthy, John J. and Alan Prince. 1995. Faithfulness and
Reduplicative Identity. UMOPL 18: Papers in Optimality
Theory. [Rutgers Optimality Archive#60].
McCarthy, John J. and Alan Prince. 1999. “Faithfulness and
Identity in Prosodic Morphology.” In Kager, R., van der Hulst,
H. and W. Zonneveld, eds. The Prosody-morphology Interface,
218-309. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
McCarthy, John J. and Alan Prince. 2004a. “Generalizaed
Alignment: Introduction to the Theory.” In McCarthy, John J.
ed., Optimality Theory in Phonology- A Reader, 72-76.
Malden, MA: Blackwell.
McCarthy, John J. and Alan Prince. 2004b. “Generalizaed
Alignment: Prosody.” In McCarthy, John J. ed., Optimality
Theory in Phonology- A Reader, 167-177. Malden, MA:
Blackwell.
29
McCarthy, John J. and Alan Prince. 2004c. “Generalizaed
Alignment: The Prosody-Morphology Interface.” In McCarthy,
John J. ed., Optimality Theory in Phonology- A Reader, 451-
463. Malden, MA: Blackwell.
McCarthy, John J. and Alan Prince. 2004d. “The Emergence of the
Unmarked.” In McCarthy, John J. ed., Optimality Theory in
Phonology- A Reader, 483-494.Malden, MA: Blackwell.
Moktadir, Khalid. 1989. The Passive Form in Tashlhiyt Berber- A
Prosodic Approach. D.E.S. diss., M5 University, FLSH, Rabat.
Morén, Bruce T. 1999. Distinctiveness, Coercion and Sonority: A
Unified Theory of Weight. Ph.D. diss., University of Maryland
at College Park.
Morén, Bruce T. 2003. “Weight Typology: An Optimality
Theoretic Approach.” The Linguistic Review 20: 281-304.
Ní Chiosáin, Maire. 1995. “Barra Gaelic Vowel Copy and (non-)
Constituent Spreading”. West Coast Conference on Formal
Linguistics 13: 3-15.
Ní Chiosáin, Maire. and Jaye Padgett. 1997. “Markedness,
Segment Realization and Locality in Spreading.” Report n°
LRC-97-01, Linguistics Research Center, UCSC, Santa Cruz,
CA.
Padgett, Jaye. 1995. “Feature Classes.” University of
Massachusetts Occasional Papers 18: Papers in Optimality
Theory: 385-420.
Penchoen, Thomas G. 1973. Tamazight of the Ayt Ndhir. Los
Angeles: Undena Publications.
Prince, Alan and Paul Smolensky. 1993/2004. Optimality Theory:
Constraint Interaction in Generative Grammar. Malden, MA,
and Oxford, UK: Blackwell. [Revision of 1993 technical report,
Rutgers University Center for Cognitive Science.]
Renisio, A. 1932. Etude sur les Dialectes Berbères des Beni
Iznassen, du Rif et des Senhaja de Sraïr. Paris: Leroux.
Ridouane, Rachid. 2008. “Syllables without Vowels: Phonetic and
Phonological Evidence from Tashlhiyt Berber.” Phonology 25:
321-359.
Rosenthall, Sam. 1994. Vowel/Glide Alternation in a Theory of
Constraint Interaction. Ph.D. diss., UMass, Amherst.
Rosenthall, Sam. 1997. “The Distribution of Prevocalic Vowels.”
Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 15: 139-180.
30
Saib, Jilali. 1982. “Initial Vowel Syncope and Reduction in
Tamazight-Berber Nouns.” Langues et Littératures 2: 159-184.
Saib, Jilali. 1986. “Noun Pluralization in Berber: A Study of
Internal Reconstruction.” Langues et Littératures 5: 109-133.
Selkirk, Elizabeth. 1981. “Epenthesis and Degenerate Syllables in
Cairene Arabic.” MIT Working Papers in Linguistics 3:
Theoretical Issues in the Grammar of Semitic Languages: 209-
232.
Selkirk, Elizabeth. 1993. “[Labial] Relations.” Ms. University of
Massachusetts, Amherst.
Selkirk, Elizabeth. 1994. “Optimality Theory and Featural
Phenomena.” Lecture notes, LING 730, University of
Massachusetts, Amherst.
Selkirk, Elizabeth. 1995. “Language-Particular Violation of a
Universal Constraint: The OCP from the Perspective of
Optimality Theory.” Paper read at Table Ronde, Marrakech,
13-15 January 1995.
Smolensky, Paul. 1993. “Harmony, Markedness, and Phonological
Activity.” Handout from ROW-1, Revised. [Rutgers Optimality
Archive#87].
Smolensky, Paul. 1995. “On the Internal Structure of the Constraint
Component Con of UG.” Handout of talk, UCLA, April 7,
1995. [Rutgers Optimality Archive#86].
Suzuki, Keiishiro. 1998. A Typological Investigation of
Dissimilation. Ph.D. diss., The University of Arizona.
Walker, R. 1998. A Minimal Triplet in Altaic: Round Licensing,
Harmony, and Bisyllabic Triggers. Ms. University of
California, Santa Cruz. [Rutgers Optimality Archive #263].
Yip, Moira. 1988. “The Obligatory Contour Principle and
Phonological Rules: A Loss of Identity.” Linguistic Inquiry 19:
65-100.
Yip, Moira. 1995. “Repetition and Its Avoidance: The Case of
Javanese.” Ms. University of California, Irvine. [Rutgers
Optimality Archive#83]
Yip, Moira. 1998. “Identity Avoidance in Phonology and
Morphology.” In Lapointe, S., Brentari, D. and P. Farrell, eds.
Morphology and its Relation to Phonology and Syntax, 216-
246. Stanford: CSLI publications.
31