Chapter Motivation
Chapter Motivation
12
1. What are some of the different motivations people might have for learning
language?
2. Some people say that we can do anything if we are sufficiently motivated.
Do you agree?
3. Some authors make the point that motivation is a complex phenomenon
which depends on numerous other individual and socio-ecological variables.
What are some of the other factors that might affect motivational level?
4. Motivation can also be dynamic (that is, it can change). Can you think of
any examples where this has happened in your own experience or anyone
else you know?
Background
refers to the combination of effort plus desire to achieve the goal of learning the language plus
favourable attitudes towards learning the language. That is, motivation to learn a second lan-
guage is seen as referring to the extent to which the individual works or strives to learn the
language because of a desire to do so and the satisfaction experienced in this activity. (p. 10)
• integrative (arising from a desire to identify with those who speak the language,
perhaps in a social context, in a workplace or as an immigrant)
• instrumental (arising from a desire to benefit practically from acquiring the lan-
guage, for instance by gaining access to desired educational institutions, by get-
ting a better job, higher salary, etc).
• intrinsic (originating from within the learner—it is something the learner wants
for his/her own satisfaction)
• extrinsic (arising from outside the learner, for instance from a parent, a teacher/
school system or an employer).
Using this theoretical base, Noels, Pelletier, Clement, and Vallerand (2000) sur-
veyed 159 Anglophone learners of French at a university in Canada. They discov-
ered that intrinsic motivation was more strongly correlated with the criterion
measures than extrinsic motivation.
A meta-analysis carried out by Masgoret and Gardner (2003) investigated the
relationship of target language achievement to 5 other variables: integrativeness,
attitudes towards the learning situation, motivation, integrative orientation and
instrumental orientation, as well as age and availability of the language in the com-
munity. Altogether, 10,489 participants were involved. According to the authors, the
The Complex Nature of Motivation 183
results clearly demonstrated that the correlations between achievement and motiva-
tion were uniformly higher than those between any of the other variables.
A study which supports the idea of the critical importance of motivation is the
one by Griffiths (2018b). The participants in this study were 14 non-native speakers
of English, judged to be “successful” since they were all either teaching English or
teaching in English, the assumption being that those who are capable of performing
at this level must be at least reasonably competent (i.e. “good”) in English. Nine of
the participants were women and 5 were men, and in order to help minimize cultural
bias they were deliberately chosen to represent a variety of national backgrounds:
Turkey, Russia, the Czech Republic, India, Korea, China, Japan, Iran, Kyrgystan,
Pakistan, Poland, Kenya, Brazil and Finland. A Likert-type questionnaire was con-
structed using findings from the literature regarding the importance of motivation,
investment, beliefs, autonomy and strategy use. Respondents were asked to respond
to a series of statements on a scale of 5 (strongly agree) to 1 (strongly disagree). The
questionnaire was sent out by e-mail and returned by respondents at their conve-
nience. Given that Likert-style ratings are ordinal, median ratings are quoted
(Cohen, Manion, & Morrison, 2018; Dörnyei, 2007), which provides an indication
of the overall agreement or disagreement with the given statements. As a result of
the data analysis, with only one exception, all respondents strongly agreed that they
were motivated, producing a maximum median rating of 5.0 (strongly agree). From
this result we can infer that these good language learners, almost without exception,
were motivated, adding yet more support to the idea that motivation is an important
contributor to successful language learning.
A new study
Although this result might seem contradictory, it was also found to be the case in
another study (Tugan, 2015, another aspect of which was described in the chap-
ter on affect), described below.
(continued)
The Dynamic Nature of Motivation 185
(continued)
Table 12.1 Students’ (N = 406) median levels of agreement (out of 10) for each type
of motivation, plus the relationships with achievement (Spearman’s rho), the probability
values (p), and the percentage of variance or effect size (E = rs2)
Motivation type Median Spearman’s correlation Probability E
Intrinsic 8 rs = 0.174** p = 0.000 3.03
Extrinsic 9 rs = 0.009 p = 0.849 0.01
Integrative 7 rs = 0.146** p = 0.003 2.13
Instrumental 9 rs = 0.146** p = 0.003 2.13
NB: **indicates significance at the p < 0.01 (99%) level
As we can see from Table 12.1, although extrinsic and instrumental motiva-
tional types attracted the highest median agreement, it was intrinsic motiva-
tion which was most strongly correlated with achievement, although the effect
size is reasonably low (Plonsky & Oswald, 2014), a result which concurs with
the result of Griffiths and Özgür’s (2013) study described above. ◄
Making the point that many studies fail to consider the dynamic character and tem-
poral variability of motivation, Pawlak (2012) undertook a 4-week classroom-based
study which investigated the motivational dynamics of 28 senior high school students in
terms of their goal-orientation as well as their participation in classroom activities. Data
were collected by means of questionnaires, observations and interviews. According to
the results, although the reasons for learning remained relatively stable over the time
scale involved, the intensity of motivation varied on a minute-to-minute basis.
The case of Lily (Griffiths, 2003) may well serve as a useful example of motiva-
tional dynamism.
Lily
Lily was a 26-year-old Swiss-German student who arrived in New
Zealand with her husband planning to immigrate. In order to strengthen
her eligibility for immigration and also for employment as a teacher, she
enrolled at a language school. Her English level was already high, and she
was an excellent student, working hard, enthusiastic in class, conscientious
with homework, and also willing to support other students whose English
was at a lower level than hers.
However, an interesting change occurred about half-way through the
course when she and her husband decided to apply for permanent residence.
Suddenly she urgently needed proof of her English level, so she sat the
IELTS (International English Language Testing System) exam and was
delighted to obtain a band score of 8.5 (The top band of 9 is often reckoned
to be at or near native-speaker level). Nevertheless, their residence applica-
tion was declined, which left Lily bitterly disappointed.
This rejection seemed to really affect Lily’s motivation. She was much less
positive in class, and homework was less carefully attended to. In addition,
when her mother came to visit from Switzerland, she became very homesick.
Although she had been expected to do very well in the exam for which she
was studying (Cambridge Proficiency in English/CPE), she did not achieve as
high a grade as had been anticipated.
About 18 months later, however, Lily and her husband came into the
school to show us their new baby and to tell us that they had just received
their permanent residence. Lily was back to her bright, bubbly self, confi-
dently anticipating getting a job as a teacher in the near future, and her
English was excellent.
Lily’s case would therefore seem to support the idea that rather than being
a static phenomenon, motivation can and does change dynamically in response
to changes in other factors which impact a learner’s life and state of mind. ◄
Discussion
The studies presented above contribute to the idea that motivation is not a simple
phenomenon. On the contrary, it is extremely complex, as suggested by Griffiths
and Özgür’s (2013) study which found that students’ own assessment of their moti-
vation did not accord with the motivational type which produced the best course
results. The same inconsistency was found by Tugan (2015).
Motivation is also dynamic, that is, it can and does change. This dynamism is
demonstrated by the case of Lily, who went from being a highly motivated student
at the beginning of the course, to losing her motivation after a rejection, to regaining
her motivation and continuing on her successful language learning trajectory.
Another student who demonstrated motivational dynamism was J., who began as an
unmotivated and unsuccessful student, but who responded to an extrinsic motivator
(offered by his father) and became a highly successful student, continuing under the
impetus of intrinsic motivation beyond the point where he had achieved his prize (a
brand-new sports car). This accords with the conclusions of a study by Mercer
(2011) who conducted 21 interviews with her single participant (Joana) over 2 years,
and found that Joana’s motivation appeared to vary dynamically, depending on the
specific parameters (such as language domain) under consideration. Mercer (ibid.)
therefore argues that a complex system (such as motivation) is dynamic and always
in a constant state of flux.
188 12 Motivation
Socio-ecological Context
Goal-orientation
Related to goal-orientation may well be the vision that one holds of oneself for the
future, which is well-recognized as a powerful source of motivation (e.g. Dörnyei &
Ushioda, 2009). As Lamb (2011, p. 177) explains, “the self-identity we wish for in
the future can be a source of motivation…which will help us achieve that identity”.
Lamb (ibid.) describes a study which he undertook in Indonesia among 12 learners
of English, as a result of which he found that the motivated learners took personal
Other Factors Related to Motivation 189
control of their learning and exploited the available resources; they also had clear
goals for the future. By comparison, the future vision of the less motivated students
remained
vague and tentative. A common feature of their talk about the future is frequent hedging,
indicating feelings of uncertainty about what will happen and a lack of personal agency in
securing favourable outcomes…there is a sense of obligation in their statements as if they
are motivated more by fear of failure than a true vision of a future English-speaking self.
(p. 177)
Volition
Associated with motivation, the idea of volition has been around for some time
(e.g. Corno, 1989). Although a familiar concept in the psychological literature
(e.g. Kuhl, 1984), and in other fields (such as health, e.g. Karasaki, Fraser, Moore,
& Dietze, 2013), for some reason it has failed to make a great deal of impact in
the language learning field. One reason for this may be the similarity with motiva-
tion, which has, of course, absorbed a great deal of interest from language educa-
tors, and this may have led to the feeling that we really don’t need another similar
term. Nevertheless, volition, although it may overlap with motivation, is not
exactly the same.
According to Corno (1989, p. 111), volition is “the capacity to protect one’s own
psychological states”. Oxford and Lee (2008) explain volition as what good learners
do to “keep themselves going when the going gets tough” (p. 313). A similar ques-
tion is asked by Svinicki (2016, p. 11): “When the going gets tough, what do good
students do?” She points to what she calls the Rubicon Model,
190 12 Motivation
alluding to the observation that striving for a goal progresses in two stages: the assertion of
an intention to act and, on the other side of the Rubicon, the realities of following through.
A lot of research has been conducted on the motivational side of the Rubicon model, what
motivates the learner initially, but not as much on the volitional side, the processes that sup-
port working toward the goal.
Investment
It would seem self-evident that learners’ level of motivation will be a major factor
in determining the level of investment they are prepared to devote to the language
learning process. The investment concept was introduced to the language learning
literature by Norton Peirce (1995), following the economic metaphors of Bourdieu
(1977) who describes linguistic development as cultural capital. As Norton Peirce
(1995) explains, when learners invest in language learning, an identity-enhancing
return on their investment is expected. Ushioda (2008, p. 24) further explains:
When learners invest in learning a new language, they do so with the understanding that
they will acquire a wider range of symbolic and material resources, which will enhance
their cultural capital, their conception of themselves, and their desires for the future. (p. 24)
Investment can take the form of time, attention and effort (in addition, perhaps,
to money). When a number of successful learners of English were surveyed by
Griffiths (2018b), the majority of the participants gave responses to the item on
investment in the “agree” (rating=4) to “strongly agree” (rating=5). However, in
order for students to be able to maintain the motivation to continue to invest time
and effort, they need to be able to see a return on their investment (Darvin & Norton,
2015, 2018; Norton, 2012, 2017).
Cohen and Dörnyei (2002) suggest that teachers should actively address the issue of
student motivation in order to increase the effectiveness of instruction, and at times
teachers may feel guilty that unmotivated students are somehow their fault. However,
although the role of motivator is commonly listed among the roles that teachers are
Implications for Learning and Teaching 191
expected to perform (e.g. Harmer, 2012), one needs, perhaps to ask the extent to
which such an expectation is reasonable or even possible.
The indications are that the students whose interview data is reported in Griffiths
(2018b) succeeded or not largely on the basis of the motivation which they brought
with them into the classroom situation. We might therefore wonder whether it is
reasonable for teachers to be expected to motivate extremely unmotivated students,
a number of which are described in Griffiths (2003), for instance:
How reasonable is it to blame the teacher for these students’ lack of motivation? It
would, nevertheless, certainly seem reasonable to expect that anything a teacher might
be able to do in terms of increasing students’ motivation (for instance by establishing a
good classroom atmosphere and relationships with the students) may well bear divi-
dends in terms of successful learning and in terms of students’ willingness to invest the
time and effort required. This certainly appeared to be the case in a study by Ryan and
Patrick (2001), who investigated how students’ (N = 233) perceptions of changes in
their classroom environment related to changes in motivation and engagement. They
found that students’ perceptions of teacher support, and the teacher as promoting inter-
action and mutual respect were related to positive changes in motivation and engage-
ment, while students’ perceptions of the teacher as promoting performance goals were
related to negative changes. Similarly, when Wu (2003) examined the influence of
classroom environment on intrinsic motivation of young foreign language learners
using a quasi-experimental study triangulated with observation and interviews, it was
found that a predictable learning environment, moderately challenging tasks, necessary
instructional support, and evaluation emphasizing self-improvement which attributed
success or failure to controllable variables were effective ways to enhance learners’
self-perceptions of competence; in addition, freedom to choose the content, methods
and learning performance outcomes, as well as strategy training, led to increased
autonomy and higher levels of motivation.
Meeting students’ deficiency needs, including love, self-esteem or respect, as in
line with humanistic psychology (e.g. Maslow & Rogers, 1979) could also help the
students to feel more motivated, more responsible and more independent, so that
they can start to reflect on what they need to know in order to actualize their aims.
In other words, when freedom is allowed as well as responsibility given, the stu-
dents are most likely to feel valued and important, which will further help them to
express agency and develop volitional competence to act or deal with the problems
encountered. When they discover their self-determining agency or innate potential
192 12 Motivation
for a fully developed self, they will feel more ready to change and more motivated
to do what they want and need to do.
Conclusion
Questions to Consider
1. Why do you think there might be inconsistency between the kind of motivation
that students attribute to themselves and the way the phenomenon actually
relates to successful outcomes?
2. Do you think that the instrumental/integrative and/or the intrinsic/extrinsic
dichotomies are useful ways of viewing motivation?
3. Do you agree that motivation is contextualized? Can you think of any examples?
4. Do you agree that motivation is complex and dynamic? Can you think of
examples?
5. Do you think it was right for J.’s father to try to motivate his son as he did?
6. What do you think the future might be for Lily?
7. Do you agree that the concept of the ideal self might be a useful motivator?
8. How do you think the concept of volition might be more actively employed by
educators?
9. How are motivation and investment related?
10. What instructional strategies can teachers use to motivate extremely unmoti-
vated students as illustrated in Griffiths (2003)?
► Follow-up Task Conduct a case study of a learner you know whose motivation
has varied over time.
► Suggestions for Further Reading In addition to the references cited in text and
in the reference list, readers might like to consider:
Dear Participant: Please rate each item according to the following scale:
There is also a column for your comments. The items questions do not have a
right or wrong answer. Thank you for your contribution.
References
Al-Hoorie, A. (2016). Unconscious motivation. Part II: Implicit attitudes and L2 achievement.
Studies in Second Language Learning and Teaching, 6(4), 619–649.
Bourdieu, P. (1977). The economics of linguistic exchanges. Social Science Information, 16,
645–668.
Brown, H. D. (1994). Principles of language learning and teaching. Englewood Cliffs, NJ:
Prentice Hall.
Cohen, A., & Dörnyei, Z. (2002). Focus on the language learner: Motivation, styles and strat-
egies. In N. Schmitt (Ed.), An introduction to applied linguistics (pp. 170–190). London:
Edward Arnold.
Cohen, L., Manion, L., & Morrison, K. (2018). Research methods in education (8th ed.). London
and New York: Routledge.
Corno, L. (1989). Self-regulated learning: A volitional analysis. In B. Zimmerman & D. Schunk
(Eds.), Self-regulated learning and academic achievement: Theory, research and practice
(pp. 111–141). New York: Springer.
Darvin, R., & Norton, B. (2015). Identity and a model of investment in applied linguistics. Annual
Review of Applied Linguistics, 35, 36–56.
Darvin, R., & Norton, B. (2018). Identity, investment, and TESOL. In The TESOL Encyclopedia
of English Language Teaching (pp. 1–7). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Deci, E., & Ryan, R. (1980). The empirical exploration of intrinsic motivational processes. In
L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 13, pp. 39–80).
New York: Academic Press.
Dörnyei, Z. (2007). Research methods in applied linguistics. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Dörnyei, Z., MacIntyre, P., & Henry, A. (Eds.). (2015). Motivational dynamics in language learn-
ing. Bristol, UK: Multilingual Matters.