EBFinal Thesis
EBFinal Thesis
EBFinal Thesis
by
Emily Baldwin
May, 2020
Thesis written by
Emily Baldwin
Approved by
_____________________________________________________________________,
Advisor
Accepted by
ii
TABLE OF CONTENTS
LIST OF TABLES………..………………………………………………………………iv
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS……………………………………….………………………..v
CHAPTER
I. INTRODUCTION ………………………………….…………….………1
II. METHOD…………………………………………….………………….10
III. RESULTS………………………………………………………………..15
IV. DISCUSSION……………………………………………………………17
REFERENCES…………………………………………………………………………..26
APPENDIX
1. TABLES…………………………………………………………………30
iii
LIST OF TABLES
iv
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
The completion of this project would not have been possible without the support
and guidance of so many people. I would first like to thank my advisor, Dr. Mary Ann
Devine. She has been a wonderful mentor to me through her patience, encouragement,
and immense knowledge. I learned a great deal from her open-minded, creative approach
to research, and I am grateful to have had the opportunity to work with her. Alongside my
advisor, I would like to thank the rest of my defense committee: Dr. Lisa Audet, Dr. Amy
Miracle, and Dr. Shannon Ciesla, for their kind words of support, thoughtful questions,
I also want to give thanks to all my friends and family for their patience and
support throughout this process. I am so grateful to my parents, Ken and Angel Baldwin,
for always believing I could achieve my goals and expressing their unlimited care during
moments of uncertainty. A special thanks to Louise Bowers, who has always made me
feel like family and is a diligent proofreader. Finally, I want to thank my fiancé, Andrew
Bowers II, for listening to my every concern with patience and compassion, and for his
v
1
INTRODUCTION
known to discuss and be concerned with the effects of stress and are likely to employ
the internet (Bland et al., 2012). With college years being considered one of the most
stressful periods in a person’s life due to changes in health habits and greater academic
responsibilities, college students may use different stress management strategies to cope
(Hales, 2009). One strategy that has emerged in recent years is a sensory phenomenon
called Autonomous Sensory Meridian Response (ASMR), which has grown in popularity
due to its apparent ability to help people relax and/or fall asleep through stimulating
visuals and sounds, known as ASMR triggers (Poerio et al, 2018). Online, many people
seek out this experience by watching and creating videos that include common triggers
videos and a change in emotional response in college students with high sensory
processing sensitivity (SPS). SPS was described by a review by Greven and colleagues as
a common, heritable trait involving increased information processing in the brain. This
population was chosen based on previous studies that found a relationship between high
SPS and negative affect, which is the terminology used that means experience of
2
emotion. Evidence from previous studies demonstrated that college students with high
SPS may be under an even greater level of stress than a typical college student, and
therefore have a greater need for effective, accessible emotional regulation strategies
positive and negative experiences, with negative experiences increasing risk for negative
from negative environment conditions (Greven et al, 2019). These conditions can include
physical, social, and sensory stimuli, as well as internal events such as one’s thoughts,
feelings, and sensations. During the ages between 18-24 years old, college students with
SPS may need relief from the extra stress that comes with pursing a higher education
degree. It is reasonable to conclude that those with high SPS would be more affected by
Several studies have found a relationship between individuals having high SPS
and negative affect. In a study by Engel-Yeger and Dunn (2011), typical adult population
aged 18-50 years completed the Adolescent/Adult Sensory Profile (AASP) to measure
sensory processing sensitivity levels and the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule
(PANAS) to measure affect. The aim of the study was to strengthen previous findings
regarding sensory processing patterns and affect in typical adults, as most evidence prior
3
to this study was related to children. Researchers made a distinction between different
behavior strategies, including two high neurological threshold groups and two low
neurological groups, based on their AASP scores. The high neurological threshold groups
were low registration, individuals with a passive behavior strategy who fail to detect
stimuli that others notice, and sensation seeking, individuals with an active behavior
strategy who experience pleasure from a rich sensory environment. Similarly, the low
neurological threshold groups are defined by their behavior strategies with sensory
The results of the study by Engel-Yeger and Dunn (2011) supported their
hypotheses that negative affect was positively correlated with low neurological threshold
sensory processing patterns, positive affect was positively correlated with sensation
positive/negative affect between the four sensory processing patterns. Researchers noted
that most studies examining sensory sensitivity and affect primarily analyzed low
neurological threshold groups, and speculated that these individuals are “more noticeable
in research and are referred more frequently to therapy, because of their hypervigilance,
aggression, need to control or avoid situations and the emotional burden and distress that
they experience” (Engel-Yeger & Dunn, 2011, p. 461). This is relevant to the present
study as the main interest was with a sensory sensitive low threshold group, due to the
4
potential presence of maladaptive traits and noted correlational findings with negative
the relationship between sensory processing difficulties and anxiety as both a trait and a
state. State anxiety refers to emotional response to stressful environments that is subject
individual differences in anxiety tendency that is relatively stable. Using the AASP and
the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory self-reporting measures, findings were that participants
with sensory hypersensitivity demonstrated both elevated trait anxiety and state anxiety
(Engel-Yeger & Dunn 2011, Spielberger 1972). The significant predictor for state anxiety
researchers based on these conclusions were that interventions that utilize sensory
profiles and employ strategies that consider sensory needs could be helpful in improving
anxiety levels. These findings support the assumption of the current study that college
students with sensory processing sensitivity would be more anxious than what is typical
the relationship between SPS and symptoms of anxiety and stress through using different
emotional regulation measures to measure anxiety and depression. They then explored if
5
emotional regulation can contribute to negative affect (Barlow, 1991). Using the Highly
Sensitive Person Scale (HSPS) and the Depression, Anxiety, and Stress Scales (DASS-
21), researchers found that there was a positive relationship between SPS and negative
psychological symptoms which was consistent with previous studies. They also found
that this relationship was partially mediated through emotional regulation, as aversive
internal states experienced by highly sensitive persons may limit their belief that they can
tolerate or cope with negative affective states (Brindle et al, 2015). Researchers
regarding dealing with emotions as well as therapeutic strategies such as mindfulness and
affective states. This is relevant to the current study as researchers are interested in
mediating the relationship between negative emotional states and SPS through watching
ASMR, which has been associated with mindfulness (Fredborg, Clark, & Smith, 2018).
Understanding ASMR
ASMR has been defined by some studies as the tingling sensations reported by
individuals after experiencing triggers (Fredborg, Clark, & Smith, 2018). However, often
the goal in experiencing ASMR is achieving a more positive emotional state. Under this
context, ASMR videos are the sensory tool that researchers are using to establish if there
6
is a correlation between engaging with ASMR content and changes in affect in the target
In a study by Barratt and Davis (2015), ASMR was examined to explore the
extent to which engagement with ASMR may ease symptoms of depression, which is
related to negative affect as the constructs are related (Watson, Clark, & Carey, 1988).
and mood, as well as various triggers and sensations participant could have experienced.
The study found that 80% of participants reported a positive influence in mood after
watching ASMR (Barratt & Davis, 2015). Another important finding from this study was
that 50% of participants said their mood improved even in sessions when no tingling
sensation was produced, while 30% said that achieving this sensation was vital to mood
improvement. This supports the idea that tingling sensations are not essential to the
moderate to severe on the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI II), 69% reported using
ASMR content to ease their symptoms of depression and the overall mean improvement
21.33 (STD=13.58) in non-depressed participant (Barratt & Davis, 2015). The study
suggests further investigation into ASMR for improving mood and pain symptoms as “a
potential therapeutic measure similar to meditation and mindfulness” (Barratt & Davis,
2015).
7
related effects. The purpose of the study was to investigate the relationship between
tendency to view the world in a mindful way. Participants completed the Toronto
Mindfulness Scale (TMS) and the Mindful Attention and Awareness Scale (MAAS), as
well as a questionnaire about ASMR experiences. The results were that participants who
experienced ASMR scored significantly higher on the Mindfulness scales than the Non-
ASMR control group, which suggests ASMR and mindfulness are related constructs.
These results are relevant because improving mindfulness has been an established tool to
help emotional regulation, and the present study is looking to see if ASMR elicits similar
Another study has also used ASMR to explore why people engage with this
content, what individuals report feeling after watching ASMR, and whether there are
measurable therapeutic benefits. Poerio, Blakely, Hostler and Veltri (2018) studied
ASMR in a large-scale online experiment to test the emotional correlates of the ASMR
response. Their aim was to use ASMR to elicit measurable affective responses from
groups of individuals who reported experiencing tingling sensations and those who did
not, for the purpose of comparing differences in outcomes. Participants watched two
ASMR clips and one control clip, and then self-reported their affective response on
several dimensions using the Multi-affect Indicator as well as how frequently they
8
experienced tingling sensations during the videos. Results indicated relative affective
differences between individuals who reported experiencing tingling sensations and those
who did not, but both groups reported overall higher levels of positive affect such as
excitement and calmness and lower levels of stress after watching ASMR. There were no
differences in affective response to the control videos for either participant group, which
demonstrates the change in affect was based on viewing ASMR videos and not a general
response to any video. This relates to the current study as researchers are aiming to
Dunn (2011) and Poerio et al (2018), using the Highly Sensitive Person (HSP) scale and
the Multi-affect Indicator to collect data on a broader sample of college students from
Kent State University. These studies were chosen because researchers are trying to
determine if their results can be replicated with this population. The Engel-Yeger and
Dunn study found a correlation between sensory processing patterns and negative affect
by analyzing data given by participants through the AASP and PANAS surveys. Poerio
and colleagues studied relationships between watching ASMR and affect, in which affect
was improved after watching ASMR videos and not after watching the control videos. In
combining ideas from both studies, researchers hope to find relationships between
9
watching ASMR and affect in participants with high SPS using these self-reporting
scales. With this population as the focus as opposed to tingle sensitivity, we could
compare the changes in affect to see how this group is affected by ASMR.
negative affect and anxiety and the trait of sensory processing sensitivity, the present
study aimed to examine if college students with high SPS would elicit similar positive
affective outcomes after watching ASMR videos (Brindle et al, 2015; Engel-Yeger &
Dunn, 2011; Greven et al, 2019). In studying the effect of high SPS on responses to
sensitivity.
METHOD
Participants
Study Eligibility
Participants eligible for inclusion in this study were college students between the
ages of 18-24 years enrolled at Kent State University. Researchers omitted data from
participants who did not complete the study, and participants who took less than 14
minutes or longer than 1 hour to complete the study. This omission was to ensure all
videos were watched and questions were answered in one sitting to limit confounding
variables.
Recruitment
Participants were recruited through flyers as well as mass emailing 7,500 students
who met the age criteria. Flyers were posted in 16 different buildings across the Kent
State University main campus inviting interested individuals to email the researchers in
order to participate. After FERPA approval, the Kent State Registrar’s office provided
researchers with a list containing the emails of all eligible students. Mass emails were
sent to 7,500 eligible students that were chosen randomly through Qualtrics. An initial
email was sent to invite students to participate, followed by three reminder emails to
within a two-month period (during which the survey was active online). To qualify for
analysis in this study, a threshold score of 4.7 on the HSPS needed to be met for
participants to be considered “highly sensitive”, resulting in the final sample size of 29.
Data was collected anonymously along with other college students who may have lower
levels of SPS in order for individuals with high SPS to not be singled out to do this study.
Participant Demographics
through the survey. The study participants ranged in age from 18 to 24, with a mean age
of 21. A majority of the participants were female (82.8%), 13.8% were male, and 3.4%
were non-binary. Also, most of the participants were Caucasian (93.1%), and 6.9%
reported a Biracial or Multi-racial ethnicity. The class rank distribution of the participants
was 41.4% seniors, 27.6% were juniors, 20.7% were sophomores, and 10.3% were
freshmen. In regards to familiarity with ASMR videos, 44.8% of the participants reported
that they were extremely familiar with ASMR and watch ASMR videos on a regular
basis, 17.2% were very familiar and have watched more than one ASMR video, 20.7%
were moderately familiar and have watched at least one ASMR video, 10.3% were
slightly familiar and have never watched an ASMR video, and 6.9% were not familiar at
Instruments were chosen based on their availability, number of items, and ability
to measure the constructs of affect and SPS in previous studies. The scales measuring
affect and SPS were freely available for research purposes by Warr (2014) and Aron &
Aron (1997) respectively. Also, the scales were chosen because they contained fewer
items than other instruments that measured the same constructs in the interest of
accessibility and reducing the time needed to take the survey. On average, the survey
class standing, gender, and ethnicity. It also included a question regarding how familiar
the individual was with ASMR videos on a scale from 1 to 5, with 1 being “Extremely
The Highly Sensitive Person Scale (HSPS) (Aron & Aron, 1997): All subjects
completed this instrument, which is a self-reporting tool including 27 items that are rated
according to how sensitive subjects are to certain situations or stimuli. All items are
scored in the same direction from 1 to 7, with 1 being the least sensory sensitive and 7
being the most sensitive. Participants’ sensitivity levels scored by calculating an item
mean score. A score of 4.7 was the threshold for the “highly sensitive” group, the subject
group included in this study. This scale was chosen instead of the AASP measure used in
the Engel-Yeger and Dunn study to determine the level of SPS because while they
measure similar constructs, HSPS is more accessible and includes fewer items.
13
The Multi-Affect Indicator (MAI) (Warr, 2014): Items from this instrument were
used to measure the affective outcomes of watching each video. It included 16 items that
specify different kinds of feeling not only in terms of the conventional negative-to-
positive affect but also through low-to-high mental arousal or activation (Remington,
Fabrigar, & Visser, 2000; Russell, 1980, 2003). Item responses are scored from 1 to 7 to
rate how often an emotion is felt, with 1 indicating “never” and 7 indicating “always”.
While scores for 4 different variations of affect can be measured with the items, for the
purpose of this study researchers are only examining mean scores for positive and
negative affect without considering activation level. This scale was used in the study by
Poerio et al and has been reported to have high content validity and reliability (Warr,
2014).
watched three ASMR videos, as well as two control videos. Video clips were each
approximately three minutes in length and included: six soft-spoken ASMR videos (three
with a female voice and three with a male voice), three ASMR videos with sound, but no
speaking, and, six control (non-ASMR) videos (three with a speaking component and
three with sound, but no speaking). ASMR videos were taken from YouTube and were
selected by the author on the basis that they contained multiple ASMR triggers (Poerio et
al, 2018). Control (non-ASMR videos) were also selected from YouTube and were
chosen in order to mimic the content of ASMR videos as closely as possible without
Design
The study used a pre-post design where research participants completed the
Multi-Affect Indicator before and after viewing of videos. The dependent variable was
self-reported changes in affect experienced after watching the videos. The Highly
Sensitive Person scale was also completed before the videos were presented.
Procedure
After receiving ethical approval from the Kent State University institutional
review board, participants were recruited through flyers posted across the Kent State
University main campus as well as mass emailing 7,500 students who met the age
online survey on Qualtrics, participants gave an initial response to items on the MAI
pretest to provide a comparative measure of affect. Then, participants took the HSPS to
determine sensory sensitivity level. Scores on these measures were not available for
participants to view. Then participants watched one of each type of video, randomly
chosen from the preselected videos. After each video, participants completed a posttest
RESULTS
The results of this study were corrected from Qualtrics to be analyzed through
SPSS. Pearson’s correlation tests were calculated to examine the relationship between
HSPS score and negative and positive affect scores on the pretest MAI in individuals who
scored a 4.7 or above on the HSPS. Paired samples T-tests were also computed, in order
to examine whether significant differences in MAI scores exist between the different
Hypothesis 1
Hypothesis 1 (H₁): Negative affect will be positively correlated with high levels of
sensory sensitivity.
Results of this study did not support H₁. As seen in Table 2, correlation between
pretest negative affect and HSPS scores were not statistically significant. While not
hypothesized, there was a significant correlation between HSPS score and pretest positive
affect, r=.419, p<.05. This suggests that within this sample a high score on the HSPS is
Hypothesis 2
Hypothesis 2 (H₂): Affect will improve significantly after watching ASMR videos.
16
T-test were conducted on the results of research subjects who scored a 4.7 or
higher on the HSPS to examine the change in positive affect from pre to post testing of
MIA after viewing ASMR videos. The results from the paired sample t-test for positive
affect revealed no change in positive affect after watching ASMR videos, but there was a
statistically significant change in the control video between the no voice control video
type and feelings of positive affect. This means the data does not support the hypothesis
of significant improved positive affect after watching ASMR type videos. See Table 3,
which displays t-test results between the pretest positive affect and each post-video
positive affect.
Second, another t-test was conducted on the results of research subjects who
scored a 4.7 or higher on the HSPS to examine the change in negative affect from pre to
post testing of MIA after viewing ASMR videos. Results show significant changes in
negative affect after watching all video types. This suggests that negative affect was
reduced throughout the survey compared to the initial pretest MAI measurement, but it
does not support the hypothesis that watching ASMR type videos caused this change as
negative affect improved after watching control videos as well. As seen in table 4, there
were changes in the mean between level of the pretest negative affect and each post-video
DISCUSSION
The objective of the present study was to replicate findings from a previous study
that examined a relationship between negative affect and high SPS, as well as the effects
of watching ASMR videos on affect people who scored a 4.7 on the HSPS. The
hypotheses of this study were that negative affect would be positively correlated with
high levels of sensory sensitivity, and that affect would improve significantly after
watching ASMR videos. While the results of the present study did not support these
hypotheses, there are important implications and critiques that could be utilized in future
The present study examined the relationship between negative affect score on the
MAI scale and HSPS score in individuals who scored a 4.7 or above. While it was
hypothesized that negative affect would be positively correlated with high levels of
sensory sensitivity, no significant correlation between these two constructs were found.
This may be because of the size of the sample, and environmental factors outside of the
study. There was, however, a significant correlation between positive affect and HSPS
score in this sample, r=.419, p<.05. This is inconsistent with findings from previous
studies that found a positive relationship between negative affect and HSPS (Brindle et
al, 2015; Engel-Yeger & Dunn, 2011). If high SPS was related to positive affectivity
instead of negative affectivity, then the population would not necessarily need an
18
emotional regulation tool like ASMR videos. However, due to how consistently the
relationship between high SPS and negative affect has been found in previous literature,
researchers speculate that their findings were different because of limitations (Aron &
Aron, 1997; Bakker & Moulding, 2012; Liss, Timmel, Baxley, & Killingsworth, 2005).
When analyzing a relationship between negative affect and high SPS, the small
size of the sample (N=29) could have been a contributing factor. Individual differences in
affect would have been more pronounced, meaning there could be outlier individuals
with high SPS who abnormally low negative affect scores had affecting the results.
Another rational for the absence of this relationship could be explained in a study by Liss
et al. (2005), which suggested that highly sensitive people are not necessarily predisposed
they are more likely to experience negative psychological symptoms compared with
individuals with normative sensory processing (Liss et al.). The individuals in this sample
may have scored high on the HSPS but were not experiencing a situation that would have
caused negative affect. Another related factor is that college can be a challenging
environment for any individual, but these challenges are not constant over time. The
survey was distributed in January of 2020, during the beginning of the semester which
could elicit different responses if it was completed during another time in the semester
such as final exam week. Given the relationship found between HSPS score and positive
affect, it is possible that at this time college was a positive environment for this sample of
Positive Affect
Results of research subjects who scored a 4.7 or higher on the HSPS were
examined to determine if there were changes in positive affect from pre to post testing of
MIA after viewing ASMR videos. After watching ASMR videos, there was no overall
change in positive affect between the pretest MAI and posttest after watching the no
voice control video type. This means the data does not support the hypothesis of
significant improved positive affect after watching ASMR type videos. Researchers
speculate these results could be related to the choice of materials as well as instrument
limitations. The type of ASMR videos used in this study was replicated from a study by
Poerio et al., (2018) but the exact videos used in the previous study were not able to be
obtained. While researchers chose video clips that were representative of common ASMR
content, it would have been preferable to use video clips that have already been shown to
influence affect. Similarly, the no voice control video eliciting positive change in positive
affect could be contributed to the video clips being too different from the others, which
Negative Affect
The results of research subjects who scored a 4.7 or higher on the HSPS were
examined to identify changes in negative affect from pre to post testing of MIA after
viewing ASMR videos. Results show statistically significant changes in negative affect
20
after watching all video types for this population. This suggests that while negative affect
decreased post-test, it does not support the hypothesis that the type of video watched was
relative to this change as negative affect improved after watching the control videos as
well. Given that the control videos also resulted in a decrease in negative affect it could
be speculated that the process in and of itself may reduce negative affect.
These results could have also been influenced by the order the video clips were
presented in, as video clips were always viewed starting with female voice ASMR, then
male voice ASMR, no voice ASMR, voice control, and lastly no voice control. In
showing the videos in this order that starts with the ASMR types, participants could have
negative affect over time. Given that the no voice control video was the last video to
shown and resulted in the greatest positive change in negative affect, this outcome could
be due to the cumulative intervention effect of the ASMR videos instead of the control
video itself. If the videos had been presented in a random order, it would represent the
Despite the inability to state ASMR videos as the contributing factor in the
decrease of negative affect, these results do have significant implications regarding the
effect of ASMR and people with high SPS. Negative affect was reduced through an
videos in general can improve negative affect in this population. ASMR videos are
intended to help remove one’s self from the negativity of everyday life and focus on the
21
ASMR triggers that are presented. Evidence of reduced negative affect while watching all
videos could be explained through the context of the study having participants focus on
videos while bringing awareness to how they are feeling, which reflects the way ASMR
videos are meant to be watched. Perhaps individuals with high SPS are positively
impacted by focusing on videos in this context, or by a potential change the person’s state
of mind when viewing. Although the exact cause of reduced negative affect cannot be
determined, the results show a potential link between effect of ASMR and people who
This study found a link between a reduction in negative affect and watching
ASMR videos. Applications of ASMR could be used with high SPS college students as
well as other populations give the ease of access of these videos. Students could be
introduced to ASMR as a tool to reduce negative affect alongside a list of other mental
health resources on a college website. For example, students could use these videos
occasionally, in times of stress such as final exam week, to manage stress or improve
could be produced to present ASMR to any population that could benefit from this tool. If
the results of the current study were reflected on a larger scale by introducing more
people to ASMR, negative affect could be reduced for those are vulnerable to negative
psychological outcomes.
22
The findings need to be considered with respect to the limitations of the current
study. A convenience sample of Kent State University students was used as the research
subject pool, which could have led to a homogeneous subject pool that does not reflect
the population. This also could have contributed to the small number of students who met
the 4.7 HSPS threshold. There were challenges using the MAI scale, despite its use in
previous inquiries examining ASMR and affect. Specifically, the instrument used a
confusing scoring process that made comparing positive and negative difficult in terms of
numerical value. Authors of the measure instruct that negative values should be reverse
scored, so that higher scores always represent higher well-being (Warr, 2014). Since
researchers were not measuring well-being, inverting the scores was not relevant for
measuring affect. Other studies that used this instrument also did not invert scores if the
aim was not to examine well-being, thus the scores were not inverted for this study
(Poerio et al, 2018). While the ideas about low-to-high mental activation being measured
alongside positive and negative affect could be relevant in a study about the effects of
ASMR, this method was not used in this study as researchers were focused on emotional
state as opposed to arousal to stimuli. Another scale such as the PANAS survey may have
measured positive and negative affect score with less difficulty. One other design-related
limitation was that the survey did not include any questions regarding participants’ use of
medications, which could have mitigated emotional response to the ASMR videos and
Another set of limitations of the study were related to the implementation of the
online survey. There was a lack of incentive for students to complete a relatively long
online survey, with the average time to complete the study being more than 20 minutes.
Without providing course extra credit or potential financial gain, it was a challenge to
recruit participants. Students who self-selected to participate in the study may have been
interested because they were already familiar with ASMR, thus had pre-disposed
opinions or feelings about this type of video, which could have influenced the subject
group. Also, the issue of limited response rate was exacerbated by mistakes in creating
the online survey through Qualtrics. Participants were able to skip past video clips
without watching them, which was reflected in their quick completion time. This created
more invalid responses than there may have been otherwise. Also, while each type of
video had 3 videos that were randomly selected from, the types of video were not in a
randomized order. These mistakes in the online survey’s design could have affected the
Future Research
With interest in studying ASMR increasing for potential use in decreasing stress,
anxiety, and nervousness, future research should focus on how different populations
could benefit from experiencing ASMR. Groups with net negative affect should be
beneficial in achieving a more positive emotional state. Studying ASMR in this way
24
could lead to greater understanding regarding how and why many people consider it
Implications from this study suggest changes in study design and composition if
replicated. A larger sample size should be used in order to account for more variables and
a more accurate reflection of the population in terms of diversity. Researchers should also
consider exploring different materials to measure affect, or utilize the measures of low-
to-high mental activation alongside positive and negative affect within the MAI that
could be relevant in a study about the effects of ASMR that were not used in this study.
Future researchers should also consider using laboratory-based methods to address these
constructs in order to control for more confounds or redesigning the online survey to
Conclusion
between viewing ASMR videos and a change in emotional response in college students
with high sensory processing sensitivity (SPS). Researchers hypothesized that negative
affect would be positively correlated with high levels of sensory sensitivity, and that
affect would improve significantly after watching ASMR videos. While the hypotheses of
the present study were not supported, a larger scale study with a more representative
25
sample should be conducted to further explore ASMR videos and their effects on
populations with a need for an accessible tool for emotional regulation. Accessibility
through technology is one of the most important developments for all people, especially
those who may be more vulnerable. It is important to study the potential outcomes of an
organically developed online intervention like ASMR videos because researchers can
learn if it works to reduce negative affectivity, why it works, and how it could be best
REFERENCES
345-368.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2012.04.006
Barratt EL, Davis NJ. 2015. Autonomous Sensory Meridian Response (ASMR): a flow-
Belsky, J., Jonassaint, C., Pluess, M., Stanton, M., Brummett, B., & Williams, R. (2009).
Bishop SR, Lau M, Shapiro S, Carlson L, Anderson ND, Carmody J, Segal ZV, Abbey S,
BLAND, H. W., MELTON, B. F., WELLE, P., & BIGHAM, L. (2012). Stress Tolerance:
New Challenges for Millennial College Students. College Student Journal, 46(2),
http://search.ebscohost.com.proxy.library.kent.edu/login.aspx?direct=true&db=s3
h&AN=77698067&site=ehost-live
27
Brindle, K., Moulding, R., Bakker, K., & Nedeljkovic, M. (2015). Is the relationship
Dimock, M. (2019, January 17). Defining generations: Where Millennials end and
tank/2019/01/17/where-millennials-end-and-generation-z-begins/
Engel-Yeger, B., & Dunn, W. (2011). Exploring the Relationship between Affect and
Engel-Yeger, Batya, and Winnie Dunn. (2011). "The relationship between sensory
Fredborg BK, Clark JM, Smith SD. 2018. Mindfulness and autonomous sensory meridian
Greven, C., Lionetti, F., Boothe, C., Aron, E., Fox. E., Schendang, H., Pluess, M.,
Bruining, H., Acevedo, B., Bijttebier, P., and Homberga, J. (2019), Sensory
http://college.cengage.com/health/course360/personal_health_1111764875/versio
n_2/ebook/hales_27001_ch01.pdf
Lionetti, F., Aron, A., Aron, E. N., Burns, G. L., Jagiellowicz, J., & Pluess, M. (2018).
Liss, M., Timmel, L., Baxley, K., & Killingsworth, P. (2005). Sensory
.2005.05.007
Pluess, M., Assary, E., Lionetti, F., Lester, K. J., Krapohl, E., Aron, E. N, & Aron, A.
Poerio GL, Blakey E, Hostler TJ, Veltri T (2018) More than a feeling: Autonomous
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0196645
Remington, N. A., Fabrigar, L. R., & Visser, P. S. (2000). Re-examining the circumplex
ed. Anxiety: current trends in theory and research (Vol. 1). New York:
Academic Press.
Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Carey, G. (1988). Positive and negative affectivity and their
97(3), 346.
30
TABLES
Table 1
Demographic Characteristic Statistics of Individuals who scored at or above 4.7 on the
HSPS
Descriptive Characteristics Frequency Percent
1 Gender
Female 24 82.8
Male 4 13.8
Non-Binary 1 3.4
2 Ethnicity
Caucasian 27 93.1
3 Class Rank
Freshmen 3 10.3
Sophomore 6 20.7
Junior 8 27.6
Senior 12 41.4
4 ASMR Familiarity
Table 2
Correlations Between HSPS and Positive/Negative Affect of Individuals who scored at or
above 4.7 on the HSPS
Paired Samples Correlation Sig.
Table 3
Change in Positive Affect between Pre-Posttest MAI Video
Paired Samples Mean Std. Deviation t df Sig. (2-tailed)
Table 4
Changes in Mean Negative Affect Level Between Pre-Posttest MAI Video
Paired Samples Mean Std. Deviation Std. Error Mean
Table 5
Change in Negative Affect between Pre-Posttest MAI Video MAI
Paired Samples Mean Std. Deviation t df Sig. (2-tailed)