Foundations of Qualitative Methods 5
Foundations of Qualitative Methods 5
METHODS
KEY POINTS IN THIS CHAPTER
T Phenomenologists
and experiences.
attempt to understand
the person’s perceptions
1
FOUNDATIONS OF QUALITATIVE METHODS
73
This can be used to understand people’s feelings, thoughts, ways of
under- standing the world, or ways of communicating with others.
A simplified illustration of the difference between the quantitative and
the qualitative approach is shown in the differing responses to the
question ‘‘How are you feeling today?’’ A quantitatively oriented
researcher might ask the participant to respond on a seven-point scale,
ranging from 1 = ‘‘Very unhappy’’ to 7 = ‘‘Very happy,’’ and receive an
answer of 5, signifying ‘‘Somewhat happy.’’ A qualitative researcher
might ask the same person the same question, ‘‘How are you feeling
today?’’, but request an open-ended answer, which could run something
like ‘‘Not too bad, although my knee is hurting me a little, and I’ve just
had an argument with my boyfriend. On the other hand, I think I might
be up for promotion at work, so I’m excited about that.’’ In other words,
the quantitative approach yields data which are relatively simple to
process, but are limited in depth and hide ambiguities; the qualitative
approach yields a potentially large quantity of rich, complex data
which may be difficult and time consuming to analyze.
However, the difference between quantitative and qualitative
approaches to research is about more than the difference between
numbers and words; it is also about epistemology, the theory of what
knowledge consists of (see Chapter 2). As we noted in Chapter 4,
quantitative research is largely based on the philosophy of positivism.
Qualitative researchers usually reject positivism, often quite
vehemently, instead preferring non-realist epistemological positions
based on developing understanding rather than on testing hypotheses
(Bryman, 1988). We will describe these positions more fully below.
It is worth noting in passing that there is one potential source of
confusion over the meaning of the word ‘‘qualitative,’’ as it also has a
second distinct meaning in research terminology. In quantitative
research, the term ‘‘qualitative data’’ is used to refer to nominal scale
data, to distinguish it from ordinal or interval scale data (see Chapter 4
on psychometric theory). Thus, census categories measuring ethnic
background (white European, black African, etc.) may be referred to as
a ‘‘qualitative’’ variable (because they have no ordering property), even
though the data are analyzed by quantitative methods. However, in this
book we will reserve the term ‘‘qualitative’’ for data that are collected
by open-ended questions or by observations that yield verbal
descriptions. Simple yes–no responses or nominal categories will be
considered as a form of quantitative data, since they are narrowly
delimited. The qualitative–quantitative distinction, as we are using it,
boils down to whether the data are collected and analyzed as words or
numbers (including counts, proportions, multiple choice, and yes–no
responses).
Advantages
The main advantages of using qualitative methods are:
● They avoid the simplifications imposed by quantification, since
some things cannot be easily expressed numerically. That is, they
enable more complex
73
FOUNDATIONS OF QUALITATIVE METHODS
74
aspects of experience to be studied and impose fewer restrictions on the
data or the underlying theoretical models than quantitative approaches.
● They allow the researcher to address research questions that do not
easily lend themselves to quantification, such as the nature of
individual experiences of a psychological condition (e.g., eating
disorders) or event (e.g., being a victim of crime).
● They enable the individual to be studied in depth and detail.
● The raw data are usually vivid and easy to grasp: good qualitative
research reports make the participants come alive for the reader. In
general, the reports of qualitative studies are often more readable
than those of quantitative studies (except that some qualitative
researchers, especially those with postmodernist or existential-
phenomenological leanings, tend to write in an impenetrable jargon
all of their own).
● Qualitative methods are good for hypothesis generation, and for
exploratory, discovery-oriented research. They permit a more flexible
approach, allowing the researcher to modify his or her protocol in
mid-stream. The data collection is not constrained by pre-existing
hypotheses.
● Qualitative self-report methods usually give more freedom to the
participant than structured quantitative methods. For example, open-
ended questions give interviewees a chance to respond in their own
words and in their own way.
● Since the data collection procedures are less constrained, the
researchers may end up in the interesting position of finding things
that they were not originally looking for or expecting.
Historical Background
Qualitative methods can be traced back to the ancient Greek historians.
For example, Herodotus, who is often called the father of history, traveled
widely in the ancient world and recounted in his Histories the stories he
had heard from the people he met. His successors down the ages
recorded their observations of people that they encountered in their
travels. These kinds of observations eventually became formalized in the
discipline of anthropology.
In their modern form, qualitative methods were first used in ethnographic
fieldwork. In the early decades of the 20th century, the founders of
cultural anthropology, such as Malinowski and Boas, conducted
ethnographic observa- tions on cultural groups that were remote from
their own: Malinowski in the Trobriand Islands in Papua New Guinea and
Boas with the Kwakiutl tribe in the Pacific Northwest Coast of North
America. They spent many months living with and observing the cultures
they were studying. In the 1920s and 1930s, sociologists adapted these
methods to study subcultures within their own society. For example, the
‘‘Chicago school’’ of sociology tended to focus on people at the fringes
of society, such as criminals and youth gangs. A classic
74
FOUNDATIONS OF QUALITATIVE METHODS
75
example of this genre is Whyte’s (1943) Street corner society, which
was based on fieldwork with an Italian-American youth gang in Boston,
Massachusetts. Ethnographic methods started out being used to study
the ‘‘weird and wonderful’’ (from a Eurocentric viewpoint), e.g., Pacific
Island tribal cultures, and have been brought progressively closer to
bear on the investigators’ own culture, culminating in such
contemporary specialties as medical anthropology, which use
anthropological methods to study health and illness in our own culture
(Helman, 1990).
Some ethnographic work is located on the rather fuzzy boundary between
social science and journalism. A good example is Blythe’s (1979) The view
in winter, a moving account of people describing how they experience
being old. The distinction is that journalism seeks to report accurately
and produce an engaging story, whereas social science brings a body of
theory to bear on the subject matter, or seeks to develop theory
from the data, and it articulates its assumptions and procedures in
order to enable replication.
In clinical research, qualitative methods were first used in case histories
(see Chapter 9), for instance, Breuer and Freud’s (1895/1955) first
cases, which began the psychoanalytic tradition, and Watson and
Rayner’s (1920) study of ‘‘Little Albert,’’ which helped establish the
behavioral tradition. There is also a tradition of participant observation
methods in mental health research, though they are more often
conducted by sociologists than by psychologists. Classic examples of
participant observation studies are Goffman’s (1961) Asylums and
Rosenhan’s (1973) ‘‘Sane in insane places’’ study.
The two main qualitative data collection methods currently used in
clinical psychology research are in-depth interviewing (see Chapter 6)
and qualitative observation (see Chapter 7). Common data analytic
methods are grounded theory, various types of phenomenological
analysis, and discourse analysis (general principles of qualitative data
analysis are covered in Chapter 12).
PHENOMENOLOGICAL APPROACHES
Philosophical Background
Phenomenological approaches in psychology derive from the
phenomenological movement in philosophy, which developed in the late-
19th and early-20th centuries. It in turn is descended from the rationalist,
idealist philosophical tradition of Plato and Kant. Husserl was its
founder; Brentano, Heidegger,
76
PHENOMENOLOGICAL APPROACHES
77
Merleau-Ponty, and Sartre were key figures in its development
(Jennings, 1986; Spinelli, 1989). Their ideas were introduced into
psychology by Giorgi, Laing, May, and others (e.g., Giorgi, 1975; Laing,
1959; May et al., 1958), and were a major influence on the client-
centered, humanistic, and existential approaches to psychological
therapy.
Assumptions
We can distinguish four central assumptions of phenomenology. First,
percep- tion is regarded as the primary psychological activity, since our
perceptions give rise to what we do, think, and feel. Because of this,
perceived meaning is more important than objective reality, facts, or
events.
Second, understanding is regarded as being the true end of science (in
contrast, for example, to the aim of prediction and control that more
traditional hypothetico-deductive approaches espouse). The goal is to
produce explana- tions of the person’s experiences and actions in terms
of intentions, purposes, and meanings.
A third key assumption is that of multiple perspectives, also known as
‘‘epistemological pluralism.’’ Each person’s perspective has its own
validity (i.e., it is how they see things); therefore, multiple, differing
perspectives are equally valid and of interest for study. These multiple
perspectives constitute different life-worlds (in German, umwelten). For
example, the same aging oak tree is radically different when perceived
by the forester, the lost child, the fox, or the wood beetle. These life-
worlds are the object of study for the phenomenologist (Pollio, 1982).
Fourth, individuals’ perceptions of their life-worlds are based on implicit
assumptions or presuppositions, which phenomenologists also try to
understand. That is, what we perceive is built on multiple assumptions
about ourselves, others, and the world. These assumptions are the
taken-for-granted, unques- tioned context for our actions and
perceptions. For example, if an acquaintance greets you with ‘‘How are
you?’’, you are not usually expected to give an accurate or detailed
answer; in fact, to anyone but a close friend, it would seem quite odd to
do so—the underlying assumption is that we respond with a brief,
positive answer. Although we accept these underlying assumptions, we
are not generally aware of them and do not question them. In other
words, they are believed to be ‘‘known to all’’ and part of what
‘‘everybody knows that everybody knows’’ (Garfinkel, 1967).
77
PHENOMENOLOGICAL APPROACHES
78
Table 5.1 Types of phenomenological research
Empirical Phenomenology
The oldest systematic approach to phenomenological psychology is
associated with Duquesne University (Pittsburgh, USA), and published in
the Journal of Phenomenological Psychology (see also Duquesne studies
of phenomenological psychology, 1971 to 1983). Giorgi, Wertz, and
Fischer are three of the better known proponents. The approach stresses
in-depth analysis, often at first of single cases, aiming to describe the
main defining features of an experience (e.g., that of being criminally
victimized), and the different variations that the experience may have in
the population (analogous to the statistical ideas of mean and standard
deviation). Carrying out research in this tradition involves the practice of
the phenomenological method, a rigorous procedure that is based on two
key processes: bracketing and describing.
Bracketing is an attempt to set aside one’s assumptions and
expectations, as far as is humanly possible. However, because one’s
underlying assumptions are often hidden, it requires a special act of
reflection to identify them. This act has been described in several
different ways. The most common is ‘‘bracketing the natural attitude’’ (or
‘‘bracketing’’ for short). It involves a process of stepping back from the
phenomenon in order to see it as if from the outside, as if we were the
proverbial observer from Mars. Bracketing involves a special kind of
turning
78
PHENOMENOLOGICAL APPROACHES
79
away from the natural attitude, in which the researcher does not accept
a description as a statement about the world, but simply as a statement
about an experience of the world.
In the clinical context, bracketing is one aspect of the process of
empathy in such exploratory psychotherapies as client-centered and
experiential therapy. When a client says that she is ‘‘trapped’’ in a
situation, the therapist is not interested in determining whether this is
factually the case; what is important is that the client feels trapped
(Rogers, 1975). In contrast, beginning therapists generally prefer to
stay ‘‘within the natural attitude’’ by trying to talk the client out of such
presumed irrational beliefs, often questioning the facts of the situation.
One important component of empathy is letting go of one’s own
presuppositions, in order to understand what the client is trying to say.
A similar idea is found in the ideal therapist state of ‘‘evenly hovering,
free-floating attention’’ referred to in the psychoanalytic literature (e.g.,
Greenson, 1967).
A naive approach to bracketing might be to mentally steel oneself and
promise to give up one’s biases. However, a more fruitful alternative is
to begin by carefully investigating one’s assumptions. At the beginning
of a study, the researcher can conduct a thought experiment of carrying
out the study in imagination in order to identify expectations of what it
might find. This thought experiment might also be repeated at the end
of the study in order to identify additional expectations that only
became clear in the course of the study. These expectations take the
place of hypotheses in traditional research, but they are not the same.
In phenomenological research, expectations are not given a place of
honor at the end of the introduction, instead, they are figuratively
locked in a drawer until the study is over. Phenomenological research is
perhaps most exciting when it uncovers understandings that are
unexpected or even startling.
The second step in the empirical phenomenological method is
describing. Several principles are involved (see, for example, Spinelli,
1989). First, good descriptions focus on concrete or specific
impressions, as opposed to the abstract or general. Second, they avoid
evaluative terms such as ‘‘good or bad’’ and their many synonyms and
euphemisms (e.g., ‘‘ineffective,’’ ‘‘helpful’’), except where these are
part of the experience itself. Third, they tend to avoid explanations,
particularly early in the research. The task is to discover meaning,
not invent it. This means avoiding ‘‘why’’ questions or anything that
encourages the informant to speculate on causes or reasons: such
questions encourage intellectualization and interfere with the slow,
careful process of attending to concrete experience.
Grounded Theory
Grounded theory is probably the most common form of qualitative
analysis used today. It was developed by two North American
sociologists, Glaser and Strauss, in their 1967 book, The discovery of
grounded theory: Strategies for qualitative research. As the title
suggests, Glaser and Strauss were attempting to articulate how
qualitative data could be used not just to provide rich descriptions,
79
PHENOMENOLOGICAL APPROACHES
but also to 80
80
PHENOMENOLOGICAL APPROACHES
81
generate theory. Grounded theory can be used with a range of qualitative
material, such as semi-structured interviews, focus groups, participant
observation, and diaries.
The term ‘‘grounded theory’’ is potentially confusing, as it refers both to a
method—a set of systematic procedures for analyzing data—and also to
the outcome or product of the analysis, which is theory ‘‘grounded’’ in the
data. The basic process involves identifying categories at a low level of
abstraction and then building up to more abstract theoretical concepts.
The end point is often one or more core categories, which capture the
essence of the phenomenon (see Chapter 12). This process of analysis
occurs concurrently with the process of data collection, and the
developing theory guides the sampling strategy (‘‘theoretical sampling’’:
see Chapter 10).
The original Glaser and Strauss (1967) volume was more theoretical and
polemical rather than practical; it was aimed at challenging the prevailing
quantitative paradigm in American sociology. The practical implications
for researchers, i.e., the steps in actually carrying out a grounded theory
study, are developed in Glaser (1978) and Strauss and Corbin (1998).
The grounded theory method was taken up by psychologists in the 1980s
and 1990s. Articles by Rennie et al. (1988) and by Henwood and Pidgeon
(1992) were aimed at introducing the grounded theory approach to an
audience of psychologists. Rennie and Brewer’s (1987) study entitled
‘‘A grounded theory of thesis blocking’’ (i.e., writer’s block among
research students) may well be of personal interest to some readers of
this text! As more psychologists have taken up the invitation of Rennie et
al., and of Henwood and Pidgeon, grounded theory has become a popular
approach to qualitative research.
One example of the method in clinical psychology is Bolger’s (1999) study
of the phenomenon of emotional pain. The participants were women in a
therapy group for adult children of alcoholics; they were interviewed on
several occasions following group therapy sessions in which they had
explored painful life experiences. The interviews focused on how pain was
experienced and what was significant in that experience for them. The
core category that emerged from the analysis was labeled the ‘‘broken
self,’’ characterized by four sub-categories of woundedness,
disconnection, loss of self, and awareness of self.
Another, well-known, example of a grounded theory study, in a more
popularized book format, is Charmaz’s (1991) analysis of the experience
of living with chronic illness (see box).
Hermeneutic Approaches
A somewhat more flexible approach to qualitative research is represented
by approaches that bill themselves as hermeneutic (e.g., Packer &
Addison, 1989). Researchers who describe themselves in this way find
grounded theory and empirical phenomenology too restrictive and use a
wider range of methods. They argue that it is important to go
beyond the surface meaning of research
81
SOCIAL CONSTRUCTIONIST APPROACHES
81
81
SOCIAL CONSTRUCTIONIST APPROACHES
82
82
SOCIAL CONSTRUCTIONIST APPROACHES
83
traditional realist approaches, who are usually concerned with whether
statements expressed in language are true or not. Thus if a psychiatrist
says, ‘‘This patient is paranoid’’, a realist approach would be to see
whether the statement was accurate—i.e., is the patient paranoid or
not? In constructionism the focus shifts towards looking at how people
construct their arguments and what work their constructions do: for
example, what rhetorical devices does the psychiatrist use to convince
us of the validity of her position, that the patient is indeed paranoid
(see Harper, 1994)?
The type of focus on language also distinguishes the constructionist
approach from the phenomenological one. Phenomenologists and social
constructionists may share the assumption that objective reality is not
of primary concern. Furthermore, phenomenologists also use spoken
language, in the form of qualitative interviews, as their primary medium
of research. However, the phenomenologist is using the language to
understand the thoughts and feelings of the participants—to try and
understand their inner world. For radical constructionists, this act of
understanding, too, is a social construction, leaving us with only the
process of construction to study, especially as this plays out in
83
SOCIAL CONSTRUCTIONIST APPROACHES
84
language use. As Reicher (2000) puts it ‘‘. . . language is a form of social
action which we use in order to create our social world. The focus is on
how apparent descriptions serve to manage our social relations.
Psychological categories such as beliefs, desires, and even experience,
are only of interest in so far as participants themselves put them to use in
their discourse’’ (pp. 3–4).
The other feature of many constructionist theories is that they stress the
reflexivity (from ‘‘reflection,’’ a function of mirrors) of psychological
theorizing. By this is meant that psychologists are doing the theorizing,
but the psychologists themselves, as human beings, are also the object of
the theory. Psychological research is thus a circular process, which is
what the reflexivity metaphor is attempting to capture. Some
constructionists maintain that the existence of reflexivity fatally
undermines any claims of psychology to be an objective science.
Postmodernism
Social constructionism is closely aligned with the body of thought known
as postmodernism, or sometimes poststructuralism (the two terms
overlap, but are not synonymous). This can be rather difficult territory.
One major difficulty is that it is often hard to pin down exactly what many
of the authors writing in this tradition are actually saying, as their prose
style is often opaque, and the ratio of useful ideas to verbiage can seem
frustratingly low. However, postmodern thought is currently fashionable
and much discussed within several fields of study, so it is important to
come to grips with it.
A second difficulty, however, is that the term ‘‘postmodernism’’ itself is
hard to define. It refers to a rather loose collection of ideas that have
found expression in a number of different fields, e.g., literary theory,
sociology, and architecture. The key figures are all French: the literary
theorist Derrida, the historian Foucault, the psychoanalyst Lacan. Some of
its central themes are:
● A rejection of ‘‘grand theories’’ that provide overarching
explanations, such as psychoanalysis or Marxism; instead micro- or
composite theories are favored. This is coupled with a questioning of
the personal and social interests that lie behind scientific theories,
particularly where those theories seem to serve the interests of those
in power (Prilleltensky, 1997).
● An intellectual playfulness, that borrows from many different
traditions within the same piece of work. For example, postmodernist
architecture often ‘‘quotes’’ from earlier traditions, inserting a Gothic
turret here, a Georgian window there. This is exemplified in the
image of the qualitative researcher as ‘‘bricoleur,’’ a sort of handy-
person who uses whatever is at hand to construct things that are
useful but not elegant (Levi-Strauss, 1958/1963; McLeod, 2001).
● A focus on language. Lyotard (1979) borrows Wittgenstein’s phrase,
‘‘language games,’’ to capture both the aspect of playfulness and the
idea that language is governed by rules.
● The indeterminacy (ambiguity) of language. Post-modernists stress
that all communication carries multiple meanings, so that
84
SOCIAL CONSTRUCTIONIST APPROACHES
understanding is always 85
85
SOCIAL CONSTRUCTIONIST APPROACHES
86
an act of interpretation, and what one reader makes of a text may differ
from another reader’s understanding. Eagleton (1983) gives the
example of a sign seen in the London Underground: ‘‘Dogs must be
carried on escalators.’’ Does this mean one cannot step on the
escalator unless one is carrying a dog?
Constructionism is not identical to postmodernism, and it was first
articulated before postmodernism became popular (e.g., Berger &
Luckmann, 1966). However, the postmodernist viewpoint has been
adopted by many social constructionists (e.g., Gergen, 1994, 2001), and
it provides a useful framework in which to understand their ideas.
Critiques of Postmodernism
The postmodernist position (and also the extreme versions of
constructionism) have been fiercely criticized, in a debate which has
generated much heat, and perhaps a little light. The main lines of
argument are:
● The lack of interest in people’s mental states is open to the same
criticisms that were earlier leveled at the methodological
behaviorists, who adopted an identical stance for completely
different reasons (i.e., to become, as they saw it, more scientific). A
psychology that sidesteps the role of inner experiencing is severely
limited, and ultimately presents an alienating view of the person.
● Likewise, the non-realist emphasis can make research into an ivory
tower exercise. Reality is important, especially unpleasant reality.
Studying rape, child abuse, racism, or genocide purely from a
discursive viewpoint can easily seem to diminish their importance or
even appear to deny their existence or the need to prevent them.
● The underlying model of the person is that of a fragmented,
unintegrated self; it is not the model that many psychological
therapists, who are trying to help their clients feel more whole, would
endorse. Likewise, the person can be viewed as a manipulator of
language, whose goal is to manage the impression they make or to
get others to act in a certain way. This is not an image of human
beings that can support the enterprise of helping people lead more
fulfilling, meaningful lives.
● The language that postmodernists employ is often riddled with
impenetrable jargon, which seems designed to convey an impression
of erudition and profundity. Sokal and Bricmont (1999) exposed the
ridiculousness of much postmodernist writing, especially its use of
scientific metaphors, in their critique, Intellectual impostures.
Conclusion
In summary, the strong points of the constructionist position are that
they remind us to look closely and critically at how language is used to
construct reality and to accomplish practical purposes. What ‘‘position’’
is the speaker or writer trying to adopt, how is their language being
used to bolster this position, and what is
86
SOCIAL CONSTRUCTIONIST APPROACHES
87
Table 5.2 Main types of social constructionist
references
Conversation analysis Sacks (1995); Schegloff (1999)
Critical approaches Lather (1991); Reason and Rowan (1981)
Deconstructionism Slife and Williams (1995)
Discourse analysis Potter and Wetherell (1987)
Radical feminist research Belenky et al. (1986)
Social representations Farr and Moscovici (1984)
Discourse Analysis
There are many kinds of discourse analysis (see van Dijk, 1997a,
1997b); it is an interdisciplinary field spanning psychology, sociology,
communication,
87
SOCIAL CONSTRUCTIONIST APPROACHES
88
linguistics, and literature. Within psychology, the most popular
approach is that articulated by Potter and Wetherell (1987). In general,
discourse analysis involves rigorously examining texts, in order to
analyze the repertoires of discourse that a speaker is drawing upon, and
the kinds of ‘‘subject positions’’ that the speaker is adopting (see box
for an example).
Conversation Analysis
Although sometimes grouped with discourse analysis, conversation
analysis has its own tradition and its own particular methods. An
outgrowth of the work of sociologists Garfinkel and Goffman, it was
developed by Harvey Sacks (1995) as a rigorous method for identifying
the common conversational sequences and strategies used by people to
accomplish practical purposes. Conversation analysis attempts to
study how speakers perceive each other’s utterances, based on how
they respond to each other. Although it attempts to develop general
models of the strategies people use to accomplish practical work in
conversation, it emphasizes the ad hoc, contextually embedded nature
of ‘‘talk- in-interaction’’ (Schegloff, 1999). Over the past 25 years,
conversation analysis has built up a large repertoire of provisional
understandings of everyday and professional speech, including many
investigations of psychotherapy (see Mondada, 1998, for a review).
One interesting clinical application of conversation analysis is Rapley
and Antaki’s (1996) British study of how psychologists administered a
standardized quality-of-life instrument to people with learning
disabilities (mental retardation in US terminology). They demonstrated
how the interviewers’ questioning strategies and reformulations shaped
the answers that respondents produced. Their analysis challenged the
traditional notion of acquiescence in this population (see Chapter 6) and
suggested a more complex picture of the conversational strategies used
by both interviewers and respondents.
88
SOCIAL CONSTRUCTIONIST APPROACHES
89
Moscovici and promulgated by Farr (e.g., Farr & Moscovici, 1984), social
representations research involves the examination of how new ideas,
especially scientific ideas, are perceived and assimilated within the
culture at large. For instance, social representations researchers might
examine the metaphors that people use to understand psychological
disorders and their treatment.
Critical Approaches
Critical researchers attempt not only to understand but also to
emancipate their research participants; they try to minimize the
distinction between researcher and ‘‘subject’’ in order to create research
in which participant and researcher are co-researchers who interact as
equals. They argue that the act of studying another human being
establishes an alienated relationship between them: the researcher treats
the ‘‘subject’’ like an object.
This viewpoint is forcefully articulated by Reason and Rowan (1981). Their
approach is sometimes described as the human inquiry approach and
sometimes as new paradigm research (they use both labels in the title of
their book). As McLeod (2001) notes, human inquiry research emphasizes
disciplined subjec- tivity, respect for the whole person, linking research to
action, and creative forms of presenting the results of research (e.g., in
the form of art or poetry). An example is an investigation that Wilshaw
(1996) carried out in collaboration with a support group for individuals
who had been sexually abused as children. In this study, he acted as a co-
participant in the support group and as a research consultant; members
of the group selected the research questions, methods, and means of
presenting the results (which consisted in part of group members’
paintings, depicting the impact that their abuse had had on them).
Other critical research approaches include neo-Marxist approaches, as
well as participatory action research and Freirian research (Lather, 1991).
89
WAYS OF EVALUATING QUALITATIVE STUDIES
89
controlling, is seen as a masculine activity; communal research,
involving sensitivity and personal participation, is seen as feminine.
However, in a feminist critique of this stance, Peplau and Conrad (1989)
argue that attempts to identify a distinctive set of feminist research
methods are mistaken, and that feminist researchers should avail
themselves of the full range of research methods, including quantitative
and experimental approaches.
Deconstructionism
Finally, deconstructionist researchers engage in self-critique, embracing
a postmodern view of the research process. They see the major task of
researchers as being ‘‘deconstruction’’ of the cultural, social, or
epistemological assumptions of their work and that of others. They
embrace radical pluralism, and attempt to speak or give air to multiple
voices while eschewing any attempt to bring these voices together into
a single message. In essence, they attempt to mirror fragmented,
postmodern, multicultural society in their research. For example, a
deconstructionist researcher such as Lather (1991) might present her
findings as a kind of research collage.
Perhaps most importantly, deconstruction is an essential component of
the process of evaluating research, in which one attempts to identify
the implicit assumptions that drive a research study. Slife and Williams
(1995) provide an excellent introduction to this approach. In our view,
deconstructionism is less useful as a primary research method than as a
method for reflecting on and critiquing research. This takes us to our
final topic.
89
WAYS OF EVALUATING QUALITATIVE STUDIES
90
Table 5.3 Summary of Elliott et al.’s (1999) evolving guidelines
good and bad practice under each. In summary, their guidelines for
qualitative studies are:
Owning one’s perspective. The authors describe their theoretical
orientations and biases, in order to help readers evaluate the researchers’
interpretation of the data. For example, they would state if they were
coming to the research from a psychoanalytic, or from a feminist,
perspective.
Situating the sample. The authors describe the research participants so
that readers can judge how widely the findings might apply.
Grounding in examples. The authors provide enough examples of their
raw data to illustrate the analytic procedures used and to allow the reader
to evaluate their findings. They also stay close to the data; any
speculations that exceed the data are clearly labeled as such.
Providing credibility checks. The researchers use methods for checking
the credibility of the results, for example, analytic auditing (e.g., using
multiple researchers or an additional person who checks the results
against the data), triangulation (examining the phenomenon from
multiple, varied perspectives) and testimonial validity (checking the
results with the original informants or similar others).
Coherence. The interpretation of the data is coherent and integrated, but
at the same time it does not oversimplify the data.
Accomplishing general versus specific research tasks. If the research
aims to achieve a general understanding, then the appropriate range of
people or situations is sampled. If it aims to achieve a specific
understanding of a particular case, that case is described thoroughly
enough for the reader to gain a full understanding.
90
CONCLUSIONS
91
Resonating with the reader. From the point of view of the reader, the
results are not only believable but seem to capture or make sense of
the phenomenon, enabling the reader to understand the phenomenon
more fully.
CONCLUSIONS
Qualitative methods have now become much more fully accepted within
psychology, and the heat seems to be dying out of the old quantitative
versus qualitative debate. Research methodologists are now focusing
their attention on when best to use either a quantitative or a qualitative
approach, what is the appropriate qualitative method for any given
research question, and how best to appraise qualitative studies.
However, given that we have now described the fundamentals of both
quantitative and qualitative approaches, it is worth briefly considering
how researchers might decide between them.
CHAPTER SUMMARY
FURTHER READING
Many treatments of qualitative methods have recently been published.
Richardson (1996), Smith (in press), and Willig (2001) give accessible
treatments of the theory and practice of the commonly used approaches.
Some of the old stalwarts, such as Lincoln and Guba (1985) and Patton (2002),
still hold up, and Taylor and Bogdan’s (1998) sociologically oriented text
includes some illustrative studies, although their current edition omits the
fascinating and moving single case account (originally published in Bogdan &
Taylor, 1976) of an articulate ‘‘mentally retarded’’ man, ‘‘Ed Murphy’’, who
describes his life in state institutions. Potter and Wetherell (1987) is a core
reference for the social- psychological approach to discourse analysis. For an
extensive but accessible treatment of the quantitative versus qualitative
debate, see Bryman (1988) and Polkinghorne (1983). Since many qualitative
approaches have their roots in literary theory, it is also worth reading about
them in that context. Eagleton (1983) gives an excellent exposition and
critique of, among other things, phenomenology, hermeneutics, and
poststructuralism as applied to the analysis of literary texts.
93