Dung-Discourse Assignment
Dung-Discourse Assignment
Dung-Discourse Assignment
Abstract
This research is launched to research adverb which is considered . Ten conversations of
different topics in the text book are chosen at random for data analysis. Searle’s (1976)
classification of speech acts is applied as the framework for data analysis with a combination
with Bach and Harnish’s (1979) framework and appropriate modification. The results show that
representatives, directives, and expressives are the most commonly-used single speech act types.
Also, the possibility for speech act combinations in an utterance is really high with various forms
of combination.
1. Introduction
Discourse analysis is a new linguistics field realized in studies of different
disciplines (Nguyen Hoa, 2003; Hoang Van Van, 2006; Ton Nu My Nhat, 2010 …). There
have been a wide variety of approaches to discourse analysis such as contextual analysis,
grammatical analysis, cohesion analysis, conversational analysis, and pragmatic approach.
Among them, pragmatic approach has proven to be so strong a discipline, of which speech
act studies have been conducted worldwide, contributing a great deal to the interpretation
and use of speech acts across cultures.
This research contributes a specific investigation into speech acts realized in
conversations in the text book New Interchange 1, written by Richards et al., first
published in 1997 and introduced to Vietnam in 2005. The reason for choosing the
textbook for my study lies in the fact that it is a popular textbook used to teach English for
communication in plenty of schools and English centres in Vietnam. Thus, a better insight
into the use and interpretation of speech acts in New Interchange 1 is expected to facilitate
teachers and students using it. Also, it provides the researcher with sufficient spoken data
necessary for a study of speech act types. Besides, no studies of speech acts have been
conducted with the data in the textbook, as far as the researcher knows.
2. A review of notion and classification of speech acts
First suggested by Austin (1962), speech acts are defined as actions performed via
language and can be analyzed in three levels: locutionary act, illocutionary act, and
1
A Study of Speech Act Types in Conversations in New Interchange 1
perlocutionary act, with the first act being the production of a meaningful linguistic
utterance, the second the speaker’s meaning or intention via the utterance, and the last the
effect of that utterance on the hearer. Of the three levels, it is the illocutionary act that
counts because it conveys the speaker’s meaning (illocutionary force) which can be
realized in different language structures of various semantic meanings. It is for this reason
that Yule (1997: 52) claims, “‘speech act’ is generally interpreted quite narrowly to mean
only the illocutionary force of an utterance”.
Speech acts have been classified according to their functions (Austin, 1962; Searle,
1976; Bach and Harnish, 1979). One of the widely known and applicable classifications of
speech acts is suggested by Searle (1976) who classifies speech acts into declarations,
representatives, expressives, directives, and commissives which are, to some extent, in turn
similar to the terms effectives, constatives, acknowledgements, directives, and commisives
in Bach and Harnish’s classification (1979: 41). There are some differences in the two
ways of classification, however. For example, while Searle (1976) puts suggestions and
predictions in the categories of directives and commissives, Bach and Harnish (1979) put
both of them in constatives, the similar term to representatives by Searle.
Speech acts have also been classified into two broader categories, named: direct
and indirect speech acts by Yule (1997) and others, but it is not the focus of this research.
The classified categories of speech act can, however, be criticized for their limited
ability to cover all communicative functions, their analysis of out-of-context utterances
failing to reflect the correct illocutionary acts, and their exclusion of possibility for
combination of several illocutionary acts in one utterance Gajaseni (1994, in Yu, 1999: 15-
16).
2
A Study of Speech Act Types in Conversations in New Interchange 1
1 2 11 72 Describing work
2 3 10 67 Shopping
3 2 9 75 Likes and dislikes
4 2 7 68 Invitations
5 2 7 62 Family
6 2 10 83 Describing exercise
7 2 8 73 On vacation
8 2 9 66 The neighborhood
9 2 8 72 Describing cities
10 2 7 69 Health problems
Total 86 707 10
3
A Study of Speech Act Types in Conversations in New Interchange 1
Table 2: Speech act types (Adapted from Searle, 1976 and Bach and Harnish, 1979)
Data analysis is done both qualitatively and quantitatively with the statistic,
descriptive methods being employed. The unit of analysis is utterance existing in one
speaking turn and varying from one word to a phrase, a clause, a sentence, and a
combination of sentences. The illocutionary force is based on for the classification of
speech acts in the collected data
As shown in Table 3, there are 54 single speech acts out of the 86 turns under
4
A Study of Speech Act Types in Conversations in New Interchange 1
investigation, accounting for 62.8%. The combined group also appears quite frequently
with 32 combinations, making up 37.2%.
N/86 % N/86 %
As regards the single speech act types which are described in Table 4, there are no
single declarations and commissives. While the absence of declarations can be predicted
because of their conventional characteristics in felicity conditions, it is a surprise that we
cannot find any commissives. Perhaps, it results from the limited source of the collected
data.
Directives are the second most frequently-used single speech acts, most of which
can be noticed in questions (Ex 5, 6, 7) and one in commands (Ex 8).
5
A Study of Speech Act Types in Conversations in New Interchange 1
Expressives account for 10.5 %, being realized in the collected data when people
greet (Ex 9), accept thanks (Ex 10), thank (Ex 11), express likes (Ex 12), and so on.
Returning now to combined speech acts, it can be seen in Table 5 that there are 5
combinations of speech acts realized in the collected data and abbreviated as Rep + Exp +
Dir, Dir + Rep, Exp + Dir, Exp + Rep, and Rep + Com.
Rep + Exp + Dir Dir + Rep Exp + Dir Exp + Rep Rep + Com
As is shown in Table 5, the highest percentage of the combined group, 14%, goes
to Dir + Rep, followed by Exp + Dir at 11.6%, Exp + Rep at 7%, and Rep + Exp + Dir
6
A Study of Speech Act Types in Conversations in New Interchange 1
3.5 %. Rep + Dir just accounts for a small percentage of 1.2%, with only one
representative.
To be more specific, the 14% of Dir + Rep (also used to mean Rep + Dir),
representing the combination of directives and representatives, are illustrated with
different sub-speech acts of each type, as can be seen in Ex 13, Ex 14, and Ex 15.
- Ex 13: It’s $ 499. Would you like to try it on? ([Rep + Dir], Conversation 2)
- Ex 14: Look! These jackets are nice. Which one do you like better? ([Dir + Rep
+Dir], Conversation 2)
- Ex 15: I have two tickets to the Phantom of the Opera on Friday night. Would
you like to go? ([Rep + Dir], Conversation 4)
Ex 16 and Ex 17 below are given for the demonstration of the second combined
group, Exp + Dir (referring to combinations of expressives and directives), accounting for
11.6%. “Really?” in Ex 17 is in the structure of a question, but I do not put in the group of
directives but expressives because based on the illocutionary force of the utterance in that
context, it is used to express a surprise.
- Ex 16: Thanks. I’d love to. What time is the show? ([Exp + Dir], Conversation 4)
Following are some examples to illustrate other combined groups, including Exp +
Rep meaning combinations between expressives and representatives (Ex 18, Ex 19), Rep
+ Exp + Dir for combinations of representatives, expressives and directives (Ex 20, Ex
21), and Rep + Com for combinations between representatives and commissives (Ex 22).
- Ex 18: Well, I prefer the leather one. It’s more attractive than the wool one.
([Exp + Rep], Conversation 2)
- Ex 20: Yes, it’s a great job. And I love it. What do you do? ([Rep + Exp + Dir],
7
A Study of Speech Act Types in Conversations in New Interchange 1
Conversation 1)
- Ex 21: The Cranberies. I love their music. How about you? Do you like them?
([Rep + Exp + Dir], Conversation 3)
- Ex 22: No problem, Rod. I won’t play too hard. ([Rep + Com], Conversation 6).
6. Summary
The representatives and expressives are realized with various sub-speech acts,
whereas the directives are mainly in the forms of question.
There are various ways for speech act combination, of which the most possible
ones are Dir + Rep, Exp + Dir, and Exp +Rep.
7. Implications
The results of this study can help to suggest the following implications:
In teaching English, the analysis of speech act types can help teachers have a
better insight into their use and interpretation of speech acts, thus improving
their quality of teaching. Teachers are, hence, expected to study speech act types
8
A Study of Speech Act Types in Conversations in New Interchange 1
In doing research of speech act types, it is the illocutionary force that must be
based on for the appropriate classification.
The possibility for speech act combinations is really high and needs to be taken
into consideration for any research of this type.
9
A Study of Speech Act Types in Conversations in New Interchange 1
References
Austin, J. L. (1962). How to do things with words. New York: Oxford University Press.
Bach, K. and Harnish, R. (1979). Linguistic communication and speech acts. Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
Hoang Van Van (2006). Introducing discourse analysis. Hanoi: Hanoi Open
University
Nguyễn Hòa (2003). Phân tích diễn ngôn: Một số vấn đề lý luận và phương pháp. Hà Nội : Nxb
ĐHQH Hà Nội.
Richards, J. C. et al. (2005). New Interchange: English for International Communication (Book
1). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Searle, J. R. (1976). The classification of illocutionary acts. Language in Society 5, pp. 1-23.
Ton Nu My Nhat (2010). An introduction to discourse analysis. Quy Nhon: Quy Nhon University.
Yu, M. C. (1999). Cross-cultural and interlanguage pragmatics: Developing communicative
competence in a second language, PhD. Dissertation. Harvard: Harvard University.
Yule, G. (1997). Pragmatics. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
10
A Study of Speech Act Types in Conversations in New Interchange 1
Conversation 2 – Shopping, p. 17
Anne: Look! These jackets are nice. Which one do you like better? [Dir + Rep]
Sue: I like the wool one better. [Exp]
Anne: Really? Why? [Exp + Dir]
Sue: It looks warmer. [Rep]
Anne: Well, I prefer the leather one. It's more attractive than the wool one. [Exp + Rep]
Sue: Hmm. There's no price tag. [Rep]
Anne: Excuse me. How much is this jacket? [Exp + Dir]
Clerk: It's $ 499. Would you like to try it on? [Rep + Dir]
Anne: Oh, no. That's OK! But thank you anyway. [Rep + Exp]
Clerk: You're welcome. [Exp]
11
A Study of Speech Act Types in Conversations in New Interchange 1
Conversation 4 – Invitations, p. 23
Dave: I have tickets to The Phantom of the Opera on Friday night. Would you like to go?
[Rep + Dir]
Susan: Thanks. I'd love to. What time is the show? [Exp + Dir]
Dave: It's at 8:00. [Rep]
Susan: That sounds great. So, do you want to meet at the theater? [Exp + Dir]
Dave: Uh, I'd like to, but I have to work late. [Exp + Dir]
Susan: Oh, that's OK. Let's just meet at the theater before the show, around 7:30. [Rep +
Dir]
Dave: That sounds fine. [Rep]
Conversation 5 – Family, p. 29
Rita: Tell me about your brother and sister, Sue. [Dir]
Sue: Well, my sister is a lawyer. [Rep]
Rita: Really? Does she live here in Seattle? [Exp + Dir]
Sue: Yes, she does. But she’s working in Washington DC, right now. Her job is top
secret. [Rep]
Rita: Wow! And what does your brother do? [Exp + Dir]
Sue: He’s a painter. He’s working in Argentina this month. He has an exhibition there.
[Rep]
Rita: What an interesting family! [Exp]
Conversation 7 – On vacation, p. 43
Mike: Hi, Celia. How was your trip to the United States? [Exp + Dir]
Celia: It was terrific. I really enjoyed it. [Rep + Exp]
Mike: Great. How long were you away? [Rep + Dir]
Celia: I was there for about three weeks. [Rep]
Mike: That's a long time! Was the weather OK? [Rep + Dir]
Celia: Yes, most of the time. But it snowed a lot in Chicago. [Rep]
Mike: So, what was the best thing about your trip? [Dir]
Celia: Oh, that's difficult to say. But I guess I like Nashville the best. [Rep + Exp]
12
A Study of Speech Act Types in Conversations in New Interchange 1
13