Memory
Memory
One theory, a simple one agreed by most psychologists, was used in the definition of memory.
Three distinct processes of memory have been identified. These are an encoding process, a
storage process, and retrieval. Encoding is the process of receiving sensory input and
transforming it into a form, or code, which can be stored; storage is the process of actually
putting coded information into memory; and retrieval is the process of gaining access to stored,
To illustrate these three memory processes, imagine that on the way to work, your car was
bumped by a bus and slightly dented. You encoded your visual impressions of the accident in a
form that you could store in your memory. Later, when you talk to the insurance adjuster, you
This simple process theory helps explain why your memory of an accident may be inaccurate.
The encoding you do may be faulty, perhaps due to the emotion and distress you experience at
the time of the accident; important encoded information may not be well fixed in your memory,
or it may be distorted by events occurring after the accident; your retrieval of the information
stored in your memory may be biased; or, since the processes of encoding, storage, and retrieval
are related, all three memory processes may be faulty. Memory is seldom an accurate record of
INFORMATION-PROCESSING THEORIES
Imagine yourself a device something like a digital computer that takes items of information in;
processes them in steps, or stages, and then produces an output. Models of memory based on this
idea are called information-processing theories. A number of such models of memory have been
proposed. We will use one of the most prominent and influential of these models the
1
information-processing theory developed by Richard Atkinson and Richard Shiffrin (1968)-to
In the Atkinson-Shiffrin theory, memory starts with a sensory input from the environment. This
input is held for a very brief time-several seconds at most-in a sensory register associated with
the sensory channels (vision, hearing, touch, and so forth). Information that is attended to and
recognized in the sensory register may be passed on to short-term memory (STM), where it is
held for perhaps 20 or 30 seconds. Some of the information reaching short-term memory is
processed by being rehearsed that is, by having attention focused on it, perhaps by being
repeated over and over, or perhaps by being processed in some other way that will link it up with
other information already stored in memory. Information that is rehearsed may then be passed
along to long-term memory; information not so processed is lost. When items of information are
placed in long-term-memory, they are organized into categories where they may reside for days,
months, years, or for a life-time. When you remember something, a representation of the item is
There are some interesting parallels between this information processing theory and some of the
brain processes involved in memory. This model of memory fits well with our subjective
impressions when we are trying to remember something. Imagine yourself asking someone for a
telephone number you d not know. (It's in the other person's long-term memory, but not in
yours.)
The person tells you the number and off you go to dial it. Unless you rehearse number you will
probably forget it soon after hearing it. If something interrupts you on your way to the telephone,
thus interrupting your rehearsal you will probably forget the number. Or imagine yourself at a
party: Unless you take pains to rehearse the names of the new people you meet you will not
2
remember them long. Without rehearsal and with the information overload caused by all the
things happening at the party, transfer to long-term memory will not occur.
Retrieval from long-term memory is also experienced subjectively.Try to remember where your
bedroom was located in all of the houses or apartments you have lived in. As you do this, you
will find yourself searching through your long-term memory, and you will probably develop
search strategy. Perhaps you will search chronologically from the first Be you remember to later
ones. You may then imagine the house and locate your bedroom in relation to the other rooms. If
you cannot remember a bedroom using one search method, you may shift to another search
strategy. In any case, you will have the subjective impression of having searched through your
storehouse of memories.
Information can be held for a very brief time in the sensory channels themselves. This storage
function of the sensory channels is called the sensory register. Most of the information briefly
held in the sensory register is lost; what has been briefly stored simply decays from the register.
However, we pay attention to and recognize some of the information in the sensory register;
when we do this, the attended-to information is passed on to short-term memory for further
processing.
Some ingenious experiments have shown that the visual sensory register holds information for
up to about 1 second, while the auditory (hearing) register holds information somewhat longer-
up to about 4 or 5 seconds. Studies with the visual sensor register have also shown that it can
hold at least 11 to 16 items of information during the second before it loses the information
through decay. Furthermore, in vision at least, the sensory storage seems to be in the form of a
faint image called an iconic image (from the Greek word meaning "likeness") which is a copy of
replaced by new information. Although it is rare, some people do have what is properly called
eidetic imagery, or the ability to access a visual sensory memory over a long period of time.
Although the popular term photographic memory is often used to mean this rare ability, some
people claiming to have photographic memory actually mean that they have an extremely good
memory. Having a very good memory and having eidetic imagery ability are two very different
things. People with eidetic imagery ability might be able to look quickly at a page in a book, then
by focusing on a blank wall or piece of paper, “read” the words from the image that still lingers
Echoic Sensory Memory Another type of sensory memory is echoic memory, or the brief
memory of something a person has heard. A good example of echoic memory is the “What?”
parent,roommate, or friend walks up and says something to you. You sit there for a second two,
Echoic memory’s capacity is limited to what can be heard at any one moment and is smaller than
the capacity of iconic memory, although it lasts longer—about 2 to 4 seconds. Echoic memory is
very useful when a person wants to have meaningful conversations with others. It allows the
person to remember what someone said just long enough to recognize the meaning of a phrase.
Short-term memory
Short-term memory is a limited capacity store that can maintain and rehearse information for
about 20 to 30 seconds. It is first of the two main systems for storing information and also known
Because the information that is stored briefly in our sensory memory consists of representations
of raw sensory stimuli, it is not meaningful to us. In order for us to make sense of it and to allow
4
for the possibility of long-term retention, the information must be transferred to the next stage of
memory short-term memory. Short-term memory is the memory store in which material first has
It appears that short term memory can encode only about seven separate items (plus or minus
two) - limited capacity and can hold them for only a limited time. Estimates of how long
information can be retained in STM vary from about a few seconds to almost a minute. Since
seven in the typical number of separate items in a telephone number, STM is often referred to as
telephone number memory. Thus, if you were verbally given a phone number, you could most
probably you could hold it in STM long enough to pick up the phone and dial the number,
However, if the other party didn't answer the ring or there was a busy signal, you probably could
Even though STM is restricted to about seven items; the items may be grouped together or
chunked in this way several items may be packaged together and processed as a single element.
For example you might be asked to repeat a series of nine digits, a number that is pushing the
limit of the magical value of seven plus or minus two by chunking the digits into groups of three,
however you then have to hold only three separate items in mind, not a difficult task The same is
true for letters Whereas a random set of seventeen letters pushes for beyond its limit, by
chunking the letters, retention can be made easy. Take the letters OPECIBMSONARRADAR,
which is a total of seventeen letters. If you chunk them into OPEC, IBM, SONAR, and RADAR,
entering short-term memory, the information cannot be held there very lang. Most psychologists
believe that information in short-term memory is lost after fifteen to twenty-five seconds-unless
Rehearsal accomplishes two things. First, as long as the information repeated, it is kept alive in
short-term memory. More important, however, rehearsal allows us to transfer the material into
long-term memory.
Whether the transfer is made from short-to long term memory seems to depend largely on the
kind of rehearsal that is carried out. If the material is simply repeated over and over again-as we
might do with a telephone number while we rush from the phone book to the telephone-it is kept
current in short-term memory, but it will not necessarily be placed in long-term memory. Instead,
as soon as we stop dialing, the number is likely to be replaced by other information and will be
completely forgotten.
On the other hand, if the information in short-term memory is rehearsed using a process called
Elaborative rehearsal occurs when the material is considered and organized in some fashion. The
organization might include expanding the information to make it fit into a logical framework,
linking it to another memory, turning it into an image, or transforming it in some other way. For
example, a list of
vegetables to be
purchased at a store
could be woven
together in memory
prepare an elaborate
6
linked to the items bought on an earlier shopping trip; or they could be thought of in terms of the
Although short-term memory has traditionally been considered as a single. system, more recent
evidence suggests that it may actually consist of several components. According to psychologist
Alan Baddeley, short-term memory is better thought of as a three-part working memory. In this
view, one component is the central executive, which coordinates the material to focus on during
reasoning and decision-making. The central executive makes use of two sub-components: the
visuo-spatial sketch pad and the phonological loop. The visuo-spatial sketch pad concentrates on
visual and spatial information, while the phonological loop is responsible for holding and
Some researchers suspect that a breakdown in the central executive may result in the memory
losses that are characteristic of Alzheimer's disease, the progressively degenerative disorder that
Long-term Memory (LTM) is an unlimited capacity store that can hold information over lengthy
periods of time. The information from STM can be passed along to be processed and
consolidated into long-term memory (LTM). Encoding into LTM can typically take upto twenty
minutes, but this time interval can be dramatically shortened in certain situations such as in flash
bulb storage. LTM has the potential for holding the encoded information for a lifetime. Material
that makes its way from short-term memory to long-term memory enters a storehouse of almost
unlimited capacity. Like a new book delivered to a library, the information in long-term memory
7
TWO LTM SYSTEMS
Researchers in the area both cognitive and brain science are now suggesting that we may have at
least two long term memory systems, one for declarative memory and the other for procedural
1. Declarative Memory
stated or declared in the sense of a fact, proposition, or image. There are two types of declarative
consists of one's memory for personal past episodes in one's life. It is made up mostly of images
from personal experiences organized on the basis of when and where they occurred -
chronologically organized.
It is as though our lifetime experiences were on a long reel of movie film, and we rewind the reel
to go back and look at the images on a few frames. Your ability to recall what you did last night
or last week or even years ago is based on your retrieval of episodic memory.
Try to remember your childhood home, its colour and shape, your bedroom and where your
parents slept. Or you can recall your school days, the classroom you were in, the area you played
in at recess and your first fight with your neighbourhood bully. These memories are stored as
8
images and are related to one another by space and time, and so if you shut your eyes and relax,
b. Semantic memory: It contains our storehouse of words and the meaning of words, facts
general information, concepts, and the rules for learning most of the things that we were
“memoree" is misspelled.
The organization of this memory is grounded, not on an ordered - time basis, but on net works of
interconnected relationships and ideas called schemata, as a plural, or schema in the singular. For
example you might have a schema which covers virtually everything you know about football:
that there are referees, that there are eleven players on each side, that players may get injured,
that a touchdown counts for six points and soon. That is, anybody of information to a central
2. Procedural memory
It contains memory of motor skills, typically learned through repetitive practice and
conditioning. A person relies on procedural memory when tying shoes, playing a musical
Any one who doubts that procedural memory is a valid phenomenon should try asking typists
where certain letters on the keyboard. Watch as their fingers flex out the answer. That this
individuals who have undergone bruin surgery and apparently, on occasion, lost one type of
memory but not the other. Also, patients with Alzheimer's disease often show no impairment of
their procedural memory, but cannot recall the names of their children.
9
Recalling from long-term- memory
Have you ever tried to remember someone's name, convinced that you knew it but unable to
recall it no matter how hard you tried. This not infrequent occurrence- known as the tip-of the
tongue phenomenon-exemplifies the difficulties that can occur in retrieving information stored in
long-term memory. Thus is the inability to recall information that one realizes one knows-a
a. Retrieval Cues
The capacity of long-term memory is vast, given the broad range of people's experiences and
educational backgrounds. For instance, if you are like the average college student, your
vocabulary includes some 50,000 words, you know hundreds of mathematical "facts," and you
are able to conjure up images-such as the way your childhood home looked-with no trouble at
all. In fact, simply cataloging all your memories would probably take years of work.
How do we sort through this vast array of material and retrieve specific information at the
appropriate time? One of the major ways is through the use of retrieval cue. A retrieval cue is a
stimulus that allows us to recall information that is located in long-term memory more easily It
may be a word, an emotion, a sound, whatever the specific cue, a memory will suddenly come to
mind when the retrieval cue is present. For example, the smell of rousting turkey may evoke,
Retrieval cues guide people through the information stored in long-term memory in much the
same way as the cards in a card catalogue guide people through a library. They are particularly
important when we are making an effort to recall information, as opposed to our being asked to
10
contrast, recognition occurs when people are presented with a stimulus and asked whether they
have been exposed to it previously, or are asked to identify it from a list of alternatives.
b. Flashbulb memories
Flashbulb memories are memories centered on a specific, important, or surprising even that are
so vivid it is as if they represented a snapshot of the event. Several types of flashbulb memories
are common among college students. For example, involvement in a car accident, meeting one's
roommate for the first time, and the day of graduation are all flashbulb memories.
11